Next Article in Journal
The Association between Serum 25(OH)D Status and Blood Pressure in Participants of a Community-Based Program Taking Vitamin D Supplements
Next Article in Special Issue
Reduced Educational Outcomes Persist into Adolescence Following Mild Iodine Deficiency in Utero, Despite Adequacy in Childhood: 15-Year Follow-Up of the Gestational Iodine Cohort Investigating Auditory Processing Speed and Working Memory
Previous Article in Journal
A Cross-Sectional Study of Nutrient Intake and Health Status among Older Adults in Yogyakarta Indonesia
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Impact of Iodine Status on the Recall Rate of the Screening Program for Congenital Hypothyroidism: Findings from Two National Studies in Iran
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Maternal Iodine Intake and Offspring Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Results from a Large Prospective Cohort Study

1
Division of Infection Control and Environmental Health, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, 0456 Oslo, Norway
2
Department of Nursing and Health Promotion, Faculty of Health Sciences, Oslo and Akershus University College of Applied Sciences, 0167 Oslo, Norway
3
Department of Research and Development, TINE SA, 0902 Oslo, Norway
4
Division of Mental and Physical Health, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, 0456 Oslo, Norway
5
Section of Health, Developmental and Personality Psychology, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, 0315 Oslo, Norway
6
Pharmaco Epidemiology and Drug Safety Research Group, School of Pharmacy, University of Oslo, 0315 Oslo, Norway
7
Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, 0315 Oslo, Norway
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2017, 9(11), 1239; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu9111239
Submission received: 10 October 2017 / Revised: 3 November 2017 / Accepted: 8 November 2017 / Published: 13 November 2017
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Iodine and Health throughout the Lifecourse)

Abstract

:
Current knowledge about the relationship between mild to moderately inadequate maternal iodine intake and/or supplemental iodine on child neurodevelopment is sparse. Using information from 77,164 mother-child pairs in the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study, this study explored associations between maternal iodine intake and child attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) diagnosis, registered in the Norwegian Patient Registry and maternally-reported child ADHD symptoms at eight years of age. Pregnant women reported food and supplement intakes by questionnaire in gestational week 22. In total, 1725 children (2.2%) were diagnosed with ADHD. In non-users of supplemental iodine (53,360 mothers), we found no association between iodine intake from food and risk of child ADHD diagnosis (p = 0.89), while low iodine from food (<200 µg/day) was associated with higher child ADHD symptom scores (adjusted difference in score up to 0.08 standard deviation (SD), p < 0.001, n = 19,086). In the total sample, we found no evidence of beneficial effects of maternal use of iodine-containing supplements (n = 23,804) on child ADHD diagnosis or symptom score. Initiation of iodine supplement use in gestational weeks 0–12 was associated with an increased risk of child ADHD (both measures). In conclusion, insufficient maternal iodine intake was associated with increased child ADHD symptom scores at eight years of age, but not with ADHD diagnosis. No reduction of risk was associated with maternal iodine supplement use.

1. Introduction

Iodine deficiency (ID) is among the most common micronutrient deficiencies worldwide and is recognized by the World Health Organization (WHO) as the number one cause of potentially preventable brain damage [1]. Iodine is essential for the production of thyroid hormones, which in turn are involved in multiple pathways in neurodevelopment [2]. Severe maternal ID is associated with impaired brain development, but less is known about the potential consequences of mild to moderate ID, commonly seen in populations of both low and high income countries [3].
The worldwide prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children and adolescents is estimated to be in the range 6.7–7.8%, based on the criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV) [4]. ADHD is in the fifth edition of DSM (DSM-V), defined as a persistent pattern of inattention and/or hyperactivity-impulsivity, that interferes with functioning or development [5]. It is associated with significant morbidity and disability, and impairments persist into adulthood in the majority of cases [6]. The children have an increased risk of school failure, emotional difficulties, poor peer relations, and trouble with the law [6]. ADHD is also often linked with comorbidities, such as oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, autism spectrum disorders, anxiety, depression, and substance use disorders [6]. Causes of ADHD are multifactorial and largely unknown and involve both genetic and environmental factors [7]. The heritability of ADHD is estimated to be 83–92% in children and 56–84% in adults [8]. Several nutritional factors have been investigated as potential causal factors (e.g., zinc, magnesium and polyunsaturated fatty acids), but currently there is no consistent evidence linking diet to ADHD [7]. An association between maternal iodine status and child ADHD has been suggested in several studies [9,10,11].
This paper is a follow-up of a previous publication from The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study (MoBa), where we found that inadequate iodine intake in pregnancy was associated with maternally reported child language delay, behavior problems (externalizing and internalizing) and fine motor delay, but not with gross motor delay at three years of age, or risk of not walking unaided at 17 months of age [12]. Regarding the maternal use of iodine-containing supplements, we found no evidence of beneficial effects. However, the results indicated a negative impact on child behavior problems when mothers had inadequate iodine intake from food and initiated use of supplemental iodine in the first trimester of pregnancy [12].
The main aim of the current study was to explore the association between iodine intake from food in pregnancy (as a proxy for long-term iodine intake and status) and (i) risk of specialist-diagnosed ADHD in the child and (ii) maternal report of child ADHD symptoms at eight years of age. A second aim was to explore the associations between maternal use of iodine-containing supplements prior to and during pregnancy and the same outcome measures.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Subjects and Design

This study is based on data from MoBa, a prospective population-based pregnancy cohort study, conducted by the Norwegian Institute of Public Health [13]. Women pregnant in their first trimester were recruited from all over Norway during the years 1999 to 2008. Participants were recruited to the study by postal invitation before the routine free ultrasound examination at around gestational week 18. The women were asked to provide blood and urine samples at baseline and to answer questionnaires (in Norwegian) at regular intervals during pregnancy and after birth. More than 99% of the participants were of Caucasian origin. Pregnancy and birth records from the Medical Birth Registry of Norway are linked to the MoBa database [14]. The women consented to participation in 41% of the pregnancies. The cohort now includes 114,500 children, 95,200 mothers and 75,200 fathers. The current study is based on version 9 of the quality-assured data files released for research in 2016.
To be included in this study, mothers had to have responded to a general questionnaire at around gestational week (GW) 17, and a food frequency questionnaire (FFQ) at around GW 22. Only singleton pregnancies were included. Mothers reporting the use of thyroid medication at any time during pregnancy were excluded from the study. Only participants with information on all covariates were included in the analysis because of the large sample size and low rates of missing values. FFQs with more than three blank pages or with calculated energy intakes <4.5 MJ or >20 MJ were excluded [15].
A total of 77,164 mother-child pairs were included in this study, and for 27,945 there were data on maternally reported ADHD scores when the child was aged eight years (Figure 1). For the main analysis, restricted to participants who had not reported use of iodine-containing supplements in the FFQ, 53,360 mother-child pairs were included (19,086 for ADHD score).

2.2. Exposure Variables—Iodine Intake from Food and Supplements

The MoBa FFQ [16] was specifically designed for the MoBa study and was in use from 2002. It was completed by participating women at around GW 22. It is a semi-quantitative questionnaire, designed to capture dietary habits and use of dietary supplements during the first half of pregnancy and included questions about the intake of 255 food items or dishes [15]. Intake of specific foods and nutrients were calculated based on standard Norwegian portion sizes, the Norwegian food composition table, analysis of Norwegian milk and food samples [17,18] and data on the content of more than 1000 food supplements collected from suppliers [19].
As reported previously [12], the MoBa FFQ has been shown to be a valid tool for ranking pregnant women, according to high and low intakes of energy, nutrients and foods [20]. Iodine was validated separately and iodine intake by the FFQ, including supplemental iodine, showed good agreement with the reference methods (triangular validity coefficient for total iodine intake by the FFQ was 0.62 (95% confidence interval (CI): 0.46, 0.77)) [21,22]. When dividing participants into quintiles of their iodine intake estimates, 67% were correctly classified by the FFQ compared to the 4-day weighed food diary (classified into the same or adjacent quintiles), and 63% were correctly classified by the FFQ compared to 24-h urinary iodine. Less than 5% were grossly misclassified [20]. In non-users of iodine supplements, estimated median iodine intake from food was 122 µg/day, calculated from the FFQ, 120 µg/day from the 4-day food diary, and 122 µg/day, based on 24-h urinary iodine excretion (assuming that 90% is excreted in the urine) [21,22].
An analysis of urinary iodine in spot samples from GW 18 has also been performed in a MoBa subsample comprising women with singleton deliveries (Abel et al. [23]). Median spot urinary iodine concentration (UIC) in non-users of iodine supplements and thyroid medication (n = 1950) was 61 µg/L (interquartile range (IQR): 32–104 µg/L). In iodine supplement users (n = 988), the median UIC was 86 µg/L (IQR: 43–140 µg/L). Total iodine intake, calculated by the FFQ, correlated with spot UIC (µg/g creatinine) (Spearman’s correlation: r = 0.36, p < 0.001).
Iodine intake from supplements was categorized into three groups (0, 1–200 and >200 µg/day), and the timing of initiation of iodine containing supplements up to GW 22 was divided in four categories (never, week 0–26 before pregnancy, GW 0–12 and GW ≥ 13).

2.3. ADHD Diagnosis

We obtained information about children’s ADHD diagnoses from the Norwegian Patient Registry (NPR) [24]. From 2008, all government-owned and government-financed hospitals and outpatient clinics have mandatorily reported individual level diagnoses, defined in the tenth revision of the International Classification of Disease (ICD-10) [25], to the NPR, in order to receive financial reimbursement. Using individual personal identification numbers, diagnostic information from NPR was linked to MoBa. Thus, all MoBa children registered with an ICD-10-diagnosis of hyperkinetic disorder (HKD, coded as F90.0, F90.1, F90.8, or F90.9) between 2008 and 2015 were identified and regarded as having ADHD.
In an international meta-study, prevalence estimates were 4.1% lower using the ICD-10 than the DSM-IV criteria [26]. HKD requires the combination of persisting inattentive and hyperactive symptoms before the age of six and impairment in two or more settings, and as a result HKD is a severe subtype nested within ADHD, as defined by the DSM [27]. In comparison to ADHD, as defined by the DSM, HKD is characterized by a higher proportion of individuals with impaired language and motor development [28].

2.4. ADHD Symptom Score

Child ADHD symptoms were assessed in the eight-year-old questionnaire from MoBa on a four-point Likert scale (never/rarely, sometimes, often, or very often) covering inattention problems (nine items) and hyperactivity/impulsivity (nine items) from the ADHD Rating Scale [29]. Mean scores for inattention symptoms, hyperactivity symptoms, and total ADHD symptoms were calculated and standardized. The ADHD subscales of inattention and hyperactivity were correlated (Spearman’s correlation coefficient: r = 0.53; p < 0.001). There was high agreement between maternally reported ADHD scores at eight years of age and registered ADHD diagnosis, and the median score was +2.4 SD (IQR: 1.3, 3.6) in children with ADHD diagnoses.

2.5. Covariates

A predefined set of covariates were included in the analysis, based on previous knowledge and a theoretic causal diagram (Supplemental Figure S1). Variables on birth outcomes and data from after birth were not included, as they could represent potential mediators on the causal pathway. Also, maternal mental health was not included since it could be an indicator of thyroid dysfunction [30]. For models with continuous outcome measures, child sex and birth season were included, to improve the precision of effect estimates, as these are important determinants of ADHD. Included covariates were obtained from different sources: Maternal age, child sex, and birth season (January–April, May–August, September–December) were obtained from the Medical Birth Registry of Norway. Maternally reported pre-pregnancy body weight and height, for the calculation of body mass index (BMI), maternal education (≤12, 13–16, ≥17 years), parity (previous pregnancies ≥22 weeks: 0, 1, ≥2), and use of folic acid supplements within the interval from 4 weeks beforehand, to 8 weeks after conception (yes/no) were obtained from the MoBa questionnaire 1 at GW 17. Energy intake, fiber intake (as a marker of a healthy dietary pattern), and total intake of the long chain polyunsaturated n-3 fatty acids, eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA) and docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) from food and dietary supplements, were calculated, based on the FFQ. Information on smoking in pregnancy was obtained from questionnaire 1 and, if available, questionnaires 3 (GW 30) and 4 (child’s age: six months) for three categories: no reported smoking in pregnancy, reported occasional smoking or stopped smoking before GW 12, and daily smoking at any time in pregnancy and had not stopped smoking before GW 12.
Other potential covariates were explored, but not included in the final analysis, since they had no/only negligible effects on the estimates of interest—maternal intake of alcohol (g/day), year of birth, marital status, paternal education, parents’ incomes, bilingual parent(s) (mother tongue other than Norwegian: yes/no), and maternal chronic illness (asthma, diabetes, inflammatory bowel disease, rheumatic disease, epilepsy, multiple sclerosis or cancer: yes/no).

2.6. Ethics

The MoBa was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and written informed consent was obtained from all participants. MoBa has obtained a license from the Norwegian Data Inspectorate. The current study was approved by The Regional Committee for Medical Research Ethics South East Norway 2013/594.

2.7. Statistics

The association between iodine intake and risk of ADHD diagnoses was explored with Cox proportional hazards regression. Associations to maternally reported ADHD symptoms at eight years of age were modelled by generalized linear models, with gamma family and log link functions. All models were adjusted for random effects of sibling clusters, since some mothers participated with more than one pregnancy. Results are reported as hazard ratios (HR) for ADHD diagnoses and standardized betas for ADHD symptom scores and include robust 95% confidence intervals (CI). A p-value < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
To isolate the effect of long term iodine intake, we performed analyses on associations between iodine intake from food and ADHD outcomes, restricted to participants who had not reported the use of supplemental iodine in the FFQ. We examined a potential nonlinear dose-response relationship between iodine intake from food and ADHD diagnosis, by modelling iodine intake using restricted cubic splines with four knots (at percentiles 5, 35, 65 and 95, corresponding to iodine intakes of 54, 102, 143 and 245 µg/day).
All regression models (including crude models) were adjusted for energy intake (as two piecewise linear splines, knot position at 10.5 MJ) to control for measurement error in calculated iodine intake. Adjusted models also included the following baseline maternal and family characteristics based on a causal diagram: maternal age, education, parity, pre-pregnancy BMI (including BMI squared), fiber intake, and smoking in pregnancy. Child sex and birth season were also included in models with continuous outcome variables, since they are important predictors of ADHD. Possible interaction effects were explored for maternal BMI, age, smoking, child sex and parity. The reference level of iodine intake was set at 160 µg/day, the estimated average requirement (EAR) for iodine during pregnancy by the Institute of Medicine [31]. P-values are reported for overall associations between exposure and outcomes, by testing the coefficients of all spline transformations equal to zero. The tests for non-linearity were performed by testing the coefficients of the second and third spline transformations equal to zero. Potential interactions were explored by testing all interaction coefficients equal to zero. Graphs and tabular results were calculated based on the spline models.
We also explored associations between iodine intake from food and ADHD outcome with iodine intake categorized (six categories), and the results were in agreement with results from the flexible spline models (results not included).
The impact of dosage of iodine from supplements was explored by including interaction terms between iodine from supplements (divided in three categories: 0, 1–200 and >200 µg/day) and iodine from food (in two categories: less than the EAR (<160) and above (≥160 µg/day)). The models were adjusted with the same covariates as described above, but in addition, maternal folic acid supplement within the interval from 4 weeks beforehand to 8 weeks after conception and total EPA/DHA intake were included in the adjusted models.
The impact of the timing of introduction of iodine-containing supplements (reported use 0–26 weeks before conception, first reported use GW 0–12, or GW ≥ 13) was explored in participants who had reported an intake of 1–200 µg/day of iodine from supplements in the FFQ, and who had also reported timing of use in the general questionnaires. Timing was explored in the same way as dosage, including an interaction term with iodine from food (above/below the EAR) and adjusting for the same covariates.
Additionally, we performed matched control analyses to further assess the potential causal effects of iodine supplement use (both for dosage and timing). Since supplemental iodine was not generally recommended for pregnant women in Norway at the time, we restricted the control group to participants who had reported the use of supplemental vitamins and minerals other than the recommended (which included folic acid, vitamin D, and iron (only in iron deficient individuals)). This was possible since some multi-supplements contained iodine whereas others did not. More comparable controls enabled us to control for the health-seeking behavior of taking an additional supplement as a precaution, and, to some extent, to control for confounding by other nutrients in the multi-supplements.
Statistical analyses were performed using STATA software (version 14.0; Stata Corp., College Station, TX, USA). Including the package xblc for calculating tabular estimates based on the flexible spline models [32].

3. Results

3.1. Background Characteristics

The calculated iodine intake from food (not supplements) in the first half of pregnancy ranged from 9 to 792 µg/day (median: 121 µg/day; IQR: 89–162 µg/day) and 74% had an estimated intake from food lower than the EAR in pregnancy (160 µg/day). Supplemental iodine was reported by 31% of the pregnant women (range of the average intake during GW 0–22: 1–1264 µg/day, median: 107 µg/day; IQR: 58–150 µg/day). ADHD diagnosis was registered in 1725 children (2.2%) by December 2015, and the median age at diagnosis was 8.2 years (IQR: 7.0, 9.5 years). The median age of all children in our MoBa sample (n = 77,164) was 9.9 years in December 2015 (range: 6.4–13.8 years).
The maternal and child characteristics by iodine intake from food and supplements are shown in Table 1 and Table 2. There were only minor differences in background characteristics between exposure groups. Mothers with the estimated highest iodine intake from food (>250 µg/day) included more mothers under the age of 25 years (17% vs. 10% of mothers with iodine intake ≤250 µg/day), more mothers with ≤12 years education (42% vs. 30%), and a higher prevalence of any smoking in pregnancy (26% vs. 21%). Maternal iodine intake from food was mostly determined by the intake of milk and yoghurt (Spearman’s correlation coefficient: r = 0.85; p < 0.001), but iodine intake from food was also related to the calculated total energy intake (r = 0.56, p < 0.001) and to other nutrients and foods (Table 2). Iodine supplement use was more commonly reported in mothers with no previous pregnancies (35% vs. 27% in mothers with parity ≥1), and less frequently in the mothers with ≤12 years education (27% vs. 32% in mothers with >12 years education). The use of folic acid supplements and/or n-3 fatty acid supplements were more prevalent in mothers who used supplemental iodine (95% vs. 82% in non-users). Iodine intake from food did not differ between iodine supplement users and non-users (difference in means 0.3 µg/day, p = 0.56).

3.2. Iodine Intake from Food and Risk of ADHD

Associations between maternal iodine intake from food and child ADHD are illustrated in Figure 2 and Figure 3. Iodine from food was significantly associated with maternally reported child ADHD symptoms at eight years of age (adjusted p overall = 0.001) (Figure 3), but not with risk of child ADHD diagnosis (Figure 2). The negative effect associated with a low iodine intake (<200 µg/day) on ADHD symptoms was primarily seen in the inattention subscale (p overall < 0.001), and it did not reach statistical significance for the hyperactivity subscale (p overall = 0.19). Tabular results from unadjusted and adjusted analyses are provided in Supplemental Tables S1 and S2.
No significant interaction effects were detected for iodine with the covariates, BMI, education, parity, smoking, or child sex. The associations between maternal iodine intake from food and ADHD outcomes by child sex are presented in Supplemental Figure S2.

3.3. Iodine Intake from Supplements and Risk of ADHD

Supplemental iodine originated almost exclusively from multi-nutrient supplements, and only nine mothers reported use of supplements only containing iodine.
Among iodine supplement users who reported taking up to 200 µg supplemental iodine per day in the FFQ, 64% also gave information on timing of use in the general questionnaires. Of these mothers, 39% reported their first use at 0–26 weeks before pregnancy, 29% in GW 0–12 and 32% in GW 13 or later. There was no data on the dosage or frequency of use before pregnancy, only information on any use (yes or no) in the given time period.
The potential impact of iodine from supplements was explored in two groups by maternal iodine intake from food (less than or above the EAR of iodine from food) (Table 3 and Table 4).
The maternal use of supplemental iodine was associated with an increased risk of child ADHD diagnosis (Table 3) and a higher mean ADHD symptom score (Table 4). The effect estimates were somewhat attenuated when restricting the reference group to participants who had reported taking supplements containing one or more vitamins or minerals other than the recommended supplements (folic acid, vitamin D, and iron) (matched controls). In participants with low iodine intakes from food, iodine supplement use initiated in GW 0–12 was associated with a ~29% increased risk of ADHD diagnosis (95% CI: 0–67%, p = 0.053) and a 0.06 SD higher average score on ADHD symptoms at eight years of age (95% CI: 0.01–0.11, p = 0.037) (Table 4). In participants with high iodine intakes from food, the results were not consistent. Initiating iodine supplement use in the first trimester was associated with an increased risk of ADHD diagnosis, whereas use before pregnancy was associated with increased child ADHD symptom scores.

4. Discussion

The main findings in this study were that maternal iodine intake of less than ~200 µg/day was associated with an increased risk of maternally reported child ADHD symptoms at eight years of age, but not significantly with risk of child ADHD diagnosis. Also, we found no evidence of any beneficial effect of supplemental iodine in pregnancy. On the contrary, initiating iodine supplement use within the first trimester in mothers with inadequate iodine intake from food (<EAR) was associated with both an increased risk of ADHD diagnosis and higher ADHD symptom score at eight years of age. A negative effect of iodine supplement use was also indicated for mothers with adequate iodine in their diet.

4.1. Iodine from Food and ADHD

Short-term use of supplemental iodine might have a different impact on thyroid function than long-term iodine intake [12,33]. We therefore only included non-users of iodine supplements when exploring the effects of iodine intake from food.
We have previously reported that low maternal iodine intake from food in pregnancy (below ~200 µg/day) was related to increased scores on maternally reported child behavior problems at three years of age in a dose-response manner (p < 0.001) [12]. The current study shows that this result prevails for maternally reported child ADHD symptoms at eight years of age. In comparison, the recommended iodine intake in pregnancy by the WHO is 250 µg/day [34]. The association curve for maternal iodine intake and child ADHD diagnosis displayed a similar shape as the symptom scores, but did not reach significance. This might indicate that the change in risk of ADHD diagnosis was too low to be detected in our sample, but given the wide confidence intervals, we cannot exclude an effect. Another possible explanation might be that there is a tendency that children with both ADHD and more pervasive neurodevelopmental disorders are registered under their primary diagnosis only (e.g., autism) in the NPR. The rate of registered comorbidities to ADHD in the NPR is much lower than would be expected [35]. This could result in a selection bias for the outcome, potentially also related to our exposure of interest, and thus influence the association that we study. In administrative registries like the NPR, we find only those individuals that actually are assessed and diagnosed by a specialist. The “true” prevalence of ADHD in Norway is not known, as not all suffering from ADHD seek specialist evaluation. Also, as the prevalence of formally diagnosed ADHD shows large regional differences, it is assumed that other factors, apart from symptom levels and impairment, influence the diagnostic process [36]. Thus, we might hypothesize that the variation in ADHD symptom scores is more closely related to the impact of iodine levels than the formal diagnosis.
Iodine status is closely linked to risks of thyroid dysfunction and thyroid autoimmunity [37,38]. In addition, ID might make the maternal thyroid more vulnerable to environmental goitrogens, i.e., substances inhibiting the uptake of iodine in the thyroid or the production of the thyroid hormones, present both in the diet and in cigarette smoke [39]. Several previous studies have explored maternal iodine nutrition and/or thyroid hormone status in pregnancy and the risk of child externalizing behavior or ADHD symptoms. In 1993, Hauser et al. [40] documented a link between thyroid hormones and risk of ADHD. They reported a strong association between the genetic disease, generalized resistance to thyroid hormones—characterized by reduced responsiveness to the actions of thyroid hormones—and risk of ADHD. In 2004, Vermiglio et al. [9] performed a non-randomized prospective study, comparing participants from an ID area, to participants from an iodine sufficient area (mean UIE in schoolchildren: 48 µg/day and 95 µg/day). Eight of the eleven mothers from the iodine deficient area became hypothyroxinemic early in pregnancy, and seven of their children were later diagnosed with ADHD. None of the 16 children from the iodine sufficient area were diagnosed with ADHD. Similarly, a Russian study (n = 2397 children) reported a higher prevalence of attention deficit syndrome (without hyperactivity) in an area with ID compared to an area without ID [11] (only the abstract available in English).
In the Generation R study there have been several publications reporting associations between maternal thyroid or iodine measures and child behavior. In a sub-study (n = 692 mother-child pairs), van Mil et al. [10] found that mothers with low urinary iodine early in pregnancy (below the 10th percentile; UIC < 136 µg/g creatinine) gave birth to children with a higher risk of impaired executive functioning at four years of age (mainly inhibition and working memory, which are both related to ADHD symptomatology). Ghassabian et al. [41] reported that elevated maternal thyroid-stimulating hormone concentration (within the normal range), but not hypothyroxinaemia, was associated with higher scores on externalizing problems up to three years of age. Later, Modesto et al. [42] reported that maternal hypothoroxinemia around GW ~14 was associated with increased child ADHD symptoms at eight years of age. Ghassabian et al. [43] reported that children of mothers testing positive for thyroid peroxidase antibodies (identified in 4.7% of 3139 mothers measured in GW ~14) had an increased risk of scoring high on parent-reported ADHD-symptoms at three years of age (odds ratio = 1.77, 95% CI: 1.15, 2.72).
To our knowledge, the largest study to date is a Danish register-based cohort study including n = 857,014 singleton births between 1991 and 2004 [44]. Children born to mothers who were diagnosed with thyroid disorders in pregnancy or later (3.5%) had a higher risk of ADHD diagnosis (HR 1.18, 95% CI 1.03, 1.36 for hyperthyroidism, and HR 1.10, 95% CI: 0.98, 1.25 for hypothyroidism).
Taken together, an increased risk of symptoms of ADHD in the child when the maternal diet provides inadequate iodine seems plausible, and the results in our study add supporting evidence for a link.

4.2. Iodine from Supplements and ADHD

The results in this study support our previous findings, where we reported that mothers with insufficient iodine intake from food, who started using iodine-containing supplements in pregnancy, gave birth to children with increased behavior problems at three years of age [12]. Effect estimates in the present study were however, attenuated when restricting the control group to mothers who used supplements without iodine (other than the recommended vitamin D, folic acid and iron). This most likely indicates a confounding effect of maternal “health seeking behavior”. Alternatively, it might indicate confounding or effect modification by other substances in multi-supplements (i.e., other nutrients in the supplements exerting a negative effect on the child’s brain development). However, the associations with ADHD outcomes were robust and remained borderline significant for participants with a low iodine intake from food who initiated iodine supplement use in the first trimester.
A Cochrane review, published in 2017, entitled “Iodine supplementation for women during the preconception, pregnancy and postpartum period” summarized findings from relevant randomized controlled trials (RCT) on iodine supplementation [45]. The authors concluded that there is not enough data to reach any meaningful conclusions on the potential benefits and harms of supplementing women in areas with mild to moderate ID, and that more studies are needed. They reported nonsignificant increased risks of both hypo- and hyperthyroidism in pregnancy in supplemented mothers, but the confidence intervals were wide, due to the small number of participants included. It is however, not unlikely that an abrupt increase in iodine intake, particularly in women with low iodine intakes from food, could cause a “stunning effect” of the thyroid and a temporary imbalance in thyroid hormones [33]. The developing brain might be most vulnerable to such imbalances during the first trimester, since the fetus is entirely dependent on the maternal supply of thyroid hormones in early pregnancy.
Just recently, the world’s first RCT study, exploring the effect of iodine supplementation in mildly iodine deficient pregnant women (median UIC: 131 µg/L) on child neurodevelopment was published [46]. Gowachirapant et al. did not find any effects on a range of outcomes, including intelligence quotient (IQ), executive functioning, and behavior problems. However, it is important to notice that a substantial part of the study participants had a UIC higher than 150 µg/L; supplement use was not initiated until gestational week 10.7 (SD: 2.7), and the study was underpowered to detect differences of less than five IQ points between the groups. The results showed that the children of the supplemented women scored more poorly on the tests, but the differences were small and not statistically significant.

4.3. Strengths and Limitations

Strengths of this study include the large sample size, prospective design, extensive collection of data, and the possibility of linking the cohort to national registries like the NPR securing minimal loss to follow up. Maternally reported child ADHD symptoms provided an alternative, continuous outcome measure, strengthening the potential of identifying risks of ADHD.
The considerable variation in iodine intake between mothers and the high prevalence of low intake make MoBa ideal for exploring maternal iodine intake as an exposure. In Norway, tap water and iodized salt contribute only negligible amounts of iodine and there are only few food sources of iodine. A dietary survey method is thus suitable for estimating iodine intake at an individual level, as was previously documented in a validation study in MoBa (referenced in the Methods section [20,22]). However, there will always be uncertainty related to the dietary estimates, due to potential recall bias when reporting average food and supplement intakes in the first half of pregnancy, and to variation in iodine concentration in food items.
There were no substantial differences in maternal age, BMI, parity, or socioeconomic status indicators, either by maternal iodine intake or by iodine supplement use. This might be explained by a very low awareness of iodine among pregnant women in Norway [47], and no existing guidelines for iodine supplement use. Also, milk consumption is not closely related to having a healthy diet. This reduces the risk of confounding by lifestyle or dietary factors.
Some multi-supplements reported by the MoBa mothers contained iodine, whereas others did not. This allowed us to apply a quasi-experimental design with matched controls, which strengthened the evidence for a causal link.
Weaknesses included the observational design, meaning that we cannot rule out the possibility of residual confounding. For example, heritable psychological traits are associated with medication use during pregnancy (e.g., herbal preparations) [48]. Passive genetic transmission of ADHD-like traits could therefore confound the parent-offspring associations. Furthermore, the associations identified in our study might be affected by selection bias and by misclassification of both exposure and outcome variables. The low participation rate (41%) is a concern and participants in MoBa are not representative of the general pregnant population [13]. The possible impact of self-selection in MoBa has been evaluated and the results showed that the non-representativeness does influence prevalence estimates, but does not necessarily affect exposure-outcome associations [49,50].

4.4. Clinical Relevance and Implications

This study adds supporting evidence that insufficient maternal iodine intake might be a risk factor for offspring ADHD. Our analyses also indicate an increase in the risk of ADHD in children of mothers using iodine-containing supplements, and this is alarming given the high rate of supplement use in many countries. The current recommendation from the WHO is to promote iodine supplements in areas of insufficient iodine intake for women of childbearing age and in pregnancy and lactation [34]. The results observed in this study emphasize the urgent need for large and well-designed RCTs on the impact of initiating iodine supplementation in the first trimester, and ideally well before GW 12. Our study indicates that iodine-containing supplements should not be encouraged for pregnant women in the first trimester, especially in areas with mild to moderately insufficient iodine intakes. Our results also emphasize the need for ensuring sufficient iodine intake at the population level, in order to ensure sufficient iodine status in women of childbearing age before they enter pregnancy.

5. Conclusions

This study showed that low maternal iodine intake during pregnancy was associated with increased ADHD symptom score at eight years of age, but not with the risk of child ADHD diagnosis. There was no indication of any beneficial effects of maternal use of supplemental iodine on child ADHD, and initiating iodine supplement use in the first trimester was associated with an increased risk.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at www.mdpi.com/2072-6643/9/11/1239/s1, Figure S1: Conceptual model (simplified directed acyclic diagram (DAG)), Figure S2: Sex specific associations between maternal iodine intake from food (in non-supplement users) and child ADHD, Table S1: Associations between maternal iodine intake from food in participants who did not report use of supplemental iodine in the FFQ and risk of child ADHD diagnosis, Table S2: Association between maternal iodine intake from food in participants who did not report use of supplemental iodine in the FFQ) and score on maternally reported ADHD-symptoms at age 8 years.

Acknowledgments

The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study is supported by the Norwegian Ministry of Health and the Ministry of Education and Research, NIH/NIEHS (contract No. N01-ES-75558), NIH/NINDS (grant No. 1 UO1 NS 047537-01 and grant No. 2 UO1 NS 047537-06A1). M.H.A. was supported by a 4 year grant from the Norwegian Research Council (grant No. 241430) and the Norwegian dairy company TINE SA which also covers the costs for open access publication. E.Y. is funded by European Research Council (grant No. 678033) and the Norwegian Research Council (grant No. 231105). We are grateful to all the participating families in Norway who take part in this on-going cohort study.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to designing the study, interpreting the results, and read and approved the final manuscript. M.H.A. analyzed the data with support from E.Y. and I.H.C. M.H.A. wrote the manuscript. A.L.B. had primary responsibility for the final content.

Conflicts of Interest

The first author of this paper is employed by a Norwegian dairy company (TINE SA), and she participates in this project as an industrial PhD-student financed partly by the dairy company and partly by The Research Council of Norway. This project is designed, owned and administered by The Norwegian Institute of Public Health and analysis of the data follow from protocol. All results of analysis in the project are to be published regardless of the results. The dairy company supports the study to raise awareness on the importance of iodine and to gain more knowledge about the potential health effects of milk in the Norwegian diet. Apart from the PhD-student, no one from the dairy company has been involved in the study, and in itself, the company had no direct influence on the analysis and interpretation of the results. The other authors had no conflicts of interest.

Abbreviations

ADHDAttention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
DSMThe diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
EAREstimated average requirement
FFQFood frequency questionnaire
GWGestational week
HKDHyperkinetic disorder
ICDInternational Classification of Disease
IDIodine deficiency
IQIntelligence quotient
MoBaThe Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study
NPRNorwegian Patient Registry
UICUrinary iodine concentration

References

  1. World Health Organization; United Nations Children’s Fund; International Council for Control of Iodine Deficiency Disorders. Assessment of Iodine Deficiency Disorders and Monitoring Their Elimination: A Guide for Programme Managers; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  2. Redman, K.; Ruffman, T.; Fitzgerald, P.; Skeaff, S. Iodine deficiency and the brain: Effects and mechanisms. Crit. Rev. Food Sci. Nutr. 2016, 56, 2695–2713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Pearce, E.N.; Lazarus, J.H.; Moreno-Reyes, R.; Zimmermann, M.B. Consequences of iodine deficiency and excess in pregnant women: An overview of current knowns and unknowns. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2016, 104, 918S–923S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Thomas, R.; Sanders, S.; Doust, J.; Beller, E.; Glasziou, P. Prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics 2015, 135, e994–e1001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; American Psychiatric Association: Arlington, VA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  6. Spencer, T.J.; Biederman, J.; Mick, E. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Diagnosis, lifespan, comorbidities, and neurobiology. Ambul. Pediatr. 2007, 7, 73–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Thapar, A.; Cooper, M.; Eyre, O.; Langley, K. Practitioner review: What have we learnt about the causes of ADHD? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2013, 54, 3–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Larsson, H.; Chang, Z.; D’Onofrio, B.M.; Lichtenstein, P. The heritability of clinically diagnosed attention deficit hyperactivity disorder across the lifespan. Psychol. Med. 2014, 44, 2223–2229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Vermiglio, F.; Lo Presti, V.P.; Moleti, M.; Sidoti, M.; Tortorella, G.; Scaffidi, G.; Castagna, M.G.; Mattina, F.; Violi, M.A.; Crisa, A.; et al. Attention deficit and hyperactivity disorders in the offspring of mothers exposed to mild-moderate iodine deficiency: A possible novel iodine deficiency disorder in developed countries. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2004, 89, 6054–6060. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Van Mil, N.H.; Tiemeier, H.; Bongers-Schokking, J.J.; Ghassabian, A.; Hofman, A.; Hooijkaas, H.; Jaddoe, V.W.; de Muinck Keizer-Schrama, S.M.; Steegers, E.A.; Visser, T.J.; et al. Low urinary iodine excretion during early pregnancy is associated with alterations in executive functioning in children. J. Nutr. 2012, 142, 2167–2174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Zhukov, A.O. Mental development disorders and attention-deficit syndrome caused by iodine deficiency: A clinical and epidemiological study. Zh. Nevrol. Psikhiatr. Im. S. S. Korsakova 2007, 107, 4–16. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  12. Abel, M.H.; Caspersen, I.H.; Meltzer, H.M.; Haugen, M.; Brandlistuen, R.E.; Aase, H.; Alexander, J.; Torheim, L.E.; Brantsaeter, A.L. Suboptimal maternal iodine intake is associated with impaired child neurodevelopment at 3 years of age in the Norwegian mother and child cohort study. J. Nutr. 2017, 147, 1314–1324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Magnus, P.; Birke, C.; Vejrup, K.; Haugan, A.; Alsaker, E.; Daltveit, A.K.; Handal, M.; Haugen, M.; Hoiseth, G.; Knudsen, G.P.; et al. Cohort profile update: The Norwegian mother and child cohort study (Moba). Int. J. Epidemiol. 2016, 45, 382–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Irgens, L.M. The medical birth registry of Norway. Epidemiological research and surveillance throughout 30 years. Acta Obstet. Gynecol. Scand. 2000, 79, 435–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Meltzer, H.M.; Brantsaeter, A.L.; Ydersbond, T.A.; Alexander, J.; Haugen, M. Methodological challenges when monitoring the diet of pregnant women in a large study: Experiences from the Norwegian mother and child cohort study (Moba). Matern. Child Nutr. 2008, 4, 14–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Norwegian Institute of Public Health Website. MoBa Food Frequency Questionnaire (English Translation). Available online: http://www.webcitation.org/6u5JMPcZg (accessed on 9 October 2017).
  17. Dahl, L.; Johansson, L.; Julshamn, K.; Meltzer, H.M. The iodine content of Norwegian foods and diets. Public Health Nutr. 2004, 7, 569–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Dahl, L.; Opsahl, J.A.; Meltzer, H.M.; Julshamn, K. Iodine concentration in Norwegian milk and dairy products. Br. J. Nutr. 2003, 90, 679–685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Haugen, M.; Brantsaeter, A.L.; Alexander, J.; Meltzer, H.M. Dietary supplements contribute substantially to the total nutrient intake in pregnant Norwegian women. Ann. Nutr. Metab. 2008, 52, 272–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Brantsaeter, A.L.; Haugen, M.; Alexander, J.; Meltzer, H.M. Validity of a new food frequency questionnaire for pregnant women in the Norwegian mother and child cohort study (Moba). Matern. Child Nutr. 2008, 4, 28–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Brantsaeter, A.L.; Haugen, M.; Hagve, T.A.; Aksnes, L.; Rasmussen, S.E.; Julshamn, K.; Alexander, J.; Meltzer, H.M. Self-reported dietary supplement use is confirmed by biological markers in the Norwegian mother and child cohort study (Moba). Ann. Nutr. Metab. 2007, 51, 146–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Brantsaeter, A.L.; Haugen, M.; Julshamn, K.; Alexander, J.; Meltzer, H.M. Evaluation of urinary iodine excretion as a biomarker for intake of milk and dairy products in pregnant women in the Norwegian mother and child cohort study (Moba). Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2009, 63, 347–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Abel, M.H.; Korevaar, T.I.M.; Caspersen, I.H.; Villanger, G.D.; Meltzer, H.M.; Torheim, L.E.; Alexander, J.; Arohonka, P.; Erlund, I.; Brantsæter, A.L. Iodine intake and thyroid function in pregnancy. Unpublished work. 2018. [Google Scholar]
  24. Norwegian Institute of Public Health. Norwegian Prescription Database. Available online: www.norpd.no (accessed on 1 June 2016).
  25. World Health Organization. International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  26. Polanczyk, G.V.; Willcutt, E.G.; Salum, G.A.; Kieling, C.; Rohde, L.A. ADHD prevalence estimates across three decades: An updated systematic review and meta-regression analysis. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2014, 43, 434–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Sonuga-Barke, E.J.S.; Taylor, E. Adhd and hyperkinetic disorder. In Rutter’s Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 6th ed.; Thapar, A., Pine, D.S., Leckman, J.F., Scott, S., Snowling, M.J., Taylor, E., Eds.; John Wiley and Sons Ltd.: Chichester, UK, 2015; pp. 738–756. [Google Scholar]
  28. Taylor, E.; Schachar, R.; Thorley, G.; Wieselberg, H.M.; Everitt, B.; Rutter, M. Which boys respond to stimulant medication? A controlled trial of methylphenidate in boys with disruptive behaviour. Psychol. Med. 1987, 17, 121–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Silva, R.R.; Alpert, M.; Pouget, E.; Silva, V.; Trosper, S.; Reyes, K.; Dummit, S. A rating scale for disruptive behavior disorders, based on the DSM-IV item pool. Psychiatr. Q. 2005, 76, 327–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Bauer, M.; Goetz, T.; Glenn, T.; Whybrow, P.C. The thyroid-brain interaction in thyroid disorders and mood disorders. J. Neuroendocrinol. 2008, 20, 1101–1114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Institute of Medicine. Iodine. In Dietary Reference Intakes for Vitamin A, Vitamin K, Arsenic, Boron, Chromium, Copper, Iodine, Iron, Manganese, Molybdenum, Nickel, Silicon, Vanadium, and Zinc; The National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2001; pp. 258–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Orsini, N.; Greenland, S. A procedure to tabulate and plot results after flexible modeling of a quantitative covariate. Stata J. 2011, 11, 1–29. [Google Scholar]
  33. Moleti, M.; Di Bella, B.; Giorgianni, G.; Mancuso, A.; De Vivo, A.; Alibrandi, A.; Trimarchi, F.; Vermiglio, F. Maternal thyroid function in different conditions of iodine nutrition in pregnant women exposed to mild-moderate iodine deficiency: An observational study. Clin. Endocrinol. (Oxf.) 2011, 74, 762–768. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Andersson, M.; de Benoist, B.; Delange, F.; Zupan, J. Prevention and control of iodine deficiency in pregnant and lactating women and in children less than 2-years-old: Conclusions and recommendations of the technical consultation. Public Health Nutr. 2007, 10, 1606–1611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Oerbeck, B.; Overgaard, K.R.; Aspenes, S.T.; Pripp, A.H.; Mordre, M.; Aase, H.; Reichborn-Kjennerud, T.; Zeiner, P. Adhd, comorbid disorders and psychosocial functioning: How representative is a child cohort study? Findings from a national patient registry. BMC Psychiatry 2017, 17, 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Suren, P.; Bakken, I.J.; Aase, H.; Chin, R.; Gunnes, N.; Lie, K.K.; Magnus, P.; Reichborn-Kjennerud, T.; Schjolberg, S.; Oyen, A.S.; et al. Autism spectrum disorder, ADHD, epilepsy, and cerebral palsy in Norwegian children. Pediatrics 2012, 130, e152–e158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Shi, X.; Han, C.; Li, C.; Mao, J.; Wang, W.; Xie, X.; Li, C.; Xu, B.; Meng, T.; Du, J.; et al. Optimal and safe upper limits of iodine intake for early pregnancy in iodine-sufficient regions: A cross-sectional study of 7190 pregnant women in China. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2015, 100, 1630–1638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Zimmermann, M.B.; Boelaert, K. Iodine deficiency and thyroid disorders. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol. 2015, 3, 286–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Roman, G.C. Autism: Transient in utero hypothyroxinemia related to maternal flavonoid ingestion during pregnancy and to other environmental antithyroid agents. J. Neurol. Sci. 2007, 262, 15–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Hauser, P.; Zametkin, A.J.; Martinez, P.; Vitiello, B.; Matochik, J.A.; Mixson, A.J.; Weintraub, B.D. Attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder in people with generalized resistance to thyroid hormone. N. Engl. J. Med. 1993, 328, 997–1001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Ghassabian, A.; Henrichs, J.; Tiemeier, H. Impact of mild thyroid hormone deficiency in pregnancy on cognitive function in children: Lessons from the Generation R Study. Best Pract. Res. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2014, 28, 221–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Modesto, T.; Tiemeier, H.; Peeters, R.P.; Jaddoe, V.W.; Hofman, A.; Verhulst, F.C.; Ghassabian, A. Maternal mild thyroid hormone insufficiency in early pregnancy and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms in children. JAMA Pediatr. 2015, 169, 838–845. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Ghassabian, A.; Bongers-Schokking, J.J.; de Rijke, Y.B.; van Mil, N.; Jaddoe, V.W.; de Muinck Keizer-Schrama, S.M.; Hooijkaas, H.; Hofman, A.; Visser, W.; Roman, G.C.; et al. Maternal thyroid autoimmunity during pregnancy and the risk of attention deficit/hyperactivity problems in children: The generation r study. Thyroid 2012, 22, 178–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Andersen, S.L.; Laurberg, P.; Wu, C.S.; Olsen, J. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder in children born to mothers with thyroid dysfunction: A Danish nationwide cohort study. BJOG 2014, 121, 1365–1374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Harding, K.B.; Pena-Rosas, J.P.; Webster, A.C.; Yap, C.M.; Payne, B.A.; Ota, E.; De-Regil, L.M. Iodine supplementation for women during the preconception, pregnancy and postpartum period. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2017, 3, CD011761. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Gowachirapant, S.; Jaiswal, N.; Melse-Boonstra, A.; Galetti, V.; Stinca, S.; Mackenzie, I.; Thomas, S.; Thomas, T.; Winichagoon, P.; Srinivasan, K.; et al. Effect of iodine supplementation in pregnant women on child neurodevelopment: A randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol. 2017, 5, 853–863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Garnweidner-Holme, L.; Aakre, I.; Lilleengen, A.M.; Brantsaeter, A.L.; Henjum, S. Knowledge about iodine in pregnant and lactating women in the Oslo area, Norway. Nutrients 2017, 9, 493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Ystrom, E.; Vollrath, M.E.; Nordeng, H. Effects of personality on use of medications, alcohol, and cigarettes during pregnancy. Eur. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 2012, 68, 845–851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Nilsen, R.M.; Vollset, S.E.; Gjessing, H.K.; Skjaerven, R.; Melve, K.K.; Schreuder, P.; Alsaker, E.R.; Haug, K.; Daltveit, A.K.; Magnus, P. Self-selection and bias in a large prospective pregnancy cohort in Norway. Paediatr. Perinat. Epidemiol. 2009, 23, 597–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Nilsen, R.M.; Suren, P.; Gunnes, N.; Alsaker, E.R.; Bresnahan, M.; Hirtz, D.; Hornig, M.; Lie, K.K.; Lipkin, W.I.; Reichborn-Kjennerud, T.; et al. Analysis of self-selection bias in a population-based cohort study of autism spectrum disorders. Paediatr. Perinat. Epidemiol. 2013, 27, 553–563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Flow-chart of inclusion. 1 Questionnaire 1 was answered around gestational week 17. 2 The FFQ (questionnaire 2) used in the present study was included in The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study (MoBa) from 2002 and was answered around gestational week 22. FFQ: Food frequency questionnaire, BMI: Body Mass Index, ADHD: attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Figure 1. Flow-chart of inclusion. 1 Questionnaire 1 was answered around gestational week 17. 2 The FFQ (questionnaire 2) used in the present study was included in The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study (MoBa) from 2002 and was answered around gestational week 22. FFQ: Food frequency questionnaire, BMI: Body Mass Index, ADHD: attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Nutrients 09 01239 g001
Figure 2. Association between maternal iodine intake from food and proportional risk of child ADHD diagnosis. Results are from multivariable regression analysis and are restricted to non-users of iodine supplements during first half of pregnancy (n = 53,360 mother–child pairs). Iodine intake was modelled by restricted cubic splines (four knots), and the reference level was set to 160 µg/day. Dashed lines represent 95% confidence limits. The histogram (b) illustrates the distribution of iodine intake. Both models (a,b) were adjusted for random effects of sibling clusters and for energy intake to control for measurement error. The adjusted model (b) was additionally adjusted for maternal age, BMI, parity, education, smoking in pregnancy, and fiber intake. The vertical axis on hazard ratios are on the log scale. ADHD: attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Figure 2. Association between maternal iodine intake from food and proportional risk of child ADHD diagnosis. Results are from multivariable regression analysis and are restricted to non-users of iodine supplements during first half of pregnancy (n = 53,360 mother–child pairs). Iodine intake was modelled by restricted cubic splines (four knots), and the reference level was set to 160 µg/day. Dashed lines represent 95% confidence limits. The histogram (b) illustrates the distribution of iodine intake. Both models (a,b) were adjusted for random effects of sibling clusters and for energy intake to control for measurement error. The adjusted model (b) was additionally adjusted for maternal age, BMI, parity, education, smoking in pregnancy, and fiber intake. The vertical axis on hazard ratios are on the log scale. ADHD: attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Nutrients 09 01239 g002aNutrients 09 01239 g002b
Figure 3. Association between maternal iodine intake from food and standardized score on maternally reported child ADHD symptoms at age eight years. Results are from multivariable regression analysis and restricted to non-users of iodine supplements during the first half of pregnancy (n = 19,086 mother-child pairs). Iodine intake was modelled by restricted cubic splines (four knots), and the reference level was set to 160 µg/day. Dashed lines represent 95% confidence limits. The histograms (e,f) illustrate the distribution of iodine intake. Crude models (a,c,e) were adjusted for maternal energy intake and for random effects of sibling clusters. Adjusted models (b,d,f) were additionally adjusted for maternal age, parity, education, body mass index, smoking in pregnancy, fiber intake, child sex, and birth season.
Figure 3. Association between maternal iodine intake from food and standardized score on maternally reported child ADHD symptoms at age eight years. Results are from multivariable regression analysis and restricted to non-users of iodine supplements during the first half of pregnancy (n = 19,086 mother-child pairs). Iodine intake was modelled by restricted cubic splines (four knots), and the reference level was set to 160 µg/day. Dashed lines represent 95% confidence limits. The histograms (e,f) illustrate the distribution of iodine intake. Crude models (a,c,e) were adjusted for maternal energy intake and for random effects of sibling clusters. Adjusted models (b,d,f) were additionally adjusted for maternal age, parity, education, body mass index, smoking in pregnancy, fiber intake, child sex, and birth season.
Nutrients 09 01239 g003aNutrients 09 01239 g003b
Table 1. Maternal and child characteristics by maternal iodine intake from food and from supplements in first half of pregnancy 1.
Table 1. Maternal and child characteristics by maternal iodine intake from food and from supplements in first half of pregnancy 1.
Iodine Intake from Food (µg/Day)Supplemental Iodine (µg/Day)Total
<100100–159.9160–250>25001–200>200
Mother-child pairs, n (%)25,637 (33.2)31,688 (41.1)16,322 (21.1)3517 (4.6)53,360 (69.2)21,940 (28.4)1864 (2.4)77,164 (100)
Maternal age at delivery, years30.1 (4.5)30.4 (4.4)30.1 (4.6)29.4 (4.9)30.1 (4.6)30.3 (4.4)30.0 (4.8)30.2 (4.5)
Pre-pregnancy BMI, kg/m224.2 (4.4)23.9 (4.2)24.0 (4.2)24.4 (4.6)24.1 (4.3)23.9 (4.2)23.9 (4.3)24.0 (4.3)
Parity, %
 049.946.245.348.444.752.758.247.3
 134.936.335.832.436.733.431.035.6
 2 or more15.217.418.919.218.613.910.817.1
Maternal education
 ≤12 years31.127.932.141.732.226.430.330.5
 13–16 years42.443.943.339.042.344.942.943.1
 >16 years26.528.224.619.325.528.726.826.5
Married/cohabitant96.797.096.694.996.796.895.996.7
Smoking in pregnancy
 No77.879.878.574.078.179.878.278.6
 Occasionally16.214.915.116.515.515.216.115.5
 Daily6.05.36.49.56.45.05.75.9
Chronic illness11.69.59.310.59.811.012.010.2
Parents’ income
 Low 25.125.129.033.427.124.723.526.3
 Medium40.741.242.042.641.440.941.341.3
 High31.831.226.019.828.732.132.529.8
 Missing2.42.53.04.32.82.42.72.6
Child sex
 Boys51.251.251.051.651.550.551.251.2
 Girls48.848.849.048.448.549.548.848.8
Bilingual parent(s)10.710.38.99.39.810.612.810.1
ADHD diagnosis by December 20152.12.12.63.42.22.32.12.2
1 Values are presented as mean ± standard deviation (SD) or percentages unless otherwise indicated. BMI: body mass index; ADHD: attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Table 2. Maternal dietary characteristics by maternal iodine intake from food and from supplements (in micrograms per day) during the first half of pregnancy 1.
Table 2. Maternal dietary characteristics by maternal iodine intake from food and from supplements (in micrograms per day) during the first half of pregnancy 1.
Iodine Intake from FoodSupplemental IodineTotal
<100100–159.9160–250>25001–200>200
Energy intake, MJ/day8.2 (2.0)9.7 (2.1)11.3 (2.4)13.5 (2.8)9.7 (2.6)9.7 (2.6)9.9 (2.6)9.7 (2.6)
Iodine from food, µg/day74 (18)127 (17)193 (24)304 (55)132 (61)131 (61)133 (64)132 (61)
Food intake, g/day
  Milk/yoghurt 162 (123)411 (168)756 (246)1435 (443)447 (360)448 (362)463 (386)448 (362)
  Fish, lean16 (11)22 (13)25 (15)28 (18)21 (14)20 (13)21 (15)21 (14)
  Fish, fatty9 (9)12 (13)16 (18)19 (23)12 (14)12 (14)12 (12)12 (14)
  Eggs9 (9)12 (12)13 (14)15 (17)11 (12)12 (13)12 (13)11 (12)
  Fruits and vegetables388 (217)453 (246)502 (280)559 (344)443 (253)451 (254)489 (308)447 (255)
Nutrient intake, g/day
  Protein71 (14)86 (14)104 (16)132 (21)87 (21)87 (21)89 (22)87 (21)
  Sugar55 (37)60 (36)70 (40)84 (50)62 (39)62 (37)62 (37)62 (38)
  Fiber27 (9)31 (10)35 (11)39 (13)31 (10)31 (11)33 (12)31 (10)
  Alcohol0.1 (0.6)0.1 (0.7)0.1 (0.5)0.1 (0.7)0.1 (0.6)0.1 (0.7)0.1 (0.3)0.1 (0.6)
Iodine from source, µg/day
  Milk including yoghurt 20 (16)56 (23)107 (35)206 (64)62 (52)61 (52)63 (55)62 (52)
  Fish16 (11)24 (14)30 (19)35 (27)23 (16)23 (16)22 (16)23 (16)
  Eggs4 (4)5 (6)6 (6)7 (8)5 (5)5 (6)5 (6)5 (6)
  Supplements35 (72)35 (72)35 (71)37 (77)-95 (51)336 (135)35 (72)
Iodine supplement
  0 µg/day69.069.169.469.3100--69.2
  1–99 µg/day15.815.114.814.8-53.7-15.3
  100–199 µg/day12.613.413.413.1-46.3-13.2
  ≥200 µg/day2.52.32.42.8--1002.4
n-3 FA supplement 264.969.769.766.463.477.486.068.0
Folic acid supplement 373.973.971.066.368.682.684.572.9
Any supplement (in FFQ)83.886.786.383.779.110010085.5
1 Values are presented as mean ± standard deviation (SD) or percentages unless otherwise indicated; 2 Long chain n-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids (FA), eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA) and docosahexaenoic acid (DHA); 3 Any reported use of folic acid from 4 weeks before to 8 weeks after conception reported in questionnaire 1 (not in FFQ). FFQ: Food frequency questionnaire.
Table 3. Use of iodine-containing supplements in pregnancy and risk of child ADHD diagnosis (n = 77,164) 1.
Table 3. Use of iodine-containing supplements in pregnancy and risk of child ADHD diagnosis (n = 77,164) 1.
ADHD Diagnosis
nCrude ModelAdjusted Model 2Adjusted Model 2
Matched Controls
Iodine from food <160 µg/day
 Iodine from supplement:
  No (reference)39,597 (11,057 3)111
  1–200 µg/day16,3551.03 (0.91, 1.17)1.13 (0.99, 1.28)1.01 (0.86, 1.20)
  >200 µg/day13731.05 (0.71, 1.55)1.07 (0.72, 1.59)0.95 (0.63, 1.43)
 First report of iodine 4
  Before pregnancy 540181.02 (0.81, 1.28)1.24 (0.99, 1.56)1.11 (0.86, 1.43)
  Gestational week 0–1229701.34 (1.07, 1.68)1.47 (1.17, 1.85)1.29 (1.00, 1.67)
  Gestational week ≥1334021.04 (0.82, 1.32)1.11 (0.87, 1.41)0.98 (0.75, 1.27)
Iodine from food ≥160 µg/day
 Iodine from supplement:
  No (reference)13,763 (4152 3)111
  1–200 µg/day55851.11 (0.92, 1.34)1.18 (0.98, 1.43)1.08 (0.84, 1.37)
  >200 µg/day4911.15 (0.66, 2.00)1.16 (0.66, 2.01)1.03 (0.58, 1.83)
 First report of iodine 4
  Before pregnancy 514600.97 (0.69, 1.37)1.21 (0.85, 1.71)1.09 (0.74, 1.59)
  Gestational week 0–1210201.40 (1.01, 1.95)1.50 (1.07, 2.10)1.35 (0.93, 1.96)
  Gestational week ≥1311020.99 (0.68, 1.44)1.04 (0.71, 1.52)0.93 (0.61, 1.40)
1 Values are hazard ratios (95% CIs) unless otherwise indicated. Significant associations (p < 0.05) are highlighted. All models (including crude models) were adjusted for random effects of sibling clusters and for energy intake, to control for measurement error. Models estimating the impact of supplemental iodine (dosage and timing) included interaction terms between iodine from diet and iodine from supplements; 2 The adjusted models were additionally adjusted for maternal age, BMI, parity, education, smoking in pregnancy, fiber intake, folic acid supplement within the interval from 4 weeks beforehand to 8 weeks after conception, and total EPA and DHA intake; 3 Matched controls: controls restricted to mothers who reported the intake of supplemental vitamins and/or minerals other than the recommended; 4 Restricted to participants who reported taking up to 200 µg/day of supplemental iodine in the food frequency questionnaire and who also gave information on timing of supplement use in the general questionnaires; 5 0–26 weeks before conception.
Table 4. Use of iodine-containing supplements in pregnancy and maternally reported ADHD symptoms at eight years of age (n = 27,945) 1.
Table 4. Use of iodine-containing supplements in pregnancy and maternally reported ADHD symptoms at eight years of age (n = 27,945) 1.
NADHD ScoreInattention ScoreHyperactivity Score
Crude ModelAdjusted ModelAdjusted ModelCrude ModelAdjusted ModelCrude ModelAdjusted Model
Matched Controls 2
Iodine from food <160 µg/day
 Iodine from supplement:
  No (reference)14,089 (4133)0000000
  1–200 µg/day61150.06 (0.04, 0.09)0.05 (0.03, 0.08)0.02 (−0.01, 0.05)0.06 (0.03, 0.08)0.05 (0.02, 0.07)0.06 (0.03, 0.10)0.06 (0.03, 0.10)
  >200 µg/day4570.07 (−0.00, 0.15)0.06 (−0.02, 0.13)0.02 (−0.06, 0.10)0.07 (−0.00, 0.15)0.06 (−0.02, 0.13)0.07 (−0.03, 0.17)0.06 (−0.03, 0.16)
 First report of iodine supplement 3:
  Before pregnancy 416500.04 (0.00, 0.09)0.04 (0.00, 0.08)0.01 (−0.04, 0.05)0.06 (0.02, 0.10)0.05 (0.01, 0.09)0.02 (−0.03, 0.08)0.03 (−0.02, 0.08)
  Gestational week 0–1212030.12 (0.07, 0.17)0.10 (0.05, 0.15)0.06 (0.01, 0.11)0.11 (0.06, 0.15)0.07 (0.03, 0.12)0.14 (0.08, 0.20)0.13 (0.07, 0.19)
  Gestational week ≥1312640.06 (0.01, 0.11)0.05 (0.00, 0.10)0.01 (−0.04, 0.07)0.05 (0.01, 0.10)0.04 (−0.01, 0.09)0.07 (0.01, 0.14)0.07 (0.01, 0.13)
Iodine from food ≥160 µg/day
 Iodine from supplement:
  No (reference)4997 (1593)0000000
  1–200 µg/day21330.02 (−0.02, 0.06)0.02 (−0.02, 0.07)0.02 (−0.04, 0.07)0.02 (−0.02, 0.07)0.02 (−0.02, 0.06)0.02 (−0.04, 0.07)0.02 (−0.03, 0.08)
  >200 µg/day1540.00 (−0.13, 0.14)0.01 (−0.11, 0.13)0.01 (−0.11, 0.13)0.02 (−0.11, 0.15)0.05 (−0.07, 0.17)−0.02 (−0.20, 0.17)−0.04 (−0.21, 0.13)
 First report of iodine supplement 3:
  Before pregnancy 46110.06 (−0.02, 0.13)0.08 (0.00, 0.15)0.07 (−0.02, 0.15)0.05 (−0.02, 0.13)0.06 (−0.01, 0.14)0.06 (−0.03, 0.16)0.09 (−0.00, 0.19)
  Gestational week 0–123970.03 (−0.05, 0.12)0.02 (−0.07, 0.10)0.00 (−0.09, 0.10)0.04 (−0.04, 0.13)0.02 (−0.06, 0.10)0.02 (−0.09, 0.13)0.02 (−0.09, 0.13)
  Gestational week ≥134390.02 (−0.07, 0.10)0.01 (−0.07, 0.09)0.00 (−0.09, 0.09)0.01 (−0.07, 0.09)0.00 (−0.08, 0.08)0.03 (−0.08, 0.14)0.02 (−0.09, 0.13)
1 Values are standardized beta coefficients (95% CIs) unless otherwise indicated. Significant associations (p < 0.05) are highlighted. Models included interaction terms between iodine from diet and iodine from supplements. All models (including crude models) were adjusted for random effects of sibling clusters and for energy intake, to control for measurement error. The adjusted models were additionally adjusted for maternal age, BMI, parity, education, smoking in pregnancy, fiber intake, child sex, and birth season, folic acid supplement within the interval from 4 weeks beforehand to 8 weeks after conception, and total EPA and DHA intake; 2 Matched controls: controls restricted to mothers who reported the intake of supplemental vitamins and/or minerals other than the recommended; 3 Restricted to participants who reported taking up to 200 µg/day of supplemental iodine in the food frequency questionnaire and who also gave information on the timing of supplement use in the general questionnaires; 4 0–26 weeks before conception.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Abel, M.H.; Ystrom, E.; Caspersen, I.H.; Meltzer, H.M.; Aase, H.; Torheim, L.E.; Askeland, R.B.; Reichborn-Kjennerud, T.; Brantsæter, A.L. Maternal Iodine Intake and Offspring Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Results from a Large Prospective Cohort Study. Nutrients 2017, 9, 1239. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu9111239

AMA Style

Abel MH, Ystrom E, Caspersen IH, Meltzer HM, Aase H, Torheim LE, Askeland RB, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Brantsæter AL. Maternal Iodine Intake and Offspring Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Results from a Large Prospective Cohort Study. Nutrients. 2017; 9(11):1239. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu9111239

Chicago/Turabian Style

Abel, Marianne Hope, Eivind Ystrom, Ida Henriette Caspersen, Helle Margrete Meltzer, Heidi Aase, Liv Elin Torheim, Ragna Bugge Askeland, Ted Reichborn-Kjennerud, and Anne Lise Brantsæter. 2017. "Maternal Iodine Intake and Offspring Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Results from a Large Prospective Cohort Study" Nutrients 9, no. 11: 1239. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu9111239

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop