Next Article in Journal / Special Issue
Demonstration and Characterization of a Persistent Pheromone Lure for the Navel Orangeworm, Amyelois transitella (Lepidoptera: Pyralidae)
Previous Article in Journal
A New Visual Trap for Rhagoletis cerasi (L.) (Diptera: Tephritidae)
Previous Article in Special Issue
Exceptional Use of Sex Pheromones by Parasitoids of the Genus Cotesia: Males Are Strongly Attracted to Virgin Females, but Are No Longer Attracted to or Even Repelled by Mated Females
 
 
Correction published on 20 August 2015, see Insects 2015, 6(3), 743-745.
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Semiochemical and Vibrational Cues and Signals Mediating Mate Finding and Courtship in Psylloidea (Hemiptera): A Synthesis

by
Umar K. Lubanga
1,*,
Christelle Guédot
2,
Diana M. Percy
3 and
Martin J. Steinbauer
1
1
Department of Zoology, La Trobe University, Melbourne, Victoria 3086, Australia
2
Department of Entomology, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI 53706, USA
3
Natural History Museum, London, SW7 5BD, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Insects 2014, 5(3), 577-595; https://doi.org/10.3390/insects5030577
Submission received: 12 May 2014 / Revised: 7 July 2014 / Accepted: 10 July 2014 / Published: 21 July 2014
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Pheromones and Insect Behaviour)

Abstract

:
Mate finding and courtship involve complex interactions that require close coordination between individuals of the opposite gender. Well-organized signalling systems, sometimes involving a combination of signal modalities, are required to convey species-specific and individual information to members of the opposite gender. Previous studies of psyllids have focused on single-signal modalities and have largely ignored the potentially interdependent nature of different types of signals. Several studies have shown that semiochemicals play a role in psyllid mate finding. However, long-range semiochemical sex attractants, such as the highly volatile sex pheromones used by many Lepidoptera (molecular weights <300), are yet to be identified. The compounds identified thus far, namely 13-methylheptacosane (from Cacopsylla pyricola) and dodecanoic acid (from Diaphorina citri), seem to have short range activity or no activity under field conditions. The possible role played by cuticular hydrocarbons in psyllid courtship remains largely ignored. Conversely, many psyllid species rely on vibrational signals for mate finding and mate assessment during courtship. This apparent disproportional reliance on vibrational rather than semiochemical signals suggests that vibrational signals have been more influential in sexual selection in psyllids. However, male fitness, female choice and benefits accrued from selecting fitter males remain poorly understood.

1. Signals and Strategies of Insect Sexual Selection

Mating behaviours are typically complex and comprise a series of interdependent cascading events [1]. Individuals must search for mates, engage in courtship, copulate and might need to recover from the cumulative costs of prior events. Searching and courtship involve communication and coordination between individuals that may not necessarily share similar intentions [2,3,4]. Sexual conflict arises because males and females have divergent interests linked to gender-based differences in size and the mobility of gametes, i.e., anisogamy [1,5].
Generally, males can afford to produce relatively larger numbers of gametes compared to females, whose gametes are usually larger and more costly to produce [6]. Consequently, males optimise their reproductive fitness by maximising the number of mating events, whereas females optimise fitness by selecting and mating with higher quality males, i.e., females are choosy [5,6,7]. Female choosiness is widely reported in several insect orders [8,9,10]. In some species, males may invest significantly in reproduction, leading to an apparent reversal of sex roles [11]. In such systems, female fitness is often an increasing function of the number of matings, especially when females receive direct benefits (e.g., nuptial gifts, access to food resources defended by males) from males during copulation [11]. For example, virgin and mated female Pieris protodice (Lepidoptera: Pieridae) solicit copulations to obtain fresh spermatophores from males [12].
Mating systems, such as polygyny (males mate with several females), polyandry (females mate with several males) and monoandry (an exclusive association with a single member of the opposite sex) have been reported in several insect orders, such as Orthoptera, Diptera and Hemiptera [13,14,15]. Similarly, male mating strategies, including female defence (males defend one or more females), i.e., male dominance and competition for choosy mates, resource defence (males defend resources critical to female survival) and self-advertising or leks (males position themselves around locations frequented by females) are widely reported among various insect orders [16,17,18]. Mating systems and male mating strategies determine sex roles (which sex searches and which sex signals), the efficacy of mate finding and the frequency of courtship [1,18].
Insects rely on a wide range of signalling modalities to communicate [19]. Orders, such as Lepidoptera and Hymenoptera, rely heavily on chemical and visual signals [20,21,22,23]. Orthoptera and some families of Hemiptera, most notably Cicadidae, primarily rely on airborne acoustic signals (e.g., [24,25]). Other hemipteran families (e.g., Pentatomidae and Cydnidae) are known to utilize substrate borne vibrations for mate location and assessment [26]. Orders in which chemical signalling is dominant usually possess, not surprisingly, highly developed olfactory systems [27], while those that rely on vibrational signals tend to possess well developed sound producing and perception organs [28,29]. The relative degree of complexity of morphological traits associated with signalling usually reflects the relative importance of a signal modality within a species [27,30]. The diversity of signals utilised reflects phylogenetic traits retained over evolutionary time, often as a consequence of physical constraints imposed by their environment [1].
Long-range signals are required to convey species and gender-specific information during searching, while short-range signals convey information about a specific individual, which is important during courtship [4,31]. In the case of chemical signalling, highly volatile semiochemicals (those with molecular weights (MWs) <300) are involved in long-range transmission of information, while heavier compounds (such as cuticular hydrocarbons with MWs >300) are considered more effective for short-range communication [1,32,33]. Single-signal modalities may be capable of conveying information during both mate finding and courtship. However, distortions (due to contamination or background noise (e.g., [34]) and, in some cases, deception from senders have driven some species to rely on more than a single signal modality, i.e., multimodal signalling systems [35]. For example, a multimodal signalling system involving semiochemicals and vibrational signals has been reported in the green vegetable bug, Nezara viridula (Hemiptera: Pentatomidae) [36,37]. Furthermore, mating and courtship signals are often under strong stabilising selection pressure to maintain species-specific information [38]. Consequently, species and mate recognition signals are expected to show little intraspecific variation, thus limiting the information available for mate assessment. Species, such as N. viridula, address this dilemma by utilizing a multimodal signalling system or, in cases where a single modality is utilized (e.g., vibrational signals), by varying mate finding and courtship signals [36,37]. Variation in mate finding and courtship signal characteristics has also been reported in field crickets and cicadas [39,40].

2. Introduction to the Psylloidea

Psylloids (psyllids or jumping plant lice) are small, exclusively phytophagous insects (Figure 1). The superfamily, Psylloidea, is highly diverse, comprising over 3800 described species distributed worldwide in all major zoogeographical regions [41,42]. Psyllid feeding can harm host plants by causing leaf chlorosis, necrosis and premature abscission [43,44]. Species, such as Diaphorina citri (Liviidae), Bactericera cockerelli (Triozidae) and Cacopsylla picta (Psyllidae), are economically important, because of their ability to vector harmful plant pathogens [45,46,47,48,49,50]. Psyllids reproduce sexually, and the immature ones pass through five nymphal instars before becoming adults [44]. Nymphal biologies include species that are free-living (nymphs do not develop beneath a formed shelter or in a gall), gall-forming (nymphs induce and develop inside a plant gall), lerp-forming (nymphs develop beneath a shelter of their own making) or, in some cases, inquilines (nymphs may reside beneath shelters or in galls made by other species) [44,51,52]. Seasonal dormancy in the form of either reproductive diapause or an “oligopause” have been reported in species endemic to regions with moderate to severe winters [53,54,55].

3. Psyllid Reproductive Biology and Mating Systems

In some species, both genders reach reproductive maturity within 24–48 h post-eclosion [56,57,58]. Oviposition usually commences within 24 h after mating, but may be delayed when females mate within 48 h post eclosion [56,57]. Studies of vibrational communication have shown that only unmated females responded to male signals, implying that there may only be a single copulatory event in some species [59,60]. However, it has been shown that females of some species must mate multiple times, in most cases with different males, to realize their reproductive potential [58,61,62]. Cacopsylla pyricola females have a requirement to mate at least twice within ten days, while Trioza erytreae (Triozidae) females require at least four matings to continuously produce fertile eggs [58,62]. In addition to increased fertility, females can benefit from increased longevity, as was reported in C. pyricola [62]. Following mating, males resume searching, while females may take longer before becoming sexually receptive again [58,59,63,64]. In B. cockerelli, the male refractory period lasts less than 24 h, while that of females may last a minimum of 48 h [64].
Figure 1. A pair of blue gum psyllids (Ctenarytaina eucalypti) in copula; female at top, male at bottom. Photograph courtesy of Ben Twist.
Figure 1. A pair of blue gum psyllids (Ctenarytaina eucalypti) in copula; female at top, male at bottom. Photograph courtesy of Ben Twist.
Insects 05 00577 g001
Greater numbers of receptive males relative to receptive females (male-biased operational sex ratios) usually result from gender-related differences in refractory periods and sets up conditions for male-male competition. In extreme cases, male-male competition may lead to female harassment and reduced oviposition [61,65]. Females of some insects, including psyllids, negate the effects of a biased operational sex ratio by not emitting signals [64,66]. This may explain why females of B. cockerelli were unattractive and even repellent to males during the refractory period [64] and may also account for the observation from vibrational studies of only a single mating event for females versus multiple events for males [59].
Whether or not females of a particular psyllid species typically mate once or more than once may be linked to the length of time from adult emergence to reproductive maturity and/or receptivity and to protandry (the seasonal emergence of males prior to females) [56,62]. Typically, protandry is more common in temperate [62,67] compared to tropical species [56].

4. Male Mating Strategies

Most studies of psyllid mating behaviour provide no distinction between mate finding and courtship events (see Figure 2). Here, we use the term mate finding synonymously with searching to refer to all activities leading to mate location and contact between males and females. Courtship is used to refer to close-range activities between males and females from initial contact to copulation. Generally, mate finding involves random movements by males followed by directed searching once they have perceived a female signal. In contrast, females are sedentary and, in most cases, only signal in response to male signals if receptive to mating [58,59,62,63,67,68,69].
While searching and mate finding may be protracted, courtship is typically brief [68,70] and, in most cases, lacks extravagant behaviours associated with courtship in other insect taxa, e.g., dances, exchange of nuptial gifts and serenades [71,72,73]. The small size of most psyllid species (~2–8 mm) complicates studies of courtship and partly explains why these behaviours are poorly understood. Nevertheless, rapid wing vibration and tarsal oscillation, such as observed in Cardiaspina densitexta and Cacopsylla pyricola courtship [67,68,70], suggest the use of vibrational signalling during courtship (note: the functional significance of tarsal oscillations to the production of substrate-borne vibrations has not been demonstrated). Antennation of the bodies of individual B. cockerelli and C. pyricola prior to copulation could suggest a role of cuticular hydrocarbons in mate assessment during courtship [68,70,74].
Figure 2. Generalised schema of behaviours from mate location through to mating and the refractory period for male and female psyllids, respectively. Key: * = signal studied, but role and transmission distance remains unclear; * = signal studied and believed to play a role in both mate finding and courtship; * = putative signal; [?] = probable information conveyed. The dotted arrow in the schema for females indicates that some species may mate only once.
Figure 2. Generalised schema of behaviours from mate location through to mating and the refractory period for male and female psyllids, respectively. Key: * = signal studied, but role and transmission distance remains unclear; * = signal studied and believed to play a role in both mate finding and courtship; * = putative signal; [?] = probable information conveyed. The dotted arrow in the schema for females indicates that some species may mate only once.
Insects 05 00577 g002
In most psyllid species, male-male competition is characterized by active searching for females, but not physical combat [58,63,68]. As mentioned, females are often sedentary and solitary or occur in loose aggregations [58,63,67,68]. Female choice seems to play an important role in determining the outcomes of courtship events [58,67,68]. Indeed, in some cases, males are rejected several times before successful mating occurs [67,68]. In some species, such as A. dobsoni, B. cockerelli and C. pyricola, males often attempt to mate with other males or with mating pairs without any obvious aggressive behaviour. These behavioural characteristics suggest that psyllid males maximise their fitness via competition for choosy mates, i.e., scramble competition (see [16]). Males are apparently the “search and signal” sex and females are the “sedentary and signal” sex. Most vibrational studies have found that females do not initiate signalling, but signal only in response to the perception of male signals [59,60,63,75,76]. However, in a few species, and only on rare occasions, females signal in the absence of males [77,78]. Thus, females may attract males using long-range signals (i.e., volatile semiochemicals or vibrational signals), and males may then be assessed by females using short-range signals (cuticular hydrocarbons or vibrational signals) (see Figure 2). Short range, sex-specific signals that are used for mate assessment are therefore expected to vary more in males compared to females. (Note: We propose that for psyllid semiochemical signals, long range should refer to distances greater than the length of a host plant module (e.g., photosynthetic branchlet or leaf), since these signals should be able to attract conspecific insects from neighbouring plants. For vibrational signals, long range should refer to distances greater than the length of a plant module, but limited to the same host, since these signals cannot transmit across open spaces. In contrast, short range for both semiochemical and vibrational signals should mean less than one body length of a conspecific insect.
Mating systems are unlikely to be similar across Psylloidea and male behaviours, such as attempting to mate with other males and with in copula pairs, suggesting the potential existence of alternative mating strategies, such as female mimicry and mate guarding, which have been found in other insects, but not considered in psyllids [2,16].

5. Semiochemical Signalling in Psyllids

Studies of the psyllid olfactory system have revealed a relatively simplified system. Unlike the complex antennae of insects in taxa, such as Lepidoptera and Hymenoptera, psyllid antennae are typified by sparse antennal sensilla and correspondingly few olfactory glomeruli [79,80,81]. It was only relatively recently that semiochemical signalling was reported to play a role in psyllid mate attraction [82]. Earlier studies had mainly focused on the role of semiochemicals in host plant location and selection [83,84,85]. Nevertheless, males of C. bidens (Psyllidae) were shown to be attracted to host plants supporting females, and their antennae produced greater electroantennographic responses to semiochemicals from pear hosts infested with females [82]. Additionally, while investigating the aggregation behaviour by post-diapause winter forms of C. pyricola, Horton and Landolt [86] found that males were more abundant on pear shoots currently or previously occupied by females compared to un-infested shoots or shoots previously occupied by males only. From these studies, it was not clear whether the males were more attracted to host plants or to female-produced semiochemicals. Subsequently, Horton et al. [87] confirmed that in summer forms of C. pyricola, it was female semiochemicals rather than plant semiochemicals that attracted conspecific males. Later, Guédot et al. [88] obtained similar results using the winter form of C. pyricola, suggesting the potential existence of female semiochemicals attractive to males. The existence of female-produced semiochemicals was confirmed in field trials, which showed that C. pyricola males of both types were more attracted to sticky traps baited with live females compared to traps baited with live males or traps that were left unbaited [89].
To-date, males of four psyllid species have been shown to be attracted to female-produced semiochemicals, namely: C. bidens, C. pyricola, B. cockerelli and D. citri (Table 1). In contrast, females are usually neither attracted nor repelled by male semiochemicals, with the exception of female B. cockerelli, which are repelled by male odour [90]. Although males and females are generally neither attracted nor repelled by same sex odours, cases of female-female repulsion (e.g., C. pyricola [91]), male-male attraction (e.g., B. cockerelli [90]) and male-male repulsion (e.g., C. pyricola [88]) have been reported. These results suggest that semiochemicals are utilized differently in different psyllid species. Furthermore, olfactometer bioassays using C. pyricola and B. cockerelli showed that whole body extracts of females were equally as attractive to males as live females [90,91].
These findings by Guédot [90] and colleagues suggest that female semiochemicals could be isolated and identified from whole body extracts using a combination of gas chromatography and mass spectrometry (GC-MS) and olfactometer bioassays. (Note: the term “whole body extracts” has been used by some authors synonymously with other terms, such as insect extracts, extracts and cuticular extracts to refer to extracts obtained by soaking whole bodies of freshly killed insects in non-polar organic solvents [90,92]. We propose a more precise application of the terms whole body extracts and cuticular extracts; whole body extracts is used to refer to extracts obtained by soaking or rinsing whole bodies of freshly killed insects. Such extracts may include compounds not typically associated with the insect’s cuticle, e.g., internal lipids and exocrine gland secretions. The term cuticular extracts is used to refer to those extracts obtained exclusively from the insect’s cuticle and likely to be composed entirely of cuticular hydrocarbons (CHCs)).
Male C. pyricola were shown to prefer post-diapause females to diapausing females [93]. Subsequently, by comparing the chemical profiles of whole body extracts using GC-MS of diapausing and post-diapause males and females, Guédot et al. [94] found that post-diapause females produced significantly larger quantities of 13-methylheptacosane. In olfactometer assays, live females were shown to be as attractive as a comparable quantity of synthetic 13-methylheptacosane, while field trials showed that males were attracted to synthetic 13-methylheptacosane. This provided the first evidence of a female-produced semiochemical capable of attracting male psyllids from neighbouring host plants. Using a similar approach, dodecanoic acid was identified as the female semiochemical attractive to male D. citri [92]. However, field trials using traps baited with dodecanoic acid did not increase the total catch of D. citri males compared to blank traps [92].
The physical properties of semiochemicals can be used to infer their likely suitability as long-range mate attractants. An effective long-range sex attractant should be highly volatile. The volatility of organic compounds is primarily a function of their molecular weight with an upper weight limit for airborne pheromones of about 300 [95]. With a molecular weight of 395, 13-methylheptacosane is, at best, weakly volatile and, therefore, most likely to function as a short-range attractant. Conversely, dodecanoic acid with a molecular weight of 200 seems sufficiently volatile to be a long-range attractant, although it failed to attract D. citri males to traps in the field [92]. Dodecanoic acid has been found in several other insect species, but to date, there is no record of its use as a sex attractant.
Table 1. Summary of signals mediating mate finding and courtship in Psylloidea.
Table 1. Summary of signals mediating mate finding and courtship in Psylloidea.
FamilySpeciesSemiochemical (s)Vibrational SignalNature of Vibrational SignalMechanism of Vibrational Signal ProductionRef.
Substrate Borne VibrationsAir Borne Vibrations
AphalaridaeAnoeconeossa sp. Anoeconeossa unicornuta RWV, TO [?][96,97,98]
Aphalara affinis, Aphalara polygoni RWV[60,96]
Australopsylla sp. RWV/TO [?][96,97,98]
Blastopsylla sp. ●? TO [?][97]
Cardiaspina albitextura, C. retator, C. tenuitela, C. densitexta Faint whirring soundRWV[67,98]
Cardiaspina maniformis, C. fiscella RWV[96,99]
Craspedolepta gloriosa, C. campestrella, C. nervosa, C. flavipennis, C. nebulosa RWV[60]
Creiis spp. High pitched buzzing callsRWV[76,98]
Ctenarytaina sp. ●? TO [?][97]
Glycaspis spp. G. brimblecombei, G. johnsoni, G. neureta RWV/TO [?][97,98]
Lasiopsylla rotundipennis RWV[96,98]
Spondyliaspis sp. RWV[96,98]
Phellopsylla sp. RWV[96,98]
CarsidaridaeProtyora sterculiae ●? TO [?][97]
LiviidaeDiaphorina citri▲ (A) RWV[78,92,100]
Eremopsylloides amirabilis RWV[63]
Livia juncorum Short buzzing soundsRWV[60,101]
Pachypsylloides citreus RWV[63]
PsyllidaeCacopsylla bidens ?[82]
Cacopylla pyri RWV[77]
Cacopsylla pyricola▲ (B) RWV[88,91,94]
Colposcenia aliena RWV[63]
Livilla ulicis RWV[60,96]
TriozidaeAacanthocnema dobsoni RWV[59]
Bactericera cockerelli ?[90]
Bactericera perrisii, B. kratochvili, B. calcarata, Eryngiofaga deserta RWV[63]
Schedotrioza apicobystra, S. cornuta, S. distorta, S. marginata, S. multitudinea, Schedotrioza sp. RWV[59]
Trioza tricornuta, T. percyae RWV[59]
Trioza spp. Trioza acutipennis, T. nigricornis, Trioza urticae Short buzzing soundsRWV[59,63,101]
Key: ▲ = species in which semiochemicals have been found to play a role in mate finding; A = dodecanoic acid; B =13-methylheptacosane; ● = species investigated for use of vibrational signals; ●? = species in which tarsal oscillations have been observed, but no vibrations detected; ■ = species from which vibratory signals have been recorded; RWV = rapid wing vibrations; TO [?] = tarsal oscillations (putative mechanism of vibrational signal production); ? = unknown mechanism of vibrational signal production.
Long-range volatile semiochemicals have previously been collected and identified after excising whole pheromone glands and analysing their chemical contents [102]. However, pheromone glands have not yet been identified in Psylloidea. Cuticular hydrocarbons (CHCs) are a complex blend of n-alkanes, methyl-branched alkanes and alkenes, typically with chain lengths of about 21–37 carbon atoms. If biologically active, these compounds are likely perceived at short range, possibly via contact chemoreception [33,103,104].
Clearly, further work is required to isolate semiochemicals responsible for the behaviours observed during experimental studies. Additionally, the possibility that high MW semiochemicals may have only short-range biological activity has been ignored. Nevertheless, semiochemicals with short-range (contact) activity are believed to be ideal indicators of transitory states relevant to mate quality and assessment, e.g., reproductive status/age and receptivity [4,105,106,107]. Although the ultimate function of cuticular hydrocarbons is to protect insects from desiccation [32], they are well known for their role in intraspecific communication [4,32,33].

6. Vibrational Signalling in Psyllids

Psyllids rely on simple stridulatory mechanisms to produce vibratory signals. By vibrating their forewings, a single row of ridges on the anal vein rubs against similar structures on protruding ridges on the meso- and meta-thorax to produce vibrations [76,96,108]. Such wing movements are commonly reported in the literature as wing flicks [63,67,78], but because some types of wing flicks are performed without producing distinct vibrational signals (during wing flexing or stretching), we refer to wing movements that produce signals as rapid wing vibrations. Signal characteristics, such as call (syllable) length, pulse number and reply latency, are used for species and gender recognition (Figure 3) [59,78].
Figure 3. Vibrational duetting in triozid psyllids. (A) Aacanthocnema dobsoni; long, simple male call (syllable) and short female reply (syllable). (B) Schedotrioza apicobystra (published with permission from CSIRO publishing) short and complex, tightly synchronized male-female duet. s = seconds.
Figure 3. Vibrational duetting in triozid psyllids. (A) Aacanthocnema dobsoni; long, simple male call (syllable) and short female reply (syllable). (B) Schedotrioza apicobystra (published with permission from CSIRO publishing) short and complex, tightly synchronized male-female duet. s = seconds.
Insects 05 00577 g003
Studies have indicated that temporal variations (e.g., call length, latency period and rate of call) and frequency modulation are more readily detected than structural variation [59,67,77,78]. It may be that subtle temporal and frequency shifts, rather than overall structural variation, distinguish changes in signal information associated with searching (at longer range) or courtship (at shorter range). However, under this scenario, there may still be limited intraspecific signal variation and, therefore, reduced information available to females for effective mate assessment and discrimination. Nevertheless, if signal structure in psyllids is under strong stabilizing pressure to maintain species-specific information, then varying signals via temporal or frequency shifts during mate finding and courtship may be sufficient for effective mate assessment and may be an effective compromise between different selection pressures.
Although evidence for variation in signals associated with mate proximity is limited in Psylloidea, the variation in signal structure that has been reported highlights the potential value of vibrational signals in qualitative mate assessment (sexual selection). If females use vibrational signals to assess and select between males, then there should be a correlation between desirable male traits and specific signal characteristics. More studies are needed to compare signals from solitary males with those of males producing mate finding and courtship signals and at varying distances from receiving females. Wenninger et al. [78] reported a significant correlation between fundamental frequency (lowest frequency of a periodic waveform) and male body mass in D. citri, which could potentially convey information about male fitness to receiving females.
Selective pressure on signal stabilization to convey species-specific information may vary between species and may be influenced by certain aspects of the species’ ecology. Percy et al. [59] showed that divergence in the signal of species sympatric on the same plants varies depending on whether the species are closely or distantly related. It is likely that species that live in sympatry with closely related species are under more signal stabilization pressure compared to species that live in sympatry with distantly related species or without other psyllid species on the same host plants. We currently do not know how species living in contrasting habitats respond to the challenges imposed by either stronger or more relaxed signal stabilization pressure. Percy et al. [59] found that species with short, complex signals exhibit less intraspecific variation than species with long, simple signals and that the former tend to engage in more tightly synchronized male-female duets (e.g., Schedotrioza Figure 3B). If species with long, simple calls exhibit greater intraspecific variation and less synchronized duetting (e.g., Aacanthocnema; Figure 3A), this may suggest weaker stabilizing selection.
Species that have a short male signal with limited intraspecific variation may carry less qualitative information to females, reducing the effectiveness of female choice in mate selection [109]. Under these circumstances, other signal modalities, such as visual signals and semiochemicals, may be important (e.g., [110,111]). Wenninger et al. [78] suggested that males rely on vibrational signalling in the absence of olfactory cues, but that they may also, conversely, rely less on vibrational signals in the presence of olfactory cues. This could imply a trade-off in the use of acoustic versus olfactory signals, especially where acoustic signals may be associated with greater risk, e.g., attraction of unwanted competition by eavesdropping males or of predation [109]. Percy et al. [59] reported longer female reply latencies in species with long simple male calls (e.g., A. dobsoni). Increased temporal variation in reply latencies, together with the absence of tightly synchronized duetting, may allow for the transmission of qualitative information. Additionally, female signals generally remain short, even when male signals are longer [59], perhaps because the risk of predation for the more sedentary female is greater, and therefore, variation in the reply latency may be used to transmit information with less risk. Thus, although the production of long male signals may be associated with higher energy costs, as well as increased risk of competition and predation [109], they may be more effective for the transmission of intraspecific information.

7. Multimodal Signalling

Although most research has focused on single-signal systems, it is unlikely that mate finding and selection is always based on a single modality. Moreover, different habitats are likely to promote the use of one type of signal over another, because vibrational and chemical signals have different transmission properties. Indeed, in several vibrational studies, not all mating events were preceded by vibrational signalling, suggesting that other signals may have been used [59,63]. Likewise, rapid wing vibrations have been observed in the olfactometer during chemical signalling studies [100]. Several studies report observing antennation and rapid wing vibration during mate finding and courtship [67,68,70]. Clearer evidence of multimodal signalling systems in Psylloidea was provided by Wenninger et al. [78], since males of D. citri were shown to increase their calling rate in the absence of female odours. Such a modulation of vibrational signals in response to the presence or absence of female odorants has also been reported in other hemipterans, e.g., Nezara viridula [37]. The varying role and interdependence of signal modalities remain under-investigated. More studies are needed to investigate how widespread multimodal signalling is within Psylloidea and whether the operation of these mating strategies is influenced by the presence of conspecific or heterospecific individuals.

8. Future Directions

Future studies of signals mediating mate finding and courtship should consider the possibility that some psyllid species may utilize a polyandrous mating system in which elevated levels of male-male competition may result from longer female refractory periods between matings. Both males and females may initiate signalling, but more work is needed to understand the frequency of female initiated signalling. To minimise contamination associated with solvent extraction and the loss of smaller molecules, analytical techniques for capturing volatiles (including headspace collection using solid-phase micro-extraction (SPME)) or direct sampling of weakly or non-volatile compounds by direct injection of whole insects into a GC should be considered. Surface cuticular extracts should be collected by rubbing of the cuticle with SPME fibres and/or silica-rubbing [112]). GC linked Electroantennogram Detection (EAD) or Single-Sensillum Recording (SSR)studies should be used to validate the physiological activity of putative sex attractants. To elucidate the existence of contact chemoreception, behavioural bioassays in appropriately-sized arenas should be used to compare male responses to freshly killed females, with versus without cuticular hydrocarbons [113,114,115]. The possible role played by visual cues has been almost entirely ignored. Studies by Burts et al. [62] and Wenninger et al. [56] have both suggested the possible use of visual cues by C. pyricola and D. citri, respectively, during mate finding. Species, such as Casuarinicola australis (Triozidae) and Casuarinicola warrigalensis (Triozidae), exhibit sexual dimorphism of wing patterns (maculation) [116], raising the possibility that they could provide mate finding and perhaps courtship cues.
Since the Psylloidea are so rich in species, exhibit unpredictable fluctuations in abundance and are so economically important, it is vital that we better understand the mechanisms that facilitate aggregation and enhance their reproductive success. The global importance of some psyllid species has already begun this process, but we need to extend it to those psyllid species waiting for us to alter conditions in their native habitats or to relocate them and their hosts overseas.

Acknowledgments

Umar Kombo Lubanga would like to acknowledge Richard Peters and Rachael Wood (La Trobe University)for assistance with the recording and analysis of psyllid vibrational signals and Falko Drijfhout (Keele University) and Kevin Farnier (La Trobe University) for guidance and assistance with the sampling and analysis of psyllid cuticular hydrocarbons. Umar Kombo Lubanga acknowledges the financial support provided by a La Trobe University Postgraduate Research Scholarship and a La Trobe University Full Fee Research Scholarship.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to the writing of the manuscript and approved its content.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Bradbury, J.W.; Vehrencamp, S.L. Mate attraction and courtship. In Principles of Animal Communication, 2nd ed.; Sinauer Associate Inc.: Massacusets, MA, USA, 1998; Volume 12. [Google Scholar]
  2. Thornhill, R. Male and female sexual selection and the evolution of mating strategies in insects. In Sexual Selection and Reproductive Competition in Insects; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1979; pp. 81–121. [Google Scholar]
  3. Parker, G.A. Sperm competition and the evolution of animal mating strategies. In Sperm Competition and the Evolution of Animal Mating Systems; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1984; pp. 1–60. [Google Scholar]
  4. Johansson, B.G.; Jones, T.M. The role of chemical communication in mate choice. Biol. Rev. 2007, 82, 265–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Chapman, T.; Arnqvist, G.; Bangham, J.; Rowe, L. Sexual conflict. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2003, 18, 41–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Schärer, L.; Rowe, L.; Arnqvist, G. Anisogamy, chance and the evolution of sex roles. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2012, 27, 260–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Pashler, H.; Mozer, M.; Harris, C.R. Mating strategies in a Darwinian microworld: Simulating the consequences of female reproductive refractoriness. Adapt. Behav. 2001, 9, 5–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Kaitala, A.; Wiklund, C. Female mate choice and mating costs in the polyandrous butterfly Pieris napi (Lepidoptera: Pieridae). J. Insect Behav. 1994, 8, 355–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Karlström, A. Female mate choice and modulation of oviposition preference in the moth Spodoptera littoralis. BSc. Thesis, Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences, Uppsala, Sweden, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  10. Crean, C.S.; Dunn, D.W.; Day, T.H.; Gilburn, A.S. Female mate choice for large males in several species of seaweed fly (Diptera: Coelopidae). Anim. Behav. 2000, 59, 121–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Bonduriansky, R. The evolution of male mate choice in insects: A synthesis of ideas and evidence. Biol. Rev. 2001, 76, 305–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Rutowski, R.L. Courtship solicitation by females of the checkered white butterfly, Pieris protodice. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1980, 7, 113–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Gwynne, D.T.; Jamieson, I. Sexual selection and sexual dimorphism in a harem-polygynous insect, the Alpine weta Hemideina maori (Orthoptera Stenopelmatidae). Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 1998, 10, 393–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Helinski, M.E.H.; Valerio, L.; Facchinelli, L.; Scott, T.W.; Ramsey, J.; Harrington, L.C. Evidence of polyandry for Aedes aegypti in semifield enclosures. Am. J. Trop. Med. Hyg. 2012, 86, 635–641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Franco, K.; Jauset, A.; Castañé, C. Monogamy and polygamy in two species of mirid bugs: A functional-based approach. J. Insect Physiol. 2011, 57, 307–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Thornhill, R.; Alcock, J. The Evolution of Insect Mating Systems; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  17. Emlen, S.T.; Oring, L.W. Ecology, sexual selection, and the evolution of mating systems. Science 1977, 197, 215–223. [Google Scholar]
  18. Shuster, S.M.; Wade, M.J. Mating Systems and Strategies; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 2003; pp. 262–443. [Google Scholar]
  19. Greenfield, M.D. Signalers and receivers. In Mechanisms and Evolution of Arthropod Communication; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2002; p. 414. [Google Scholar]
  20. Jacobson, M. Occurrence of sex pheromones in the female. In Insect Sex Pheromones; Academic Press, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 5–27. [Google Scholar]
  21. KonDo, Y.; Naka, H.; Tsuchida, K. Pheromones and body coloration affect mate recognition in the Japanese nine-spotted moth Amata fortunei (Lepidoptera: Arctiidae). J. Ethol. 2012, 30, 301–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Benelli, G.; Giunti, G.; Messing, R.H.; Wright, M.G. Visual and olfactory female-borne cues evoke male courtship in the aphid parasitoid Aphidius colemani Viereck (Hymenoptera: Braconidae). J. Insect Behav. 2013, 26, 695–707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Itadani, H.; Ueno, T. Chemically mediated mate finding of the polyphagous politary parasitoid Itoplectis naranyae (Hymenoptera: Ichneumonidae). Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 2014, 107, 288–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Cooley, J.R. Long-range acoustical signals, phonotaxis, and risk in the sexual pair-forming behaviors of Okanagana canadensis and O. rimosa (Hemiptera: Cicadidae). Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 2001, 94, 755–760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Virant-Doberlet, M.; Cokl, A. Vibrational communication in insects. Neotrop. Entomol. 2004, 33, 121–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Čokl, A.; Virant-Doberlet, M. Communication with substrate-borne signals in small plant-dwelling insects 1. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 2003, 48, 29–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Hansson, B.S.; Stensmyr, M.C. Evolution of insect olfaction. Neuron 2011, 72, 698–711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Bennet-Clark, H.C. Songs and the physics of sound production. Cricket Behav. Neurobiol. 1989, 227, 261. [Google Scholar]
  29. Young, D.; Bennet-Clark, H.C. The role of the tymbal in cicada sound production. J. Exp. Biol. 1995, 198, 1001–1020. [Google Scholar]
  30. Martin, J.P.; Beyerlein, A.; Dacks, A.M.; Reisenman, C.E.; Riffell, J.A.; Lei, H.; Hildebrand, J.G. The neurobiology of insect olfaction: Sensory processing in a comparative context. Prog. Neurobiol. 2011, 95, 427–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Ringo, J. Sexual receptivity in insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 1996, 41, 473–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Singer, T.L. Roles of hydrocarbons in the recognition systems of insects. Am. Zool. 1998, 38, 394–405. [Google Scholar]
  33. Martin, S.; Drijfhout, F. A review of ant cuticular hydrocarbons. J. Chem. Ecol. 2009, 35, 1151–1161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Cocroft, R.B.; Rodríguez, R.L. The behavioral ecology of insect vibrational communication. Bioscience 2005, 55, 323–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Higham, J.P.; Hebets, E.A. An introduction to multimodal communication. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2013, 67, 1381–1388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Miklas, N.; Čokl, A.; Renou, M.; Virant-Doberlet, M. Variability of vibratory signals and mate choice selectivity in the southern green stink bug. Behav. Process. 2003, 61, 131–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Miklas, N.; Lasnier, T.; Renou, M. Male bugs modulate pheromone emission in response to vibratory signals from conspecifics. J. Chem. Ecol. 2003, 29, 561–574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Paterson, H.E.H. The recognition concept of species. In Species and Speciation; Transvaal Museum: Pretoria, South Africa, 1985; pp. 21–29. [Google Scholar]
  39. Wagner, W.E.; Reiser, M.G. The importance of calling song and courtship song in female mate choice in the variable field cricket. Anim. Behav. 2000, 59, 1219–1226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Fonseca, P.J. Cicada acoustic communication. In Insect Hearing and Acoustic Communication; Hedwig, B., Ed.; Berlin Heidelberg, Springer: Berlin, Heidelberg, Germany, 2014; pp. 101–121. [Google Scholar]
  41. Austin, A.D.; Yeates, D.K.; Cassis, G.; Fletcher, M.J.; La Salle, J.; Lawrence, J.F.; McQuillan, P.B.; Mound, L.A.; Bickel, D.J.; Gullan, P.J.; et al. Insects “Down Under”—Diversity, endemism and evolution of the Australian insect fauna: Examples from select orders. Aust. J. Entomol. 2004, 43, 216–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Ouvrard, D. Psyl’list. The World Psylloidea Database. Available online: http://www.hemiptera-databases.com/psyllist (accessed on 5 July 2014).
  43. Woodburn, T.L.; Lewis, E.E. A comparative histological study of the effects of feeding by nymphs of four psyllid species on the leaves of eucalypts. Aust. J. Entomol. 1973, 12, 134–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Hodkinson, I.D. The biology of the Psylloidea (Homoptera): A review. Bull. Entomol. Res. 1974, 64, 325–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Munyaneza, J.E.; Crosslin, J.M.; Upton, J.E. Association of Bactericera cockerelli (Homoptera: Psyllidae) with “zebra chip,” a new potato disease in southwestern United States and Mexico. J. Econ. Entomol. 2007, 100, 656–663. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Munyaneza, J.E.; Goolsby, J.A.; Crosslin, J.M.; Upton, J.E. Further evidence that zebra chip potato disease in the lower Rio Grande Valley of Texas is associated with Bactericera cockerelli. Subtrop. Plant Sci. 2007, 59, 30–37. [Google Scholar]
  47. Hung, T.H.; Hung, S.C.; Chen, C.N.; Hsu, M.H.; Su, H.J. Detection by PCR of Candidatus Liberibacter asiaticus, the bacterium causing citrus huanglongbing in vector psyllids: Application to the study of vector-pathogen relationships. Plant Pathol. 2004, 53, 96–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Halbert, S.E.; Manjunath, K.L. Asian citrus psyllids (Sternorrhyncha: Psyllidae) and greening disease of citrus: A literature review and assessment of risk in Florida. Fla. Entomol. 2004, 87, 330–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Mayer, C.J.; Vilcinskas, A.; Gross, J. Phytopathogen lures its insect vector by altering host plant odor. J. Chem. Ecol. 2008, 34, 1045–1049. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Mayer, C.J.; Vilcinskas, A.; Gross, J. Chemically mediated multitrophic interactions in a plant—insect vector-phytoplasma system compared with a partially nonvector species. Agric. For. Entomol. 2011, 13, 25–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Burckhardt, D.; Ouvrard, D.; Queiroz, D.; Percy, D. Psyllid host-plants (Hemiptera: Psylloidea): Resolving a semantic problem. Fla. Entomol. 2014, 97, 242–246. [Google Scholar]
  52. Yang, M.-M.; Raman, A. Diversity, richness, and patterns of radiation among gall-inducing psyllids (Hemiptera: Psylloidea) in the Orient and Eastern Palearctic. Oriental Insects. 2007, 41, 55–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Prophetou-Athanasiadou, D.A.; Tzanakakis, M.E. Diapause termination in the olive psyllid Euphyllura phillyreae, in the field and in the laboratory. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 1986, 40, 263–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Krysan, J.L.; Higbee, B.S. Seasonality of mating and ovarian development overwintering Cacopsylla pyricola (Homoptera: Psyllidae). Environ. Entomol. 1990, 19, 544–550. [Google Scholar]
  55. Purvis, G.; Chauzar, M.P.; Segonds. Pichon, A.; Dunne, R. Life history and phenology of the eucalyptus psyllid, Ctenarytaina eucalypti in Ireland. Ann. Appl. Biol. 2002, 141, 283–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Wenninger, E.J.; Hall, D.G. Daily timing of mating and age at reproductive maturity in Diaphorina citri (Hemiptera: Psyllidae). Fla. Entomol. 2007, 90, 715–722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Guédot, C.; Horton, D.R.; Landolt, P.J. Age at reproductive maturity and effect of age and time of day on sex attraction in the potato psyllid Bactericera cockerelli. Insect Sci. 2012, 19, 585–594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Van den Berg, M.A.; Deacon, V.E.; Thomas, C.D. Ecology of the citrus psylla, Trioza erytreae (Hemiptera: Triozidae). 3. Mating, fertility and oviposition. Phytophylactica 1991, 23, 195–200. [Google Scholar]
  59. Percy, D.M.; Taylor, G.S.; Kennedy, M. Psyllid communication: Acoustic diversity, mate recognition and phylogenetic signal. Invertebr. Syst. 2006, 20, 431–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Tishechkin, D.Y.; Drosopoulos, S.; Claridge, M.F. Vibratory communication in Psylloidea (Hemiptera). In Insect Sounds and Communication; Drosopoulos, S., Claridge, M.F., Eds.; CRS Press: New York, NY, USA, 2005; pp. 357–363. [Google Scholar]
  61. Wenninger, E.J.; Hall, D.G. Importance of multiple mating to female reproductive output in Diaphorina citri. Physiol. Entomol. 2008, 33, 316–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Burts, E.C.; Fischer, W.R. Mating behavior, egg production, and egg fertility in the Pear psylla. J. Econ. Entomol. 1967, 60, 1297–1300. [Google Scholar]
  63. Tishechkin, D.Y. New data on vibratory communication in jumping plant lice of the families Aphalaridae and Triozidae (Homoptera, Psyllinea). Entomol. Rev. 2007, 87, 394–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Guédot, C.; Horton, D.R.; Landolt, P.J.; Munyaneza, J.E. Effect of mating on sex attraction in Bactericera cockerelli with evidence of refractoriness. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 2013, 149, 27–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Horton, D.R.; Lewis, T.M. Interplant movement by pear psylla (Homoptera: Psyllidae): Effects of sex ratio and reproductive status. J. Insect Behav. 1995, 8, 687–700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Andersson, J.; Borg-Karlson, A.-K.; Wiklund, C. Sexual cooperation and conflict in butterflies: A male-transferred anti-aphrodisiac reduces harassment of recently mated females. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 2000, 267, 1271–1275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. White, T.C.R. Some aspects of the life history, host selection, dispersal and ovipostion of adult Cardiaspina densitexta (Homoptera: Psyllidae). Aust. J. Zool. 1970, 18, 105–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Krysan, J.L. Laboratory study of mating behavior as related to diapause in overwintering Cacopsylla pyricola (Homoptera: Psyllidae). Environ. Entomol. 1990, 19, 551–557. [Google Scholar]
  69. Cook, P.P. Mating behaviour of Psylla pyricola Forster (Hom. Psyllidae). Pan-Pac. Entomol. 1963, 39, 175. [Google Scholar]
  70. Brown., R.L. Chemical and Behavioral Ecology of the Pear Psylla, Cacopsylla pyricola Förster (Hemiptera: Psyllidae). M.Sc. Thesis, Washington State University, Washington, DC, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  71. Kozlowski, M.W.; Aoxiang, S. Ritual behaviors associated with spermatophore transfer in Deuterosminthurus bicinctus (Collembola: Bourletiellidae). J. Ethol. 2006, 24, 103–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Kessel, E.L. The mating activities of balloon flies. Syst. Biol. 1955, 4, 97–104. [Google Scholar]
  73. Nelson, C.M.; Nolen, T.G. Courtship song, male agonistic encounters, and female mate choice in the house cricket, Acheta domesticus (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). J. Insect Behav. 1997, 10, 557–570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Knowlton, G.F.; Janes, M.J. Studies on the biology of Paratrioza cockerelli (Sulc). Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 1931, 24, 283–292. [Google Scholar]
  75. Yang, M.M.; Yang, C.T.; Chao, J.T. Reproductive isolation and taxonomy of two Taiwanese Paurocephala species (Homoptera: Psylloidea). Monogr. Taiwan Musuem 1986, 6, 177–203. [Google Scholar]
  76. Taylor, K.L. A possible stridulatory organ in some Psylloidea (Homoptera). Aust. J. Entomol. 1985, 24, 77–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Eben, A.; Mühlethaler, R.; Gross, J.; Hoch, H. First evidence of acoustic communication in the pear psyllid Cacopsylla pyri L. (Hemiptera: Psyllidae). J. Pest Sci. 2014, in press. [Google Scholar]
  78. Wenninger, E.J.; Hall, D.G.; Mankin, R.W. Vibrational communication between the sexes in Diaphorina citri (Hemiptera: Psyllidae). Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 2009, 102, 547–555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Kristoffersen, L.; Hallberg, E.; Wallén, R.; Anderbrant, O. Sparse sensillar array on Trioza apicalis (Homoptera, Triozidae) antennae-an adaptation to high stimulus levels? Arthropod Struct. Dev. 2006, 35, 85–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Onagbola, E.O.; Meyer, W.L.; Boina, D.R.; Stelinski, L.L. Morphological characterization of the antennal sensilla of the Asian citrus psyllid, Diaphorina citri Kuwayama (Hemiptera: Psyllidae), with reference to their probable functions. Micron 2008, 39, 1184–1191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Arras, J.; Hunter, W.; Bextine, B. Comparative analysis of antennae sensory arrays in asian citrus psyllid, Diaphorina citri, and potato psyllid, Bactericera cockerelli (Hemiptera). Southwest. Entomol. 2012, 37, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Soroker, V.; Talebaev, S.; Harari, A.R.; Wesley, S.D. The role of chemical cues in host and mate location in the pear psylla Cacopsylla bidens (Homoptera: Psyllidae). J. Insect Behav. 2004, 17, 613–626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Moran, V.C.; Brown, R.P. The antennae, host plant chemoreception and probing activity of the citrus psylla, Trioza erytreae (Del Guercio) (Homoptera: Psyllidae). J. Entomol. Soc. South. Afr. 1973, 36, 191–202. [Google Scholar]
  84. Valterová, I.; Nehlin, G.; Borg-Karlson, A.-K. Host plant chemistry and preferences in egg-laying Trioza apicalis (Homoptera, Psylloidea). Biochem. Syst. Ecol. 1997, 25, 477–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Horton, D.R.; Krysan, J.L. Host acceptance behavior of pear psylla (Homoptera: Psyllidae) affected by plant species, host deprivation, habituation, and eggload. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 1991, 84, 612–627. [Google Scholar]
  86. Horton, D.R.; Landolt, P.J. Attraction of male pear psylla, Cacopsylla pyricola, to female infested pear shoots. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 2007, 123, 177–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Horton, D.R.; Guédot, C.; Landolt, P.J. Attraction of male summerform pear psylla to volatiles from female pear psylla: Effects of female age, mating status, and presence of host plant. Can. Entomol. 2008, 140, 184–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Guédot, C.; Horton, D.R.; Landolt, P.J. Attraction of male winterform pear psylla to female produced volatiles and to female extracts and evidence of male-male repellency. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 2009, 130, 191–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Brown, R.L.; Landolt, P.J.; Horton, D.R.; Zack, R.S. Attraction of Cacopsylla pyricola (Hemiptera: Psyllidae) to female psylla in pear orchards. Environ. Entomol. 2009, 38, 815–822. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Guédot, C.; Horton, D.R.; Landolt, P.J. Sex Attraction in Bactericera cockerelli (Hemiptera: Triozidae). Environ. Entomol. 2010, 39, 1302–1308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Guédot, C.; Horton, D.R.; Landolt, P.J. Response of summerform pear psylla (Hemiptera: Psyllidae) to male-and female-produced odors. Can. Entomol. 2011, 143, 245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Mann, R.S.; Rouseff, R.L.; Smoot, J.; Rao, N.; Meyer, W.L.; Lapointe, S.L.; Robbins, P.S.; Cha, D.; Linn, C.E.; Webster, F.X. Chemical and behavioral analysis of the cuticular hydrocarbons from Asian citrus psyllid, Diaphorina citri. Insect Sci. 2012, 20, 367–378. [Google Scholar]
  93. Horton, D.R.; Guédot, C.; Landolt, P.J. Diapause status of females affects attraction of male pear psylla, Cacopsylla pyricola, to volatiles from female infested pear shoots. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 2007, 123, 185–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Guédot, C.; Millar, J.G.; Horton, D.R.; Landolt, P.J. Identification of a sex attractant pheromone for male winterform pear psylla, Cacopsylla pyricola. J. Chem. Ecol. 2009, 35, 1437–1447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Bradbury, J.W.; Vehrencamp, S.L. Chemical signals. In Principles of Animal Communication, 2nd ed.; Sinauer Associate Inc.: Massacusets, MA, USA, 1998; Volume 12, pp. 193–228. [Google Scholar]
  96. Taylor, K.L. The Australian Genera Cardiaspina Crawford and Hyalinaspis Taylor, (Homoptera: Psyllidae). Aust. J. Zool. 1962, 10, 307–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Carver, M. Distinctive motory behaviour in some adult psyllids (Homoptera: Psylloidea). Aust. J. Entomol. 1987, 26, 369–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Percy, D.M. Psyllid communication. Available online: http://www.psyllids.org/ (accessed on 5 July 2014).
  99. Campbell, K.G. Sound production by Psyllidae (Hemiptera). Aust. J. Entomol. 1964, 1, 3–4. [Google Scholar]
  100. Wenninger, E.J.; Stelinski, L.L.; Hall, D.G. Behavioral evidence for a female-produced sex attractant in Diaphorina citri. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 2008, 128, 450–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Ossiannilsson, F. Sound production in psyllids (Hem. Hom.). Opusc. Entomol. 1950, 15, 202. [Google Scholar]
  102. Niño, E.L.; Malka, O.; Hefetz, A.; Tarpy, D.R.; Grozinger, C.M. Chemical profiles of two pheromone glands are differentially regulated by distinct mating factors in honey bee queens (Apis mellifera L.). PLoS One 2013, 8, e78637. [Google Scholar]
  103. Ginzel, M.D.; Blomquist, G.J.; Millar, J.G.; Hanks, L.M. Role of contact pheromones in mate recognition in Xylotrechus colonus. J. Chem. Ecol. 2003, 29, 533–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Lockey, K.H. Insect cuticular hydrocarbons. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 1980, 65, 457–462. [Google Scholar]
  105. Everaerts, C.; Farine, J.-P.; Cobb, M.; Ferveur, J.-F. Drosophila cuticular hydrocarbons revisited: Mating status alters cuticular profiles. PLoS One. 2010, 5, e9607. [Google Scholar]
  106. Monnin, T. Chemical recognition of reproductive status in social insects. Ann. Zool. Fenn. 2006, 43, 515–530. [Google Scholar]
  107. Kuo, T.-H.; Yew, J.Y.; Fedina, T.Y.; Dreisewerd, K.; Dierick, H.A.; Pletcher, S.D. Aging modulates cuticular hydrocarbons and sexual attractiveness in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Exp. Biol. 2012, 215, 814–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Heslop-Harrison, G. Sound production in the Homoptera with special reference to sound producing mechanisms in the Psyllidae. Ann. Mag. Nat. Hist. 1960, 3, 633–640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Bailey, W.J. Insect duets: Underlying mechanisms and their evolution. Physiol. Entomol. 2003, 28, 157–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Claridge, M.F. Acoustic recognition signals: Barriers to hybridization in Homoptera Auchenorrhyncha. Can. J. Zool. 1990, 68, 1741–1746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Webb, J.C.; Sivinski, J.; Litzkow, C. Acoustical behavior and sexual success in the Caribbean fruit fly, Anastrepha suspensa (Loew) (Diptera: Tephritidae). Environ. Entomol. 1984, 13, 650–656. [Google Scholar]
  112. Choe, D.-H.; Ramírez, S.R.; Tsutsui, N.D. A silica gel based method for extracting insect surface hydrocarbons. J. Chem. Ecol. 2012, 38, 176–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Ruther, J.; Döring, M.; Steiner, S. Cuticular hydrocarbons as contact sex pheromone in the parasitoid Dibrachys cavus. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 2011, 140, 59–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Olaniran, O.A.; Sudhakar, A.V.S.; Drijfhout, F.P.; Dublon, I.A.N.; Hall, D.R.; Hamilton, J.G.C.; Kirk, W.D.J. A male-predominant cuticular hydrocarbon, 7-methyltricosane, is used as a contact pheromone in the western flower thrips Frankliniella occidentalis. J. Chem. Ecol. 2013, 39, 559–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Guarino, S.; de Paswuale, C.; Peri, E.; Alonzo, G.; Colazza, S. Role of volatile and contact pheromones in the mating behaviour of Bagrada hilaris (Heteroptera: Pentatomidae). Eur. J. Entomol. 2008, 105, 613–617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Taylor, G.S.; Austin, A.D.; Jennings, J.T.; Purcell, M.F.; Wheeler, G.S. Casuarinicola, a new genus of jumping plant lice (Hemiptera: Triozidae) from Casuarina (Casuarinaceae). Zootaxa 2010, 2601, 1–27. [Google Scholar]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Lubanga, U.K.; Guédot, C.; Percy, D.M.; Steinbauer, M.J. Semiochemical and Vibrational Cues and Signals Mediating Mate Finding and Courtship in Psylloidea (Hemiptera): A Synthesis. Insects 2014, 5, 577-595. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects5030577

AMA Style

Lubanga UK, Guédot C, Percy DM, Steinbauer MJ. Semiochemical and Vibrational Cues and Signals Mediating Mate Finding and Courtship in Psylloidea (Hemiptera): A Synthesis. Insects. 2014; 5(3):577-595. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects5030577

Chicago/Turabian Style

Lubanga, Umar K., Christelle Guédot, Diana M. Percy, and Martin J. Steinbauer. 2014. "Semiochemical and Vibrational Cues and Signals Mediating Mate Finding and Courtship in Psylloidea (Hemiptera): A Synthesis" Insects 5, no. 3: 577-595. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects5030577

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop