Next Article in Journal
Comparison of Psychometric Functions Measured Using Remote Testing and Laboratory Testing
Previous Article in Journal
Evolution of Hyperventilation-Induced Nystagmus in Acute Unilateral Vestibulopathy—Interpretative Model and Etiopathogenetic Hypotheses
Previous Article in Special Issue
Development of New Open-Set Speech Material for Use in Clinical Audiology with Speakers of British English
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Hebrew Digits in Noise (DIN) Test in Cochlear Implant Users and Normal Hearing Listeners

by
Riki Taitelbaum-Swead
1,2,* and
Leah Fostick
3
1
Department of Communication Disorders, Speech Perception and Listening Effort Lab in the Name of Prof. Mordechai Himelfarb, Ariel University, Ariel 4070000, Israel
2
Meuhedet Health Services, Tel Aviv 6203854, Israel
3
Department of Communication Disorders, Auditory Perception Lab in the Name of Laurent Levy, Ariel University, Ariel 4070000, Israel
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Audiol. Res. 2024, 14(3), 457-468; https://doi.org/10.3390/audiolres14030038
Submission received: 1 April 2024 / Revised: 13 May 2024 / Accepted: 15 May 2024 / Published: 20 May 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Rehabilitation of Hearing Impairment: 2nd Edition)

Abstract

:
This study aimed to compare the Hebrew version of the digits-in-noise (DIN) thresholds among cochlear implant (CI) users and their normal-hearing (NH) counterparts, explore the influence of age on these thresholds, examine the effects of early auditory exposure versus its absence on DIN threshold, and assess the correlation between DIN thresholds and other speech perception tests. A total of 13 children with CI (aged 5.5–11 years), 15 pre-lingual CI users (aged 14–30 years), and 15 post-lingual CI users (aged 22–77 years), and their age-matched NH controls (n = 45) participated in the study. Speech perception tasks, including the DIN test, one-syllable word test, and sentence identification tasks in various auditory conditions, served as the main outcome measures. The results indicated that CI users exhibited higher speech reception thresholds in noise across all age groups compared to NH peers, with no significant difference between pre-lingual and post-lingual CI users. Significant differences were also observed in monosyllabic word and sentence accuracy in both quiet and noise conditions between CI and NH groups. Furthermore, correlations were observed between the DIN and other speech perception tests. The study concludes that CI users require a notably higher signal-to-noise ratio to discern digits in noise, underscoring the DIN test’s utility in assessing speech recognition capabilities in CI users while emphasizing the need for a comprehensive test battery to fully gauge their speech perception abilities.

1. Introduction

Over the last four decades, cochlear implantation has become the preferred rehabilitation option for children and adults with severe-to-profound hearing loss [1]. Currently, approximately 1,000,000 cochlear implant (CIs) users are registered worldwide [2], and this number is anticipated to increase substantially in the coming years. The success of this procedure due to improvements in the technology of implant devices has led to the broadening of candidacy criteria, including a decrease in the age of implantation in young infants, most of them with bilateral CIs [3,4], and an increase in the age of older adults who receive CI [5]. This also led to the implantation of CIs in candidates with better hearing thresholds and speech perception abilities.
Speech perception in degraded listening situations, such as noisy environments, remains a significant challenge for CI users [6,7,8,9]. CI recipients experience difficulties in noisy environments due to the degraded signal transmitted by the CI device (poor “bottom-up” processing). Therefore, there is a substantial need to evaluate their speech perception abilities, particularly in noisy conditions. However, due to the degraded bottom-up information in these situations, CI users must rely on top-down information, such as semantic and syntactic knowledge. In turn, this adds cognitive function as a confounding factor that can influence the results [10]. The need for linguistic knowledge can also limit the range of participants performing speech-perception-in-noise tests, such as children and nonnative speakers. Accordingly, there is a need for a short and efficient test that will evaluate speech-perception-in-noise while matching the CI users’ age and auditory abilities.
The digit in noise (DIN) test emulates everyday conditions by using speech stimuli embedded in noise but with minimal linguistic knowledge. This test was originally developed as a screening tool for hearing loss; however, it has recently become an acceptable measure of speech perception abilities in adverse listening conditions. The test presents a series of three digits (typically constructed from digits 0 to 9) in the presence of speech-shaped noise constructed from the digits’ spectrum. The listener is then asked to repeat the digits in the correct order. The intensity level of the digits varies amidst the noise to assess an individual’s ability to understand speech in increasingly difficult listening conditions. Since the test stimuli are digits, it is appropriate for young children or others with limited language skills as well as for adults who possess advanced language proficiency [11]. The other advantages of this short and efficient test include the ability to change the signal-to-noise ratio adaptively and its applicability in various auditory abilities.
The results of the DIN test have been examined in various languages and populations of CI users and were shown to correlate with the outcomes of other speech perceptions in noise tests [12,13,14]. However, it is unclear whether the DIN test, primarily designed as a screening tool, can be sensitive enough to distinguish between different age groups and detect the onset of deafness, making it suitable for inclusion in the speech perception battery for follow-up in CI centers. Thus, the present study aimed (1) to compare the DIN thresholds between CI users and normal-hearing (NH) peers, (2) to test the effect of age on DIN thresholds, (3) to investigate the impact of early auditory exposure (post-lingual CI users) versus its absence (pre-lingual CI users) on the DIN threshold, and (4) to explore the association between DIN thresholds and other open-set speech perception tests. Due to group differences in age and, consequently, linguistic and auditory abilities, speech perception was assessed in children using monosyllabic words and sentences without background noise, and in adults using sentences with background noise.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

A total of 88 children and adults participated in the study: 13 prelingual CI children and 15 NH peers aged 5.5–11 years, 15 pre-lingual CI users (aged 14–30 years), and 15 post-lingual CI users (aged 22–77 years), and their age-matched NH controls (n = 30). All 43 CI users were recruited via associations for hearing-impaired individuals, whereas the NH controls were recruited via social media advertisements. The inclusion criteria for all participants were as follows: (a) native Hebrew speaker, (b) no reported cognitive or attention deficit disorders, and (c) at least 12 years of education for all adult participants and the children’s parents. These inclusion criteria were assessed using self-report questionnaires completed by adult participants or the children’s parents. The study was approved by the institutional ethics committee and all adult participants and the children’s parents provided informed consent before the experiment commenced. Participants received an equivalent of 25 USD as compensation for the time spent participating in the study.

2.1.1. CI Groups

Three groups of CI users participated in the present study. One group was of children with CI and included 13 CI users aged 5.5–11 years (mean, 7.6 years). All the children had bilateral severe to profound hearing loss before 2.5 years of age, and their age at implantation (first or only CI) ranged between 0.75 and 6.5 years (mean, 2.4 years). Eight children used bilateral CIs, and five used bimodal amplification (CI+ hearing aid [HA]); eight used cochlear devices, and five used advanced bionic devices. A second group included 15 pre-lingual CI users aged 14–30 (mean, 22.6 years). These pre-lingual young adults had bilateral severe to profound hearing loss before 1.5 years of age, and their age at implantation (first or only CI) ranged between 1 and 8 years (mean, 2.95 years). Thirteen young adults used bilateral CIs, one used bimodal amplification (CI+HA), and one used only one CI; 14 used a cochlear device, and one used an advanced bionics device. The third group included 15 post-lingual CI users aged 22–77 years (mean, 51 years), and their age at implantation (first or only CI) ranged between 17 and 73 years (mean, 46 years). All the post-lingual CI users had bilateral severe to profound deafness after eight years of age. Three adults used bilateral CIs, ten used bimodal amplification (CI+HA), and two used only one CI; six participants used the cochlear device, four used the advanced bionics device, and five used the Med-El device. The individual demographic information and background data for each CI user are presented in Appendix A.

2.1.2. NH Groups

The NH participants were matched to the participants in the CI groups in terms of age (within 6 months), sex, and level of education. The additional inclusion criterion for the NH group was a mean pure-tone average (PTA; 0.5, 1, 2, 4, and 8 kHz) of less than 20 dB HL in both ears for participants under 60 years of age and less than 30 dB HL for participants aged 60 years or older, as verified by an audiology student.

2.2. Speech Perception Measures

Speech perception was measured using the Hebrew DIN test, monosyllables (Hebrew Arthur Boothroyd [HAB] test), and sentences (the HeBio test for adults and sentences for children).

2.2.1. Hebrew Digits in Noise (DIN) Test

This test was originally developed in the Netherlands by Smits et al. (2004) [11]. It included 24 broadband homogeneous digit triplets which were used to test digit recognition. In the Hebrew version of the DIN test, the digits are pronounced by a female speaker and presented in long-term average speech spectrum (LTASS) masking noises [11]. The development and adaptation of the Hebrew version of the DIN test were similar to that of the Dutch version and followed its stages [15]. In the adaptive procedure, the overall intensity level was kept constant at 65 dBA and the first digit triplet was presented at 0 dB signal-to-noise ratio (SNR). The intensity of the triple digits varied adaptively according to the correctness of the response, following a standard one-up, one-down procedure with a step size of 2 dB. The outcome SRTn (dB) was the average presentation SNR of the final ten reversals. All three digits had to be pressed correctly for the triplet to qualify as correct (four triplets for training at a fixed SNR of 0 dB and 20 for testing).

2.2.2. HAB Words

The Hebrew version of the Arthur Boothroyd Words Test (HAB) [16,17] is composed of 15 lists of ten meaningful, one-syllable consonant-vowel-consonant phonetically balanced Hebrew words (i.e., in each list, every consonant appears once and every vowel appears twice). For each participant, two lists of ten words were presented. The participants were instructed to repeat each word they heard and to guess if they were unsure. The results were manually recorded using the experimenter and expressed as a percentage of correct words.

2.2.3. HeBio Sentence in Quiet

This test included 33 sentence lists. Each list included 20 sentences, with five sentences from each of the four speakers (two men and two women). Sentences were composed without restrictions on complexity, vocabulary, or phonemic content. They did not include names of people, places, or objects. The mean sentence length was 6.12 with a standard deviation of 1.70. The sentences were aimed at reflecting examples of the daily discourse of adults and were meaningful, yet not predictable. They were scored according to the number of words that were repeated correctly out of the total number of words in the sentence. Errors related to tense, person (number and sex), stem, or verb pattern were counted as incorrect, but reporting the correct words in a different order than in the original sentence (e.g., reporting ‘the dog I bought’ instead of ‘I bought the dog’) did not affect the scoring. The intelligibility score for each sentence was expressed as a percentage of correct words.

2.2.4. Adaptive HeBio in Noise

In the adaptive paradigm, the intensity of the speech-shaped noise (SSN) varied adaptively using a one-down-one-up procedure to target the SNR at which 50% of the speech was identified correctly, that is, the SRTn. The initial SNR was +10 dB, with a step size of 5 dB. When participants did not repeat a sentence correctly (according to the criterion), the noise decreased or increased by 2 dB steps. The intensity steps were maintained at 2 dB until the end of the list. The estimated SRTn thresholds for each list were calculated according to the mean SNR of the last ten sentences in each list. The intensity of the speech was kept constant at 65 dB SPL, which was chosen so that the utterances were clearly audible for all participants. The SSN was a steady-state narrow-band noise that matched the existing speech signals and was created using the Fast Fourier Transform of all speech files. The attenuation rate of the SSN was 12 dB/octave at 1000 Hz. Both noises had similar frequency ranges and long-term-average-speech spectra as the sentences and were normalized to have similar root mean square (RMS) amplitudes.

2.2.5. Sentences for Children

This included eight lists of 20 sentences each, which were taken from a pool of sentences in Hebrew created according to the hearing-in-noise test (HINT) criteria [18]. The sentences in each list were equal in length (five to nine syllables per sentence) and presented at 65 dB SPL by a recorded female voice. The test was conducted in quiet conditions for all children.

2.3. Design and Procedure

All participants were tested individually in a quiet room, at a distance of one meter from a loudspeaker. Upon arrival, all participants received an explanation of the experimental tasks. During the tests, CI users used their devices with the program and setting they preferred for everyday use. If participants wore a contralateral hearing aid, they were allowed to wear it during testing. Demographic and background data were obtained (via questionnaire) and the following tests were performed: Children were evaluated on the Hebrew DIN test, as well as HAB and sentences in quiet tests. Adults were evaluated on the Hebrew DIN test, HAB in quiet and noise tests, and HeBio sentences in quiet and noise tests. The HeBio sentences in the quiet and in noise tests were delivered using the Advanced Bionics ListPlayer software on a Lenovo laptop computer via a Genelec 810A loudspeaker and a MAYA 44 external sound card. For the adaptive procedure, the ListPlayer software controlled the noise level according to the relevant SNRs. The sentence intensity was calibrated to 65 dB SPL at the participant’s location using a sound-level meter with a calibration tone of 1000 Hz. The HAB and sentences for the children were delivered from the same laptop, loudspeaker, and external sound card. The Hebrew DIN was delivered via an I-PAD connected directly to a Genelec 810A loudspeaker. The calibration of the intensity was performed using a sound-level meter. In the NH group, pure-tone audiometric thresholds were assessed using an Interacoustics-AD629 audiometer with headphones. The full experimental session lasted no more than one hour, and breaks were offered upon the participants’ request.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

A statistical analysis was conducted using SPSS Statistics 26, IBM Corp (Armonk, NY, USA). Age (children, adults) and hearing status (NH, CI users) effects were analyzed using univariate ANOVA. Among the CIs, independent-sample t-tests were used to test the age differences between children and prelingual adults, and the onset-of-deafness differences between pre- and post-lingual adults. Participants in different age groups performed tests of speech perception appropriate to their respective levels to avoid ceiling/floor effects. Therefore, comparisons between hearing status in these tests, as well as correlations between them, were conducted separately for each age group, using MANOVA and Pearson correlation.

3. Results

3.1. Hebrew DIN in CI Users and NH Aged-Matched Controls

3.1.1. Age and Hearing Status

Main effects were observed for both age (F(1,87) = 17.371, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.173) and hearing status (F(1,87) = 29.362, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.261), but not for hearing status × age interaction (F(1,87) = 0.121, p = 0.729, partial η2 = 0.001). Figure 1 presents the mean SRTn for each of the four groups. All CI users had higher SRTn (mean = −3.160, SE = 0.542) than their NH peers (mean = −7.160, SE = 0.502). The children had a higher SRTn (mean = −3.622, SE = 0.614) than the adults (mean = −6.698, SE = 0.409).

3.1.2. Age and Onset-of-Deafness

Figure 2 presents Hebrew DIN SRTn for the three CI groups. Pre-lingual children had a higher Hebrew DIN SRTn than pre-lingual adults (t(26) = −1.966, p = 0.03). Pre-lingual children also had a higher Hebrew DIN SRTn than the post-lingual adults (t(26) = −1.637, p = 0.05). No difference was found between pre- and post-lingual adults (t(28) = 0.736, p = 0.766).

3.2. Hebrew DIN and Other Speech Perception Tests

3.2.1. Children

A significant main effect was found for hearing status (F(3,23) = 17.849, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.700). Table 1 section A shows that children with CIs had a higher Hebrew DIN SRTn and lower word and sentence accuracy than NH children. No correlation was found between the Hebrew DIN SRTn and HAB accuracy (Figure 3A). A significant negative association was found between the Hebrew DIN SRTn and sentence accuracy in the quiet test for children with CIs (Figure 3B).

3.2.2. Pre-Lingual CI Users Adults

A significant main effect was found for the hearing status (F(2,25) = 31.338, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.715). Table 1 section B and Figure 4A show that young adults with pre-lingual CIs had higher Hebrew DIN and HeBio SRTn values than young adults with NH.
Significant negative associations were found between the Hebrew DIN SRTn values and the accuracy of the HAB and Hebio in quiet tests (Figure 3C,D). A marginally significant correlation was found between the Hebrew DIN test and adaptive HeBio in noise SRTn (Figure 3E).

3.2.3. Post-Lingual CI Adults Users

A significant main effect was found for the hearing status (F(2,27) = 35.033, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.722). Table 1 section C and Figure 4B show that older adults with post-lingual CI had higher Hebrew DIN and HeBio SRTn values than NH older adults. A significant negative association was found between Hebrew DIN SRTn and HeBio in quiet test accuracy, and a significant positive association was found between the SRTn of Hebrew DIN and adaptive HeBio in noise (Figure 3G,H). A marginally significant correlation was found between Hebrew DIN SRTn and HAB in quiet test accuracy (Figure 3F).

4. Discussion

In the current study, we examined the Hebrew DIN test thresholds in groups of CI users in different age groups, along with different onsets of deafness (pre-lingual versus post-lingual deafness) and their NH peers. As expected, we found that the Hebrew DIN SRTn was higher among all the CI groups than among their NH peers. There was also an effect of age, as children had a higher SRTn than adults, but there was no difference between pre-lingual CI and post-lingual CI adults. Finally, we observed a significant correlation between Hebrew DIN scores and most speech perception tests.
The finding that children (both NH and CI users) achieved a higher Hebrew DIN SRTn than adults is supported by previous studies showing that children struggle more than adults in noisy environments and require more favorable SNRs to perform as well as adults [19,20]. Also, the finding that CI users required a higher mean Hebrew DIN SRTn than NH individuals is supported by previous studies [12,13,14]. Nevertheless, in more difficult tests, such as sentences in noise (HeBio), the difference in SRTn between CI adult users and their NH peers was much larger (mean of 13–14 dB as opposed to 3–5 dB in the DIN test). Additionally, approximately half of the children using CIs and 20–33% of the adult CI groups had Hebrew DIN SRTn within the range of their NH peers, and no overlap was observed in the more challenging sentences in the noise (HeBio) test. Therefore, although the Hebrew DIN test differentiates between CI users and NH peers, its discerning is insufficient. Additional tests with a range of listening conditions and linguistic redundancies are warranted to fully assess the speech perception abilities of CI users in daily life.
Post-lingual CI users develop linguistic skills through full auditory input, whereas pre-lingual users rely on degraded auditory stimuli via their implant devices. Consequently, post-lingual CI users were hypothesized to have better SRTn than pre-lingual CI users. However, the results did not support this hypothesis, as there was no significant difference in the mean Hebrew DIN SRTn between these groups. One possible explanation is that the DIN test may not be sufficiently sensitive to measure the effects of early auditory exposure. Another explanation is that the large variability in the results of the pre-lingual CI users, with some individuals receiving very poor results (positive SRTn) and others receiving normal thresholds, may have blurred the differences between the two groups. This also suggests that factors other than auditory exposure (e.g., duration CI usage, auditory training, etc.) may play a role in speech perception abilities.
Marginal to significant associations were found between the Hebrew DIN test and other standard speech perception tests among CI users. This finding is consistent with other studies that have shown correlations between the DIN test and standard speech perception tests using words and sentences [13,14]. The high correlation observed underscores the DIN test’s utility in comprehensively assessing speech perception abilities in CI users. Furthermore, the alignment between the DIN test outcomes and those of traditional speech perception tests reinforces the validity of using the DIN test as a complementary tool in clinical assessments.
The DIN test was originally developed for hearing screening [11,21]. Nevertheless, it has been increasingly used as a part of speech perception evaluation battery for CI users [22]. Therefore, it is important to note that the DIN test alone is insufficient for a comprehensive evaluation of both pre- and post-implantation, and other speech perception tests in both quiet and noisy environments should be incorporated. A notable application of the DIN is its use as a remote assessment to gauge CI user function by CI device manufacturers. This practice significantly benefits CI users by reducing the need for routine visits to distant CI centers. However, it is important to remember that while valuable, this method provides a relatively nonspecific measure of function.
The relatively limited number of participants in each group may lead to biased results. However, this sample size is accepted in relevant studies [22,23]. Future research may benefit from replicating this study with larger groups. Additionally, not all tests were administered to both children and adults, as some tests are not suited to the linguistic abilities and working memory capacity of children. Longitudinal studies can track changes in speech perception in noise over time with CI use. The current study compared children and adults, and adding a comparison between young and older adults can expand the results of the existing findings.

5. Conclusions

The results of the present study indicate that the Hebrew DIN test is an efficient and rapid measure for evaluating speech perception under noisy conditions with minimal linguistic requirements. It is highly correlated with other speech perception measures and can differentiate between CI users and NH controls, although it may not be sufficiently sensitive to differentiate between CI users with different onsets of deafness. The Hebrew DIN test is sensitive to age differences, with children performing worse than adults in both the NH and CI groups. Taking these characteristics together, we recommend the Hebrew DIN test as an additional test to the standard battery of speech perception tests for both CI candidates and users.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, R.T.-S. and L.F.; methodology, R.T.-S. and L.F.; validation, R.T.-S. and L.F.; formal analysis, L.F.; investigation, R.T.-S. and L.F.; resources, R.T.-S. and L.F.; data curation, R.T.-S. and L.F.; writing—original draft preparation, R.T.-S. and L.F.; writing—review and editing, R.T.-S. and L.F.; visualization, R.T.-S.; supervision, R.T.-S. and L.F.; project administration, R.T.-S. and L.F. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of Ariel University (AU-HEA-RTS-20201103, 3 November 2020).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data for this study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Ziv Mordechai Avshalom, Daniel Koren, Tali Bronshtein, Rotem Ben-Haim, Daniel Zuarets, Shaked Alek, Michal Mualem for collecting data for this study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Demographic information and background data for the three CI groups.
Table A1. Demographic information and background data for the three CI groups.
No.Age (y)GenderNo. of CIsImplanted EarAge at OnsetAge at 1st CIAge at 2nd CIDuration Implant Use (y)HA Use Type of CIEtiology
Pre-lingual CI Children
17.8M1L2.56.5--5.3YABUnknown
25.9M1L1.21.5--4.4YABMeningitis
37.3F1L00.9--6.4YCCMV
46.5F2R, L00.750.755.75ABGenetic
56.5M2R, L01.41.45.1ABUnknown
66.4F2R, L00.90.95.5CUnknown
77.4F1R03.8--3.6YABUnknown
86.6M2R, L1.21.51.55.1CInfection
97.9M2R, L00.90.97CGenetic
105.5M2R, L2.33.13.12.4CMeningitis
1110.5F2R, L01.11.19.4CUnknown
129.6M1L04.5--5.1YCCMV
1311F2R, L0147 CUnknown
Pre-lingual CI adults users
125.5F2R, L1.541421.5CCMV
223F2R, L021321CFamilial
323F2R, L021421CFamilial
428M1R06--22CUnknown
530F2R, L031427CGenetic
627M2R, L032624CGenetic
728.5F1R08--20.5YABGenetic
816F2R, L011015CUnknown
916M2R, L01615CUnknown
1026M2R, L042522CUnknown
1130F2R, L031427CFamilial
1220F2R, L03717CUnknown
1323F2R, L021121CUnknown
1415F2R, L01414CGenetic
1514M2R, L01.251.2512.55 CGenetic
Post-lingual CI adults users
136F1L1531--5ABUnknown
249F1L3240--9YMOtosclerosis
360F1L1045--15CGenetic
422F1R1417--3MUnknown
524F2R, L819215ABUnknown
668F1R5064--4YCGenetic
755F1L3545--10YCMeniere
856F2R, L1851545MUnknown
962M2R, L3559603MOtosclerosis
1029F1L1028--1YABMeningitis
1162M1L4058--4YMOtosclerosis
1277F1R4773--4YCGenetic
1374F1R5772--2YABUnknown
1460F1L3858--2YCAutoimmune
1532F1R1031--1YCUnknown
No. = number, (y) = years; Gender: F = female, M = male; Implanted Ear: R = right, L = left; HA = hearing aid; CI = cochlear implant; Type of CI: C = Cochlear, AB = Advanced Bionics, M = Med-EL, CMV = Cytomegalovirus.

References

  1. Wilson, B.S. Getting a decent (but sparse) signal to the brain for users of cochlear implants. Hear. Res. 2015, 322, 24–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Zeng, F.G. Celebrating the one millionth cochlear implant. JASA Express Lett. 2022, 2, 077201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Carlson, M.L.; Sladen, D.P.; Haynes, D.S.; Driscoll, C.L.; DeJong, M.D.; Erickson, H.C.; Sunderhaus, L.W.; Hedley-Williams, A.; Rosenzweig, E.A.; Davis, T.J.; et al. Evidence for the expansion of pediatric cochlear implant candidacy. Otol. Neurotol. 2015, 36, 43–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Gifford, R.H.; Dorman, M.F.; Shallop, J.K.; Sydlowski, S.A. Evidence for the expansion of adult cochlear implant candidacy. Ear Hear. 2010, 31, 186–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Zwolan, T.A.; Basura, G. Determining Cochlear Implant Candidacy in Adults: Limitations, Expansions, and Opportunities for Improvement. Semin. Hear. 2021, 42, 331–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Caldwell, A.; Nittrouer, S. Speech perception in noise by children with cochlear implants. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2013, 56, 13–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Srinivasan, A.G.; Padilla, M.; Shannon, R.V.; Landsberger, D.M. Improving speech perception in noise with current focusing in cochlear implant users. Hear. Res. 2013, 299, 29–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Taitelbaum-Swead, R.; Fostick, L. The Effect of Age, Type of Noise, and Cochlear Implants on Adaptive Sentence-in-Noise Task. J. Clin. Med. 2022, 11, 5872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Taitelbaum-Swead, R.; Dahan, T.; Katzenel, U.; Dorman, M.F.; Litvak, L.M.; Fostick, L. AzBio Sentence test in Hebrew (HeBio): Development, preliminary validation, and the effect of noise. Cochlear Implant. Int. 2022, 23, 270–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Rönnberg, J.; Lunner, T.; Zekveld, A.; Sörqvist, P.; Danielsson, H.; Lyxell, B.; Dahlström, O.; Signoret, C.; Stenfelt, S.; Pichora-Fuller, M.K.; et al. The Ease of Language Understanding (ELU) model: Theoretical, empirical, and clinical advances. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Smits, C.; Theo Goverts, S.; Festen, J.M. The digits-in-noise test: Assessing auditory speech recognition abilities in noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2013, 133, 1693–1706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Vroegop, J.; Rodenburg-Vlot, M.; Goedegebure, A.; Doorduin, A.; Homans, N.; van der Schroeff, M. The Feasibility and Reliability of a Digits-in-Noise Test in the Clinical Follow-Up of Children With Mild to Profound Hearing Loss. Ear Hear. 2021, 42, 973–981. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Kaandorp, M.W.; Smits, C.; Merkus, P.; Goverts, S.T.; Festen, J.M. Assessing speech recognition abilities with digits in noise in cochlear implant and hearing aid users. Int. J. Audiol. 2015, 54, 48–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Willberg, T.; Sivonen, V.; Linder, P.; Dietz, A. Comparing the Speech Perception of Cochlear Implant Users with Three Different Finnish Speech Intelligibility Tests in Noise. J. Clin. Med. 2021, 10, 3666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Fostick, L.; Berlin, M.; Taitelbeum-Swead, R. Digit in Noise Test: Validation in Hebrew. Presented at the 56th Congress of the Israeli Speech, Hearing and Language Association, Tel Aviv, Israel, 12–13 February 2020. [Google Scholar]
  16. Boothroyd, A. Statistical theory of the speech discrimination score. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1968, 43, 362–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Taitelbaum-Swead, R.; Kishon-Rabin, L.; Kaplan-Neeman, R.; Muchnik, C.; Kronenberg, J.; Hildesheimer, M. Speech perception of children using Nucleus, Clarion or Med-El cochlear implants. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 2005, 69, 1675–1683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Nilsson, M.; Soli, S.D.; Sullivan, J.A. Development of the Hearing in Noise Test for the measurement of speech reception thresholds in quiet and in noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1994, 95, 1085–1099. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Crandell, C.C.; Smaldino, J.J. Classroom Acoustics for Children with Normal Hearing and with Hearing Impairment. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2000, 31, 362–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Trau-Margalit, A.; Fostick, L.; Harel-Arbeli, T.; Nissanholtz-Gannot, R.; Taitelbaum-Swead, R. Speech recognition in noise task among children and young-adults: A pupillometry study. Front. Psychol. 2023, 14, 1188485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Smits, C.; Kapteyn, T.S.; Houtgast, T. Development and validation of an automatic speech-in-noise screening test by telephone. Int. J. Audiol. 2004, 43, 15–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Cullington, H.E.; Aidi, T. Is the digit triplet test an effective and acceptable way to assess speech recognition in adults using cochlear implants in a home environment? Cochlear Implant. Int. 2017, 18, 97–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. de Graaff, F.; Huysmans, E.; Qazi, O.U.; Vanpoucke, F.J.; Merkus, P.; Goverts, S.T.; Smits, C. The Development of Remote Speech Recognition Tests for Adult Cochlear Implant Users: The Effect of Presentation Mode of the Noise and a Reliable Method to Deliver Sound in Home Environments. Audiol. Neurootol. 2016, 21 (Suppl. 1), 48–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Box-and-whisker plot of Hebrew digits in noise (DIN) speech receptive thresholds in noise (SRTn) (dots beyond the whiskers symbolize outliers) for CI users (children, and pre- and post-lingual adults) and NH matched groups. CI, cochlear implants; NH, normal hearing; SRTn, speech reception thresholds in noise. Higher SRTn means worse performance. ** p < 0.01.
Figure 1. Box-and-whisker plot of Hebrew digits in noise (DIN) speech receptive thresholds in noise (SRTn) (dots beyond the whiskers symbolize outliers) for CI users (children, and pre- and post-lingual adults) and NH matched groups. CI, cochlear implants; NH, normal hearing; SRTn, speech reception thresholds in noise. Higher SRTn means worse performance. ** p < 0.01.
Audiolres 14 00038 g001
Figure 2. Box-and-whisker plot of Hebrew digits in noise (DIN) speech receptive thresholds in noise (SRTn) for three groups of CI users (pre-lingual CI children, pre-lingual CI and post-lingual CI adults users). CI, cochlear implants; NH, normal hearing; SRTn, speech reception thresholds in noise. Higher SRTn means worse performance. * p < 0.05.
Figure 2. Box-and-whisker plot of Hebrew digits in noise (DIN) speech receptive thresholds in noise (SRTn) for three groups of CI users (pre-lingual CI children, pre-lingual CI and post-lingual CI adults users). CI, cochlear implants; NH, normal hearing; SRTn, speech reception thresholds in noise. Higher SRTn means worse performance. * p < 0.05.
Audiolres 14 00038 g002
Figure 3. Correlations between speech receptive thresholds in noise (SRTn) on the Hebrew digits in noise (DIN) and results in speech perception tests for the CI study groups: (A) percentage of correct correct words on HAB word recognition test in quiet for children with CIs; (B) percentage of sentences in quiet for children with CIs, (C) percentage of correct words on HAB word recognition test in quiet for pre-lingual CI adults users, (D) percentage of HeBio sentences in quiet tests for pre-lingual CI adults users, (E) SRTn for adaptive HeBio sentences for pre-lingual CI adults users, (F) percentage of correct words on HAB word recognition test in quiet for postlingual CI adults users, (G) percentage of HeBio sentences in quiet tests for postlingual CI adults users, (H) SRTn for adaptive HeBio sentences for postlingual CI adults users. Hebrew DIN, digits in noise; SRTn, speech reception thresholds in noise, HAB, Hebrew Arthur Boothroyd. The lines on the figures represent linear regression trend lines.
Figure 3. Correlations between speech receptive thresholds in noise (SRTn) on the Hebrew digits in noise (DIN) and results in speech perception tests for the CI study groups: (A) percentage of correct correct words on HAB word recognition test in quiet for children with CIs; (B) percentage of sentences in quiet for children with CIs, (C) percentage of correct words on HAB word recognition test in quiet for pre-lingual CI adults users, (D) percentage of HeBio sentences in quiet tests for pre-lingual CI adults users, (E) SRTn for adaptive HeBio sentences for pre-lingual CI adults users, (F) percentage of correct words on HAB word recognition test in quiet for postlingual CI adults users, (G) percentage of HeBio sentences in quiet tests for postlingual CI adults users, (H) SRTn for adaptive HeBio sentences for postlingual CI adults users. Hebrew DIN, digits in noise; SRTn, speech reception thresholds in noise, HAB, Hebrew Arthur Boothroyd. The lines on the figures represent linear regression trend lines.
Audiolres 14 00038 g003
Figure 4. Box-and-whisker plot of the SRTn for the adaptive HeBio sentence test and the SRTn for the DIN test (dots beyond the whiskers symbolize outliers), for pre-lingual CI users (white) and NH adults (black) (A) and for post-lingual CI users (white) and NH adults (black) (B). CI, cochlear implants; NH, normal hearing; SRTn, speech reception thresholds in noise. ** p < 0.01.
Figure 4. Box-and-whisker plot of the SRTn for the adaptive HeBio sentence test and the SRTn for the DIN test (dots beyond the whiskers symbolize outliers), for pre-lingual CI users (white) and NH adults (black) (A) and for post-lingual CI users (white) and NH adults (black) (B). CI, cochlear implants; NH, normal hearing; SRTn, speech reception thresholds in noise. ** p < 0.01.
Audiolres 14 00038 g004
Table 1. Mean (standard deviations, SD) and results of univariate ANOVAs (F, p, and partial η2) between cochlear implant (CI) and normal hearing (NH) children (A), pre-lingual CI adult users (B), and post-lingual CI older adult users (C) in Hebrew digits in noise (DIN) and speech perception tests.
Table 1. Mean (standard deviations, SD) and results of univariate ANOVAs (F, p, and partial η2) between cochlear implant (CI) and normal hearing (NH) children (A), pre-lingual CI adult users (B), and post-lingual CI older adult users (C) in Hebrew digits in noise (DIN) and speech perception tests.
CINHF (1,27)pPartial η2
A. Children
DIN_SRTn−1.75 (5.98)−5.49 (2.18)5.080.0330.17
HAB_quiet0.78 (0.09)0.97 (0.04)52.87<0.0010.68
Sentences_quiet0.88 (0.10)0.97 (0.09)5.310.0300.18
B. Pre-lingual CI adult users
DIN_SRTn−5.15 (2.98)−9.29 (1.37)23.44<0.0010.47
HeBio_SRTn12.88 (6.91)−1.55 (1.68)61.47<0.0010.70
C. Post-lingual CI adult users
DIN_SRTn−4.44 (2.19)−8.36 (1.53)32.26<0.0010.54
HeBio_SRTn12.03 (5.48)−0.99 (2.24)72.53<0.0010.72
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Taitelbaum-Swead, R.; Fostick, L. Hebrew Digits in Noise (DIN) Test in Cochlear Implant Users and Normal Hearing Listeners. Audiol. Res. 2024, 14, 457-468. https://doi.org/10.3390/audiolres14030038

AMA Style

Taitelbaum-Swead R, Fostick L. Hebrew Digits in Noise (DIN) Test in Cochlear Implant Users and Normal Hearing Listeners. Audiology Research. 2024; 14(3):457-468. https://doi.org/10.3390/audiolres14030038

Chicago/Turabian Style

Taitelbaum-Swead, Riki, and Leah Fostick. 2024. "Hebrew Digits in Noise (DIN) Test in Cochlear Implant Users and Normal Hearing Listeners" Audiology Research 14, no. 3: 457-468. https://doi.org/10.3390/audiolres14030038

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop