Next Article in Journal
Relationship between Thinking Dispositions, Working Memory, and Critical Thinking Ability in Adolescents: A Longitudinal Cross-Lagged Analysis
Previous Article in Journal
Seeing without a Scene: Neurological Observations on the Origin and Function of the Dorsal Visual Stream
Previous Article in Special Issue
Exploring Actual and Presumed Links between Accurately Inferring Contents of Other People’s Minds and Prosocial Outcomes
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Opinion

Ability-Related Emotional Intelligence: An Introduction

by
Michael D. Robinson
Psychology, NDSU Department 2765, P.O. Box 6050, Fargo, ND 58108-6050, USA
J. Intell. 2024, 12(5), 51; https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12050051
Submission received: 2 April 2024 / Revised: 15 May 2024 / Accepted: 17 May 2024 / Published: 19 May 2024

Abstract

:
Emotionally intelligent people are thought to be more skilled in recognizing, thinking about, using, and regulating emotions. This construct has garnered considerable interest, but initial enthusiasm has faded and it is time to take stock. There is consensus that ability-related measures of emotional intelligence (EI) can be favored to self-report tests, in part because the resulting scores cannot be equated with personality traits. However, there are questions surrounding measurement as well as predictive value. Experts in the field were encouraged to chart new directions, with the idea that these new directions could reinvigorate EI scholarship. Special Issue papers speak to theory, mechanism, measurement, and training. In addition, these papers seek to forge links with research traditions focused on interpersonal perception, emotional awareness, and emotion regulation. As a result of these efforts, new insights into what EI is and how it works can be anticipated in upcoming years.

1. Special Issue on Ability-Related Emotional Intelligence: An Introduction

Emotions figure prominently in many realms such as decision making (Lerner et al. 2015), relationships (Engelberg and Sjöberg 2004), and well-being (Watson 2000). Owing to such links, emotion-related capacities might be expected to help individuals succeed rather than fail as they negotiate the complexities of daily life. Modern interest in emotional intelligence (EI)—which is thought to encompass skills related to the identification, understanding, management, and use of emotions (Kotsou et al. 2019)—began with a definitional effort by Salovey and Mayer (1990). Goleman (1995) then popularized the construct by arguing, without sufficient evidence, that EI could be more important than general mental ability in determining whether lives were successful or not. These popularization efforts, which culminated in a Times magazine piece and an Oprah Winfrey episode (Roberts et al. 2010), inevitably led some to suggest that interest in EI could be likened to a fad that would surely perish, like the dodo bird (Antonakis et al. 2009). This has not happened, but there are major questions concerning the construct as well as its value in predicting real-world outcomes (Zeidner et al. 2008).
It is often suggested that there is confusion about whether emotional intelligence should be thought of in terms of personality traits, which can be self-reported, or abilities, which require performance-based tests (Matthews et al. 2004). Although it is useful to compare the predictive validities of these two types of tests (MacCann et al. 2020a; Martins et al. 2010), there seems to be enough consensus to state that these two modes of assessment need to be distinguished from each other, in part because self-reports of EI rarely correlate highly with ability-based assessments of EI (Roberts et al. 2010). In many cases, self-reports of EI display greater predictive validity (e.g., Martins et al. 2010), but such tests also correlate highly with standard personality trait measures, rendering their discriminant validity suspect (Joseph et al. 2015). And, if one endorses an ability-based perspective on EI, which one arguably should (Mayer et al. 2008), the field will need to prioritize the ability-based model and its assessments (Daus and Ashkanasy 2005; Roberts et al. 2010). The current Special Issue does so.
Ability-based tests seek to determine whether individuals are good at perceiving emotions, whether they understand how emotions work, and whether they can (at least as inferred from their responses to standardized test materials) manage emotions in effective ways (Joseph and Newman 2010). These tests, more or less, assess emotion-related knowledge and its application (Hoemann et al. 2021) and they seek to place individuals along a continuum, from low to medium to high levels of emotion-related ability (Joseph and Newman 2010). The first widely used test was the Multifactor Emotional Intelligence Scale (MEIS: Mayer et al. 1999). This test was followed by the Mayer–Salovey–Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT: Mayer et al. 2003), the Situational Test of Emotional Understanding (STEU: MacCann and Roberts 2008), the Situational Test of Emotion Management (STEM: MacCann and Roberts 2008), the North Dakota Emotional Abilities Test (NEAT: Krishnakumar et al. 2016), and the Geneva Emotional Competence Test (GECo: Schlegel and Mortillaro 2019). Assessment-inclined researchers have also developed a number of tests of emotion recognition ability, which will relate to the perception branch of EI (Mayer et al. 2003), including the Matsumoto and Ekman’s Japanese and Caucasian Brief Affect Recognition Test (JACBART: Matsumoto et al. 2000), the Multimodal Emotion Recognition Test (MERT: Bänziger et al. 2009), and the Geneva Emotion Recognition Test (GERT: Schlegel et al. 2014). These tests tend to correlate with each other, but perhaps not so highly that the tests could be considered interchangeable (Krishnakumar et al. 2016; Mayer et al. 2016).
Goleman (1995) proposed that emotional intelligence would prove to be a strong predictor of workplace success, relationship success, and well-being. We have now conducted enough research to evaluate this proposal. Ability-based assessments of EI have displayed some predictive power with respect to workplace performance (O’Boyle et al. 2011), relationship quality (Lopes et al. 2004), and well-being (Sánchez-Álvarez et al. 2016), but these correlations are often fairly small (around .2) as well as inconsistent (Miners et al. 2018; Roberts et al. 2010). To give some examples, Di Fabio and Kenny (2016) found correlations between the MSCEIT and well-being in the .04 range (very small) and Miao et al. (2017) reported, in a meta-analysis, that ability EI was a weak predictor of workplace citizenship behavior (r = .17) and a non-significant predictor of counterproductive work behavior (r = .01). Such weak correlations often disappear when controlling for personality and/or cognitive ability (Roberts et al. 2010) and there appears to be uncertainty as to what to do next (e.g., Côté 2014; Matthews et al. 2012; Mayer et al. 2016; Ybarra et al. 2014). Special Issue papers provide relevant answers.

2. In Search of Theory

Please see Table 1 for some of the major questions that will be considered in this Special Issue. To begin, much of the knowledge that people have—such as concerning countries in Africa or types of tea—could be considered relatively circumscribed, barely affecting their lives as a whole. People who score high in emotional intelligence presumably have more extensive or accurate knowledge about emotion, yet much of this knowledge may be largely semantic in nature, raising questions about whether or how this knowledge affects the course or tenor of lives, whether in the moment or over longer time frames (Ybarra et al. 2014). Among other points, it should probably be recognized that most behaviors are multiply-determined (e.g., by the situation, by personality, by cognitive abilities) and EI-related influences could be subtle, depending on the situation and/or the behavior (Mayer et al. 2016).
Given such complexities (Mayer et al. 2016), we will simply need to develop some theoretical perspectives on EI, which are surprisingly scarce, in order to understand what these individual differences should predict. Some theorizing could be imported from personality psychology, social psychology, the emotion literature, and/or clinical science. As an example, research on reactive aggression (Wilkowski and Robinson 2010) and emotional impulsivity (Carver et al. 2009) makes the point that emotions often trigger urges to act in an impulsive manner, owing to their links to primitive approach and avoidance systems (Carver et al. 2009; Frijda 2010). People with higher levels of EI, because they possess more extensive knowledge about emotion and its management, may be capable of down-regulating their tendencies toward emotional impulsivity in ways that people with lower levels of EI cannot (Heatherton and Wagner 2011). In support of such theorizing, research has shown that high-EI individuals are less vulnerable to reactive aggression (Gutiérrez-Cobo et al. 2018), distress-influenced suicidal behavior (Cha and Nock 2009), and counterproductive work behavior, particularly when triggered by job negative affect (Krishnakumar et al. 2017).
A related line of work has started to show that individual differences in ability EI, but not self-reported EI, can be linked to cognitive control in emotion-related contexts (Checa and Fernández-Berrocal 2019; Gutiérrez-Cobo et al. 2017). What these process-related abilities would predict in more molar terms is not entirely certain, but these abilities could explain why levels of EI seem to be beneficial to performance under conditions of stress (Udayar et al. 2020). As Udayar et al. (2020) emphasize, retaining control under stressful circumstances could impart a certain sense of self-efficacy in handling emotional arousal, which should render behavior more effective (Bandura 1982). Relatedly, one might expect high-EI individuals to gravitate toward rather than away from emotional stimuli as they would be more confident in their abilities to handle the resulting emotional arousal (Appel et al. 2012).
As suggested by the first two Special Issue papers, another sort of theory is possible. As the materials presented on EI tests require individuals to attach emotional meaning to events or stimuli, it is reasonable to suggest that high-EI individuals (relative to low-EI individuals) are more skilled at doing so. Assuming that similar skills are applied in daily life, the relevant skills should result in higher levels of well-being (e.g., experiences of positive affect) under favorable circumstances, but lower levels of well-being (e.g., experiences of negative affect) under unfavorable circumstances (Cacioppo and Berntson 1999). This framework can explain why high-EI individuals sometimes display higher levels of emotional reactivity in response to stressful events (Bechtoldt and Schneider 2016; Ciarrochi et al. 2002; Matthews et al. 2006). It can also explain the links between EI and well-being (Sánchez-Álvarez et al. 2016), with the presumption that positive events (which would generate higher levels of positive affect among high-EI individuals) tend to be more common than negative events (Alves et al. 2017).
Viewed another way, though, average tendencies (e.g., to report higher or lower levels of well-being in some type of general sense) should not be emphasized. Rather, EI should be associated with dynamic operations (MacCann et al. 2020b), sometimes linking positive evaluations to current conditions (when they are pleasant) and sometimes linking negative evaluations to current conditions (when they are unpleasant). In other words, variations in EI should produce emotional states that are “attuned” to current conditions, as emphasized in functional accounts of emotion (Keltner and Gross 1999). A related construct is psychological flexibility. According to this line of theorizing, psychological health is marked by flexibility, meaning that the person is attuned to situational demands and capable of reconfiguring the self to respond to them (Kashdan and Rottenberg 2010). An important component of psychological flexibility is emotional flexibility, defined in terms of situation-appropriate emotional states (Beshai et al. 2018; Hardy and Segerstrom 2017). Emotional intelligence is very likely to be linked to emotional flexibility, but the implications of this connection are just now being studied (Vanuk et al. 2019).
The point of this section has not been to argue for any particular theory of ability EI. Rather, it has been to argue that we need some theorizing, at the present time, so that we can better understand what EI is and what it should do. People with low versus high levels of EI are likely to differ in multiple ways and linking these variations to theory will allow us to make new predictions that can reinvigorate the field.

3. Mechanisms and Processes

The “Big Idea” approach contends that emotional intelligence will lead to success in one’s life (Goleman 1995). Given the modest nature of the results that have followed from this perspective (Miners et al. 2018; Ybarra et al. 2014), and given the need for theorizing concerning this class of individual differences, it would seem valuable to identify mechanisms or processes that may represent more proximal, and therefore reliable, correlates. Above, we suggested that emotional intelligence may facilitate processing under emotional circumstances (Checa and Fernández-Berrocal 2019), likely giving rise to a sense of emotion-related self-efficacy, which should benefit self-regulation and performance under stressful circumstances (Schwarzer 2001; Udayar et al. 2020).
Other mechanisms and processes also suggest themselves. When people feel self-efficacious (which we suggest should be linked to higher levels of EI), they are likely to tackle stressful circumstances using a mode of coping termed problem-focused coping (Lazarus and Folkman 1984). This form of coping tends to be adaptive because problems get resolved, clearing a pathway for long-term goal pursuit (Carver and Scheier 2014). In support of this model, MacCann et al. (2011) linked variations in ability EI to variations in problem-focused coping, which in turn predicted higher grade-point averages among students. Results of this type have been replicated (MacCann et al. 2020a) and they suggest that EI is likely to be beneficial in the many circumstances in which problem-focused coping can be advocated (see Carver and Scheier 2014, for a relevant analysis).
Other relevant mechanisms can be found in the psychological flexibility literature. According to this clinical model, human suffering increases as the result of at least two pathological processes (Hayes et al. 2012). Some people are scared of their feelings (i.e., experiential avoidance), which can lead them to restrict their lives in unfortunate manners (Kashdan et al. 2013). Although we are not aware of research linking EI to experiential avoidance, it seems probable that low-EI individuals would be more vulnerable to experiential avoidance given their relative incapacity to understand their feelings. As indicated previously, EI should also be related to emotional flexibility (versus lack of flexibility), defined in terms of situation-appropriate emotional reactions. Following the lead of Klein et al. (2023), high-EI individuals should display intense, but short-lived reactions to both pleasant and unpleasant events.
One can also draw from the emotion regulation literature in making predictions about how EI should operate. According to Gross (2002), emotions can be regulated at various stages of the emotion elicitation process. Much of this research has contrasted to the mechanism of reappraisal, which involves altering appraisals of an eliciting event to alter one’s emotions with suppression, which involves inhibiting the expression of emotions that are felt. Reappraisal can intervene earlier in the emotion eliciting sequence than suppression can, and a considerable body of evidence has pointed to the adaptive nature of reappraisal relative to suppression (John and Gross 2004). There are multiple reasons for thinking that high-EI individuals should be capable of regulating their emotions in more skilled manners (Peña-Sarrionandia et al. 2015), and an emerging body of evidence does in fact suggest positive relationships between ability EI and reappraisal as well as negative relationships between ability EI and suppression (e.g., Megías-Robles et al. 2019; Śmieja et al. 2011). This model can be extended to the correlates of reappraisal and suppression, which include well-being and social behavior (John and Gross 2004).
By regulating negative emotions, high-EI individuals may typically experience lower levels of negative affect (MacCann et al. 2020b). They may also experience higher levels of positive affect, possibly through mechanisms that link EI to engagement with the environment (Robinson et al. 2022). These associations could in turn mediate relationships between variations in ability EI and variations in life satisfaction, eudaimonia, and meaning (Fernández-Berrocal and Extremera 2016). One Special Issue paper explores mediational processes of this type.

4. Should We Develop New Tests?

There is an emerging consensus that ability EI consists of three separable, but correlated sets of skills (i.e., “branches”: Mayer and Salovey 1997) that are involved in the perception of emotion, the understanding of emotion, and the management of emotion (Joseph and Newman 2010; MacCann et al. 2014; Shao et al. 2015). But, there is no agreement on the exact materials or scoring procedures that should be used to assess each set of skills. With respect to the perception branch, a number of points have been made. In the MSCEIT (Mayer et al. 2003), perception is assessed with two tasks, one of which involves identifying emotions in faces and the other of which involves identifying emotions in pictures (e.g., abstract paintings). The former task is probably more central to emotion perception than the latter (Hall et al. 2009) and interventions designed to increase emotion perception have succeeded in altering face perception, but not picture perception (Herpertz et al. 2016). Hence, there are doubts about the picture perception task.
Face perception tasks often use high-intensity, prototypical expressions. In daily life, however, emotional displays tend to be less intense and less prototypical (Matsumoto and Hwang 2014). There could be value in assessing decoding ability with respect to less intense stimuli, which might capture skills that are more often used in daily interactions with others (Matsumoto and Hwang 2014). In addition, some EI experts have contended that static facial expressions are limited and have called for assessments of emotion perception accuracy in relation to more dynamic materials such as videos (Schlegel et al. 2014).
Also pertaining to ecological validity, some theorists have emphasized the importance of context in the manner in which emotion perception processes operate (Barrett et al. 2011). The idea here is that facial perception in particular, as well as person perception more broadly, rarely occurs in a context-free manner, such that many sources of contextual meaning impact the perceptions that people have (also see van Kleef and Côté 2022). Some of these contextual features of meaning can be added to emotion perception materials. Hess and Kafetsios (2021) have explored procedures of this type by presenting target expressers together with surrounding expressers (i.e., other individuals). Procedures of this type can allow one to calculate separable measures of accuracy (perceiving the intended emotions) and bias (perceiving additional emotions to those intended), with each type of score possessing social cognition significance. One of the Special Issue papers reviews this research program.
In the MSCEIT, emotional understanding (EU) is assessed by asking test-takers about combinations of emotion that are likely to be felt by target characters. But, emotional understanding encompasses a broad set of processes (Castro et al. 2016) and other assessment procedures could be used. Hellwig and Schulze (2021) describe a new EU test that incorporates appraisal information into situational descriptions. A good test-taker is able to infer the likely emotions that would be felt on the basis of the appraisals that were made, resulting in a theory-informed scoring system (also see MacCann and Roberts 2008). Another particularly ambitious test describes situations that would likely give rise to 1 of 10 emotions. With respect to each scenario, test-takers make inferences concerning the appraisals, action tendencies, expressions, and subjective feelings of each character (Sekwena and Fontaine 2018). The skills involved in these inferences are numerous and the test is, therefore, a particularly comprehensive one. As readers will encounter, one of the Special Issue papers also discusses the creation of EU tests based on core relational themes (i.e., molar summaries of the appraisals linked to a particular emotion: Smith and Lazarus 1993).
The management branch of the MSCEIT seems to capture important intrapersonal and interpersonal skills (Lopes et al. 2004; MacCann et al. 2011), but there are questions concerning the assessment of these skills. The skills involved in managing one’s own emotions, for example, are probably different than the skills involved in managing the emotions of others and these skills might be distinguished (Durham et al. 2023). The emotion regulation literature has also made a great deal of progress in understanding the different types of emotion regulation strategies that people can use (e.g., Olderbak et al. 2023), but these developments have not been incorporated into ability EI tests in any systematic manner. Finally, the emotion regulation literature has increasingly suggested that people regulate their emotions for instrumental as well as hedonic reasons (Tamir 2016) and the former sorts of reasons could be modeled in ability EI tests to a greater extent (also see Mayer et al. 2016 for additional thoughts about expanding the emotion management testing space).
In summary, EI researchers are busy developing new tests of ability EI, some of which are discussed in this Special Issue. It is uncertain whether some of the tests could be packaged together such that there is a new comprehensive test like the MSCEIT. If not, we will at least have new tests targeting particular branches that are likely to display higher levels of predictive validity. With respect to this point, one last development should be mentioned. Organizational researchers have found that altering generic personality measures such that they target a particular context (e.g., the workplace) results in higher-validity coefficients when predicting outcomes pertinent to that context (Bowling and Burns 2010; Shaffer and Postlethwaite 2012). Results of this type have inspired ability EI tests targeting the workplace (Krishnakumar et al. 2016; Schlegel and Mortillaro 2019) and one could imagine similar developments targeting other contexts (e.g., interpersonal relationships: Pratscher et al. 2019).

5. Connecting with the Other Literature

As Hoemann et al. (2021) emphasize, much of the literature has proposed variations in emotion-related expertise, with relevant constructs including alexithymia, emotional awareness, emotional clarity, emotional complexity, emotional competence, empathic accuracy, and emotional intelligence. Although the test procedures used to assess ability EI may be somewhat unique, it would be surprising if there were no links (whether empirical or theoretical) between emotional intelligence and other expertise-related constructs that have been proposed. Empathic accuracy, which conceptually overlaps with the perception branch of EI (Mayer and Salovey 1997), quantifies the extent to which inferences concerning the thoughts and feelings of a target, typically after a communication episode, overlap with the actual thoughts and feelings of the target, as reported on by the target (Hall and Schmid Mast 2007). Such skills seem to be highly dependent on who the target is and whether the perceiver is motivated to understand the thoughts and feelings of that target in a particular setting (Sassenrath et al. 2022). Aside from this point, this piece of literature is useful in highlighting several potential downsides to empathic accuracy, such as the possibility that these skills can be used for Machiavellian purposes (Hodges and Myers 2007) and that they can threaten relationships, such as when a person correctly infers that their partner is attracted to another person (Simpson et al. 2003). The potential downsides to emotional intelligence, thus far, have only received scattered attention (Davis and Nichols 2016).
Emotional awareness encompasses two constructs—attention to emotion and emotional clarity. Some people value their emotions to a greater extent and they attend to them for this reason. Such individuals tend to report stronger reactions to emotional events, but they also display emotional wisdom (e.g., by choosing to avoid events that would give rise to negative emotion: Robinson et al. 2021). Emotional clarity is meta-cognitive in nature and it occurs when people sense that they understand their emotions well (Boden and Thompson 2017). Like emotional intelligence, there are individual differences in emotional clarity and they are associated with higher levels of well-being (Lischetzke et al. 2012) as well as lower levels of psychopathology (Vine and Aldao 2014). But emotional clarity also varies quite a bit within a person, with predictable consequences (Thompson and Boden 2019). The ability EI literature has not yet focused on within-person changes in EI, with the exception of intervention studies (Durham et al. 2023). Theorizing at this within-person level might allow researchers to better understand the “fluid” (Fiori et al. 2022) aspects of EI.
Emotional intelligence should, ideally, support achievements such as wisdom, maturity, and resilience. These are difficult constructs to measure, but our understanding of optimal functioning requires such efforts (Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi 2000). Resilience seems to involve some paradoxical elements. On the one hand, resilient people are more reactive to pleasant and unpleasant events in momentary experience (Waugh et al. 2011). On the other hand, resilient people are capable of experiencing positive emotions under adverse circumstances (Fredrickson et al. 2003) and they are capable of rebounding from negative circumstances more quickly (Masten 2001). In other words, resilience seems to promote “stability through change”, which is a fundamental feature of healthy physiological and psychological systems (McEwen and Lasley 2002). Resilience clearly involves abilities and these abilities clearly involve appraisals and emotions (Tugade and Fredrickson 2004). In future research, it would seem valuable to attempt to operationalize such skills in ability-related terms.

6. Can Emotional Intelligence Be Trained?

One of the benefits of the ability EI model is that it conceptualizes EI in terms of skills that could, potentially, be trained (Hoffmann et al. 2020). We now have enough of this research that it is safe to conclude that EI can be trained, although effect sizes are medium rather than large (Hodzic et al. 2018). The literature has limitations, however. Among them, Kotsou et al. (2019) suggest the need for more studies that randomly assign participants to intervention versus control groups, that use “active” control groups to guard against expectancy effects, that use ability EI measures as outcomes, and that examine long-term as well as short-term changes. There is also the need to create standardized interventions, with a clearly specifiable content, which will facilitate comparisons among training procedures in future efforts (Kotsou et al. 2019). In addition to these developments, one Special Issue paper considers the question of whether EI training is efficacious when it is delivered online rather than in face-to-face terms. Success with such digital interventions will permit wider dissemination, though one issue is that volunteers in such studies will tend to have higher levels of education as well as (quite likely) higher levels of pre-existing EI, which would be linked to interest in volunteering for EI studies.

7. Additional Future Directions

As we reflect on the state of the ability EI literature, many questions suggest themselves. One question is whether ability EI can be linked to interoceptive abilities, defined in terms of individual differences in the accurate representation of afferent signals from the body (Herbert and Pollatos 2012). Bodily signals clearly contribute to emotional experience (Critchley and Garfinkel 2017), but EI tests also seem to assess variations in semantic knowledge concerning emotion, which would not vary as a function of current bodily experiences (Hoemann et al. 2021). Nonetheless, data do suggest that interoceptive abilities are involved in the representation (Zaki et al. 2012) and experience (Dunn et al. 2010) of emotion, such that links between ability EI and interoception might be expected (also see Klein and Robinson 2021). Given these potential links, as well as their importance to theories of emotion (Zaki et al. 2012), further work on this EI–interoception interface seems warranted.
One conception of emotional expertise contends that it involves differentiated feelings, which can be assessed by computing within-subject correlations between emotional experiences of a given valence (e.g., the anger–sadness correlation across reports of experience, with lower correlations suggesting a greater differentiation of these emotions: Smidt and Suvak 2015). Briefly, it is thought that higher levels of emotion differentiation support behavior that is more emotionally intelligent (Kashdan et al. 2015). Ability EI tests ask people to make distinctions among emotions (e.g., anger versus sadness) and it is intuitive to suggest that individuals obtaining high EI scores will exhibit greater emotion differentiation in their daily lives. However, MacCann et al. (2020b) report results that are contrary to this prediction. Hence, it would seem that more work is necessary in clarifying the relationship between ability EI and emotion differentiation. If emotion differentiation is not captured by current tests, we may need to create new ones.
More generally, we need to know a lot more about whether and how ability EI manifests itself in daily life. With respect to this point, the emotion dynamics literature has used variations of the experience-sampling paradigm (Scollon et al. 2003) to answer many questions about variations in emotional reactivity, emotion regulation, and other components of emotion change (e.g., inertia: Koval et al. 2015; see Kuppens et al. 2022, for a general review). In the future, we would like to see a greater integration of the EI and the emotion dynamics literature, whether in relation to laboratory (Klein et al. 2023; Waugh et al. 2011) or experience-sampling (MacCann et al. 2020b) paradigms. As shown in some examples, high-EI individuals may exhibit stronger emotional reactions to daily events of a given valence (Beshai et al. 2018), but they may be more capable of regulating pathological reactions to such events (Robinson et al. 2012). Clearly, there are complexities here that merit research.
A prominent neuroscience model contends that damage to emotion representation regions of the brain severely impairs everyday decision making (Naqvi et al. 2006). Such patients not only display flat affect, but they have trouble making life choices and they take unwarranted risks (Bechara 2004). Since such patients have preserved cognitive abilities, this research highlights the functional importance of feelings in decision making (Naqvi et al. 2006). Low levels of ability EI may act in a manner akin to this neurocognitive model, but very little research has focused on this possibility. Among other predictions, low-EI individuals may perform more poorly in tasks such as the Iowa Gambling Task (Bechara et al. 2000) and they may display some degree of insensitivity to hedonic considerations in tasks such as those described by Caruso and Shafir (2006) or Robinson et al. (2021).
As stated above, it is recognized that there are distinct sets of EI skills that can be broadly grouped into the perception, understanding, and management areas (Joseph and Newman 2010). Although these skill sets are separable, they tend to load onto a global EI factor (Krishnakumar et al. 2016; MacCann et al. 2014). In the future, it seems likely that some researchers will prefer to theorize at the branch level (e.g., He and Côté 2023), while others will prefer to theorize at the global level (e.g., Robinson et al. 2019). It is not clear which level of theorizing is best, but the development of new tests pertaining to particular branches (e.g., Elfenbein et al. 2017; Sekwena and Fontaine 2018) will likely fractionate the literature to a certain extent. This trend could be countered by focusing on ways in which different EI skill sets interact with each other, much as mindfulness-related skills do (Eisenlohr-Moul et al. 2012).

8. Conclusions

In this introduction to a Special Issue on ability EI, we emphasized the importance of developing new theories, of focusing on mechanisms and processes, of developing new EI tests, of connecting with the other literature, and of training studies that answer novel questions. As displayed in Table 2, the Special Issue papers cover all of these topics. Emotional intelligence should matter in diverse realms such as decision making, social behavior, and well-being, but new developments are needed to forge these links. The present Special Issue is a timely one and it is hoped that we will learn considerably more about what ability EI is and how it works in upcoming years.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not Applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not Applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not Applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Alves, Hans, Alex Koch, and Christian Unkelbach. 2017. The ‘common good’ phenomenon: Why similarities are positive and differences are negative. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 146: 512–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Antonakis, John, Neal M. Ashkanasy, and Marie T. Dasborough. 2009. Does leadership need emotional intelligence? The Leadership Quarterly 20: 247–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Appel, Markus, Timo Gnambs, and Gregory R. Maio. 2012. A short measure of the need for affect. Journal of Personality Assessment 94: 418–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Bandura, Albert. 1982. Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist 37: 122–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Barrett, Lisa Feldman, Batja Mesquita, and Maria Gendron. 2011. Context in emotion perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science 20: 286–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Bänziger, Tanja, Didier Grandjean, and Klaus R. Scherer. 2009. Emotion recognition from expressions in face, voice, and body: The Multimodal Emotion Recognition Test (MERT). Emotion 9: 691–704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Bechara, Antoine. 2004. The role of emotion in decision-making: Evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage. Brain and Cognition 55: 30–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Bechara, Antoine, Daniel Tranel, and Hanna Damasio. 2000. Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain: A Journal of Neurology 123: 2189–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Bechtoldt, Myriam N., and Vanessa K. Schneider. 2016. Predicting stress from the ability to eavesdrop on feelings: Emotional intelligence and testosterone jointly predict cortisol reactivity. Emotion 16: 815–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Beshai, Shadi, Jennifer L. Prentice, and Vivian Huang. 2018. Building blocks of emotional flexibility: Trait mindfulness and self-compassion are associated with positive and negative mood shifts. Mindfulness 9: 939–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Boden, Matthew Tyler, and Renee J. Thompson. 2017. Meta-analysis of the association between emotional clarity and attention to emotions. Emotion Review 9: 79–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Bowling, Nathan A., and Gary N. Burns. 2010. A comparison of work-specific and general personality measures as predictors of work and non-work criteria. Personality and Individual Differences 49: 95–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Cacioppo, John T., and Gary G. Berntson. 1999. The affect system: Architecture and operating characteristics. Current Directions in Psychological Science 8: 133–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Caruso, Eugene M., and Eldar Shafir. 2006. Now that I think about it, I’m in the mood for laughs: Decisions focused on mood. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 19: 155–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Carver, Charles S., and Michael F. Scheier. 2014. Dispositional optimism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 18: 293–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Carver, Charles S., Sheri L. Johnson, and Jutta Joormann. 2009. Two-mode models of self-regulation as a tool for conceptualizing effects of the serotonin system in normal behavior and diverse disorders. Current Directions in Psychological Science 18: 195–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Castro, Vanessa L., Yanhua Cheng, Amy G. Halberstadt, and Daniel Grühn. 2016. EUReKA! A conceptual model of emotion understanding. Emotion Review 8: 258–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Cha, Christine B., and Matthew K. Nock. 2009. Emotional intelligence is a protective factor for suicidal behavior. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 48: 422–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Checa, Purificación, and Pablo Fernández-Berrocal. 2019. Cognitive control and emotional intelligence: Effect of the emotional content of the task. Frontiers in Psychology 10: 195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Ciarrochi, Joseph, Frank P. Dean, and Stephen Anderson. 2002. Emotional intelligence moderates the relationship between stress and mental health. Personality and Individual Differences 32: 197–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Côté, Stéphane. 2014. Emotional intelligence in organizations. Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior 1: 459–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Critchley, Hugo D., and Sarah Garfinkel. 2017. Interoception and emotion. Current Opinion in Psychology 17: 7–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Daus, Catherine S., and Neal M. Ashkanasy. 2005. The case for the ability-based model of emotional intelligence in organizational behavior. Journal of Organizational Behavior 26: 453–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Davis, Sarah K., and Rachel Nichols. 2016. Does emotional intelligence have a ‘dark’ side? A review of the literature. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 1316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Di Fabio, Annamaria, and Maureen E. Kenny. 2016. Promoting well-being: The contribution of emotional intelligence. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 1182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Dunn, Barnaby D., Hannah C. Galton, Ruth Morgan, Davy Evans, Clare Oliver, Marcel Meyer, Rhodri Cusack, Andrew D. Lawrence, and Tim Dalgleish. 2010. Listening to your heart: How interoception shapes emotion experience and intuitive decision making. Psychological Science 21: 1835–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Durham, Michelle R. Persich, Ryan Smith, Sara Cloonan, Lindsey L. Hildebrand, Rebecca Woods-Lubert, Jeff Skalamera, Sarah M. Berryhill, Karen L. Weihs, Richard D. Lane, John J. B. Allen, and et al. 2023. Development and validation of an online emotional intelligence training program. Frontiers in Psychology 14: 1221817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Eisenlohr-Moul, Tory A., Erin C. Walsh, Richard J. Charnigo, Jr., Donald R. Lynam, and Ruth A. Baer. 2012. The ‘what’ and the ‘how’ of dispositional mindfulness: Using interactions among subscales of the Five-Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire to understand its relation to substance use. Assessment 19: 276–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Elfenbein, Hillary Anger, Daisung Jang, Sudeep Sharma, and Jeffrey Sanchez-Burks. 2017. Validating emotional attention regulation as a component of emotional intelligence: A Stroop approach to individual differences in tuning in to and out of nonverbal cues. Emotion 17: 348–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Engelberg, Elisabeth, and Lennart Sjöberg. 2004. Emotional intelligence, affect intensity, and social adjustment. Personality and Individual Differences 37: 533–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Fernández-Berrocal, Pablo, and Natalio Extremera. 2016. Ability emotional intelligence, depression, and well-being. Emotion Review 8: 311–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Fiori, Marina, Shagini Udayar, and Ashley Vesely Maillefer. 2022. Emotion information processing as a new component of emotional intelligence: Theoretical framework and empirical evidence. European Journal of Personality 36: 245–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Fredrickson, Barbara L., Michele M. Tugade, Christian E. Waugh, and Gregory R. Larkin. 2003. What good are positive emotions in crisis? A prospective study of resilience and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United States on September 11th, 2001. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 84: 365–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Frijda, Nico H. 2010. Impulsive action and motivation. Biological Psychology 84: 570–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Goleman, Daniel. 1995. Emotional Intelligence. New York: Bantam Books. [Google Scholar]
  36. Gross, James J. 2002. Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology 39: 281–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Gutiérrez-Cobo, Maria J., Alberto Megías, Raquel Gómez-Leal, Rosario Cabello, and Pablo Fernández-Berrocal. 2018. The role of emotional intelligence and negative affect as protective and risk factors of aggressive behavior: A moderated mediation model. Aggressive Behavior 44: 638–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Gutiérrez-Cobo, Maria J., Rosario Cabello, and Pablo Fernández-Berrocal. 2017. The three models of emotional intelligence and performance in a hot and cool go/no-go task in undergraduate students. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 11: 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Hall, Judith A., and Marianne Schmid Mast. 2007. Sources of accuracy in the empathic accuracy paradigm. Emotion 7: 438–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Hall, Judith A., Susan A. Andrzejewski, and Jennelle E. Yopchick. 2009. Psychosocial correlates of interpersonal sensitivity: A meta-analysis. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 33: 149–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Hardy, Jaime, and Suzanne C. Segerstrom. 2017. Intra-individual variability and psychological flexibility: Affect and health in a national US sample. Journal of Research in Personality 69: 13–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Hayes, Steven C., Jacqueline Pistorello, and Michael E. Levin. 2012. Acceptance and commitment therapy as a unified model of behavior change. The Counseling Psychologist 40: 976–1002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. He, Joyce C., and Stéphane Côté. 2023. Are empathic people better adjusted? A test of competing models of empathic accuracy and intrapersonal and interpersonal facets of adjustment using self- and peer reports. Psychological Science 34: 955–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Heatherton, Todd F., and Dylan D. Wagner. 2011. Cognitive neuroscience of self-regulation failure. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15: 132–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Hellwig, Susan, and Ralf Schulze. 2021. Emotion theories as a scoring rationale for tests of emotional understanding. Personality and Individual Differences 181: 111034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Herbert, Beate M., and Olga Pollatos. 2012. The body in the mind: On the relationship between interoception and embodiment. Topics in Cognitive Science 4: 692–704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Herpertz, Sarah, Astrid Schütz, and John Nezlek. 2016. Enhancing emotion perception, a fundamental component of emotional intelligence: Using multiple-group SEM to evaluate a training program. Personality and Individual Differences 95: 11–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Hess, Ursula, and Konstantinos Kafetsios. 2021. Infusing context into emotion perception impacts emotion decoding accuracy: A truth and bias model. Experimental Psychology 68: 285–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Hodges, Sara D., and Michael W. Myers. 2007. Empathy. In Encyclopedia of Social Psychology. Edited by R. F. Baumeister and K. D. Vohs. Thousand Oaks: Sage, pp. 296–98. [Google Scholar]
  50. Hodzic, Sabina, Jana Scharfen, Pilar Ripoll, Heinz Holling, and Franck Zenasni. 2018. How efficient are emotional intelligence trainings: A meta-analysis. Emotion Review 10: 138–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Hoemann, Katie, Catie Nielson, Ashley Yuen, J. W. Gurera, Karen S. Quigley, and Lisa Feldman Barrett. 2021. Expertise in emotion: A scoping review and unifying framework for individual differences in the mental representation of emotional experience. Psychological Bulletin 147: 1159–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Hoffmann, Jessica D., Marc A. Brackett, Craig S. Bailey, and Cynthia J. Willner. 2020. Teaching emotion regulation in schools: Translating research into practice with the RULER approach to social and emotional learning. Emotion 20: 105–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. John, Oliver P., and James J. Gross. 2004. Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: Personality processes, individual differences, and life span development. Journal of Personality 72: 1301–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Joseph, Dana L., and Daniel A. Newman. 2010. Emotional intelligence: An integrative meta-analysis and cascading model. Journal of Applied Psychology 95: 54–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Joseph, Dana L., Jing Jin, Daniel A. Newman, and Ernest H. O’Boyle. 2015. Why does self-reported emotional intelligence predict job performance? A meta-analytic investigation of mixed EI. Journal of Applied Psychology 100: 298–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Kashdan, Todd B., Antonina S. Farmer, Leah M. Adams, Patty Ferssizidis, Patrick E. McKnight, and John B. Nezlek. 2013. Distinguishing healthy adults from people with social anxiety disorder: Evidence for the value of experiential avoidance and positive emotions in everyday social interactions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 122: 645–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Kashdan, Todd B., and Jonathan Rottenberg. 2010. Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect of health. Clinical Psychology Review 30: 865–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Kashdan, Todd B., Lisa Feldman Barrett, and Patrick E. McKnight. 2015. Unpacking emotion differentiation: Transforming unpleasant experience by perceiving distinctions in negativity. Current Directions in Psychological Science 24: 10–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Keltner, Dacher, and James J. Gross. 1999. Functional accounts of emotions. Cognition and Emotion 13: 467–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Klein, Robert J., and Michael D. Robinson. 2021. Signals and their detection: Basic perceptual sensitivity as a foundation for emotional clarity. Personality and Individual Differences 180: 110991. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Klein, Robert J., Nicholas C. Jacobson, and Michael D. Robinson. 2023. A psychological flexibility perspective on well-being: Emotional reactivity, adaptive choices, and daily experiences. Emotion 23: 911–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Kotsou, I., M. Mikolajczak, A. Heeren, J. Grégoire, and C. Leys. 2019. Improving emotional intelligence: A systematic review of existing work and future challenges. Emotion Review 11: 151–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Koval, Peter, Annette Brose, Madeline L. Pe, Marlies Houben, Yasemin Erbas, Dominique Champagne, and Peter Kuppens. 2015. Emotional inertia and external events: The roles of exposure, reactivity, and recovery. Emotion 15: 625–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Krishnakumar, Sukumarakurup, Kay Hopkins, and Michael D. Robinson. 2017. When feeling poorly at work does not mean acting poorly at work: The moderating role of work-related emotional intelligence. Motivation and Emotion 41: 122–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Krishnakumar, Sukumarakurup, Kay Hopkins, Joseph G. Szmerekovsky, and Michael D. Robinson. 2016. Assessing workplace emotional intelligence: Development and validation of an ability-based measure. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied 150: 371–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Kuppens, Peter, Egon Dejonckheere, Elise K. Kalokerinos, and Peter Koval. 2022. Some recommendations on the use of daily life methods in affective science. Affective Science 3: 505–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Lazarus, Richard S., and Susan Folkman. 1984. Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  68. Lerner, Jennifer S., Ye Li, Piercarlo Valdesolo, and Karim S. Kassam. 2015. Emotion and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology 66: 799–823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Lischetzke, Tanja, Michael Eid, and Ed Diener. 2012. Perceiving one’s own and others’ feelings around the world: The relations of attention to and clarity of feelings with subjective well-being across nations. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 43: 1249–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Lopes, Paulo N., John B. Nezlek, Astrid Schütz, Ina Sellin, and Peter Salovey. 2004. Emotional intelligence and social interaction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 30: 1018–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. MacCann, Carolyn, and Richard D. Roberts. 2008. New paradigms for assessing emotional intelligence: Theory and data. Emotion 8: 540–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. MacCann, Carolyn, Dana L. Joseph, Daniel A. Newman, and Richard D. Roberts. 2014. Emotional intelligence is a second-stratum factor of intelligence: Evidence from hierarchical and bifactor models. Emotion 14: 358–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. MacCann, Carolyn, Gerald J. Fogarty, Moshe Zeidner, and Richard D. Roberts. 2011. Coping mediates the relationship between emotional intelligence (EI) and academic achievement. Contemporary Educational Psychology 36: 60–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. MacCann, Carolyn, Yixin Jiang, Luke E. R. Brown, Micaela Bucich, and Amirali Minbashian. 2020a. Emotional intelligence predicts academic performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 146: 150–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. MacCann, Carolyn, Yasemin Erbas, Egon Dejonckheere, Amirali Minbashian, Peter Kuppens, and Kirill Fayn. 2020b. Emotional intelligence relates to emotions, emotion dynamics, and emotion complexity: A meta-analysis and experience sampling study. European Journal of Psychological Assessment 36: 460–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Martins, Alexandra, Nelson Ramalho, and Estelle Morin. 2010. A comprehensive meta-analysis of the relationship between emotional intelligence and health. Personality and Individual Differences 49: 554–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Masten, Ann S. 2001. Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in development. American Psychologist 56: 227–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Matsumoto, David, and Hyisung C. Hwang. 2014. Judgments of subtle facial expressions of emotion. Emotion 14: 349–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Matsumoto, David, Jeff LeRoux, Carinda Wilson-Cohn, Jake Raroque, Kristie Kooken, Paul Ekman, Nathan Yrizarry, Sherry Loewinger, Hideko Uchida, Albert Yee, and et al. 2000. A new test to measure emotion recognition ability: Matsumoto and Ekman’s Japanese and Caucasian Brief Affect Recognition Test (JACBERT). Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 24: 179–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Matthews, Gerald, Amanda K. Emo, Gregory Funke, Moshe Zeidner, Richard D. Roberts, Paul T. Costa, Jr., and Ralf Schulze. 2006. Emotional intelligence, personality, and task-induced stress. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 12: 96–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Matthews, Gerald, Moshe Zeidner, and Richard D. Roberts. 2012. Emotional intelligence: A promise unfulfilled? Japanese Psychological Research 54: 105–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Matthews, Gerald, Richard D. Roberts, and Moshe Zeidner. 2004. Seven myths about emotional intelligence. Psychological Inquiry 15: 179–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Mayer, John D., and Peter Salovey. 1997. What is emotional intelligence? In Emotional Development and Emotional Intelligence: Educational Implications. Edited by P. Salovey and D. J. Sluyter. New York: Basic Books, pp. 3–34. [Google Scholar]
  84. Mayer, John D., David R. Caruso, and Peter Salovey. 1999. Emotional intelligence meets traditional standards for intelligence. Intelligence 27: 267–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Mayer, John D., David R. Caruso, and Peter Salovey. 2016. The ability model of emotional intelligence: Principles and updates. Emotion Review 8: 290–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Mayer, John D., Peter Salovey, and David R. Caruso. 2008. Emotional intelligence: New ability or eclectic traits? American Psychologist 63: 503–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Mayer, John D., Peter Salovey, David R. Caruso, and Gill Sitarenios. 2003. Measuring emotional intelligence with the MSCEIT V2.0. Emotion 3: 97–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. McEwen, Bruce S., and Elizabeth Norton Lasley. 2002. The End of Stress as We Know It. Washington, DC: Joseph Henry Press. [Google Scholar]
  89. Megías-Robles, Albert, María José Gutiérrez-Cobo, Raquel Gómez-Leal, Rosario Cabello, James J. Gross, and Pablo Fernández-Berrocal. 2019. Emotionally intelligent people reappraise rather than suppress their emotions. PLoS ONE 14: e0220688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Miao, Chao, Ronald H. Humphrey, and Shanshan Qian. 2017. Are the emotionally intelligent good citizens or counterproductive? A meta-analysis of emotional intelligence and its relationships with organizational citizenship behavior and counterproductive work behavior. Personality and Individual Differences 116: 144–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Miners, Christopher T. H., Stéphane Côté, and Filip Lievens. 2018. Assessing the validity of emotional intelligence measures. Emotion Review 10: 87–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Naqvi, Nasir, Baba Shiv, and Antoine Bechara. 2006. The role of emotion in decision making: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. Current Directions in Psychological Science 15: 260–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. O’Boyle, Earnest H., Jr., Ronald H. Humphrey, Jeffrey M. Pollack, Thomas H. Hawver, and Paul A. Story. 2011. The relation between emotional intelligence and job performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Organizational Behavior 32: 788–818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Olderbak, Sally, Andero Uusberg, Carolyn MacCann, Katja M. Pollak, and James J. Gross. 2023. The Process Model of Emotion Regulation Questionnaire: Assessing individual differences in strategy stage and orientation. Assessment 30: 2090–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Peña-Sarrionandia, Ainize, Moïra Mikolajczak, and James J. Gross. 2015. Integrating emotion regulation and emotional intelligence traditions: A meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Pratscher, Steven D., Phillip K. Wood, Laura A. King, and B. Ann Bettencourt. 2019. Interpersonal mindfulness: Scale development and initial construct validation. Mindfulness 10: 1044–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Roberts, Richard D., Carolyn MacCann, Gerald Matthews, and Moshe Zeidner. 2010. Emotional intelligence: Toward a consensus of models and measures. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4: 821–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Robinson, Michael D., Michelle R. Persich, and Sukumarakurup Krishnakumar. 2022. Happy and engaged: Synergies among mindfulness and ability-related emotional intelligence. Journal of Happiness Studies 23: 769–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Robinson, Michael D., Michelle R. Persich, Cassandra Stawicki, and Sukumarakurup Krishnakumar. 2019. Deviant workplace behavior as emotional action: Discriminant and interactive roles for work-related emotional intelligence. Human Performance 32: 201–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Robinson, Michael D., Robert J. Klein, Roberta L. Irvin, and Avianna Z. McGregor. 2021. Attention to emotion and reliance on feelings in decision-making: Variations on a pleasure principle. Cognition 217: 104904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Robinson, Michael D., Sara K. Moeller, Ryan L. Boyd, and Wendy Troop-Gordon. 2012. The regulatory benefits of high levels of affect perception accuracy: A process analysis of reactions to stressors in daily life. Emotion 12: 785–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Salovey, Peter, and John D. Mayer. 1990. Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality 9: 185–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Sassenrath, Claudia, Jacquie D. Vorauer, and Sara D. Hodges. 2022. The link between perspective-taking and prosociality-Not as universal as you might think. Current Opinion in Psychology 44: 94–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  104. Sánchez-Álvarez, Nicolás, Natalio Extremera, and Pablo Fernández-Berrocal. 2016. The relation between emotional intelligence and subjective well-being: A meta-analytic investigation. The Journal of Positive Psychology 11: 276–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Schlegel, Katja, and Marcello Mortillaro. 2019. The Geneva Emotional Competence Test (GECo): An ability measure of workplace emotional intelligence. Journal of Applied Psychology 104: 559–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Schlegel, Katja, Didier Grandjean, and Klaus R. Scherer. 2014. Introducing the Geneva Emotion Recognition Test: An example of Rasch-based test development. Psychological Assessment 26: 666–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Schwarzer, Ralf. 2001. Social-cognitive factors in changing health-related behaviors. Current Directions in Psychological Science 10: 47–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Scollon, Chistie Napa, Chu Kim-Prieto, and Ed Diener. 2003. Experience sampling: Promises and pitfalls, strengths and weaknesses. Journal of Happiness Studies 4: 5–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Sekwena, Eva Kefilwe, and Johnny R. J. Fontaine. 2018. Redefining and assessing emotional understanding on the componential emotion approach. South African Journal of Psychology 48: 243–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Seligman, Martin E. P., and Mihalyi Csikszentmihalyi. 2000. Positive psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist 55: 5–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  111. Shaffer, Jonathan A., and Bennett E. Postlethwaite. 2012. A matter of context: A meta-analytic investigation of the relative validity of contextualized and noncontextualized personality measures. Personnel Psychology 65: 445–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Shao, Bo, Lorna Doucet, and David R. Caruso. 2015. Universality versus cultural specificity of three emotion domains: Some evidence based on the cascading model of emotional intelligence. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 46: 229–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Simpson, Jeffy A., M. Minda Oriña, and William Ickes. 2003. When accuracy hurts, and when it helps: A test of the empathic accuracy model in marital interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 85: 881–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  114. Smidt, Katharine E., and Michael K. Suvak. 2015. A brief, but nuanced, review of emotional granularity and emotion differentiation research. Current Opinion in Psychology 3: 48–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Smith, Craig A., and Richard S. Lazarus. 1993. Appraisal components, core relational themes, and the emotions. Cognition and Emotion 7: 233–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Śmieja, Magdalena, Marta Mrozowicz-Wrońska, and Dorota Kobylińska. 2011. Emotional intelligence and emotion regulation strategies. Studia Psychhologiczne 49: 55–64. [Google Scholar]
  117. Tamir, Maya. 2016. Why do people regulate their emotions? A taxonomy of motives in emotion regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Review 20: 199–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  118. Thompson, Renee J., and Matthew Tyler Boden. 2019. State emotional clarity and attention to emotion: A naturalistic examination of their associations with each other, affect, and context. Cognition and Emotion 33: 1514–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Tugade, Michele M., and Barbara L. Fredrickson. 2004. Resilient individuals use positive emotions to bounce back from negative emotional experiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 86: 320–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Udayar, Shagini, Marina Fiori, and Elise Bausseron. 2020. Emotional intelligence and performance in a stressful task: The mediating role of self-efficacy. Personality and Individual Differences 156: 109790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. van Kleef, Gerben A., and Stéphane Côté. 2022. The social effects of emotions. Annual Review of Psychology 73: 629–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  122. Vanuk, John R., Anna Alkozei, Adam C. Raikes, John J. B. Allen, and William D. S. Killgore. 2019. Ability-based emotional intelligence is associated with greater cardiac vagal control and reactivity. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13: 181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  123. Vine, Vera, and Amelia Aldao. 2014. Impaired emotional clarity and psychopathology: A transdiagnostic deficit with symptom-specific pathways through emotion regulation. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 33: 319–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Watson, David. 2000. Mood and Temperament. New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  125. Waugh, Christian E., Renee J. Thompson, and Ian H. Gotlib. 2011. Flexible emotion responsiveness in trait resilience. Emotion 11: 1059–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Wilkowski, Benjamin M., and Michael D. Robinson. 2010. The anatomy of anger: An integrative cognitive model of trait anger and reactive aggression. Journal of Personality 78: 9–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  127. Ybarra, Oscar, Ethan Kross, and Jeffrey Sanchez-Burks. 2014. The ‘big idea’ that is yet to be: Toward a more motivated, contextual, and dynamic model of emotional intelligence. The Academy of Management Perspectives 28: 93–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Zaki, Jamil, Joshua Ian Davis, and Kevin N. Ochsner. 2012. Overlapping activity in anterior insula during interoception and emotional experience. NeuroImage 62: 493–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Zeidner, Moshe, Richard D. Roberts, and Gerald Matthews. 2008. The science of emotional intelligence: Current consensus and controversies. European Psychologist 13: 64–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Some Questions Asked in the Special Issue (and/or Introduction).
Table 1. Some Questions Asked in the Special Issue (and/or Introduction).
FocusQuestions
TheoryHow does emotional knowledge impact people’s lives?
Are low-EI individuals prone to emotionally impulsive behaviors?
How would cognitive control in emotional contexts manifest itself?
Might EI be linked to higher levels of emotional reactivity?
Is EI linked to psychological flexibility?
MechanismsDo high-EI individuals favor problem-focused coping?
Might low-EI individuals be prone to experiential avoidance?
Do high-EI individuals reappraise rather than suppress their emotions?
Is EI linked to average levels of positive and negative affect?
MeasurementShould emotion perception materials use fewer prototypical stimuli?
How can we model social context in emotion perception tasks?
Can we develop theory-informed tests of emotional understanding?
Can emotion regulation theories inform emotion management assessments?
Should we develop more context-specific assessments of EI?
Neighboring AreasAre higher perception abilities always beneficial?
Might EI levels display within-person fluctuations?
Can EI be linked to wisdom, maturity, or resilience?
Can we develop ability-based approaches to resilience?
TrainingCan emotional intelligence be trained?
What sorts of skills should be trained in intervention studies?
Can EI be trained using online training methods?
Future DirectionsDo interoception abilities contribute to emotional intelligence?
Do high-EI individuals experience emotions in a differentiated manner?
What are the daily diary signatures of ability EI?
How does ability EI impact decision making?
Do different EI skills interact with each other?
Note: EI = Emotional Intelligence.
Table 2. An Overview of Special Issue Papers.
Table 2. An Overview of Special Issue Papers.
FocusNumberBrief Summary
Theory1Presents a hypersensitivity theory of EI
2Presents an evaluation expertise theory of EI
Mechanisms3Positive and negative affect as well-being mediators
Measurement4A contextual approach to emotion perception
5Integrating emotion theory into EI measurement
6A new test of emotion perception
7A new test of emotional understanding
Neighboring Areas8A review of the correlates of empathic accuracy
9Emotional clarity as fluid EI
10Resilience from skill and well-being perspectives
Training11An online intervention for emotional intelligence
Note: EI = Emotional Intelligence.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Robinson, M.D. Ability-Related Emotional Intelligence: An Introduction. J. Intell. 2024, 12, 51. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12050051

AMA Style

Robinson MD. Ability-Related Emotional Intelligence: An Introduction. Journal of Intelligence. 2024; 12(5):51. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12050051

Chicago/Turabian Style

Robinson, Michael D. 2024. "Ability-Related Emotional Intelligence: An Introduction" Journal of Intelligence 12, no. 5: 51. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12050051

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop