Next Article in Journal
Semiochemical and Vibrational Cues and Signals Mediating Mate Finding and Courtship in Psylloidea (Hemiptera): A Synthesis
Previous Article in Journal
Ecology of the African Maize Stalk Borer, Busseola fusca (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae) with Special Reference to Insect-Plant Interactions
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

A New Visual Trap for Rhagoletis cerasi (L.) (Diptera: Tephritidae)

1
Research Institute of Organic Agriculture (FiBL), Ackerstrasse 113, Postfach 219, CH-5070 Frick, Switzerland
2
Fachstelle Pflanzenschutz Strickhof, Eschikon, Postfach, CH-8315 Lindau, Switzerland
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Insects 2014, 5(3), 564-576; https://doi.org/10.3390/insects5030564
Submission received: 26 March 2014 / Revised: 4 July 2014 / Accepted: 10 July 2014 / Published: 18 July 2014

Abstract

:
The European cherry fruit fly, Rhagoletis cerasi (L.) (Diptera: Tephritidae), is the most important pest of sweet cherries in Europe. The aim of our experiments was to develop a new, cost-efficient, lead chromate-free and more eco-friendly trap for monitoring and mass trapping of R. cerasi. Five different-colored yellow panels and three different trap shapes were compared to a standard Rebell® amarillo trap in three experimental orchards in 2012. Trap color F, with a strong increase in reflectance at 500–550 nm and a secondary peak in the UV-region at 300–400 nm, captured significantly more flies than the standard Rebell® amarillo trap. Yellow traps with increased reflectance in the blue region (400–500 nm) were least attractive. Trap shape was of minor importance, as long as the object was three-dimensional and visible from all directions. Based on economic and practical considerations, a cylinder-shaped trap “UFA-Samen Kirschenfliegenfalle” was developed for commercial use and is currently under on-farm evaluation.

1. Introduction

The European cherry fruit fly, Rhagoletis cerasi (L.) (Diptera: Tephritidae), is the most important pest of sweet cherries Prunus avium (L.) L. in Europe. The adult flies emerge from the soil in May and June [1] and begin to lay eggs about ten days after emergence [2,3]. The maggots develop inside the cherries. At the third instar, the larvae leave the fruit, drop to the soil and, within hours, start to pupate under the tree canopy [4]. R. cerasi is univoltine and overwinters as a pupa [5]. Without insecticide treatment, up to 100% of fruit can be infested [6]. R. cerasi poses a challenge to cherry growers because of the low tolerance level of the fresh market to damaged fruit, with a maximum of 2% of infested fruits. In most orchards and in most years, insecticide treatments are necessary to keep the infestation level below the threshold. The phase-out of organophosphorus insecticides now threatens cherry production throughout the European Union. The use of the systemic insecticide, dimethoate, in particular, is being revoked due to problems with ecotoxicity and residues. Registered alternative products [7] are targeting adult flies. Therefore, the detection of first flight becomes increasingly important to schedule precise insecticide applications. New methods have recently been developed for organic agriculture, such as biocontrol using the entomopathogenic fungus, Beauveria bassiana [8], or bait sprays with spinosad or Neem [9,10,11]. These methods also require an exact determination of the first flight.
R. cerasi is known to be highly responsive to visual stimuli [12], especially to yellow surfaces [13,14,15,16,17]. Remund [14] determined that daylight fluorescent yellow-colored flat surfaces are most attractive. Prokopy [18] suggested that large yellow surfaces represent a super-normal foliage-type stimulus, eliciting food-seeking behavior in R. cerasi. He also hypothesized that flies react to yellow on the basis of true color discrimination. This hypothesis was supported by Agee et al. [19], who showed that adult R. cerasi have a major peak of electroretinographically assessed spectral sensitivity at 485 to 500 nm (yellow green region) and a secondary peak at 365 nm (ultraviolet region). Traps with a sharp increase of reflectance in the 500 to 520 nm region were found to be the most attractive to R. cerasi [19,20]. Based on this knowledge, the three-dimensional, cross-shaped Rebell® amarillo trap was developed in the 1970s [21] and is now used for monitoring and mass-trapping purposes in Switzerland. In home gardens, mass trapping often is the only available method for cherry fruit fly control, although the efficacy is not always satisfactory. Depending on the tree size, several traps per tree are necessary. In old, standard trees with a canopy diameter of 10 m, approximately 10 traps are needed [14]. At a price of 3.40 € per trap, the Rebell® trap is rather expensive [7]. In addition, the yellow pigments used for the Rebell® trap still contain lead chromate. The toxicity of lead chromate has been known since about 1980 [22], and lead chromate was listed as a “substance of very high concern” (SVHC) in the EU according to the REACH Regulation (Regulation (EC) No 1907/2006) [23]. Rebell® traps can be cleaned and reused, but because cleaning is very time consuming and solvents are necessary to dissolve the glue, most farmers use Rebell® traps only once. The aim of our experiments was to develop an equally attractive, but less material-intensive, lead chromate-free, more eco-friendly and more cost-effective trap for R. cerasi.

2. Materials and Methods

Trap types: In order to estimate the performance of the new trap types, two experiments were conducted. In the first experiment, the influence of different shades of yellow was evaluated comparing five differently-colored 15 × 20 cm panels to a Rebell® amarillo (Andermatt Biocontrol, Grossdietwil, Switzerland) panel of the same size. Details on the color and material are given in Table 1 and Figure 1. Polypropylene panels (Schoellkopf, Rümlang Switzerland) were double-printed with yellow (4.2 g color/m2) on both panel sides. Polyethylene panels were produced using colored granules (Granula AG, Merenschwand, Switzerland).
In the second experiment, the influence of trap shape was evaluated by comparing differently-shaped traps to a standard Rebell® amarillo trap (Table 1, Figure 2). The Rebell® trap consists of two yellow plastic panels fastened together at right angles. This cross-shape was copied with the material and color of Panel I from the color experiment. Additional shapes—a cube with a side length of 15 cm and a cylinder with a height 20 cm and a circumference of 54 cm—were produced from the same material (Figure 2). Furthermore, the combination of yellow surfaces with red dots was evaluated by spraying red dots of 13 mm in diameter in regular distances of 75 mm onto the cylinder-shaped trap. Spray colors of one layer of Fire red” (color code: RAL3000), followed by one layer of “Neon Signalrot fluoreszierend” (Dupli Color, Hassmersheim, Germany), were applied using a stencil. All trap types were glued using Tangle-Trap® (Contec Inc., Victoria, Canada).
Table 1. Material, shape and size of the sticky surface of trap types used in the two experiments.
Table 1. Material, shape and size of the sticky surface of trap types used in the two experiments.
Trap typeMaterial (thickness)FluorescenceShape (Sticky surface)Experiment
Panel RebellRebell® amarillo Polypropylene (1 mm)NoPanel (600 cm2)Color
Panel IPolypropylene (0.8 mm)YesPanel (600 cm2)Color
Panel KPolypropylene (0.5 mm)YesPanel (600 cm2)Color
Panel FPolyethylene (1 mm)NoPanel (600 cm2)Color
Panel GPolyethylene (1 mm)YesPanel (600 cm2)Color
Panel HPolyethylene (1 mm)YesPanel (600 cm2)Color
Cross RebellRebell® amarillo Polypropylene (1 mm)NoCross (1200 cm2)Shape
CrossPolypropylene (0.8 mm)YesCross (1200 cm2)Shape
CubePolypropylene (0.8 mm)YesCube (900 cm2)Shape
CylinderPolypropylene (0.8 mm)YesCylinder (1080 cm2)Shape
Dotted CylinderPolypropylene (0.8 mm)YesCylinder + 21 red dots of 13 mm in diameter (1080 cm2)Shape
The spectral reflectance of different colored yellow panels was measured using a Camag TCL 3 scanner. The reflectance and absorption was recorded continuously from 200 to 700 nm and showed clear differences (Figure 1). The standard Rebell® amarillo trap showed a strong increase in reflectance at 500–550 nm. Trap types F and G showed a similar strong increase in the same region. However, trap type F had another pronounced peak of reflectance in the UV region at 300–400 nm, but a very low reflectance at ~450 nm (blue region). Trap types H, I and K had a similar high reflectance above 500 nm, but these traps also showed a high reflectance in the blue region (400–500 nm).
Figure 1. Spectral reflectance curves for the six differently colored panels (without Tangle-Trap) compared in Experiment 1.
Figure 1. Spectral reflectance curves for the six differently colored panels (without Tangle-Trap) compared in Experiment 1.
Insects 05 00564 g001
Experimental design: The experiments were conducted in northwestern Switzerland. All three experimental orchards were located in a 3-km radius around Arlesheim BL (47°29'46.20''N; 7°37'01.72''E). The orchards consisted of untreated trees of the varieties Schauenburger, Hedelfinger and Basler Adler. Trees were 30–40 years old, 4–5 m high and had a canopy diameter of 5–6 m. The infestation pressure with R. cerasi was high in the previous years. Each trap was tested in three replicates in each orchard for a total of nine replicates. One replicate consisted of one cherry tree: all trap types were randomly fixed on a wooden bar with a distance of 50 cm between traps. The bar was fixed at a 3.5 m height to the south-southeast periphery of each cherry tree (see Figure 2f). This experimental design was chosen in order to test all trap types within the same tree, because infestation pressure can vary greatly, even among neighboring trees. We randomly arranged traps within one replicate and recorded the neighboring trap types in order to statistically correct for any influences between neighboring traps. Experiment 1 and Experiment 2 were conducted simultaneously in the same orchards on separate trees. The first flight of the cherry fruit fly was determined by using an temperature-based forecasting model [24], as well as one Rebell® trap per orchard. These monitoring traps were checked daily, and the experimental traps were installed at the day of first R. cerasi catches: on May 15, 2012, in Orchards 1 and 2 and on May 22, 2012, in Orchard 3, which is located at a higher altitude. Traps were removed immediately before cherry harvest on June 19, 2012.
Figure 2. Trap shapes tested in the experiment: (a) Rebell® amarillo (cross); (b) cross-shaped trap; (c) cylinder-shaped trap; (d) dotted cylinder-shaped trap; (e) cube-shaped trap (the “roof” was not glued); (f) one replicate of Experiment 1: a comparison of differently-colored panels.
Figure 2. Trap shapes tested in the experiment: (a) Rebell® amarillo (cross); (b) cross-shaped trap; (c) cylinder-shaped trap; (d) dotted cylinder-shaped trap; (e) cube-shaped trap (the “roof” was not glued); (f) one replicate of Experiment 1: a comparison of differently-colored panels.
Insects 05 00564 g002
Examinations: The number of cherry fruit flies per trap was counted in weekly intervals. All insect counts were conducted on the same day in all experimental orchards. Cherry fruit flies were removed from the traps during counting. The fruit load of the experimental trees was estimated (0 = little load; 1 = low load; 2 = normal-high load), as well as the shadow on the experimental traps due to cherry leaves and branches (0 = no shadow on the trap surface from 10 am to 4 pm; 1 = little shadow). Climatic conditions during the experimental period were monitored using a Campbell CR10X meteorological station.
Statistical analysis: JMP [25] was used for all statistical analyses. If necessary, data were [log10(x + 1)] transformed to obtain a normal distribution. The normality of data and the homogeneity of variance were tested before performing an ANOVA (factors: trap type, orchard, tree, shadow). Means were compared by Tukey’s HSD post hoc tests (α = 0.05). Data are presented in the figures and the text as means with standard errors.

3. Results and Discussion

The climatic conditions and flight activity of cherry fruit flies are given in Figure 3. The highest catches were obtained in the second week after the beginning of the flight period during the warm and sunny period from May 22 to 29, 2012.
Figure 3. The climatic conditions and cherry fruit fly catches (average over all experimental traps + SE) during the experimental period in 2012.
Figure 3. The climatic conditions and cherry fruit fly catches (average over all experimental traps + SE) during the experimental period in 2012.
Insects 05 00564 g003
A total of 7962 cherry fruit flies were captured during the experimental period (May 15 to June 19, 2012) on the 99 experimental traps. Among the three experimental orchards, significant differences were observed: flight intensity over the whole experimental period was highest in Orchard 1, with 166.5 ± 20.6 flies per trap, followed by Orchard 2, with 68.7 ± 10.8 flies per trap. The lowest flight intensity was observed in Orchard 3 (6.2 ± 1.6 flies per trap; statistical analysis: data transformed [logx + 1]; one-way-ANOVA, F2,96 = 69.4, p < 0.0001; Tukey’s HSD-test α = 0.05). Within orchards, flight intensity differed among the different experimental trees. However, fruit load on the cherry trees had no influence on trap catches. This might be because the fruit load was very low in most experimental trees following cold and rainy weather conditions during pollination in early spring. Differences between the experimental trees might be the result of the infestation level in the previous year or wind exposition. Due to the experimental design, we expected an influence of neighboring traps, but these influences were very low and statistically not significant. However, shadows from cherry leaves and branches on the experimental traps could not be avoided and influenced catch counts. In order to adjust for influencing factors, we used a four-way-ANOVA for the statistical comparison of trap catches. Included factors were trap type, orchard and tree (nested within orchard) and shadow (yes/no). In order to summarize the results, we analyzed the number of flies per trap captured from May 23 to 29 (main flight activity, beginning of experimental period), from June 13 to 19 (end of experimental period), as well as over the whole experimental period (May 15 to June 19).
Experiment 1, comparison of differently colored panels: The results of the comparison of differently colored panels are given in Figure 4. Over the whole flight period (Figure 4, black bars), trap Type F captured significantly more flies than trap Types K, H and the Rebell® trap, but not more flies than trap Types I and G. Trap Types I and G did not significantly differ from trap Type F or from the Rebell® trap (statistical analysis: data transformed [logx + 1], four-way-ANOVA, Tukey’s HSD-test; May 23–29: F5,39 = 8.7, p < 0.0001; June 13–19: F5,39 = 29.3, p < 0.0001; May 15–June 19: F5,39 = 32.9, p < 0.0001).
Figure 4. A comparison of differently-colored yellow sticky panels on the numbers of captured cherry fruit flies per week (+SE) during the second week of experiments (white bars), the fifth week of experiments (grey bars) and during the whole experimental period (black bars; average per week; Tukey’s HSD test p = 0.05; different letters show significant differences).
Figure 4. A comparison of differently-colored yellow sticky panels on the numbers of captured cherry fruit flies per week (+SE) during the second week of experiments (white bars), the fifth week of experiments (grey bars) and during the whole experimental period (black bars; average per week; Tukey’s HSD test p = 0.05; different letters show significant differences).
Insects 05 00564 g004
The attractiveness of trap Type F is mainly explained by the reflectance pattern of this trap (Figure 1). According to Agee et al. [19], R. cerasi has a major peak of electroretinographically-assessed spectral sensitivity at 485 to 500 nm (yellow green region) and a secondary peak at 365 nm (ultraviolet region). Trap Type F matches these specifications most closely of all tested trap types.
Trap Types F and G were identical in color and material, apart from fluorescent pigments contained in trap Type G. No significant differences were observed between these two traps. Thus, fluorescence appears unnecessary for high R. cerasi captures. This result is contrary to literature references, which mention a positive effect of fluorescence on trap catches of R. cerasi [13,14]. However, for the western cherry fruit fly, Rhagoletis indifferens, highly fluorescent traps were found to be less attractive than traps with lower fluorescence [26].
Trap Types H and K were the least attractive. This observation is consistent with the literature: Prokopy and Boller [20] noted that pure yellow color with a sharp increase of reflectance between 500 to 520 nm is most attractive and that adding as little as 0.5% of blue or red enamel resulted in a significant decrease in fly capture. The reflectance patterns of the traps (Figure 1) show that trap Types H and K had an increased reflectance in the blue region (400–500 nm).
Captures of cherry fruit flies during the fifth week of the experiment (Figure 4, grey bars) were lower than during the second week of the experiment (Figure 4, white bars). This observation can be due to several reasons: (1) reduced fly abundance due to natural mortality; (2) reduced flight activity of the cherry fruit fly; (3) reduced attractiveness of the traps, due to fading of the yellow color; or (4) reduced attractiveness of the traps due to black pollution with already captured insects. The reduction of captures was most pronounced in trap Type K, which visually faded during the experimental period (Figure 5). Trap Type H also faded, but the reduction in captures was less pronounced. Both trap Types H and K were nearly white at the end of the experiment. Fading was less obvious in the other trap types. Trap Type I was a bit lighter at the end of the experiment and also showed a reduction in captures in the fifth week of the experiment. Trap Types F and G were very stable, because the yellow color was not printed onto the trap, but imbued within it. Reduced captures in trap Types F and G might also be due to pollution with already captured insects. Absolutely no visual fading was observed in the Rebell® amarillo trap. This observation is in accordance with other experiments [27,28]. The Rebell® amarillo trap contains highly-stable lead chromate yellow pigments and is intended to be reusable for several years after cleaning and new gluing. The Rebell® amarillo trap showed the lowest reduction in captures. However, fading might have been of minor importance, because the rank order of captures of the different trap types was the same during the second and the fifth week of the experiment. Thus, we assume that the performance of the traps was stable over one flight period of R. cerasi. However, given the observed fading, the new trap types are not reusable in consecutive years.
Figure 5. The color of trap types before and after the experiment (trap Type L = Rebell® amarillo).
Figure 5. The color of trap types before and after the experiment (trap Type L = Rebell® amarillo).
Insects 05 00564 g005
Trap Types I (polypropylene) and G (polyethylene) captured significantly more flies than trap Types K (polypropylene) and H (polyethylene). Thus, trap material was of minor importance compared to trap color.
Experiment 2, comparison of different trap shapes: All traps in this experiment (Figure 2), except the standard Rebell® amarillo trap, were made from the same material as trap Type I in the color experiment. Differences between trap shapes were not significant during the second and the fifth week of experiment (Figure 6; statistical analysis: data transformed [logx + 1], four-way-ANOVA, Tukey’s HSD-test; May 23–29: F4,31 = 0.8, p = 0.52; June 13–19: F4,31 = 1.8, p = 0.15). Over the whole experimental period, a significant difference was observed between the cylinder-shaped trap and the standard Rebell® amarillo trap; this might have been due to differences in shape or differences in color (May 15–June 19: F4,31 = 3.1, p = 0.03). Apparently, shape is of minor importance as long as the object is three-dimensional and visible from all directions.
The sticky surface was different among the tested trap types (Table 1): in the first experiment (color comparison), panels with a 600-cm2 surface were used. Trap types in the shape experiment had sticky surfaces of 900 cm2 (cube), 1080 cm2 (cylinder) and 1200 cm2 (cross), respectively. Although the cross-shaped traps had the largest sticky surface, captures were slightly lower than in cylinder-shaped traps. However, due to the shape, the outlines of the cylinder-shaped traps were the largest. In addition, the sticky surface in the inner angles of the cross-shaped traps does not catch a lot of flies, as it is difficult to reach.
Figure 6. A comparison of differently-shaped trap types on the numbers of captured cherry fruit flies per week (+SE) during the second week of experiments (white bars), the fifth week of experiments (grey bars) and during the whole experimental period (black bars; average per week; Tukey’s HSD test p = 0.05, different letters show significant differences).
Figure 6. A comparison of differently-shaped trap types on the numbers of captured cherry fruit flies per week (+SE) during the second week of experiments (white bars), the fifth week of experiments (grey bars) and during the whole experimental period (black bars; average per week; Tukey’s HSD test p = 0.05, different letters show significant differences).
Insects 05 00564 g006
Red dots on the cylinder increased total captures by 12%, but the differences were not significant. It is known that Rhagoletis flies react to red or dark-colored spheres of approximately the same size as the host fruit [18]. The attraction of fruit flies to spherical objects is believed to represent a response to mating and oviposition site-type stimuli. Red spheres of 7.5 cm in diameter are used as traps for the apple maggot fly, Rhagoletis pomonella [29]. The optimal sphere size for R. cerasi was determined to be 2.5 cm in diameter by Prokopy [12]. He also assumed that flies detect spheres solely on the basis of circular silhouette. Haisch and Forster [16], however, demonstrated that dots of 1 cm in diameter increased captures on yellow traps, whereas dots with larger diameters had no influence, and Russ et al. [30] showed that the combination of yellow surfaces with either cherry-sized discs or hemispheres did not significantly increase the attractiveness of traps. Observations by Wiesmann [31] indicate that flies detect the location of host fruit solely through vision. Dark-colored spheres are preferred, because they stand out in the strongest contrast against the background [17,32]. While fruit‑mimicking spherical traps are superior for attracting R. pomonella, yellow panels were found to be more suitable for capturing R. cerasi [12,33]. By adding the red dots to the yellow sticky trap, we tried to combine these two stimuli. However, further experiments on dot size, color and distribution are needed to improve trap efficiency.

4. Conclusions

Over the whole flight period, trap Types I and G were equally effective as the standard Rebell® amarillo trap. Trap Type F was even significantly more effective than the Rebell® amarillo trap. Thus, the new trap Type F is suitable for monitoring the beginning of the flight period, as well as for mass‑trapping purposes in home gardens. Color of trap Type F was stable during one flight period of cherry fruit fly. However, we assume that prolonged exposure to environmental conditions might lead to a desaturation of color. This might be a disadvantage compared to the standard Rebell® amarillo trap, which contains highly-stable, but toxic, lead chromate yellow pigments and is intended to be reusable for several years after cleaning and new gluing. However, most farmers dispose the Rebell® amarillo trap after one use, because cleaning the traps with solvents and re-gluing is a dirty, time-consuming task. The new trap type, made from thinner material and without lead chromate, might therefore be a good, eco-friendly alternative.
Trap shape seemed to be of minor importance. Based on economic and practical considerations, the cylinder shape seems to be the best option. This shape has several advantages: (1) it is easy to produce with a small amount of cutting loss, in contrast to the cube-shaped traps; (2) when laid out flat, it can be very economically packed and shipped; (3) the cylinder shape is stable, even if thin panels are used, unlike with cross-shaped traps; and (4) cylinder-shaped traps are easy for farmers to handle in the field.
Red dots did not significantly increase captures. Further experiments are necessary to identify the optimal dot size and distribution. However, a commercial trap type without dots might be better, because: (1) the impact of red dots is considered to be rather low; (2) applying red dots would increase trap costs; and (3) because traps without dots can also be used for other pest insects (e.g., Meligethes sp. (Coleoptera: Nitidulidae) and Ceutorhynchus sp. (Coleoptera: Curculionidae) in oilseed rape production).
A commercial product “UFA-Samen Kirschenfliegenfalle” was developed based on these experiments: a cylinder-shaped trap (height 20 cm, circumference of 54 cm) made of Color F (yellow non-fluorescent polyethylene with a strong increase in reflectance at 500–550 nm, a secondary peak in the UV-region at 300–400 nm and a very low reflectance at ~450 nm). For cherry fruit fly monitoring—the detection of the presence/absence of pests and the detection of the first flight, to schedule precise insecticide applications—this product is now available and will be further evaluated under on-farm conditions in comparison with the standard Rebell® amarillo trap during the next few years. Further experiments are needed to investigate the efficacy of this new trap type for mass-trapping of R. cerasi.

Acknowledgments

Many thanks to Regina Burger (Fenaco UFA-Samen Nützlinge, Aesch BL, Switzerland) for supporting, promoting and implementing this research project, as well as to the companies, Granula AG (Merenschwand AG, Switzerland) and Läser AG (Gontenschwil AG, Switzerland), for providing small batches of customized colors and plastic panels and for their help in creating the prototypes. Many thanks also to Cecile Thonar for operating the Camag TLC scanner and to Brian Baker for language editing of the manuscript. We would like to thank the cherry growers for permitting the trials to be conducted within their orchards.

Author Contributions

Field work was conducted by Samuel Mathis as part of his vocational training. The thesis was supervised by Georg Feichtinger. Claudia Daniel was the second supervisor and composed this manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References and Notes

  1. Ranner, H. Untersuchungen zur Biologie und Bekämpfung der Kirschfruchtfliege, Rhagoletis cerasi L. (Diptera, Trypetidae) - III. Statistischer Vergleich der Schlupfperioden und Schlupfraten der Kirschfliege. Pflanzenschutzberichte 1988, 1, 17–26. (in German). [Google Scholar]
  2. Leski, R. Studia nad biologia i ecologia nasionnicy tzresniowki Rhagoletis cerasi L. (Diptera: Trypetidae). Pol. Pismo Entomol. Ser. B 1963, 2–4, 153–240. (in Polish). [Google Scholar]
  3. Boller, E. Beitrag zur Kenntnis der Eiablage und Fertilität der Kirschenfliege Rhagoletis cerasi L. Mitt. Schweiz. Entomol. Ges. 1966, 38, 195–202. (in German). [Google Scholar]
  4. Wiesmann, R. Untersuchungen über die Lebensgeschichte und Bekämpfung der Kirschenfliege Rhagoletis cerasi Linné - II. Mitteilung. Landw. Jahrb. Schweiz. 1934, 48, 281–338. (in German). [Google Scholar]
  5. Thiem, H. Beiträge zur Epidemiologie und Bekämpfung der Kirschfruchtfliege (Rhagoletis cerasi L.). Arb. Physiol. Angew. Entomol. Berl. Dahlem. 1934, 1, 7–79. (in German). [Google Scholar]
  6. Fimiani, P. Multilarval infestations by Rhagoletis cerasi L. (Diptera: Trypetidae) in cherry fruits. In Fruit flies of economic importance; Cavalloro, R., Ed.; Balkema: Rotterdam, The Netherlands, 1983; pp. 52–59. [Google Scholar]
  7. Daniel, C.; Grunder, J. Integrated Management of European Cherry Fruit Fly Rhagoletis cerasi (L.): Situation in Switzerland and Europe. Insects 2012, 7, 956–988. [Google Scholar]
  8. Daniel, C.; Wyss, E. Field applications of Beauveria bassiana to control the European Cherry Fruit Fly Rhagoletis cerasi. J. Appl. Entomol. 2010, 134, 675–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Böckmann, E.; Hummel, E.; Vogt, H. Promising field and semi field results for cherry fruit fly control using neem. In Proceedings  of the 15th International Conference on Organic Fruit-Growing, Hohenheim, Germany, 20–22 February 2012; pp. 167–172.
  10. Böckmann, E.; Köppler, K.; Hummel, E.; Vogt, H. Bait spray for control of European cherry fruit fly—An appraisal based on semi-field and field studies. Pest Manag. Sci. 2014, 70, 502–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Thistlewood, H.; Bostanian, N.; Senger, S.; DeLury, N. Adapting to new management strategies for cherry fruit flies in British Columbia, Canada. IOBC/WPRS Bull. 2010, 54, 197–203. [Google Scholar]
  12. Prokopy, R.J. Visual responses of European cherry fruit flies—Rhagoletis cerasi L. (Diptera, Trypetidae). Pol. Pismo Entomol. 1969, 39, 539–566. [Google Scholar]
  13. Boller, E. Neues über die Kirschenfliege: Freilandversuche im Jahr 1969. Schweiz. Z. Obst-und Weinbau 1969, 105, 566–572. (in German). [Google Scholar]
  14. Remund, U. Anwendungsmöglichkeiten einer wirksamen visuellen Wegwerffalle für die Kirschenfliege. Schweiz. Z. Obst-und Weinbau 1971, 107, 199–205. (in German). [Google Scholar]
  15. Haisch, A.; Forster, S. Versuche zur Anköderung und zum Fang der Kirschenfliege (Rhagoletis cerasi L.). Anz. Schadl. J. Pest Sci. 1969, 42, 97–102. (in German). [Google Scholar]
  16. Haisch, A.; Forster, S. Erfahrungen beim Fang der Kirschenfliege mit Leimtafeln und Leimkugeln. Gesunde Pflanz. 1970, 22, 1–4. (in German). [Google Scholar]
  17. Katsoyannos, B.I. Response to shape, size and color. In Fruit flies Their Biology, Natural Enemies and Control; Robinson, A.S., Hooper, G., Eds.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1989; Volume 3A, pp. 307–324. [Google Scholar]
  18. Prokopy, R.J. Orientation of the apple maggot flies Rhagoletis pomonella (Walsh) and European cherry fruit flies R. cerasi L. (Diptera: Tephritidae) to visual stimuli. In Proceedings of the 13 International Congress of Entomology, Moscow, Russia; 1971; Volume 2, pp. 34–35. [Google Scholar]
  19. Agee, H.R.; Boller, E.; Remund, U.; Davis, J.C.; Chambers, D.L. Spectral sensitivities and visual attractant studies on the Mediterranean Fruit Fly, Ceratitis capitata (Wiedemann), Olive Fly, Dacus oleae (Gmelin), and the European Cherry Fruit Fly, Rhagoletis cerasi (L) (Diptera, Tephritidae). J. Appl. Entomol. 1982, 93, 403–412. [Google Scholar]
  20. Prokopy, R.J.; Boller, E. Response of European Cherry Fruit Flies to colored rectangles. J. Econ. Entomol. 1971, 64, 1444–1447. [Google Scholar]
  21. Remund, U.; Boller, E. Kirschenfliegenfallen für Prognosewesen und biotechnische Bekämpfung im Vormarsch. Schweiz. Z. Obst-und Weinbau 1978, 114, 229–232. (in German). [Google Scholar]
  22. Dayan, A.D.; Pajne, A.J. Mechanisms of chromium toxicity, carcinogenicity and allergenicity: Review of literature from 1985 to 2000. Hum. Exp. Toxicol. 2001, 20, 439–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Regulation (EC) No 1907/2006 of the European Parliament and of the Council concerning the Registration, Evaluation, Authorisation and Restriction of Chemicals (REACH), establishing a European Chemicals Agency, amending Directive 1999/45/EC of the European Parliament and of the Council and repealing Council Regulation (EEC) No 793/93 and Commission Regulation (EC) No 1488/94, as well as Council Directive 76/769/EEC and Commission Directives 91/155/EEC, 93/67/EEC, 93/105/EC and 2000/21/EC.
  24. Samietz, J.; Graf, B.; Höhn, H.; Schaub, L.; Höpli, H.U. Phenology modelling of major insect pests in fruit orchards from biological basics to decision support: the forecasting tool SOPRA. EPPO Bull. 2007, 37, 255–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. JMP, version 5.0.1.2.; SAS Institute Inc.: Cary, NC, USA, 2003.
  26. Yee, W.L. Preference by Rhagoletis indifference (Diptera, Tephritidae) for rectangles of various yellow colours and fluorescence. J. Appl. Entomol. 2013, 137, 225–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Liburd, O.E.; Stelinsky, L.L.; Gut, L.J.; Thornton, G. Performance of various trap types for monitoring populations of Cherry Fruit fly (Diptera: Tephritidae) species. Environ. Entomol. 2001, 30, 82–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Katsoyannos, B.I.; Papadopoulos, N.T.; Stavridis, D. Evaluation of trap types and food attractants for Rhagoletis cerasi (Diptera: Tephritidae). J. Econ. Entomol. 2000, 93, 1005–1010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Prokopy, R.J. Visual responses of Apple Maggot Flies, Rhagoletis pomonella (Diptera: Tephritidae): Orchard studies. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 1968, 11, 403–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Russ, K.; Boller, E.; Vallo, V.; Haisch, A.; Sezer, S. Developement and application of visual traps for monitoring and control of populations of Rhagoletis cerasi L. Entomophaga 1973, 18, 103–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Wiesmann, R. Die Orientierung der Kirschfliege, Rhagoletis cerasi L., bei der Eiablage. Landw. Jahrb. Schweiz. 1937, 1080–1109. (in German). [Google Scholar]
  32. Levinson, H.Z.; Haisch, A. Optical and chemosensory stimuli involved in host recognition and oviposition of the cherry fruit fly Rhagoletis cerasi L. In Fruit flies of economic importance; Cavalloro, R., Ed.; Balkema: Rotterdam, The Netherlands, 1983; pp. 268–275. [Google Scholar]
  33. Economopoulos, A.P. Use of traps based on color and/or shape. In Fruit flies their biology, natural enemies and control; Robinson, A.S., Hooper, G., Eds.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1989; Volume 3B, pp. 315–327. [Google Scholar]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Daniel, C.; Mathis, S.; Feichtinger, G. A New Visual Trap for Rhagoletis cerasi (L.) (Diptera: Tephritidae). Insects 2014, 5, 564-576. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects5030564

AMA Style

Daniel C, Mathis S, Feichtinger G. A New Visual Trap for Rhagoletis cerasi (L.) (Diptera: Tephritidae). Insects. 2014; 5(3):564-576. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects5030564

Chicago/Turabian Style

Daniel, Claudia, Samuel Mathis, and Georg Feichtinger. 2014. "A New Visual Trap for Rhagoletis cerasi (L.) (Diptera: Tephritidae)" Insects 5, no. 3: 564-576. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects5030564

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop