Next Article in Journal
Prenatal Isolated Ventricular Septal Defect May Not Be Associated with Trisomy 21
Next Article in Special Issue
Alterations in Skin Temperature and Sleep in the Fear of Harm Phenotype of Pediatric Bipolar Disorder
Previous Article in Journal
Beyond Trisomy 21: Additional Chromosomal Anomalies Detected through Routine Aneuploidy Screening
Previous Article in Special Issue
Biologism in Psychiatry: A Young Man’s Experience of Being Diagnosed with “Pediatric Bipolar Disorder”
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Cortical Volume Alterations in Conduct Disordered Adolescents with and without Bipolar Disorder

1
Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, The University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio, San Antonio, 7703 Floyd Curl Drive, TX 78229, USA
2
Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center, Institute of Living, Hartford, CT 06114, USA
3
Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT 06510, USA
4
Department of Psychology, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA
5
Department of Psychiatry, The University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, Houston, TX 77030, USA
6
Oxford University Centre for Functional MRI of the Brain, Oxford OX1 2JD, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Clin. Med. 2014, 3(2), 416-431; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm3020416
Submission received: 24 December 2013 / Revised: 28 February 2014 / Accepted: 3 March 2014 / Published: 16 April 2014
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Bipolar Disorder in Children and Adolescents)

Abstract

:
Background: There is increasing evidence that bipolar disorder (BD) and conduct disorder (CD) are co-occurring disorders. Magnetic resonance imaging has revealed differences in the structure and function of the frontal cortex in these disorders when studied separately; however, the impact of BD comorbidity on brain structure in adolescents with CD has not yet been examined. Method: We conducted an optimized voxel based morphometry (VBM) study of juvenile offenders with the following diagnoses: conduct disorder with comorbid bipolar disorder (CD-BD; n = 24), conduct disorder without bipolar disorder (CD; n = 24) and healthy controls (HC, n = 24). Participants were 13–17 years of age, in a residential treatment facility for repeat offenders. The three groups in this study were similar in age, gender, socioeconomic status and ethnicity. Results: We found CD-BD subjects had decreased volume relative to controls at the voxel level in the right medial prefrontal cortex (PFC). Using a Threshold-Free Cluster Enhancement (TFCE) technique, the CD-BD subjects had significantly decreased volumes of the right medial prefrontal cortex and portions of the superior and inferior frontal gyrus, anterior cingulate and temporal gyrus. The CD subjects did not have differences in brain volume compared to control subjects or CD-BD subjects. Conclusions: Our findings suggest the comorbidity between CD and BD is associated with neurobiological impact namely volumetric differences from healthy controls. Furthermore subjects with this comorbidity had poorer lifetime functioning, more mood and attentional dysfunction, and more medication exposure than subjects with CD who were not BD.

Graphical Abstract

1. Introduction

Despite evidence that antisocial adults have decreased volumes in the frontal regions [1,2,3,4] temporal lobes [3,5] and the insula [4], only a handful of neuroanatomical studies of conduct disorder (CD), a precursor of antisocial personality disorder have been done. These studies note a variety of neuroanatomic differences in CD children and adolescents compared to healthy adolescents, including reduced temporal lobe gray matter, a trend for reduction in the prefrontal cortex volume (PFC) [6], smaller posterior cerebellar volumes [7], reduced volume in the anterior insular cortex bilaterally and the left amygdala [8] and reduced volume of the left orbitofrontal region and bilateral medial temporal lobes (including the left amygdala and hippocampus) [9]. Reduced cortical thickness in parietal, temporal, and supramarginal regions along with gyral folding differences in frontal regions have also been reported [10]. These studies suggest both frontal and temporal lobe involvement in CD, and are of interest given the role of these networks in the modulation of emotion [11,12], autonomic arousal [13] and moral decisions [14]. The assortment of findings, however, suggests heterogeneity in the CD phenotype.
A potential source of heterogeneity in CD adolescents may be co-occurring conditions. Although the co-occurrence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [15] and substance abuse [16] is well documented in CD youth, there is growing appreciation of the co-existence of mood disorders in this population, ranging from 35% to 78% in community and clinical setting respectively [17]. Similarly in studies of incarcerated youth, there is extensive evidence of comorbid mood disorders, manic symptoms and or bipolar disorder (BD) [18,19,20,21]. Bipolar disorder may portend a particularly bad outcome in incarcerated populations with a significantly higher risk of multiple incarcerations [22].
Supporting evidence of the co-occurrence of BD and CD also comes from studies that focus on BD samples. These studies find high rates of comorbid externalizing disorders (CD, Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD) and ADHD) and/or delinquent behaviors [23,24,25,26]. Compared to adults with BD, children with BD may be a distinct category, with a high familial loading and a complex clinical presentation [27]. A review of an extensive body of work examining the phenomenology of CD-BD subjects finds that the family loading in subjects with CD and comorbid BD is high for both BD and antisocial behavior [28]. Early onset BD patients are found to have committed more crimes [29], have more CD symptoms, greater behavior problems and higher lifetime rates of substance abuse disorders relative to adult onset BD patients [30]. The presence of comorbid CD has been found to worsen the overall clinical course of BD [31], with poor functional outcomes and high rates of comorbidity with both internalizing and externalizing disorders [28].
Although the co-occurrence of BD and CD is well recognized, little is known about the neural systems underlying this phenotype. In an earlier study we demonstrated neuropsychological deficits in adolescents with CD; however, the most robust neuropsychological deficits were accounted for by the presence of comorbid BD [32]. In this study we explore the effects of comorbid BD on cortical volumes in CD adolescents with and without BD relative to each other and a healthy control group. In subjects with conduct disorder and BD (CD-BD) we expect to see neuroanatomic findings consistent with BD such as decreased PFC volume compared to healthy control subjects. Based on the limited existing data, we anticipate decreased volume in temporal lobes and orbito-medial PFC of CD subjects compared to controls. This is the first study to directly compare CD subjects with and without BD.

2. Methods

2.1. Subjects

All patients were assessed at a secure, post adjudication, residential treatment center where they arrived after a 4–8 week stay at the county juvenile detention center. Healthy controls (HC) were assessed at the outpatient clinical office of the principal investigator. Subjects were males and females between the ages of 13 and 17 years. To confirm diagnoses, all subjects underwent a semi-structured interview with the parent and child separately, then together to reconcile any discrepancies, administered by a board certified child and adolescent psychiatrist (Rene L. Olvera), with the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Present and Lifetime version (K-SAD-Pl) [33]. The interviewer showed 100% agreement on five audio taped cases with a senior child and adolescent psychiatrist (Steven R. Pliszka). The following groups were studied: Conduct disorder with comorbid bipolar disorder (CD-BD), conduct disorder without bipolar disorder (CD) and healthy controls (HC) (see Table 1). HC from the community, were similar to the patient subjects in age, sex, ethnicity and socioeconomic status, but did not have a current or past psychiatric disorder. Additional scales conducted at the time of the interview included: The Hollingshead Four Factor Index of Social Status [34], the Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS) [35], the Children’s Depression Rating Scale, Revised (CDRS-R) [36], and the Personal Experience Screening Questionnaire (PESQ, a self-report measure about alcohol and substance abuse) [37]. The Intelligence Quotient (IQ) was measured using the Differential Abilities Scales [38]. The Children’s Global Assessment Scales (C-GAS) was used to estimate all the subjects level of functioning [39]. According to the K-SADS instructions, for subjects with a current or past history of psychiatric illness, three C-GAS scores were assigned: A current C-GAS score, a past C-GAS score which estimates the level of functioning during the most severe prior episode of illness and a score which estimates the child’s mean highest past level of functioning. For control subjects only the current C-GAS is used.
Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics.
Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics.
FactorsCD-BD (n = 24)CD (n = 24)Healthy Controls (n = 24)
Mean Age years (sd)15.83 (1.05)16.23 (1.05)15.3 (1.14)
Male Gender (%)16 (66)21 (84)16 (66)
Ethnicity
Non-Hispanic White (%)5 (21)3 (12)3 (12)
Hispanic (%)13 (54)21(87)20 (83)
African American (%)4 (17)01 (4)
Other Race/Ethnic (%)2 (8)00
Hollingshead Socioeconomic Status34.31 (10.75)34.48 (10.08)37.04 (8.52)
IQ91.90 (15.45)97.75 (9.62)98.55 (10.74)
Mean YMRS (sd)6.00 (3.94) a2.60 (2.81) b0
Mean CDRS (sd)25.62 (11.39) a22.71 (7.12) a17.14 (0.47) b
Mean PESQ (sd)39.48 (15.49) a41.83 (11.85) a18.91 (1.61) b
Mean Current C-GAS63.76 (7.60) a62.54 (7.31) a85.56 (3.31) b
Mean Past C-GAS30.00 (10.66) a43.40 (11.06) b
Mean Highest Past C-GAS65.32 (6.58)63.61 (8.26)
Lifetime Comorbid Condition
Alcohol Abuse or Dependence (%)7 (29)6 (24)0
Cannabis Abuse or Dependence (%)17 (71)20 (83)0
Early Onset CD (%)10 (42)6 (24)0
ODD22 (96%)19 (76%)0
PTSD (%)6 (25)6 (25)0
GAD (%)6 (25)3 (12)0
ADHD (%)18 (75) a11 (44) b0
MDD (%)15 (63) a4 (16) b0
Medications
Lithium6 (25) a0 b0
Valproic Acid12 (50) a1 (4) b0
Atypical Antipsychotic20 (83) a3 (12) b0
Stimulants14 (58)9 (36)0
a,b Different superscripts denote significant difference between groups (p < 0.05).
A parent or legal guardian provided signed informed consent, and signed assent was obtained from each adolescent. Subjects were required to be between 13 and 17 years old, with no serious medical problems, and to meet the standard safety criteria for an MRI scan. Exclusion criteria for all subjects included a lifetime diagnosis of pervasive developmental disorder, mental retardation (IQ < 70), pregnancy or head injury with loss of consciousness. The institutional review board approved this study with a prisoner advocate included in the formal review. For ethical and safety reasons (subjects were transported to the University Research Imaging Center for the MRI portion) we did not withhold treatment from any subject and ensured that our subjects were clinically stable (YMRS < 12 and CDRS < 40) at the time of IQ testing and MRI.

2.2. Imaging Methods

All MRI images were collected on a 3T Siemens Scanner (Siemens, Malvern, PA, USA), with a T1-weighted gradient echo sequence (3D T1-GRE, Siemens, Malvern, PA, USA), repetition time (TR) of 20 ms, echo time (TE) of 5.15 ms, slice thickness of 1.0 mm, number of excitations (NEX) of 2, field of view (FOV) of 256 × (160, 176, 192), Base Resolution 256 mm × 100%, and pixel size 1 × 1 × 1 mm.

2.3. Pre-Processing

The analysis was conducted using a voxel-based morphometry approach (VBM) [40,41,42,43]. The image processing was done using the Oxford Centre for functional magnetic resonance imaging of the brain’s (FMRIB) Software Library (FSL, FMRIB, Oxford, UK), using the following processing steps: We corrected the intensity bias due to field inhomogeneities [44]; removed the non-brain tissue [45], and segmented the brain into gray matter (GM), white matter and cerebrospinal fluid [44]. The GM partition was linearly aligned to the Montreal Neurological Institute template [46] and these images were averaged producing a first-pass, affine-only (linear), study-specific template. The GM images were then non-linearly aligned (warped) to the first-pass template [47] and averaged to produce the study-specific template. Lastly the GM images were non-linearly aligned to the study-specific template [47] where they were corrected for expansions and shrinkages of GM using the local Jacobian determinants of the warps [43] and smoothed with a Gaussian filter with 4.0 mm of standard deviation (equivalent to a FHWM of 9.4 mm). After smoothing, the images were concatenated in a four-dimensional array.

2.4. Analyses

For IQ, socio-economic status (SES) and continuous clinical variables, we used an ANOVA with Bonferroni adjustment to compare our three groups. By definition the HC did not have any psychiatric diagnosis nor receive medication, therefore, we limited our Chi square analyses of these categories to our two clinical groups (CD-BD vs. CD).
The imaging statistical analysis was performed using a general linear model with the design matrix containing the three groups as predictors, with age and sex as covariates. Voxel inferences were done using permutation methods [48,49]. As permutation tests do not rely on known, a priori distributions, an empirical distribution under the null hypothesis was built by randomly permuting the group assignments for the subjects many times, refitting the model and accumulating the observed statistic for each realization. Later the statistic obtained from the correct model was compared with the distribution of the statistic after 10,000 permutations. Voxel-level p-values corrected for family-wise error rate (FWE) at 0.05 were produced by computing, at each random realization of the model, the maximum test statistic observed across the image. In addition to these models for inference, we used a new technique called Threshold-Free Cluster Enhancement (TFCE) to increase sensitivity to regions of signal that are spatially extended [45]. In TFCE, the value of the statistic at each voxel is replaced by a composition of the statistic observed in that voxel and those neighbors that follow certain spatial properties. In TFCE, both intensity and signal extent are considered. A p-value can be computed by randomization, using standard permutation testing while maintaining strong family-wise error control. The areas of significant differences had their center of mass computed in MNI coordinates, then were converted to Talairach coordinates, and finally were named using the Talairach Client (UTHSCSA Research Imaging Institute, San Antonio, TX, USA).

3. Results

The three groups in this study were similar in IQ and demographic variables. Although we required all subjects to be clinically stable, the groups differed by YMRS scores, as CD-BD subjects had a higher score on the YMRS compared to the CD group (t = 5.92, df46, p < 0.001). On the Children’s Global Assessment Scales the control group had significantly higher current C-GAS compared to the CD-BD and CD (F2,67 = 90.88, p < 0.001), but the CD-BD and CD groups did not differ from each other. Comparing just the two clinical groups revealed the most severe past C-GAS was significantly lower for the CD-BD group compared to the CD only group (t = 4.27, df46, p < 0.001). Of note the CD-BD and CD groups were similar on self-reported measures of depression and substance abuse but the CD-BD group was more likely to have a lifetime history of ADHD (χ2 = 10.08, df = 1, p < 0.001), Major Depressive episodes (χ2 = 6.59, df = 1, p = 0.009) and a trend for Oppositional Defiant Disorder (χ2 = 3.71, df = 1, p = 0.054) compared to the CD group. The CD-BD group was also more likely to be on mood stabilizers and atypical antipsychotic medications (see Table 1). Our CD-BD and CD groups differed from healthy controls on all clinical measures but were not significantly different in terms of IQ or SES.
CD/Bipolar Disorder (CD-BD) vs. Controls: At the voxel level, subjects with CD-BD had lower volume in the right medial frontal gyrus, Brodmann area (BA) 10 compared to controls (Table 2). Using TFCE CD-BD subjects had decreased volumes compared to controls in the right medial frontal gyrus (BA 9) right superior frontal gyrus (BA 10), right inferior frontal gyrus (BA 47), right anterior cingulate gyrus (BA 25), right middle frontal gyrus (BA 46), right middle temporal gyrus (BA 37) and the right inferior frontal gyrus (BA 47). (Table 2; Figure 1).
Conduct Disorder (CD) vs. Controls: Significant volumetric differences were not found after correction for multiple testing, either at the voxel level or using TFCE between subjects with CD and controls.
CD-BD vs. CD: Significant volumetric differences were not found after correction for multiple testing, either at the voxel level or using TFCE between subjects with CD-BD and CD.
Table 2. Volumetric difference between groups.
Table 2. Volumetric difference between groups.
Cluster NumberSizeLowest pXYZStructureBrodmann Area
CD-BD Compared to HC: Voxel Level
1230.00210.251.88R Medial Frontal Gyrus10
Threshold-Free Cluster Enhancement
732730.00515.249.426.6R Medial Frontal Gyrus9
64490.02723.260.8−6.8R Superior Frontal Gyrus10
51350.03145.223.4−2.6R Inferior Frontal Gyrus, B47
4730.046914−14.4R Anterior Cingulate25
3260.04449.821.421.6R Middle Frontal Gyrus46
2110.04749−65.69.8R Middle Temporal Gyrus37
110.054224−20R Inferior Frontal Gyrus, B47
Figure 1. TFCE volumetric differences between CD-BD and HC. 1 − p: Range from 0.05 (red) to 0.0001 (yellow) corrected for multiple comparisons.
Figure 1. TFCE volumetric differences between CD-BD and HC. 1 − p: Range from 0.05 (red) to 0.0001 (yellow) corrected for multiple comparisons.
Jcm 03 00416 g001

4. Discussion

Our study is the first imaging study to compare CD adolescents with and without BD. Although the CD-BD subjects had a higher score on the YMRS compared to the CD group they were still in the clinically stable range, and these groups did not differ on self-reported measures of depression and substance abuse. However on the C-GAS the CD-BD and the CD groups were still significantly lower than healthy control with scores suggesting continued moderate difficulty. Furthermore the CD-BD subjects had significantly lower C-GAS scores at “the most severe past episode” compared to subjects with just CD. The pattern of lifetime comorbid conditions (ADHD 75%, MDD 63%, and ODD 96%) are also consistent with prior studies of the BD subjects [24,26], and CD-BD subjects [28]. Our study is also consistent with studies of CD subjects where these conditions were common but less frequent (ADHD 44%, MDD 16%, and ODD 75%) than in BD subjects [18,28,50] (see Table 1). This clinical presentation with our CD group where they had definite impairment but consistently seemed to be “less severe” than the CD-BD group may account for our inability see volumetric difference between the CD subjects and our other groups (CD-BD and healthy controls) (see Table 1). As noted in the introduction there has not been a consistent anatomical finding in CD subjects. Furthermore we utilized a secure residential treatment setting as this allowed for medical stabilization, medication compliance, denied access to street drugs and allowed for routine exercise, sleep times, mandatory school attendance and access to weekly individual, family and group therapy. This raises the issue of the potential biological effects associated with treatment and a healthy environment. Whether anatomical findings in CD are a state or trait effect can only be answered in a well-controlled longitudinal protocol.
For the CD-BD subjects we found significant differences compared to healthy adolescent controls; specifically, CD-BD subjects had right hemispheric decreased volume in the anterior frontal lobe including BA 9, BA 10, BA 47 and BA 46 as well as the superior temporal gyrus (BA 39). Our volumetric findings in areas BA 9 and BA 10 of our CD-BD subjects include the medial and inferior frontal gyrus and portions of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). These regions are involved in higher integrative cognitive functions [51], as well as emotional response modulation [52,53]. Our findings are consistent with volumetric differences reported in BD adolescents without comorbid CD relative to control subjects [54,55,56,57,58,59]. Decreased volume in these PFC brain regions of BD subjects may represent disruption in higher level or top down networks, with the manifestation of poor emotional regulation [60,61,62] which is consistent with a fronto-limbic hypothesis of bipolar disorder [63,64,65]. Our finding in the ventral anterior cingulate (BA 25) the affective division of the anterior cingulate is consistent with dorsal system that is important in the regulation of affect [12]. However our findings need to be interpreted with caution as decreased PFC volumes are not specific to a single diagnosis, and can result in a number of problematic behaviors in the areas of planning and impulse control [14,66]. Given that our subjects had CD and BD as well as other externalizing disorders we cannot disentangle the exact cause for these differences. The most consistent biological finding in CD adolescents and antisocial adults is decreased autonomic responsiveness [67,68,69]. Decreased autonomic reactivity has been associated with right hemispheric, and prefrontal deficits [70,71,72] and decreased functional connectivity between the amygdala and the ventromedial PFC in boys with callous-unemotional traits, many of whom had either ODD or CD, relative to healthy controls [73].
In the temporo-parietal region we found lateralized differences relative to healthy controls where the CD-BD subjects had decreased volume in the right superior temporal gyrus (BA 39). These areas have been associated with cognitive operations such as episodic memory and visual spatial and facial perception and tend to show a lateralized pattern: With verbal stimuli being left-lateralized whereas facial and spatial encoding are right lateralized [74]. The right superior temporal gyrus is of interest as it is involved in the processing of emotional faces [52,75]. Temporo-parietal regions are also responsive to the attribution of mental states in others [76,77] with some evidence that the right side may particularly sensitive to this task [78]. The left temporo-parietal junction may be of interest in subjects with CD as this area is sensitive to paradigms that test empathic understanding [77].
Important limitations of our study include the cross sectional nature, lack of a BD-only group, and the clinical presentation of our sample. However incarcerated youth represent a particularly challenging group [18,21] therefore finding a group with only single diagnosis would have been highly uncharacteristic of this population. Similar to our study, in the largest CD-BD samples described [28] premorbid ADHD and ODD were present in most CD-BD children. Although we also included a clinical comparison group we did not have the capacity to recruit a sample of sufficient size to examine the effects of comorbid conditions. Although are findings are consistent with non-incarcerated patients with BD, we cannot wholly attribute our findings solely to the existence BD. It is also important to consider the limitation of categorical diagnoses that are given only when a syndrome, i.e., criminal behavior or manic episode are present. However given the overlap with ODD and ADHD, even relying on dimensional measures, would have been a challenge. In most BD children “mood instability” (irritability, temper outbursts) or other possible manic symptoms (increased activity and poor concentration) are first attributed to the more common conditions of childhood namely ADHD and ODD. Future work with longitudinal samples prior to the age of illness will be necessary to better understand the contribution of individual symptoms and or disorders. Given the complexity of behavior and symptom overlap a larger sample and the use of multiple biological measures may be also prove productive [79,80].
In addition to high rates of comorbidity, our CD-BD sample had extensive medication exposure. Given the level of impairment associated with these disorders, medication management is usually required. Our study was conducted in a correctional treatment setting where symptomatic patients run the risk of extending their length of stay, or accruing new charges (i.e., for assaultive behavior). We therefore felt it was unethical to interrupt their clinical care in any way. Without any unmedicated CD-BD subjects, we could not examine the effects of medication, however our rates of medication exposure are similar to other volumetric studies [81,82,83,84]. Using VBM, Dickstein et al. [56] found reduced GM volume in the left DLPFC in BD subjects who were similar to our cohort as they were euthymic, medicated, and 60% were comorbid for ADHD [56]. In many of these studies, secondary analyses failed to find a difference based on medication exposure; however, most had limited power to do so, therefore negative findings need to be interpreted with caution [82,83,85]. A recent meta-analyses found that the overall effects of medications are either in the direction of increased volume (in particular Lithium) or of no effect [86], which suggest we may have increased the risk of a Type II error by requiring our subjects to be medically stable. Further research is needed to disentangle whether the prefrontal cortical reductions in adolescent BD are independent of mood state, medication status and comorbid conditions.
The use of VBM entails certain strengths and limitations. VBM is a procedure that involves spatially normalizing high-resolution neuroanatomic images into a common stereotactic space. A voxel-wise comparison of the local concentration probability of gray or white matter is conducted and based on these measures, group differences are determined [42,87]. The automated nature of VBM differs from the manual tracing method used in volumetric studies as multiple regions can be examined in a less labor intensive manner and has a reduced potential for human error [87]. Lastly although volumetric studies often find differences relative healthy controls, volumetric differences between psychiatric disorders have not been established [55,88,89]. The inability to distinguish between disorders is often attributed to heterogeneity within disorders and symptom overlap between disorders [90] as was an issue in this study. And as noted earlier this task may be particularly difficult in child and adolescents psychiatry given the degree that behaviors such impulsivity, irritability, distractibility are seen in many conditions [26,89,91]. Of note recent imaging modalities such as Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI) [92] and functional MRI (fMRI) [89] have shown promise for such discrimination.

5. Conclusions

In summary, we found distinct patterns of cortical alterations in CD-BD subjects compared to healthy controls. These CD-BD subjects had decreased volumes in brain areas associated with impulse control and mood. Furthermore CD-BD subjects had poorer lifetime functioning, more mood and attentional dysfunction, and more medication exposure than subjects with CD who were not BD. As all of our CD-BD patients met the threshold of having full DSM-IV criteria for BD type I, our findings suggests the neurological underpinnings of bipolar disorder are consistent with other BD samples despite differences in patient settings and comorbid conditions.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by NIMH grant K23-MH068280.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Raine, A.; Lenz, T.; Bihrle, S.; LaCasse, L.; Colletti, P. Reduced prefrontal grey matter volume and reduced autonomic activity in antisocial personality disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2000, 57, 119–127. [Google Scholar]
  2. Yang, Y.; Raine, A.; Lencz, T.; Bihrle, S.; LaCasse, L.; Colletti, P. Volume reduction in prefrontal gray matter in unsuccessful criminal psychopaths. Biol. Psychiatry 2005, 57, 1103–1108. [Google Scholar]
  3. Oliveira-Souza, R.; Hare, R.D.; Bramati, I.E.; Garrido, G.J.; Azevedo, I.F.; Tovar-Moll, F.; Moll, J. Psychopathy as a disorder of the moral brain: Fronto-temporo-limbic grey matter reductions demonstrated by voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage 2008, 40, 1202–1213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Tiihonen, J.; Rossi, R.; Laakso, M.P.; Hodgins, S.; Testa, C.; Perez, J.; Repo-Tiihonen, E.; Vaurio, O.; Soininen, H.; Aronen, H.J.; et al. Brain anatomy of persistent violent offenders: More rather than less. Psychiatry Res. 2008, 163, 201–212. [Google Scholar]
  5. Dolan, M.C.; Deakin, J.F.; Roberts, N.; Anderson, I.M. Quantitative frontal and temporal structural MRI studies in personality-disordered offenders and control subjects. Psychiatry Res. 2002, 116, 133–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Kruesi, M.J.; Casanova, M.F.; Mannheim, G.; Johnson-Bilder, A. Reduced temporal lobe volume in early onset conduct disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2004, 132, 1–11. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bussing, R.; Grudnik, J.; Mason, D.; Wasiak, M.; Leonard, C. ADHD and conduct disorder: An MRI study in a community sample. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 2002, 3, 216–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Sterzer, P.; Stadler, C.; Poustka, F.; Kleinschmidt, A. A structural neural deficit in adolescents with conduct disorder and its association with lack of empathy. Neuroimage 2007, 37, 335–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Huebner, T.; Vloet, T.D.; Marx, I.; Konrad, K.; Fink, G.R.; Herpertz, S.C.; Herpertz-Dahlmann, B. Morphometric brain abnormalities in boys with conduct disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2008, 47, 540–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Hyatt, C.J.; Haney-Caron, E.; Stevens, M.C. Cortical thickness and folding deficits in conduct-disordered adolescents. Biol. Psychiatry 2012, 72, 207–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Hariri, A.R.; Bookheimer, S.Y.; Mazziotta, J.C. Modulating emotional responses: Effects of a neocortical network on the limbic system. Neuroreport 2000, 11, 43–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Phillips, M.L.; Drevets, W.C.; Rauch, S.L.; Lane, R. Neurobiology of emotion perception I: The neural basis of normal emotion perception. Biol. Psychiatry 2003, 54, 504–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Bechara, A.; Damasio, H.; Damasio, A.R.; Lee, G.P. Different contributions of the human amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making. J. Neurosci. 1999, 19, 5473–5481. [Google Scholar]
  14. Anderson, S.W.; Bechara, A.; Damasio, H.; Tranel, D.; Damasio, A.R. Impairment of social and moral behavior related to early damage in human prefrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 1999, 2, 1032–1037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Connor, D.F.; Steeber, J.; McBurnett, K. A review of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder complicated by symptoms of oppositional defiant disorder or conduct disorder. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2010, 31, 427–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Aarons, G.A.; Brown, S.A.; Hough, R.L.; Garland, A.F.; Wood, P.A. Prevalence of adolescent substance use disorders across five sectors of care. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2001, 40, 419–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Polier, G.G.; Vloet, T.D.; Herpertz-Dahlmann, B.; Laurens, K.R.; Hodgins, S. Comorbidity of conduct disorder symptoms and internalising problems in children: Investigating a community and a clinical sample. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2012, 21, 31–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Abram, K.M.; Teplin, L.A.; McClelland, G.M.; Dulcan, M.K. Comorbid psychiatric disorders in youth in juvenile detention. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2003, 60, 1097–1108. [Google Scholar]
  19. Garland, A.F.; Hough, R.L.; McCabe, K.M.; Yeh, M.; Wood, P.A.; Aarons, G.A. Prevalence of psychiatric disorders in youths across five sectors of care. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2001, 40, 409–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Wasserman, G.A.; McReynolds, L.S.; Lucas, C.P.; Fisher, P.; Santos, L. The voice disc-IV with incarcerated male youths: Prevalence of disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2002, 41, 314–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Harzke, A.J.; Baillargeon, J.; Baillargeon, G.; Henry, J.; Olvera, R.L.; Torrealday, O.; Penn, J.V.; Parikh, R. Prevalence of psychiatric disorders in the texas juvenile correctional system. J. Correct Health Care 2012, 18, 143–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Baillargeon, J.; Binswanger, I.A.; Penn, J.V.; Williams, B.A.; Murray, O.J. Psychiatric disorders and repeat incarcerations: The revolving prison door. Am. J. Psychiatry 2009, 166, 103–109. [Google Scholar]
  23. Pliszka, S.R.; Sherman, J.O.; Barrow, M.V.; Irick, S. Affective disorders in juvenile offenders: A preliminary study. Am. J. Psychiatry 2000, 157, 130–132. [Google Scholar]
  24. Geller, B.; Zimerman, B.; Williams, M.; Bolhofner, K.; Craney, J.L.; DelBello, M.P.; Soutullo, C.A. Diagnostic characteristics of 93 cases of a prepubertal and early adolescent bipolar disorder phenotype by gender, puberty and comorbid attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 2000, 10, 157–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Faraone, S.V.; Biederman, J.; Wozniak, J.; Mundy, E.; Mennin, D.; O’Donnell, D. Is comorbidity with ADHD a marker for juvenile-onset mania? J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 1997, 36, 1046–1055. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Axelson, D.; Birmaher, B.; Strober, M.; Gill, M.K.; Valeri, S.; Chiappetta, L.; Ryan, N.; Leonard, H.; Hunt, J.; Iyengar, S.; et al. Phenomenology of children and adolescents with bipolar spectrum disorders. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2006, 63, 1139–1148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Birmaher, B.; Axelson, D.; Monk, K.; Kalas, C.; Goldstein, B.; Hickey, M.B.; Obreja, M.; Ehmann, M.; Iyengar, S.; Shamseddeen, W.; et al. Lifetime psychiatric disorders in school-aged offspring of parents with bipolar disorder: The pittsburgh bipolar offspring study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2009, 66, 287–296. [Google Scholar]
  28. Biederman, J.; Mick, E.; Wozniak, J.; Monuteaux, M.C.; Galdo, M.; Faraone, S.V. Can a subtype of conduct disorder linked to bipolar disorder be identified? Integration of findings from the massachusetts general hospital pediatric psychopharmacology research program. Biol. Psychiatry 2003, 53, 952–960. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Lish, J.D.; Dime-Meenan, S.; Whybrow, P.C.; Price, R.A.; Hirschfeld, R.M. The national depressive and manic-depressive association (DMDA) survey of bipolar members. J. Affect. Disord. 1994, 31, 281–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Carlson, G.A.; Bromet, E.J.; Sievers, S. Phenomenology and outcome of subjects with early- and adult-onset psychotic mania. Am. J. Psychiatry 2000, 157, 213–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Kovacs, M.; Pollock, M. Bipolar disorder and comorbid conduct disorder in childhood and adolescence. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 1995, 34, 715–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Olvera, R.L.; Semrud-Clikeman, M.; Pliszka, S.R.; O’Donnell, L. Neuropsychological deficits in adolescents with conduct disorder and comorbid bipolar disorder: A pilot study. Bipolar Disord. 2005, 7, 57–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Kaufman, J.; Birmaher, B.; Brent, D.; Rao, U.; Flynn, C.; Moreci, P.; Williamson, D.; Ryan, N. Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 1997, 36, 980–988. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Hollingshead, A.B. Four Factor Index of Social Status; Department of Sociology, Yale University: New Haven, CT, USA, 1975. [Google Scholar]
  35. Fristad, M.A.; Weller, E.B.; Weller, R.A. The mania rating scale: Can it be used in children? A preliminary report. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 1992, 31, 252–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Poznanski, E.O.; Mokros, H.B. Children’s Depression Rating Scale, Revised (Cdrs-R); Western Psychological Services: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  37. Winters, K.C. Personal Experience Screening Questionnaire (PESQ); Western Psychological Services: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  38. Elliott, C.D. Adminstration and Scoring Manual for the Differential Ability Scales (DAS); The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
  39. Shaffer, D.; Gould, M.S.; Brasic, J.; Ambrosini, P.; Fisher, P.; Bird, H.; Aluwahlia, S. A Children’s Global Assessment Scale (CGAS). Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1983, 40, 1228–1231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Wright, I.C.; McGuire, P.K.; Poline, J.B.; Travere, J.M.; Murray, R.M.; Frith, C.D.; Frackowiak, R.S.; Friston, K.J. A voxel-based method for the statistical analysis of gray and white matter density applied to schizophrenia. Neuroimage 1995, 2, 244–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Bullmore, E.T.; Suckling, J.; Overmeyer, S.; Rabe-Hesketh, S.; Taylor, E.; Brammer, M.J. Global, voxel, and cluster tests, by theory and permutation, for a difference between two groups of structural MR images of the brain. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 1999, 18, 32–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Ashburner, J.; Friston, K.J. Voxel-based morphometry-the methods. Neuroimage 2000, 11, 805–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Good, C.D.; Johnsrude, I.S.; Ashburner, J.; Henson, R.N.; Friston, K.J.; Frackowiak, R.S. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage 2001, 14, 21–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Zhang, Y.; Brady, M.; Smith, S. Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 2001, 20, 45–57. [Google Scholar]
  45. Smith, S.M.; Nichols, T.E. Threshold-free cluster enhancement: Addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. Neuroimage 2009, 44, 83–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Jenkinson, M.; Smith, S. A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Med. Image Anal. 2001, 5, 143–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Andersson, J.; Jenkinson, M.; Smith, S. FMRIB’s Non-Linear Image Registration Tool; FMRIB Centre: Oxford, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  48. Holmes, A.P.; Blair, R.C.; Watson, J.D.; Ford, I. Nonparametric analysis of statistic images from functional mapping experiments. J. Cereb. Blood Flow. Metab. 1996, 16, 7–22. [Google Scholar]
  49. Nichols, T.E.; Holmes, A.P. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: A primer with examples. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2002, 15, 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Steiner, H. Practice parameters for the assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with conduct disorder. American academy of child and adolescent psychiatry. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 1997, 36, 122–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Fuster, J.M. The prefrontal cortex- an update: Time is of the essence. Neuroimage 2001, 30, 319–333. [Google Scholar]
  52. Narumoto, J.; Yamada, H.; Iidaka, T.; Sadato, N.; Fukui, K.; Itoh, H.; Yonekura, Y. Brain regions involved in verbal or non-verbal aspects of facial emotion recognition. Neuroreport 2000, 11, 2571–2576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Goldapple, K.; Segal, Z.; Garson, C.; Lau, M.; Bieling, P.; Kennedy, S.; Mayberg, H. Modulation of cortical-limbic pathways in major depression: Treatment-specific effects of cognitive behavior therapy. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2004, 61, 34–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Lopez-Larson, M.P.; DelBello, M.P.; Zimmerman, M.E.; Schwiers, M.L.; Strakowski, S.M. Regional prefrontal gray and white matter abnormalities in bipolar disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 2002, 52, 93–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. McIntosh, A.M.; Job, D.E.; Moorhead, T.W.; Harrison, L.K.; Forrester, K.; Lawrie, S.M.; Johnstone, E.C. Voxel-based morphometry of patients with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder and their unaffected relatives. Biol. Psychiatry 2004, 56, 544–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Dickstein, D.P.; Milham, M.P.; Nugent, A.C.; Drevets, W.C.; Charney, D.S.; Pine, D.S.; Leibenluft, E. Frontotemporal alterations in pediatric bipolar disorder: Results of a voxel-based morphometry study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2005, 62, 734–741. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Frazier, J.A.; Breeze, J.L.; Makris, N.; Giuliano, A.S.; Herbert, M.R.; Seidman, L.; Biederman, J.; Hodge, S.M.; Dieterich, M.E.; Gerstein, E.D.; et al. Cortical gray matter differences identified by structural magnetic resonance imaging in pediatric bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2005, 7, 555–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Frangou, S.; Donaldson, S.; Hadjulis, M.; Landau, S.; Goldstein, L.H. The maudsley bipolar disorder project: Executive dysfunction in bipolar disorder I and its clinical correlates. Biol. Psychiatry 2005, 58, 859–864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Houenou, J.; Frommberger, J.; Carde, S.; Glasbrenner, M.; Diener, C.; Leboyer, M.; Wessa, M. Neuroimaging-based markers of bipolar disorder: Evidence from two meta-analyses. J. Affect. Disord. 2011, 132, 344–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Ochsner, K.N.; Ray, R.D.; Cooper, J.C.; Robertson, E.R.; Chopra, S.; Gabrieli, J.D.E.; Gross, J.J. For better or for worse: Neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion. Neuroimage 2004, 23, 483–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Blair, K.S.; Smith, B.W.; Mitchell, D.G.V.; Morton, J.; Vythilingam, M.; Pessoa, L.; Fridberg, D.; Zametkin, A.; Nelson, E.E.; Drevets, W.C.; et al. Modulation of emotion by cognition and cognition by emotion. Neuroimage 2007, 35, 430–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Price, J.L.; Drevets, W.C. Neurocircuitry of mood disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 2010, 35, 192–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Strakowski, S.M.; DelBello, M.P.; Adler, C.M. The functional neuroanatomy of bipolar disorder: A review of neuroimaging findings. Mol. Psychiatry 2005, 10, 105–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Adler, C.M.; DelBello, M.P.; Strakowski, S.M. Brain network dysfunction in bipolar disorder. CNS Spectr. 2006, 11, 312–320. [Google Scholar]
  65. Phillips, M.L.; Ladouceur, C.D.; Drevets, W.C. Neural systems underlying voluntary and automatic emotion regulation: Toward a neural model of bipolar disorder. Mol. Psychiatry 2008, 13, 829–857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Bechara, A.; Damasio, H.; Damasio, A.R. Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 2000, 10, 295–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Scarpa, A.; Raine, A. The psychophysiology of child misconduct. Pediatr. Ann. 2004, 33, 296–304. [Google Scholar]
  68. Herpertz, S.C.; Mueller, B.; Qunaibi, M.; Lichterfeld, C.; Konrad, K.; Herpertz-Dahlmann, B. Response to emotional stimuli in boys with conduct disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 2005, 162, 1100–1107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Herpertz, S.C.; Vloet, T.; Mueller, B.; Domes, G.; Willmes, K.; Herpertz-Dahlmann, B. Similar autonomic responsivity in boys with conduct disorder and their fathers. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2007, 46, 535–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Damasio, A.R.; Tranel, D.; Damasio, H. Individuals with sociopathic behavior caused by frontal damage fail to respond autonomically to social stimuli. Behav. Brain Res. 1990, 41, 81–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Bechara, A.; Tranel, D.; Damasio, H.; Damasio, A.R. Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 1996, 6, 215–225. [Google Scholar]
  72. Raine, A. Annotation: The role of prefrontal deficits, low autonomic arousal, and early health factors in the development of antisocial and aggressive behavior in children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2002, 43, 417–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Marsh, A.A.; Finger, E.C.; Mitchell, D.G.; Reid, M.E.; Sims, C.; Kosson, D.S.; Towbin, K.E.; Leibenluft, E.; Pine, D.S.; Blair, R.J. Reduced amygdala response to fearful expressions in children and adolescents with callous-unemotional traits and disruptive behavior disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 2008, 165, 712–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Cabeza, R.; Nyberg, L. Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2000, 12, 1–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Narumoto, J.; Okada, T.; Sadato, N.; Fukui, K.; Yonekura, Y. Attention to emotion modulates fMRI activity in human right superior temporal sulcus. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 2001, 12, 225–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Aichhorn, M.; Perner, J.; Weiss, B.; Kronbichler, M.; Staffen, W.; Ladurner, G. Temporo-parietal junction activity in theory-of-mind tasks: Falseness, beliefs, or attention. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2009, 21, 1179–1192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Vollm, B.A.; Taylor, A.N.; Richardson, P.; Corcoran, R.; Stirling, J.; McKie, S.; Deakin, J.F.; Elliott, R. Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task. Neuroimage 2006, 29, 90–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Saxe, R.; Wexler, A. Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo-parietal junction. Neuropsychologia 2005, 43, 1391–1399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Glahn, D.C.; Thompson, P.M.; Blangero, J. Neuroimaging endophenotypes: Strategies for finding genes influencing brain structure and function. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2007, 28, 488–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Glahn, D.C.; Knowles, E.E.; McKay, D.R.; Sprooten, E.; Raventos, H.; Blangero, J.; Gottesman, I.I.; Almasy, L. Arguments for the sake of endophenotypes: Examining common misconceptions about the use of endophenotypes in psychiatric genetics. Am. J. Med. Gen. 2014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Friedman, L.; Findling, R.L.; Kenny, J.T.; Swales, T.P.; Stuve, T.A.; Jesberger, J.A.; Lewin, J.S.; Schulz, S.C. An MRI study of adolescent patients with either schizophrenia or bipolar disorder as compared to healthy control subjects. Biol. Psychiatry 1999, 46, 78–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Strakowski, S.M.; DelBello, M.P.; Zimmerman, M.E.; Getz, G.E.; Mills, N.P.; Ret, J.; Shear, P.; Adler, C.M. Ventricular and periventricular structural volumes in first- versus multiple-episode bipolar disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 2002, 159, 1841–1847. [Google Scholar]
  83. DelBello, M.P.; Zimmerman, M.E.; Mills, N.P.; Getz, G.E.; Strakowski, S.M. Magnetic resonance imaging analysis of amygdala and other subcortical brain regions in adolescents with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2004, 6, 43–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Chang, K.; Karchemskiy, A.; Barnea-Goraly, N.; Garrett, A.; Simeonova, D.I.; Reiss, A. Reduced amygdalar gray matter volume in familial pediatric bipolar disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2005, 44, 565–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Chang, K.; Barnea-Goraly, N.; Karchemskiy, A.; Simeonova, D.I.; Barnes, P.; Ketter, T.; Reiss, A.L. Cortical magnetic resonance imaging findings in familial pediatric bipolar disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 2005, 58, 197–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Hafeman, D.M.; Chang, K.D.; Garrett, A.S.; Sanders, E.M.; Phillips, M.L. Effects of medication on neuroimaging findings in bipolar disorder: An updated review. Bipolar Disord. 2012, 14, 375–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Ashburner, J.; Friston, K.J. Why voxel-based morphometry should be used. Neuroimage 2001, 14, 1238–1243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Konarski, J.Z.; McIntyre, R.S.; Kennedy, S.H.; Rafi-Tari, S.; Soczynska, J.K.; Ketter, T.A. Volumetric neuroimaging investigations in mood disorders: Bipolar disorder versus major depressive disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2008, 10, 1–37. [Google Scholar]
  89. Rubia, K. “Cool” inferior frontostriatal dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder versus “hot” ventromedial orbitofrontal-limbic dysfunction in conduct disorder: A review. Biol. Psychiatry 2011, 69, 69–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Gillihan, S.J.; Parens, E. Should we expect “neural signatures” for DSM diagnoses? J. Clin. Psychiatry 2011, 72, 1383–1389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Connor, D.F.; Doerfler, L.A. ADHD with comorbid oppositional defiant disorder or conduct disorder: Discrete or nondistinct disruptive behavior disorders? J. Atten. Disord. 2008, 12, 126–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Anderson, D.; Ardekani, B.A.; Burdick, K.E.; Robinson, D.G.; John, M.; Malhotra, A.K.; Szeszko, P.R. Overlapping and distinct gray and white matter abnormalities in schizophrenia and bipolar I disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2013, 15, 680–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Olvera, R.L.; Glahn, D.C.; O'Donnell, L.; Bearden, C.E.; Soares, J.C.; Winkler, A.M.; Pliszka, S.R. Cortical Volume Alterations in Conduct Disordered Adolescents with and without Bipolar Disorder. J. Clin. Med. 2014, 3, 416-431. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm3020416

AMA Style

Olvera RL, Glahn DC, O'Donnell L, Bearden CE, Soares JC, Winkler AM, Pliszka SR. Cortical Volume Alterations in Conduct Disordered Adolescents with and without Bipolar Disorder. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2014; 3(2):416-431. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm3020416

Chicago/Turabian Style

Olvera, Rene L., David C. Glahn, Louise O'Donnell, Carrie E. Bearden, Jair C. Soares, Anderson M. Winkler, and Steven R. Pliszka. 2014. "Cortical Volume Alterations in Conduct Disordered Adolescents with and without Bipolar Disorder" Journal of Clinical Medicine 3, no. 2: 416-431. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm3020416

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop