Next Article in Journal
Virtual Reality for Research in Social Neuroscience
Previous Article in Journal
Hippocampal Neuronal Loss in Infant Macaques Orally Infected with Virulent Simian Immunodeficiency Virus (SIV)
Previous Article in Special Issue
Intensive Sleep Re-Training: From Bench to Bedside
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

How Hyperarousal and Sleep Reactivity Are Represented in Different Adult Age Groups: Results from a Large Cohort Study on Insomnia

1
Sommeil, Addiction et Neuropsychiatrie, Univ. Bordeaux, USR 3413, F-33000 Bordeaux, France
2
Sommeil, Addiction et Neuropsychiatrie, CNRS, USR 3413, F-33000 Bordeaux, France
3
École de psychologie, Université Laval, Québec City, QC G1V 0A6, Canada
4
Centre d’Étude des Troubles du Sommeil, Centre de Recherche de l’Institut Universitaire en Santé Mentale de Québec, Quebec City, QC G1J 2G3, Canada
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2017, 7(4), 41; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci7040041
Submission received: 8 November 2016 / Revised: 6 April 2017 / Accepted: 7 April 2017 / Published: 14 April 2017
(This article belongs to the Special Issue New Research in Insomnia)

Abstract

:
Hyperarousal is a 24-h state of elevated cognitive and physiological activation, and is a core feature of insomnia. The extent to which sleep quality is affected by stressful events—so-called sleep reactivity—is a vulnerability factor for developing insomnia. Given the increasing prevalence of insomnia with age, we aimed to investigate how hyperarousal and sleep reactivity were related to insomnia severity in different adult age groups. Data were derived from a large cohort study investigating the natural history of insomnia in a population-based sample (n = 1693). Baseline data of the Arousal Predisposition Scale (APS) and Ford Insomnia Response to Stress Test (FIRST) were examined across age and sleep/insomnia subgroups: 25–35 (n = 448), 35–45 (n = 528), and 45–55 year olds (n = 717); good sleepers (n = 931), individuals with insomnia symptoms (n = 450), and individuals with an insomnia syndrome (n = 312). Results from factorial analyses of variance (ANOVA) showed that APS scores decreased with increasing age, but increased with more severe sleep problems. FIRST scores were not significantly different across age groups, but showed the same strong increase as a function of sleep problem severity. The findings indicate that though arousal predisposition and sleep reactivity increase with more severe sleep problems, only arousal decreases with age. How arousing events affect an individual during daytime thus decreases with age, but how this arousal disrupts sleep is equivalent across different adult age groups. The main implication of these findings is that treatment of insomnia could be adapted for different age groups and take into consideration vulnerability factors such as hyperarousal and stress reactivity.

1. Introduction

Insomnia is a prevalent condition in all ages, but particularly in middle-aged and older adults [1]. A core feature of insomnia is hyperarousal—a 24-h state of elevated physiological and cognitive activation [2]. Hyperarousal is well recognized as a risk factor for developing insomnia [3]. A measure particularly sensitive for measuring hyperarousal is the Arousal Predisposition Scale (APS) [4], which has provided insight into the intermediating role that arousal plays between stress and sleep [5]. Scores on this questionnaire have been shown to be higher in insomnia syndrome incident cases compared to good sleepers [3].
The extent to which sleep is affected by stressful events—so-called sleep reactivity—is another factor that has been hypothesized to increase vulnerability for developing insomnia. The degree of sleep reactivity in an individual can be assessed by the Ford Insomnia Response to Stress Test (FIRST) [6]. Higher sleep reactivity has been associated with an increased risk of incident insomnia syndrome one year later among good sleepers [7] and strongly related to arousal and emotion coping in insomnia [8].
Older adults may be less affected by stress through habituation and improved coping strategies, which are important factors driving sleep quality [5,9,10]. Few studies investigated age differences in immediate and delayed stress reactions, and those who do mostly focus on older age groups (over 65 years) [11,12]. However, a large study (n = 190) applying ecological momentary assessment (EMA) five times a day in 20- to 81-year-olds to measure positive and negative affect has shown that older adults, as opposed to younger adults, are less affected by stressors. However, no age differences were observed in the effects of the stressor three to six hours after exposure [13]. Studies that investigate how age affects the stress–arousal–sleep relationship are lacking as of yet.
Sleep becomes more disrupted with age, and part of those changes is explained by the increased incidence of health problems with aging. In addition, there are other developmental changes that may impact sleep quality with aging, including neurocognitive and hormonal changes, behavioural/sleep scheduling factors, and reduced amplitude of circadian rhythms [14]. These factors may attenuate the role of arousal as a factor accounting for sleep disturbances. Our goal was to examine the impact of hyperarousal and sleep reactivity as insomnia vulnerability factors across age groups, while at the same time minimizing the potential confound of these variables. As such, we can investigate the importance of hyperarousal and sleep reactivity as vulnerability factors for insomnia and its changes within older adult age.
In the current study, we thus investigate how hyperarousal and sleep reactivity are related to insomnia severity in a large cross-sectional study comparing different adult age groups from 25 to 55 years old. We first hypothesize that both factors are strongly related to existing sleep problems, independent of age. We expect that hyperarousal decreases with age, but sleep reactivity remains equal independent of sleep problems. Findings can give insight into factors playing a role in vulnerability to insomnia in different age groups, which may facilitate personalized treatment adapted to different age groups, aimed not just at treating sleep problems, but also at normalizing arousal and sleep reactivity.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Data were derived from a large cohort study investigating the natural history of insomnia in a population-based sample. Participants were chosen using a stratified probabilistic selection procedure based on the last Canadian census, combined with a random digit selection method and the Kish method to identify which household member was interviewed [15]. Inclusion criteria for the telephone interview were to be over 18 years of age and to speak French or English [16]. A total sample size of 3911 was acquired initially.
From this cohort, we selected baseline data for the age group between 25 and 55 years of age (n = 1693) in order to reduce the impact of potential confounding factors. Specifically, individuals in this age range may have a more regular lifestyle and day-and-night rhythm than in later life phases, and other age- and health-related problems disrupting sleep quality such as menopause or neurodegenerative diseases are not yet prevalent.

2.2. Measures

While DSM criteria are leading in the diagnosis of insomnia, the Insomnia Severity Index (ISI) [17] is a well-accepted measure that assesses severity of nocturnal symptoms and daytime impairments [17,18]. It is a seven-item self-administered questionnaire asking about severity of sleep difficulties (initial, middle, and late insomnia), satisfaction about actual sleep quality, perceived daytime impairments, as well as thoughts and worries about sleep. Responses to each item range from 0 (e.g., no difficulty) to 4 (e.g., very difficult). The ISI has adequate psychometric properties and is sensitive to measure treatment outcome. The version applied here—the French-Canadian version of the ISI—has good internal consistency, test–retest reliability (Cronbach’s alpha of 0.74), and convergent validity (r = 65 when comparing with sleep diary) [17]. Arousal was measured using the Arousal Predisposition Scale [4], which assesses through 12 items whether the respondent considers her/himself a stressful and emotionally reactive person. It is a self-report scale that inquires about typical behaviours and emotional responses to daytime events, either with or without an external trigger. Each item is scored on a scale from 1 (never) to 5 (always). The APS has good predictive validity and internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha of 0.83) [19]. Sleep reactivity was measured using the Ford Insomnia Response to Stress Test [6], which contains nine items assessing whether past and future stressful events are likely to affect sleep quality. It is a self-report scale that assesses the likelihood of having trouble sleeping either after a stressful or arousing event during the previous day or when anticipating this event for the following day. For each item, scores run from 0 (not likely) to 4 (very likely). The FIRST is a reliable measure with high internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha of 0.83) and good test–retest reliability (t-rr coefficient = 92) [6].

2.3. Analyses

Of the 1693 participants between 25 and 55 years of age, we first analysed how vulnerability to insomnia actually affected insomnia severity scores by comparing groups with high and low scores on the APS and FIRST. Groups were created based on a median split for both arousal and sleep reactivity scores, with low vulnerability defined as a score lower than median and high vulnerability as a score equal to or higher than the median. We first analysed how high and low arousal and sleep reactivity were related to insomnia severity separately. Based on the results, four groups were then created: a high insomnia vulnerability group (high vulnerability scores on both arousal and sleep reactivity scales), a low vulnerability group (low vulnerability scores on both arousal and sleep reactivity scales), and medium vulnerability groups (a high score on one and a low score on the other scale). For each of these groups, we calculated the mean and standard deviation (SD) of ISI scores. By performing the analyses in these stages, we could determine whether it was one of these vulnerability factors alone that would drive insomnia severity scores, or whether these factors combined had a much stronger effect on insomnia severity scores than either of these factors alone.
Next, we investigated the age effect on sleep reactivity and arousal. Baseline data were examined across three different age groups: 25–35 year olds, 35–45 year olds, and 45–55 year olds. In parallel and across age, the group was divided into three different sleep groups based on a previously described algorithm [20]: good sleepers, those with insomnia symptoms, and those with insomnia syndrome. The algorithm considers criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition (DSM-IV [21]) and International Classification of Diseases, 10th Edition [22]) as well as sleep-promoting medication use and scores on the ISI and Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index [23]. Responses from the Insomnia Severity Index [18] the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index [23], and from questions about the use of sleep medication were used to evaluate the presence or absence of each criterion. Participants in the group with an insomnia syndrome met all diagnostic criteria for insomnia. They were dissatisfied or very dissatisfied (score of 3 or 4 on a scale of 0–4) with their sleep patterns and had symptoms of initial, middle, or late insomnia at least three nights per week for at least 1 month. Substantial distress or daytime impairment related to sleep difficulties was also reported by those individuals (score of 3 or 4 on a scale of 0–4). Participants were also classified in the insomnia syndrome group if they used prescribed sleep-promoting medication at least three nights per week. Although not a formal criterion for insomnia diagnosis, use of sleep medication may mask the underlying symptoms. Participants classified in the group with insomnia symptoms reported initial, middle, or late insomnia at least three nights per week without fulfilling all diagnostic criteria for insomnia syndrome (i.e., they could report being satisfied with their sleep, not report distress or daytime consequences, or not meet the criterion of symptoms for at least 1 month required for a diagnosis of insomnia). This group also included individuals dissatisfied with their sleep but without symptoms of initial, middle, or late insomnia. Participants using prescribed sleep medication fewer than three nights per week or over-the-counter medication for sleep at least one night per week were classified in this group. Participants in the good sleepers group were satisfied with their sleep (i.e., score of 0–2 on a scale of 0–4) did not report symptoms of insomnia, and did not use prescribed or over-the-counter medication to promote sleep.
The group with insomnia syndrome fulfilled all criteria of insomnia diagnosis (n = 312), those with insomnia symptoms showed some but not all symptoms of insomnia (n = 450), and the group of good sleepers did not complain about their sleep nor did they take any sleep medication (n = 931). A multi-factorial ANOVA was conducted to compare the main effects of age group and sleep group and the interaction effect of both on APS and FIRST scores.

3. Results

3.1. Insomnia Severity in High and Low Vulnerability Groups

Medians for both arousal (APS, median = 30) and sleep reactivity (FIRST, median = 22) were first calculated. Defining high and low arousal and sleep reactivity groups based on these values is in line with previously published high arousal and high sleep reactivity values, and with those defined in the original test definitions [4,6]. Participants with high arousal scores (APS, n = 893) had significantly higher scores on the insomnia severity index (ISI: Mean = 10.29, SD = 5.81) than participants with low arousal scores (n = 800, Mean = 6.72, SD = 5.12) (F1, 1691 = 175.138, p < 0.001). Likewise, participants with high reactivity scores (n = 919) reported significantly higher insomnia severity (Mean = 10.57, SD = 5.68) than those with low sleep reactivity (n = 774; Mean = 6.27, SD = 5.05) (F1, 1691 = 267.523, p < 0.001).
These data supported our creation of a second level division in vulnerability groups, combining arousal and sleep reactivity scores as described in the methods. We created a group with high scores on both arousal and sleep reactivity (high vulnerability, n = 630). Medium vulnerability was defined as having high scores on one but low score on the other questionnaire, and so consisted of two groups: one group with high arousal and low sleep reactivity scores (n = 263), and one group with high sleep reactivity and low arousal scores (n = 289). Lastly, a low vulnerability group was created with low scores on both arousal and sleep reactivity measures (n = 511).
There was a significant main effect of group on ISI scores (F3, 1689 = 118.37, p < 0.001). Post hoc tests showed that most groups were significantly different from each other: the high vulnerability group reported more severe insomnia (Mean = 11.42, SD = 5.60) than the medium vulnerability-SR group (Mean = 8.73, SD = 5.40, p < 0.001, d = 0.49), the medium vulnerability-AR group (Mean = 7.57, SD = 5.40, p < 0.001, d = 0.70), and the low vulnerability group (Mean = 5.59, SD = 4.72, p < 0.001, d = 1.13). The medium vulnerability-SR group and medium vulnerability-AR group each also reported significantly more severe insomnia than the low vulnerability group; (p < 0.001, d = 0.62) and (p < 0.001, d = 0.39), respectively. Only the medium vulnerability groups did not differ significantly from each other (p = 0.052, d = 0.21) (see Figure 1).

3.2. Hyperarousal and Sleep Reactivity as a Function of Age and Insomnia Symptoms

Next, we divided the initial total sample (n = 1693) into three age groups: 25–35 year olds (n = 448), 35–45 year olds (n = 528), and 45–55 year olds (n = 717). In parallel, three sleep/insomnia groups were formed based on our previously defined algorithm: a group of good sleepers (n = 931), those with symptoms of insomnia (n = 450), and those with insomnia syndrome (n = 312).
There was no significant age × sleep status interaction effect on either sleep reactivity (F4, 1684 = 0.27, p = 0.900) or on arousal (F4, 1684 = 0.59, p = 0.673). Results of the multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA )for the sleep groups showed higher scores across each of the groups for both arousal (F2, 1684 = 68.340, p < 0.001) and sleep reactivity (F2, 1684 = 104.05, p < 0.001), confirming the results for insomnia vulnerability. Main effect of age showed that arousal scores are significantly lower with increasing age (F2, 1684 = 8.07, p < 0.001) but sleep reactivity scores are not (F2, 1684 = 0.45, p = 0.639).
Post-hoc comparisons using the Tukey’s honest significant difference (HSD) test revealed that arousal scores were significantly higher in the insomnia syndrome group (Mean = 33.69, SD = 7.14) compared to both the symptom group (Mean = 31.31, SD = 7.16, p < 0.001, d = −0.33) and to good sleepers (Mean = 28.60, SD = 6.61; p < 0.001, d = −0.74), while the insomnia symptom group also showed higher scores compared to the good sleepers (p < 0.001, d = −0.39). Significantly lower arousal scores were obtained among the 45–55 year olds (Mean = 29.78, SD = 7.27) compared to 25–35 year olds (Mean = 31.07, SD = 6.97; p = 0.005, d = 0.18), but not compared to 35–45 year olds (Mean = 30.22, SD = 7.07; p = 0.506, d = 0.06), nor did this last group differ significantly from the 25–35 year olds on arousal scores (p = 0.127, d = 0.12) (see Figure 2). For sleep reactivity, post-hoc comparisons using the Tukey HSD test revealed significantly higher scores in the insomnia syndrome group (Mean = 25.59, SD = 6.04) compared to both the symptom group (Mean = 23.72, SD = 6.06; p < 0.001, d = −0.31) and to good sleepers (Mean = 20.42, SD = 5.67; p < 0.001, d = −0.88), while the insomnia symptom group also showed higher scores compared to the good sleepers (p < 0.001, d = −0.56). Since a main effect of age was not found for sleep reactivity, post-hoc tests were not performed.

4. Discussion

The current study shows, in a large representative sample, that both arousal and sleep reactivity are strongly associated with sleep problems and that those with high scores on both measures particularly show high levels of insomnia severity. Furthermore, hyperarousal symptoms decrease as a function of age, but sleep reactivity remains similar across different age groups. How arousing events affect an individual during daytime decreases with age, but how stressful events disrupt sleep remains similar across different adult age groups.
Previous findings [24] have suggested that both arousal and sleep reactivity are relatively stable traits over shorter time periods (6 and 12 months follow-up). Though the current data are based on cross-sectional comparisons and thus limit conclusions about developmental changes over the lifespan, the data suggest that arousal but not sleep reactivity scores decrease in older adulthood. Findings are in line with a large study suggesting that the immediate stress response to an event decreases with age, but not delayed stress effects [13]. These delayed effects could—although not investigated—include effects on sleep.
Our findings have important implications for treatment adaptations for insomnia, which could be aimed at treating sleep problems but also at normalizing arousal and sleep reactivity. With stress reduction and improved emotion coping during daytime, it might be possible to reduce sleep-disrupting events during the night as well [14]. In fact, stress coping strategies are a mediating factor in the relation between stress exposure and insomnia development [25]. A new patient-based measure focusing not only on the likelihood but also on the actual frequency of sleep disrupting events might thus be warranted, in order to measure the effectiveness of such treatment. In fact, recent findings show that a reduction of insomnia symptoms through cognitive behavioural therapy is indeed associated with a reduction of sleep reactivity as measured by FIRST scores [26]. Sleep reactivity—as insomnia—is shown to be much more prevalent in women than in men, and is in turn associated with metacognitive beliefs about sleep in insomnia patients [27]. These findings emphasize the importance of reducing sleep reactivity through appropriate insomnia treatment. Longitudinal within-subject studies should further verify the hypothesis of trait and state within these different features of insomnia.
Despite insomnia being much more prevalent in older than in younger adults, the age-dependent decrease of arousal shown in the current study suggests that with increasing age, hyperarousal may play a less important role than other factors as a cause of insomnia. An interesting aim for future studies would be to map which factors are driving insomnia in older adults, improving the definition of insomnia phenotypes: reduced homeostatic drive, hormonal changes, changed diurnal rhythms due to changed lifestyles, and changes in sleep hygiene may all be factors underlying this higher insomnia prevalence in older adults instead of hyperarousal. The current study suggests that over a larger age span, the role that each of these factors play in the incidence and severity of insomnia varies with age for hyperarousal, but not for sleep reactivity.
The current study has a number of limitations: first of all, it is a cross-sectional and not a longitudinal study, so final conclusions on traits of hyperarousal and sleep reactivity await confirmation from such studies. Second, both insomnia diagnosis and reports of hyperarousal, sleep quality, and sleep reactivity were all assessed with subjective measures. This may also explain why the relative group size of those with insomnia syndrome is slightly higher than estimates from other prevalence studies, though numbers vary between studies [3,28,29]. Despite the limitations, the present findings give insight into factors playing a role in vulnerability to insomnia in different age groups, which may facilitate personalized treatment adapted to different age groups, aimed not just at treating sleep problems, but also at normalizing arousal and sleep reactivity, possibly preventing relapse of insomnia.

5. Conclusions

Our analyses from a large representative cohort study show that both arousal predisposition and sleep reactivity increase with more severe sleep problems. Comparing different age groups between 25 and 55 years of age, we show that arousal predisposition, but not sleep reactivity scores decrease with increasing age. How arousing events affect an individual during daytime may thus decrease with age, but how arousal disrupts sleep is equivalent across different adult age groups. The findings implicate that treatment of insomnia should possibly be adapted for these age groups, taking into consideration vulnerability factors such as hyperarousal and sleep reactivity.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a grant from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (MT42504). Ellemarije Altena and Yannick Daviaux are supported by LabEx BRAIN: LABEX BRAIN ANR-10-LABX-43.

Author Contributions

C.M.M. and H.I. conceived and designed the original study; E.A. designed the current study paper; C.M.M. and H.I. performed the experiments; E.A. analyzed the data; E.A. and C.M.M. wrote the paper; H.I., I.Y.C., Y.D., P.P. critically analyzed and contributed to the contents of the paper.

Conflicts of Interest

C.M.M. served as a consultant for Merck and received research funding from Novartis. All other authors declare no conflict of interest. The founding sponsors had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, and in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Ancoli-Israel, S.; Cooke, J.R. Prevalence and comorbidity of insomnia and effect on functioning in elderly populations. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2005, 53, 264–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Bonnet, M.H.; Arand, D.L. Hyperarousal and insomnia: State of the science. Sleep Med. Rev. 2010, 14, 9–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. LeBlanc, M.; Merette, C.; Savard, J.; Ivers, H.; Baillargeon, L.; Morin, C.M. Incidence and risk factors of insomnia in a population-based sample. Sleep 2009, 32, 1027–1037. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  4. Coren, S.; Mah, K.B. Prediction of physiological arousability: A validation of the arousal predisposition scale. Behav. Res. Ther. 1993, 31, 215–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Morin, C.M.; Rodrigue, S.; Ivers, H. Role of stress, arousal, and coping skills in primary insomnia. Psychosom. Med. 2003, 65, 259–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Drake, C.; Richardson, G.; Roehrs, T.; Scofield, H.; Roth, T. Vulnerability to stress-related sleep disturbance and hyperarousal. Sleep 2004, 27, 285–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Drake, C.L.; Pillai, V.; Roth, T. Stress and sleep reactivity: A prospective investigation of the stress-diathesis model of insomnia. Sleep 2014, 37, 1295–1304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Fernandez-Mendoza, J.; Vela-Bueno, A.; Vgontzas, A.N.; Ramos-Platon, M.J.; Olavarrieta-Bernardino, S.; Bixler, E.O.; De la Cruz-Troca, J.J. Cognitive-emotional hyperarousal as a premorbid characteristic of individuals vulnerable to insomnia. Psychosom. Med. 2010, 72, 397–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Kageyama, T.; Nishikido, N.; Kobayashi, T.; Kurokawa, Y.; Kaneko, T.; Kabuto, M. Self-reported sleep quality, job stress, and daytime autonomic activities assessed in terms of short-term heart rate variability among male white-collar workers. Ind. Health 1998, 36, 263–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. LeBlanc, M.; Beaulieu-Bonneau, S.; Merette, C.; Savard, J.; Ivers, H.; Morin, C.M. Psychological and health-related quality of life factors associated with insomnia in a population-based sample. J. Psychosom. Res. 2007, 63, 157–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Stawski, R.S.; Sliwinski, M.J.; Almeida, D.M.; Smyth, J.M. Reported exposure and emotional reactivity to daily stressors: The roles of adult age and global perceived stress. Psychol. Aging 2008, 23, 52–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Folkman, S.; Lazarus, R.S.; Pimley, S.; Novacek, J. Age differences in stress and coping processes. Psychol. Aging 1987, 2, 171–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Scott, S.B.; Sliwinski, M.J.; Blanchard-Fields, F. Age differences in emotional responses to daily stress: The role of timing, severity, and global perceived stress. Psychol. Aging 2013, 28, 1076–1087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Altena, E.; Micoulaud-Franchi, J.A.; Geoffroy, P.A.; Sanz-Arigita, E.; Bioulac, S.; Philip, P. The bidirectional relation between emotional reactivity and sleep: From disruption to recovery. Behav. Neurosci. 2016, 130, 336–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Kish, L. Survey Sampling; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1965. [Google Scholar]
  16. Morin, C.M.; LeBlanc, M.; Daley, M.; Gregoire, J.P.; Merette, C. Epidemiology of insomnia: Prevalence, self-help treatments, consultations, and determinants of help-seeking behaviors. Sleep Med. 2006, 7, 123–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Bastien, C.H.; Vallieres, A.; Morin, C.M. Validation of the insomnia severity index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Med. 2001, 2, 297–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Morin, C.M.; Belleville, G.; Belanger, L.; Ivers, H. The insomnia severity index: Psychometric indicators to detect insomnia cases and evaluate treatment response. Sleep 2011, 34, 601–608. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  19. Coren, S. Prediction of insomnia from arousability predisposition scores: Scale development and cross-validation. Behav. Res. Ther. 1988, 26, 415–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Daley, M.; Morin, C.M.; LeBlanc, M.; Gregoire, J.P.; Savard, J. The economic burden of insomnia: Direct and indirect costs for individuals with insomnia syndrome, insomnia symptoms, and good sleepers. Sleep 2009, 32, 55–64. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  21. Association, A.P. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, WA, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  22. Classification of Diseases. Available online: http://www.who.int/classifications/icd/en/ (accessed on 4 November 2016).
  23. Buysse, D.J.; Reynolds, C.F., 3rd; Monk, T.H.; Berman, S.R.; Kupfer, D.J. The pittsburgh sleep quality index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989, 28, 193–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Jarrin, D.C.; Chen, I.Y.; Ivers, H.; Drake, C.L.; Morin, C.M. Temporal stability of the ford insomnia response to stress test (first). J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2016, 12, 1373–1378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Pillai, V.; Roth, T.; Mullins, H.M.; Drake, C.L. Moderators and mediators of the relationship between stress and insomnia: Stressor chronicity, cognitive intrusion, and coping. Sleep 2014, 37, 1199–1208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Okajima, I.; Nakajima, S.; Ochi, M.; Inoue, Y. Association among changes in sleep-related beliefs, sleep reactivity, and improvement of insomnia following cognitive behavioral therapy. Sleep Med. 2017, 29, 96–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Palagini, L.; Bruno, R.M.; Paolo, T.; Caccavale, L.; Gronchi, A.; Mauri, M.; Riemann, D.; Drake, C.L. Association between stress-related sleep reactivity and metacognitive beliefs about sleep in insomnia disorder: Preliminary results. Behav. Sleep Med. 2016, 14, 636–649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Leger, D.; Guilleminault, C.; Dreyfus, J.P.; Delahaye, C.; Paillard, M. Prevalence of insomnia in a survey of 12,778 adults in france. J. Sleep Res. 2000, 9, 35–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Pandey, S.; Phillips, B.A. Why is the prevalence of insomnia skyrocketing? And what can be done about it? Sleep Med. 2015, 16, 555–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Insomnia severity scores vary depending on low, medium, or high insomnia vulnerability. Those with low vulnerability for insomnia (scoring low on both arousal and sleep reactivity) have significantly lower scores on insomnia severity than those with medium (scoring high on only one scale) or high vulnerability (scoring high on both arousal and sleep reactivity). Groups were defined based on a medium split of arousal scores (arousal predisposition scale, APS) and sleep reactivity scores (Ford Insomnia Response to Stress Test, FIRST); error bars represent standard error of the mean. Asterisks (*) indicate a significance level of p < 0.001. See text for details.
Figure 1. Insomnia severity scores vary depending on low, medium, or high insomnia vulnerability. Those with low vulnerability for insomnia (scoring low on both arousal and sleep reactivity) have significantly lower scores on insomnia severity than those with medium (scoring high on only one scale) or high vulnerability (scoring high on both arousal and sleep reactivity). Groups were defined based on a medium split of arousal scores (arousal predisposition scale, APS) and sleep reactivity scores (Ford Insomnia Response to Stress Test, FIRST); error bars represent standard error of the mean. Asterisks (*) indicate a significance level of p < 0.001. See text for details.
Brainsci 07 00041 g001
Figure 2. Arousal and sleep reactivity scores increase as a function of the presence and severity of sleep problems. Next to an overall significant main effect of group, post-hoc tests show significant differences between good sleepers (CON), those with insomnia symptoms (INS symp), and those with insomnia syndrome (INS synd) on both arousal and sleep reactivity scores. Arousal scores decrease with age: an overall main effect of age is supported by post-hoc test results of a significant difference between the younger (25–35 year olds) and older adult age group (45–55 year olds), although none of the other group comparisons are significantly different. Sleep reactivity scores are similar between the different age groups: there is no significant main effect nor are scores of any of the age groups significantly different from each other. Asterisks (*) indicate a significance level of p ≤ 0.005. See text for details.
Figure 2. Arousal and sleep reactivity scores increase as a function of the presence and severity of sleep problems. Next to an overall significant main effect of group, post-hoc tests show significant differences between good sleepers (CON), those with insomnia symptoms (INS symp), and those with insomnia syndrome (INS synd) on both arousal and sleep reactivity scores. Arousal scores decrease with age: an overall main effect of age is supported by post-hoc test results of a significant difference between the younger (25–35 year olds) and older adult age group (45–55 year olds), although none of the other group comparisons are significantly different. Sleep reactivity scores are similar between the different age groups: there is no significant main effect nor are scores of any of the age groups significantly different from each other. Asterisks (*) indicate a significance level of p ≤ 0.005. See text for details.
Brainsci 07 00041 g002

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Altena, E.; Chen, I.Y.; Daviaux, Y.; Ivers, H.; Philip, P.; Morin, C.M. How Hyperarousal and Sleep Reactivity Are Represented in Different Adult Age Groups: Results from a Large Cohort Study on Insomnia. Brain Sci. 2017, 7, 41. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci7040041

AMA Style

Altena E, Chen IY, Daviaux Y, Ivers H, Philip P, Morin CM. How Hyperarousal and Sleep Reactivity Are Represented in Different Adult Age Groups: Results from a Large Cohort Study on Insomnia. Brain Sciences. 2017; 7(4):41. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci7040041

Chicago/Turabian Style

Altena, Ellemarije, Ivy Y. Chen, Yannick Daviaux, Hans Ivers, Pierre Philip, and Charles M. Morin. 2017. "How Hyperarousal and Sleep Reactivity Are Represented in Different Adult Age Groups: Results from a Large Cohort Study on Insomnia" Brain Sciences 7, no. 4: 41. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci7040041

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop