Next Article in Journal
SPAST Intragenic CNVs Lead to Hereditary Spastic Paraplegia via a Haploinsufficiency Mechanism
Previous Article in Journal
OV Modulators of the Paediatric Brain TIME: Current Status, Combination Strategies, Limitations and Future Directions
Previous Article in Special Issue
Chloride/Multiple Anion Exchanger SLC26A Family: Systemic Roles of SLC26A4 in Various Organs
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Sodium Bicarbonate Decreases Alcohol Consumption in Mice

Department of Cell Biology, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA 30322, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2024, 25(9), 5006; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms25095006
Submission received: 6 March 2024 / Revised: 26 April 2024 / Accepted: 28 April 2024 / Published: 3 May 2024

Abstract

:
We previously reported that mice with low neuronal pH drink more alcohol, demonstrating the importance of pH for alcohol reward and motivation. In this study, we tested whether systemic pH affects alcohol consumption and if so, whether it occurs by changing the alcohol reward. C57BL/6J mice were given NaHCO3 to raise their blood pH, and the animals’ alcohol consumption was measured in the drinking-in-the-dark and two-bottle free choice paradigms. Alcohol consumption was also assessed after suppressing the bitterness of NaHCO3 with sucrose. Alcohol reward was evaluated using a conditioned place preference. In addition, taste sensitivity was assessed by determining quinine and sucrose preference. The results revealed that a pH increase by NaHCO3 caused mice to decrease their alcohol consumption. The decrease in high alcohol contents (20%) was significant and observed at different ages, as well as in both males and females. Alcohol consumption was also decreased after suppressing NaHCO3 bitterness. Oral gavage of NaHCO3 did not alter quinine and sucrose preference. In the conditioned place preference, NaHCO3-treated mice spent less time in the alcohol-injected chamber. Conclusively, the results show that raising systemic pH with NaHCO3 decreases alcohol consumption, as it decreases the alcohol reward value.

1. Introduction

Excessive alcohol drinking has devastating health consequences [1,2]. Between 2011 and 2015, excessive drinking was responsible for more than 95,000 deaths and an average of 29 years lost per death in the US each year [3], and the death rate increased by 39% in 2021 [4]. Excessive drinking is associated with alcohol-use disorder (AUD), which causes 3 million deaths worldwide, according to the World Health Organization [5]. Preventing excessive use of alcohol is, thus, essential for reducing the risk of AUD. The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention recommends cutting back on drinking, and many people benefit simply by controlling their habits by drinking slowly, choosing alcohol-free days, or setting a limit on how much they will drink [6]. However, maintaining new drinking habits is not an easy task, and some people regain old habits later. Medical treatment approved by the US Food and Drug Administration is prescribed only to people who have already stopped drinking and are trying to maintain abstinence [7]. In this regard, developing a non-medication strategy that can easily reduce excessive alcohol drinking will be valuable for people at risk of developing AUD and for others who want to drink moderately.
pH is an important biological factor in the body, as H+ binds to proteins and alters the structure and function of bound proteins [8,9,10,11]. In the nervous system, pH affects the activities of many proteins, such as receptors, ion channels, transporters, and signaling proteins, thus resulting in physiological and pathological consequences [12,13,14]. A small amount of H+ or HCO 3 moving across cell membranes can cause a large pH change at the synapses, where the synaptic cleft is typically closed off; as a result, synaptic activity and the complex firing pattern can be altered. In this sense, it is conceivable that pH may alter alcohol modulatory properties in the brain, such that alcohol-drinking frequency and amount are changed. For example, pH can affect dopamine transmission [15,16], which is responsible for motivation and reward [17]. pH also alters the activities of γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) receptors and N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors, both of which play key roles in synaptic plasticity relevant to the development of addictive behaviors [18,19,20].
We previously reported that alcohol consumption is high in mice with targeted inactivation of a pH-regulating protein found in many cells, including neurons [21]. These mice have low pH in neurons and display increased responses to alcohol due to the development of increased motivation to drink alcohol. These findings led us to test whether alcohol consumption could be affected by systemic pH and, if so, whether it would occur by changing the alcohol reward value. In this study, we raised blood pH in C57BL/6 mice by orally administering NaHCO3 and analyzed the animals’ alcohol consumption, effective dose of NaHCO3, effective alcohol content, and taste sensitivity. The effect of NaHCO3 on alcohol reward was also evaluated. The results show that mildly increasing blood pH with NaHCO3 decreases alcohol consumption, as it decreases the alcohol reward value.

2. Results

2.1. Acute Ingestion of NaHCO3 Decreases Alcohol Consumption in Mice

Table 1 shows the results of blood chemistry in C57BL/6J mice, measured at 0, 1, and 2 h after oral gavage of NaHCO3 (0.25 g/kg body weight). Blood pH increased from 7.35 to 7.44 at 1 h and 7.47 at 2 h (p < 0.05; one-way ANOVA with repeated measures). These changes are comparable to blood pH increases at 1–2 h after ingestion of similar amounts of NaHCO3 in humans [22,23,24]. As expected, HCO 3 and base excess (i.e., an excess in the amount of base) increased, while PCO2 remained unchanged. In parallel experiments, a different cohort of mice was given a NaHCO3 gavage and, one hour later, allowed free access to 20% alcohol in the DID paradigm. The control mice were given water instead of NaHCO3. As shown in Figure 1, NaHCO3-treated mice (i.e., animals with raised blood pH) decreased their alcohol consumption, compared with water-treated controls (p < 0.05; two-way ANOVA). The decrease was particularly prominent on day 4, when alcohol drinking was allowed for 4 h. The decrease was observed in mice at different ages, as well as males and females, indicating that the effect of NaHCO3 on alcohol consumption occurs regardless of age and sex.

2.2. Chronic Ingestion of NaHCO3 Decreases Alcohol Consumption and Preference in Mice

The DID procedure we used in the above experiment is useful to assess the effects of pharmacological compounds (NaHCO3 in our study) on binge-like alcohol consumption, especially when the action of a drug is short term [25]. On the other hand, DID is limited in testing some other properties of a drug, such as its effects on alcohol preference or taste sensitivity. For this reason, we measured alcohol consumption in mice using the two-bottle free choice paradigm in which animals were allowed free access to two bottles: one containing water and the other containing alcohol. Blood pH was raised by allowing mice free access to 0.3 M NaHCO3 for 6 days and maintained during the experiment. As shown in Table 2, chronic ingestion of NaHCO3 increased blood pH by 0.19 pH units (p < 0.05; Student t-test). As expected, HCO 3 and base excess increased while most of the other parameters, including PCO2, anion gap, BUN, hematocrit, and hemoglobin, remained unchanged. NaHCO3 decreased blood K+ level (p < 0.05), which is consistent with hypokalemia in metabolic alkalosis [26,27]. Figure 2A shows the comparison of 5–20% alcohol consumption before and after NaHCO3 ingestion. The consumption of 5–15% alcohol was similar between the two groups, but the consumption of 20% alcohol was significantly decreased after NaHCO3 ingestion (p < 0.05 for the interaction between alcohol content and NaHCO3; two-way ANOVA). Consistent with this result, there was a decrease in alcohol preference (i.e., ratio of alcohol intake–total fluid intake) at 20% (Figure 2B). These changes were observed in both males and females (Figure 2C,D), again consistent with negligible sex difference, as shown in the above DID.

2.3. NaHCO3 at the Concentrations of 0.1–0.4 M Decreases Alcohol Consumption without Causing Diarrhea

To determine the dose-dependent effects of NaHCO3 on alcohol consumption, mice were subjected to the two-bottle free choice with water and 20% alcohol in the presence of 0.05–0.4 M NaHCO3. As shown in Figure 3A, alcohol consumption began to decrease at a concentration of 0.1 M and higher (p < 0.05; one-way ANOVA with repeated measures). Calculated from the fluid volume ingested, the minimum effective dose of NaHCO3 corresponded to 0.05 g/kg body weight. Mice increased the consumption of water containing NaHCO3, as shown in Figure 3B. This inverse relationship resulted in a progressive decrease in alcohol preference as NaHCO3 concentrations increased (Figure 3C). The highest concentration in our study was 0.4 M, which can cause gastrointestinal distress, such as diarrhea in humans [28]. We examined the condition of feces produced by mice to determine whether the animals had diarrhea. We found no significant difference in diarrhea scores between NaHCO3-treated mice and water-treated mice (Figure 4A). The number of feces produced by the two groups was also similar (Figure 4B). Thus, drinking up to 0.4 M NaHCO3 decreased alcohol consumption without causing diarrhea in our study.

2.4. Decreased Alcohol Consumption Is Not Due to the Unpleasant Taste of NaHCO3

NaHCO3 has a bitter and metallic taste and is unpleasant to drink. In the above dose–response experiments, alcohol consumption was more profoundly decreased at higher NaHCO3 concentrations. While this result supports the importance of high pH for decreasing alcohol consumption, an alternative explanation is that mice might have consumed less fluid due to the unpleasant taste of NaHCO3. We conducted two sets of experiments to address this possibility. First, mice were allowed free access to NaHCO3 in the presence of sucrose, which suppresses the bitter taste [29]. Figure 5A shows the baseline consumption of pure water vs. NaHCO3-containing water in a two-bottle free choice. Mice did not prefer NaHCO3-containing water to pure water. In the presence of 1% sucrose, however, mice consumed NaHCO3-containing water as much as pure water (p > 0.05; Student t-test) (Figure 5B), indicating the suppression of NaHCO3 bitterness by sucrose. Next, alcohol consumption was measured in the presence of sucrose and compared before and after 6 days of NaHCO3 ingestion. As shown in Figure 5C–E, mice with NaHCO3 ingestion decreased alcohol consumption and preference, while increasing NaHCO3-containing water. These changes were comparable to those in the absence of sucrose (Figure 3). Thus, the ability of NaHCO3 to decrease alcohol consumption occurred regardless of sucrose, indicating that the unpleasant taste of NaHCO3 has a negligible impact on decreasing alcohol consumption.

2.5. NaHCO3 Can Decrease Alcohol Consumption without Altering Taste Sensitivity

To determine the effect of NaHCO3 on taste sensitivity, mice were allowed free access to 0.01–0.3 mM quinine and water in the two-bottle choice, and quinine consumption was measured before and after 6 days of NaHCO3 ingestion. Both bottles contained NaHCO3 when measured after NaHCO3 ingestion. NaHCO3 caused quinine preference to increase (p < 0.05; two-way ANOVA), as shown in Figure 6A. The increase was significant at 0.1 and 0.3 mM. In separate experiments, mice were given access to 1% and 10% sucrose and water, and sucrose consumption was measured before and after NaHCO3 ingestion. No difference was found in 1% sucrose preference, whereas there was an increase in 10% sucrose preference (Figure 6B). Thus, free access to NaHCO3 appears to influence taste sensitivity.
In this experiment, mice were given free access to NaHCO3, such that taste buds on the tongue were influenced by NaHCO3. This led to a question of whether the effect of NaHCO3 on alcohol consumption would be mediated by raising the blood pH or by directly changing taste sensitivity on the tongue. To address this question, we gave mice NaHCO3 by oral gavage to deliver NaHCO3 directly to the stomach and assessed quinine and sucrose preference. As shown in Figure 6C, both NaHCO3-treated mice and water-treated controls had a similar quinine preference (p > 0.05; two-way ANOVA). Both groups also had a similar sucrose preference (p > 0.05; two-way ANOVA) (Figure 6D). Thus, oral gavage of NaHCO3 had negligible impacts on taste sensitivity, indicating that the effect of NaHCO3 on alcohol consumption can occur without altering taste sensitivity.

2.6. NaHCO3 Decreases Alcohol-Reinforcing Effects

Decreased voluntary alcohol consumption can be driven by either a decreased reward value of alcohol, thus leading to reduced motivation for alcohol consumption, or an increased reward value of alcohol, thus requiring less alcohol to achieve the same level of hedonic response [30,31]. We examined the reinforcing effects of alcohol using CPP in which mice received oral gavage of NaHCO3 or water 1 h before alcohol conditioning (Figure 7A). Water-treated control mice increased the time spent in the alcohol-injected chamber after conditioning (Figure 7B), consistent with a place preference conditioned by alcohol. In contrast, NaHCO3-treated mice showed negligible increase after conditioning (p > 0.05; Student t-test), as shown in Figure 7C. NaHCO3 gavage alone without alcohol injection had no effect (Supplementary Data S1). The difference between the two groups of mice was evident when the CPP scores were calculated (Figure 7D). The scores were significantly decreased in NaHCO3-treated mice, indicating a decreased alcohol reward value. Alcohol decreased the distance traveled and the speed when measured during conditioning (p < 0.05 for each; one-way ANOVA) (Figure 7E,F). This effect was blunted in NaHCO3-treated mice. Conclusively, the CPP results demonstrate that NaHCO3 decreases the reinforcing effects of alcohol.

3. Discussion

The major findings in this study are highlighted as follows. (i) A systemic pH increase by NaHCO3 ingestion decreases alcohol consumption in mice. (ii) This effect is evident with higher alcohol contents and occurs in mice at different ages, as well as in both males and females. (iii) The unpleasant taste of NaHCO3 is not involved in decreasing alcohol consumption. (iv) Decreased alcohol consumption is driven by decreased alcohol-reinforcing effects, thus reducing the motivation to drink alcohol. (v) NaHCO3 can decrease alcohol consumption without altering taste sensitivity. The significance of our study is that we identified pH as a biological factor that can influence alcohol-drinking properties. In alcohol research, pH change has been viewed as the consequence of alcohol metabolism, given that chronic and excessive alcohol drinking causes lactic acidosis and ketoacidosis [32,33]. No study has been done to examine whether pH conversely affects alcohol drinking. In this sense, our study, for the first time, provides evidence for the role of pH in modulating alcohol reward and drinking properties and presents a different paradigm from the current understanding. Considering that a small pH difference has been considered minor in function and is often ignored, our observation of marked changes in alcohol consumption by a 0.1 pH unit increase is, thus, interesting.
We raised the blood pH in mice using NaHCO3. In humans, NaHCO3 ingestion improves pH, [ HCO 3 ], base excess, and PCO2 compared to the placebo condition [34]. HCO 3 loading is ergogenic in sporting events that heavily generate energy through anaerobic glycolysis and challenge acid–base homeostasis [35,36]. Ingestion of 0.2–0.3 g/kg NaHCO3 increases blood pH by 0.05–0.1 pH unit for 1–3 h [22,23,24]. Consistent with these changes, we observed a comparable increase in blood pH after oral gavage of 0.25 g/kg NaHCO3 (Table 1). This amount transiently increased blood [ HCO 3 ] by 5–6 mM. In humans, 0.3 g/kg NaHCO3 causes bloating and diarrhea in some studies, while the same amount has no effect in other studies. At much higher doses, NaHCO3 can cause abnormal electrical activity, volume expansion, and hypernatremia [37]. Healthy adults can tolerate up to 1700 mEq (142.8 g) NaHCO3 daily [38,39]. This corresponds to 2.1 g/kg NaHCO3 in mice. In our study, we used 0.25 g/kg NaHCO3 for oral gavage, markedly lower than the tolerance level. NaHCO3 is removed from the kidneys or converted to CO2 and H2O, and the addition of new HCO 3 is temporary and unlikely to cause a severe metabolic complication.
We found that mildly raised blood pH by NaHCO3 caused mice to consume less alcohol. This effect was observed in acute and chronic ingestions, as well as by voluntary (DID and two-bottle free choice) and involuntary (gavage) drinking paradigms. Thus, the pH change, not the procedure to administer NaHCO3, is responsible for decreasing alcohol consumption. The decrease was profound with the alcohol content of 20% while being negligible with the lower contents (Figure 3). Thus, pH probably affects the efficacy of alcohol to produce the maximum response, but not its potency to increase consumption. Drug efficacy is the intrinsic ability of a drug to activate its target molecule, whereas drug potency is a binding affinity between a drug and its target molecule [40]. Thus, we think that pH affects alcohol’s ability to activate, or inactivate, its target proteins (receptors) but has no effect on the rate of the binding between alcohol and its target proteins. Because efficacy is an intrinsic characteristic of alcohol, it is likely that pH alters the pharmacological properties of the target proteins, resulting in a decreased maximum response to alcohol. This interpretation is consistent with the fact that pH can alter protein structure and function and modulate the relevant synaptic activities and complex firing patterns in the brain.
An interesting finding from our study is the increase in the consumption of NaHCO3-containing water in the presence of alcohol (Figure 3B). Mice did not prefer NaHCO3-containing water to pure water when the two solutions were given in the two-bottle free choice. This is understandable, as NaHCO3 has a bitter and salty taste. However, when sucrose was provided, mice consumed NaHCO3-containing water as much as pure water and, more importantly, consumed less alcohol. Thus, the alcohol consumption decrease caused by NaHCO3 is unrelated to its taste. This interpretation leads to a possibility that alcohol may abolish the bitterness of NaHCO3. Alcohol has a sweet component in addition to a bitter component in animals [41] and humans [42]. Furthermore, the relationship between sucrose and alcohol intake has been documented [43,44]. A cross-sectional study by Liu et al. [43] revealed that many heavy drinkers have taste dysfunction, determined by failing to correctly identify the bitter taste of quinine in the whole-mouth test. Silva et al. [44] observed low sensitivity for sweet taste in alcoholics. Excessive alcohol drinking can change the sensitivity of taste receptors and compromise the functions of taste. Together, we think that, similar to sucrose, alcohol may suppress NaHCO3 bitterness, and this suppressive property of alcohol facilitates the consumption of NaHCO3-containing water.
Regarding the effect of NaHCO3 on taste sensitivity, quinine and sucrose preferences were similar between NaHCO3-treated mice and pair-treated mice, when NaHCO3 was given by oral gavage. However, these preferences were different between the two groups when mice were allowed free access to NaHCO3 (Figure 6). Oral gavage allows NaHCO3 to be directly delivered to the stomach with minimum contact with taste buds on the tongue. The lack of change in the preferences, thus, indicates that the decreasing effect of NaHCO3 on alcohol consumption can occur without changing taste sensitivity. Given that NaHCO3 reduces alcohol consumption regardless of the administration procedures, we think that the two properties are not closely related to each other. Additional studies are required to further assess the effect of NaHCO3 on taste sensitivity.
The results from the current study are consistent with our previous finding that alcohol consumption is enhanced in NBCn1 knockout mice [21]. These mice have decreased resting pH in neurons and cerebrospinal fluid; thus, they develop acidic brain pH. Increased alcohol consumption in these mice is due neither to a difference in taste sensitivity, as there is no significant effect of genotype on quinine and sucrose preference, nor to alcohol clearance, as blood alcohol contents are similar between genotypes. Instead, it is due to the increased reward value of alcohol because the knockout mice develop an increased propensity for the alcohol-induced conditioned place preference. The knockout mice also have an increased tolerance to alcohol-induced sedative effects. We did not test whether NaHCO3 affects a tolerance to alcohol-induced sedation.
What would be the underlying mechanism of pH-dependent changes in alcohol consumption? The neural circuit that plays a key role in alcohol reward is the mesolimbic pathway [31,45], in which alcohol stimulates dopamine (DA) release from the ventral tegmental area (VTA) nerve terminals in the nucleus accumbens (NAc) in the ventral striatum. The stimulation occurs, in part, by decreasing GABAergic inhibition in the VTA, which results in a disinhibition of DA neuronal activity [17]. The stimulation also occurs by affecting glutamate release from various glutamatergic projections that feed into the VTA and NAc and mediate the synaptic plasticity relevant to addictive behaviors [18,19]. GABAA receptors have variable pH sensitivity due to the different pharmacological and biophysical properties of their subunits [46]. NMDA receptors are also sensitive to physiological pH (IC50 = pH 7.3), and their pH dependence has been a model for pH-mediated regulation of neuronal activity [47]. It is unclear whether a similar model is involved in the mesolimbic pathway, but we note that a 0.3 pH unit change alters the current in the NAc [48]. Furthermore, pH may influence dopamine release and uptake [15], dopamine autoxidation [16], and its binding to dopamine receptors [49]. pH may cause significant effects on the dopaminergic transmission associated with alcohol motivation and reward, subsequently changing alcohol consumption. In this regard, we note that postmortem tissue of people with AUD has low brain pH, compared with controls [50,51].
In summary, our study demonstrates the importance of systemic pH for alcohol consumption and alcohol reward. The results lead to the possibility that NaHCO3 as a mediator has the potential to decrease excessive alcohol consumption in humans and improve alcohol-related behaviors. NaHCO3 is a dietary supplement and has a lower risk of adverse effects. Furthermore, NaHCO3 can enhance the anti-inflammatory response [52,53], thus suggesting that NaHCO3 not only reduces alcohol consumption but may also protect the body from alcohol-induced peripheral insults. Taken together, we propose that temporarily raising blood pH by NaHCO3 can be a useful approach to reducing excessive alcohol consumption and ultimately reducing the risk of AUD.

4. Materials and Methods

4.1. Animals

This study was conducted in accordance with the National Institute of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at Emory University. C57BL/6J male and female mice at the age of 12–24 weeks were purchased from Jackson Labs (Bar Harbor, ME, USA) and housed on a 12 h light–dark cycle. Mice were provided with standard chow and water ad libitum. Blood chemistry was analyzed using an i-STAT 1 blood analyzer (Abbott Laboratories, Abbott Park, IL, USA) with EC8+ and CG8+ cartridges.

4.2. Drinking in the Dark (DID)

DID was conducted using the protocol by Thiele et al. [54] with the following modification. One week before the initiation of testing, mice were individually housed in the reverse light-cycle room and given standard mouse chow and water ad libitum. Each mouse was weighed before DID. During the first 3 h of the dark phase, mice were given oral gavage of NaHCO3 (0.25 g/kg body weight) 1 h prior to alcohol drinking. The gavage volume was 10 × body weight (µL/g). The controls were water gavage. The mice were allowed to drink 20% alcohol for 2 h on days 1–3 and 4 h on day 4 to achieve an alcohol binge, as described by Thiele et al. [54]. Alcohol consumption was determined per body weight (g/kg body weight).

4.3. Two-Bottle Free Choice

Mice were individually housed and allowed continuous access to two bottles: one bottle containing water and another bottle containing alcohol with various concentrations of 5–20% alcohol (v/v). Each concentration was offered for 4 days. Alcohol consumption (g/kg body weight) was calculated by subtracting the volume of the solutions left in the bottles from the initial volume. Measurement was made daily at each concentration and averaged. The bottles were switched every day to minimize a potential side preference. To increase blood pH, the mice were given free access to a solution containing 0.2–0.3 M NaHCO3 for 6 days, which gradually increases blood pH and stably maintains a raised pH during alcohol-consumption experiments. The mice were then subjected to an alcohol-consumption assessment in the two-bottle free choice. NaHCO3 was added to both bottles to maintain increased blood pH. For the experiments with the suppression of NaHCO3 bitterness, 1% sucrose was added to both bottles. Alcohol preference was calculated from the ratio of alcohol intake to total fluid intake (alcohol + water). Alcohol evaporates over time and the evaporation rate varies with different alcohol contents. Our preliminary test showed 6.2–13.1% evaporation in 24 h for 5–20% alcohol contents, and the changes were similar, regardless of NaHCO3 in the solutions. The loss by evaporation was not subtracted in this study, as we compared alcohol consumption between the control group and the test group.

4.4. Diarrhea Assessment

Diarrhea was assessed using the diarrhea scoring system by Sakai et al. [55]. Scores were scaled based on the following stool conditions, 0: normal, 1: soft stool, 2: slight wet and soft stool, 3: wet and unformed stool with moderate perianal staining on the coat, and 4: watery stool with severe perianal staining on the coat. The assessment was conducted with mice that had been given 0.4 M NaHCO3 (the highest concentration in the two-bottle free choice) for 2 days. The average score and the incidence of each diarrhea score were obtained.

4.5. Taste Sensitivity

Taste sensitivity was assessed in two different ways. First, the assessment was made in the two-bottle free choice paradigm as described previously [21]. The mice were individually housed and offered two bottles containing water and 0.01–1 mM of quinine hydrochloride. Each quinine concentration was offered for 1 day; in between each quinine test day, the animals were given two bottles of water for 2 days. The bottles were weighed daily and switched. Then, the mice were given free access to 0.3 M NaHCO3 for 6 days, and the quinine experiments were repeated. NaHCO3 was added to both bottles to maintain an increased blood pH. For sucrose preference, 1% or 10% (w/v) of sucrose was used instead of quinine. Quinine or sucrose preference was determined as a ratio of taste solution intake to total fluid intake. Second, taste sensitivity was assessed in the DID paradigm. The mice were given an oral gavage of NaHCO3 (0.25 g/kg) or water during the first 3 h of the dark phase and then allowed to drink 0.01–1 mM of quinine hydrochloride for 4 h. Each concentration was offered for 1 day. For sucrose preference, 1% or 10% (w/v) of sucrose was used instead of quinine. Quinine or sucrose preference was determined as a ratio of taste solution intake to total fluid intake.

4.6. Conditioned Place Preference (CPP)

Alcohol-induced CPP was performed as described previously [56] with a slight modification. Mice were acclimated to the testing room for at least several hours a day before testing. The CPP apparatus had two chambers: one with white walls and the other with black walls separated by a small partition [57]. On day 1 (pretest), mice were given full access to the chambers for 15 min. The chamber for placing mice was random and alternated. Following the pretest, 5 min conditioning sessions were conducted daily for 3 days, with saline injections in the morning and 2 g/kg alcohol injections 4 h later. One group of mice received oral gavage of 0.25 g/kg NaHCO3 one hour prior to alcohol injection, and another group of mice received water instead of NaHCO3. The gavage was performed in the home cages to eliminate potential preference/aversion caused by NaHCO3 or the gavage itself. Conditioning was performed using an unbiased design [58], in which the animals were placed in the black chamber after saline injection and in the white chamber after alcohol injection. On day 5 (test), the mice were placed into the chamber where they were initially placed in the pretest and allowed free access to the apparatus for 15 min. The time spent in the alcohol-paired chamber was measured and compared to the time spent in the same chamber in the pretest. The results were compared between NaHCO3-treated and water-treated mice. For testing the effect of NaHCO3 on alcohol-related locomotor activity, distance traveled and speed in the conditioning sessions were analyzed. All experiments were recorded using an overhead camera and analyzed using the tracking software Toxtrac version 2.98 [59].

4.7. Statistical Analysis

Data were reported as mean ± standard error of the mean. A two-tailed, unpaired Student t-test was used when analyzing diarrhea scores and CPP scores and a two-tailed, paired test when analyzing blood chemistry before and after NaHCO3 administration and suppression of the bitterness with sucrose. One-way ANOVA with repeated measures was used when analyzing blood chemistry at different time points after NaHCO3 gavage and the effective dose of NaHCO3, and an ordinary one-way ANOVA was used when analyzing time traveled and speed in CPP. A two-way ANOVA with a Sidak post hoc test was used when analyzing alcohol consumption in DID, different alcohol contents in the two-bottle free choice, sex difference, and taste sensitivity. A p value of <0.05 was considered significant. The analysis was made using Prism 9 (GraphPad; La Jolla, CA, USA) and Microsoft Office Excel add-in Analysis ToolPak (Redmond, WA, USA). Outliers were determined using Grubbs’ test.

Supplementary Materials

The supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ijms25095006/s1.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, I.C.; Data curation, I.C.; Formal analysis, I.C., J.L., A.P.R., Y.Y., C.R., S.P. and K.J.; Funding acquisition, I.C.; Investigation, J.L., A.P.R., Y.Y., C.R., S.P. and K.J.; Methodology, I.C.; Project administration, I.C.; Resources, I.C. and S.M.G.; Supervision, I.C. and S.M.G.; Validation, I.C.; Writing-original draft, I.C.; Writing—review and editing, S.P. and S.M.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by NIH R21AA028606, R01AA030893 to I.C. and NINDS R01NS102850 to S.M.G.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The animal study protocol was approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Emory University (protocol code: 201700469 and 201700252).

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available upon request to I.C.

Acknowledgments

We thank Jesse Schank for the helpful discussion on CPP experiments. The authors also thank Ashwini Narayanan and Da Min Lee for the editorial correction.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. The US Burden of Disease Collaborators. The State of US Health, 1990–2016: Burden of Diseases, Injuries, and Risk Factors Among US States. JAMA 2018, 319, 1444–1472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Alcohol Facts and Statistics. Available online: https://www.niaaa.nih.gov/sites/default/files/publications/NIAAA_Alcohol_Facts_and_Stats_0.pdf (accessed on 1 March 2024).
  3. Esser, M.B.; Sherk, A.; Liu, Y.; Naimi, T.S.; Stockwell, T.; Stahre, M.; Kanny, D.; Landen, M.; Saitz, R.; Brewer, R.D. Deaths and Years of Potential Life Lost From Excessive Alcohol Use—United States, 2011–2015. MMWR Morb. Mortal. Wkly. Rep. 2020, 69, 1428–1433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Esser, M.B.; Sherk, A.; Liu, Y.; Naimi, T.S. Deaths from Excessive Alcohol Use—United States, 2016–2021. MMWR Morb. Mortal. Wkly. Rep. 2024, 73, 154–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. World Health Organization. Global Status Report on Alcohol and Health 2018. Available online: https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/9789241565639 (accessed on 1 March 2024).
  6. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) Preventing Excessive Alcohol Use. Available online: https://www.cdc.gov/alcohol/fact-sheets/prevention.htm (accessed on 1 March 2024).
  7. Winslow, B.T.; Onysko, M.; Hebert, M. Medications for Alcohol Use Disorder. Am. Fam. Physician 2016, 93, 457–465. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  8. Boron, W.F. Regulation of intracellular pH. Adv. Physiol. Educ. 2004, 28, 160–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Boedtkjer, E.; Ara, T. Strengthening the basics: Acids and bases influence vascular structure and function, tissue perfusion, blood pressure, and human cardiovascular disease. Pflug. Arch. 2024, 476, 623–637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Boedtkjer, E. Ion Channels, Transporters, and Sensors Interact with the Acidic Tumor Microenvironment to Modify Cancer Progression. In From Malignant Transformation to Metastasis: Ion Transport in Tumor Biology; Stock, C., Pardo, L.A., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2022; pp. 39–84. [Google Scholar]
  11. Becker, H.M.; Seidler, U.E. Bicarbonate secretion and acid/base sensing by the intestine. Pflug. Arch. 2024, 476, 593–610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Chesler, M. Regulation and modulation of pH in the brain. Physiol. Rev. 2003, 83, 1183–1221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Ruffin, V.A.; Salameh, A.I.; Boron, W.F.; Parker, M.D. Intracellular pH regulation by acid-base transporters in mammalian neurons. Front. Physiol. 2014, 5, 74282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Jo-Watanabe, A.; Inaba, T.; Osada, T.; Hashimoto, R.; Nishizawa, T.; Okuno, T.; Ihara, S.; Touhara, K.; Hattori, N.; Oh-Hora, M.; et al. Bicarbonate signalling via G protein-coupled receptor regulates ischaemia-reperfusion injury. Nat. Commun. 2024, 15, 1530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Park, H.J.; Choi, I.; Leem, K.H. Decreased Brain pH and Pathophysiology in Schizophrenia. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2021, 22, 8358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Umek, N.; Gersak, B.; Vintar, N.; Sostaric, M.; Mavri, J. Dopamine Autoxidation Is Controlled by Acidic pH. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 2018, 11, 467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Wise, R.A. Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2004, 5, 483–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Gass, J.T.; Olive, M.F. Glutamatergic substrates of drug addiction and alcoholism. Biochem. Pharmacol. 2008, 75, 218–265. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  19. Holmes, A.; Spanagel, R.; Krystal, J.H. Glutamatergic targets for new alcohol medications. Psychopharmacology 2013, 229, 539–554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Kumar, S.; Porcu, P.; Werner, D.F.; Matthews, D.B.; Diaz-Granados, J.L.; Helfand, R.S.; Morrow, A.L. The role of GABA(A) receptors in the acute and chronic effects of ethanol: A decade of progress. Psychopharmacology 2009, 205, 529–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Schank, J.R.; Lee, S.; Gonzalez-Islas, C.E.; Nennig, S.E.; Fulenwider, H.D.; Chang, J.; Li, J.M.; Kim, Y.; Jeffers, L.A.; Chung, J.; et al. Increased alcohol consumption in mice lacking sodium bicarbonate transporter NBCn1. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 11017. [Google Scholar]
  22. Boegman, S.; Stellingwerff, T.; Shaw, G.; Clarke, N.; Graham, K.; Cross, R.; Siegler, J.C. The Impact of Individualizing Sodium Bicarbonate Supplementation Strategies on World-Class Rowing Performance. Front. Nutr. 2020, 7, 138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Hilton, N.P.; Leach, N.K.; Hilton, M.M.; Sparks, S.A.; McNaughton, L.R. Enteric-coated sodium bicarbonate supplementation improves high-intensity cycling performance in trained cyclists. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 2020, 120, 1563–1573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Jones, R.L.; Stellingwerff, T.; Artioli, G.G.; Saunders, B.; Cooper, S.; Sale, C. Dose-Response of Sodium Bicarbonate Ingestion Highlights Individuality in Time Course of Blood Analyte Responses. Int. J. Sport Nutr. Exerc. Metab. 2016, 26, 445–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Thiele, T.E.; Navarro, M. “Drinking in the dark” (DID) procedures: A model of binge-like ethanol drinking in non-dependent mice. Alcohol 2014, 48, 235–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Emmett, M. Metabolic Alkalosis: A Brief Pathophysiologic Review. Clin. J. Am. Soc. Nephrol. 2020, 15, 1848–1856. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Do, C.; Vasquez, P.C.; Soleimani, M. Metabolic Alkalosis Pathogenesis, Diagnosis, and Treatment: Core Curriculum 2022. Am. J. Kidney Dis. 2022, 80, 536–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Kahle, L.E.; Kelly, P.V.; Eliot, K.A.; Weiss, E.P. Acute sodium bicarbonate loading has negligible effects on resting and exercise blood pressure but causes gastrointestinal distress. Nutr. Res. 2013, 33, 479–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Mennella, J.A.; Reed, D.R.; Mathew, P.S.; Roberts, K.M.; Mansfield, C.J. “A spoonful of sugar helps the medicine go down”: Bitter masking by sucrose among children and adults. Chem. Senses 2015, 40, 17–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Koob, G.F. Addiction is a Reward Deficit and Stress Surfeit Disorder. Front. Psychiatry 2013, 4, 72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Spanagel, R. Alcoholism: A systems approach from molecular physiology to addictive behavior. Physiol. Rev. 2009, 89, 649–705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Woods, W.A.; Perina, D.G. Alcoholic Ketoacidosis. In Tintinalli’s Emergency Medicine: A Comprehensive Study Guide, 8th ed.; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 2016; Chapter 226. [Google Scholar]
  33. Howard, R.D.; Bokhari, S.R.A. Alcoholic Ketoacidosis; StatPearls: Treasure Island, FL, USA, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  34. Calvo, J.L.; Xu, H.; Mon-López, D.; Pareja-Galeano, H.; Jiménez, S.L. Effect of sodium bicarbonate contribution on energy metabolism during exercise: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Int. Soc. Sports Nutr. 2021, 18, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Heibel, A.B.; Perim, P.H.L.; Oliveira, L.F.; McNaughton, L.R.; Saunders, B. Time to Optimize Supplementation: Modifying Factors Influencing the Individual Responses to Extracellular Buffering Agents. Front. Nutr. 2018, 5, 35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Siegler, J.C.; Midgley, A.W.; Polman, R.C.; Lever, R. Effects of various sodium bicarbonate loading protocols on the time-dependent extracellular buffering profile. J. Strength Cond. Res. 2010, 24, 2551–2557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Sabatini, S.; Kurtzman, N.A. Bicarbonate therapy in severe metabolic acidosis. J. Am. Soc. Nephrol. 2009, 20, 692–695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. DynaMed, Sodium Bicarbonate. In Dynamed, EBSCO Information Services. Available online: https://www.dynamed.com (accessed on 20 April 2024).
  39. Hughes, A.; Brown, A.; Valento, M. Hemorrhagic Encephalopathy From Acute Baking Soda Ingestion. West. J. Emerg. Med. 2016, 17, 619–622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Kenakin, T.P. Pharmacologic Analysis of Drug-Receptor Interaction; Raven Press: New York, NY, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
  41. Blizard, D.A. Sweet and bitter taste of ethanol in C57BL/6J and DBA2/J mouse strains. Behav. Genet. 2007, 37, 146–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Scinska, A.; Koros, E.; Habrat, B.; Kukwa, A.; Kostowski, W.; Bienkowski, P. Bitter and sweet components of ethanol taste in humans. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2000, 60, 199–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Liu, G.; Zong, G.; Doty, R.L.; Sun, Q. Prevalence and risk factors of taste and smell impairment in a nationwide representative sample of the US population: A cross-sectional study. BMJ Open 2016, 6, e013246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Silva, C.S.; Dias, V.R.; Almeida, J.A.; Brazil, J.M.; Santos, R.A.; Milagres, M.P. Effect of Heavy Consumption of Alcoholic Beverages on the Perception of Sweet and Salty Taste. Alcohol Alcohol. 2016, 51, 302–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Volkow, N.D.; Morales, M. The Brain on Drugs: From Reward to Addiction. Cell 2015, 162, 712–725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Huang, R.Q.; Dillon, G.H. Effect of extracellular pH on GABA-activated current in rat recombinant receptors and thin hypothalamic slices. J. Neurophysiol. 1999, 82, 1233–1243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Traynelis, S.F. pH modulation of ligand-gated ion channels. In pH and Brain Function; Kaila, K., Ransom, B.R., Eds.; Wiley-Liss, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  48. Ariansen, J.L.; Heien, M.L.; Hermans, A.; Phillips, P.E.; Hernadi, I.; Bermudez, M.A.; Schultz, W.; Wightman, R.M. Monitoring extracellular pH, oxygen, and dopamine during reward delivery in the striatum of primates. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2012, 6, 36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Bacopoulos, N.G. Dopamine receptors in rat brain regions. Optimal conditions for 3H-agonist binding, pH dependency and lack of inhibition by ascorbic acid. Biochem. Pharmacol. 1982, 31, 3085–3091. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Sheedy, D.; Garrick, T.; Dedova, I.; Hunt, C.; Miller, R.; Sundqvist, N.; Harper, C. An Australian Brain Bank: A critical investment with a high return! Cell Tissue Bank. 2008, 9, 205–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Sheedy, D.; Say, M.; Stevens, J.; Harper, C.G.; Kril, J.J. Influence of liver pathology on markers of postmortem brain tissue quality. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2012, 36, 55–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Ray, S.C.; Baban, B.; Tucker, M.A.; Seaton, A.J.; Chang, K.C.; Mannon, E.C.; Sun, J.; Patel, B.; Wilson, K.; Musall, J. Bet al. Oral NaHCO3 Activates a Splenic Anti-Inflammatory Pathway: Evidence That Cholinergic Signals Are Transmitted via Mesothelial Cells. J. Immunol. 2018, 200, 3568–3586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Kawakami, T.; Koike, A.; Maehara, T.; Hayashi, T.; Fujimori, K. Bicarbonate enhances the inflammatory response by activating JAK/STAT signalling in LPS + IFN-gamma-stimulated macrophages. J. Biochem. 2020, 167, 623–631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Thiele, T.E.; Crabbe, J.C.; Boehm, S.L., 2nd. “Drinking in the Dark” (DID): A simple mouse model of binge-like alcohol intake. Curr. Protoc. Neurosci. 2014, 68, 9–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Sakai, H.; Sagara, A.; Matsumoto, K.; Hasegawa, S.; Sato, K.; Nishizaki, M.; Shoji, T.; Horie, S.; Nakagawa, T.; Tokuyama, S.; et al. 5-Fluorouracil induces diarrhea with changes in the expression of inflammatory cytokines and aquaporins in mouse intestines. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e54788. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Nennig, S.E.; Fulenwider, H.D.; Chimberoff, S.H.; Smith, B.M.; Eskew, J.E.; Sequeira, M.K.; Karlsson, C.; Liang, C.; Chen, J.F.; Heilig, M.; et al. Selective Lesioning of Nuclear Factor-kappaB Activated Cells in the Nucleus Accumbens Shell Attenuates Alcohol Place Preference. Neuropsychopharmacology 2018, 43, 1032–1040. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Martin, K.K.; Noble, D.J.; Parvin, S.; Jang, K.; Garraway, S.M. Pharmacogenetic inhibition of TrkB signaling in adult mice attenuates mechanical hypersensitivity and improves locomotor function after spinal cord injury. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 2022, 16, 987236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Prus, A.J.; James, J.R.; Rosecrans, J.A. Conditioned Place Preference. In Methods of Behavior Analysis in Neuroscience, 2nd ed.; Buccafusco, J.J., Ed.; CRC Press/Taylor & Francis: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  59. Rodriguez, A.; Zhang, H.; Klaminder, J.; Brodin, T.; Andersson, M. ToxId: An efficient algorithm to solve occlusions when tracking multiple animals. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 14774. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Alcohol consumption before and after oral gavage of NaHCO3. Mice at 2 months (8 weeks) (A), 4 months (B), and 14 months (C) were given oral gavage of NaHCO3 (0.25 g/kg body weight) or water and, 1 h later, allowed free access to 20% alcohol. The alcohol-drinking sessions were 2 h on days 1–3 and 4 h on day 4. Alcohol consumption was measured as g/kg body weight. (D) Male vs. female. Alcohol consumption per hour on days 1–4 was compared. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Figure 1. Alcohol consumption before and after oral gavage of NaHCO3. Mice at 2 months (8 weeks) (A), 4 months (B), and 14 months (C) were given oral gavage of NaHCO3 (0.25 g/kg body weight) or water and, 1 h later, allowed free access to 20% alcohol. The alcohol-drinking sessions were 2 h on days 1–3 and 4 h on day 4. Alcohol consumption was measured as g/kg body weight. (D) Male vs. female. Alcohol consumption per hour on days 1–4 was compared. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Ijms 25 05006 g001
Figure 2. Effects of NaHCO3 on alcohol consumption in mice. (A) Effects of NaHCO3 on the consumption of 5–20% alcohol contents in the two-bottle free choice paradigm. Mice (3–4 months) were allowed free access to 5–20% alcohol for 4 days in two-bottle free choice (n = 10). Mice were then allowed free access to 0.3 M NaHCO3 for 6 days, and alcohol consumption was measured again in the continued presence of NaHCO3. Measurements were performed daily at each concentration. (B) Alcohol preference (i.e., alcohol intake/total fluid intake). (C,D) Comparison of alcohol consumption (C) and preference (D) between males and females. Consumption of 20% alcohol was compared (n = 11 males and 7 females). ** p < 0.01.
Figure 2. Effects of NaHCO3 on alcohol consumption in mice. (A) Effects of NaHCO3 on the consumption of 5–20% alcohol contents in the two-bottle free choice paradigm. Mice (3–4 months) were allowed free access to 5–20% alcohol for 4 days in two-bottle free choice (n = 10). Mice were then allowed free access to 0.3 M NaHCO3 for 6 days, and alcohol consumption was measured again in the continued presence of NaHCO3. Measurements were performed daily at each concentration. (B) Alcohol preference (i.e., alcohol intake/total fluid intake). (C,D) Comparison of alcohol consumption (C) and preference (D) between males and females. Consumption of 20% alcohol was compared (n = 11 males and 7 females). ** p < 0.01.
Ijms 25 05006 g002
Figure 3. Dose-dependent effects of NaHCO3. (A) Alcohol consumption at different concentrations of NaHCO3. Mice were given access to the two-bottle free choice with water and 20% alcohol in the presence of 0.05–0.4 M NaHCO3. Both bottles contained NaHCO3 to minimize a potential tastant effect. (B) Consumption of NaHCO3-containing water. (C) Alcohol preference. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Figure 3. Dose-dependent effects of NaHCO3. (A) Alcohol consumption at different concentrations of NaHCO3. Mice were given access to the two-bottle free choice with water and 20% alcohol in the presence of 0.05–0.4 M NaHCO3. Both bottles contained NaHCO3 to minimize a potential tastant effect. (B) Consumption of NaHCO3-containing water. (C) Alcohol preference. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Ijms 25 05006 g003
Figure 4. Effects of NaHCO3 on diarrhea. (A) Diarrhea scores. Feces were collected from mice before and after 0.4 M NaHCO3 administration for 2 days, and diarrhea scores were counted based on watery conditions (n = 8/group). Scores were scaled using the following stool conditions: 0: normal, 1: soft stool, 2: slight wet and soft stool, 3: wet and unformed stool with moderate perianal staining on the coat, and 4: watery stool with severe perianal staining on the coat. (B) Number of feces produced daily before and after NaHCO3 ingestion (n = 10). ns: not significant.
Figure 4. Effects of NaHCO3 on diarrhea. (A) Diarrhea scores. Feces were collected from mice before and after 0.4 M NaHCO3 administration for 2 days, and diarrhea scores were counted based on watery conditions (n = 8/group). Scores were scaled using the following stool conditions: 0: normal, 1: soft stool, 2: slight wet and soft stool, 3: wet and unformed stool with moderate perianal staining on the coat, and 4: watery stool with severe perianal staining on the coat. (B) Number of feces produced daily before and after NaHCO3 ingestion (n = 10). ns: not significant.
Ijms 25 05006 g004
Figure 5. Alcohol consumption after the suppression of NaHCO3 bitterness by sucrose. (A) Fluid consumption in the absence and presence of NaHCO3. The measurements were done before and after 0.3 M NaHCO3 ingestion in a two-bottle free choice (n = 8). (B) Suppression of the bitterness of NaHCO3 by sucrose. Fluid consumption was measured as shown in (A) in the presence of 1% sucrose. (CE) Effects of NaHCO3 on alcohol consumption (C), water consumption (D), and alcohol preference (E) in the presence of 1% sucrose (n = 10). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, ns: not significant.
Figure 5. Alcohol consumption after the suppression of NaHCO3 bitterness by sucrose. (A) Fluid consumption in the absence and presence of NaHCO3. The measurements were done before and after 0.3 M NaHCO3 ingestion in a two-bottle free choice (n = 8). (B) Suppression of the bitterness of NaHCO3 by sucrose. Fluid consumption was measured as shown in (A) in the presence of 1% sucrose. (CE) Effects of NaHCO3 on alcohol consumption (C), water consumption (D), and alcohol preference (E) in the presence of 1% sucrose (n = 10). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, ns: not significant.
Ijms 25 05006 g005
Figure 6. Effects of NaHCO3 on quinine and sucrose sensitivity. (A,B) Quinine and sucrose sensitivity in mice with free access to NaHCO3. Mice were offered 0.01–1 mM quinine hydrochloride and water in a two-bottle choice, and quinine consumption was measured before and after 6 days of ingestion of NaHCO3. Both bottles contained NaHCO3 when measured after NaHCO3 ingestion. Each concentration was offered for 24 h. Quinine preference was determined as a ratio of quinine intake to total fluid intake (n = 5 for each concentration except 10 for 0.01 mM). For sucrose sensitivity, the procedure was similar to that in (A), except that 1% and 10% (w/v) sucrose solutions were used (n = 8). (C,D) Quinine and sucrose sensitivity in mice with oral gavage of NaHCO3. Mice were given NaHCO3 or water by oral gavage and, 1 h later, offered 0.01–1 mM quinine hydrochloride or 1% and 10% sucrose solutions. Mice were allowed to drink for 4 h in the DID procedure (n = 5/group). * p < 0.05.
Figure 6. Effects of NaHCO3 on quinine and sucrose sensitivity. (A,B) Quinine and sucrose sensitivity in mice with free access to NaHCO3. Mice were offered 0.01–1 mM quinine hydrochloride and water in a two-bottle choice, and quinine consumption was measured before and after 6 days of ingestion of NaHCO3. Both bottles contained NaHCO3 when measured after NaHCO3 ingestion. Each concentration was offered for 24 h. Quinine preference was determined as a ratio of quinine intake to total fluid intake (n = 5 for each concentration except 10 for 0.01 mM). For sucrose sensitivity, the procedure was similar to that in (A), except that 1% and 10% (w/v) sucrose solutions were used (n = 8). (C,D) Quinine and sucrose sensitivity in mice with oral gavage of NaHCO3. Mice were given NaHCO3 or water by oral gavage and, 1 h later, offered 0.01–1 mM quinine hydrochloride or 1% and 10% sucrose solutions. Mice were allowed to drink for 4 h in the DID procedure (n = 5/group). * p < 0.05.
Ijms 25 05006 g006
Figure 7. Effects of NaHCO3 on alcohol reward. (A) Timeline of experimental treatment and behavioral measurements in alcohol-induced CPP. Following the 15 min pretest, 5 min conditioning sessions were conducted daily for 3 days, with saline injection in the morning and 2 g/kg alcohol injection 4 h later. One group of mice received a gavage of 0.25 g/kg NaHCO3 one hour prior to alcohol injection, while another group of mice received a gavage of water (n = 7–8/group). In the test session, mice were allowed to explore the chambers for 15 min. (B,C) Time spent in the alcohol-paired chamber during the pretest and test sessions in control mice (B) and NaHCO3-treated mice (C). (D) CPP scores. Scores were calculated by subtracting the time spent in the alcohol-paired chamber in the pretest from that in the test. (E) Distance traveled during the conditioning session. Travel distance was measured for 5 min after injection of saline or alcohol and averaged. (F) Speed (cm/s). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, ns: not significant.
Figure 7. Effects of NaHCO3 on alcohol reward. (A) Timeline of experimental treatment and behavioral measurements in alcohol-induced CPP. Following the 15 min pretest, 5 min conditioning sessions were conducted daily for 3 days, with saline injection in the morning and 2 g/kg alcohol injection 4 h later. One group of mice received a gavage of 0.25 g/kg NaHCO3 one hour prior to alcohol injection, while another group of mice received a gavage of water (n = 7–8/group). In the test session, mice were allowed to explore the chambers for 15 min. (B,C) Time spent in the alcohol-paired chamber during the pretest and test sessions in control mice (B) and NaHCO3-treated mice (C). (D) CPP scores. Scores were calculated by subtracting the time spent in the alcohol-paired chamber in the pretest from that in the test. (E) Distance traveled during the conditioning session. Travel distance was measured for 5 min after injection of saline or alcohol and averaged. (F) Speed (cm/s). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, ns: not significant.
Ijms 25 05006 g007
Table 1. Blood chemistry before and after oral gavage of NaHCO3 (0.25 g/kg body weight).
Table 1. Blood chemistry before and after oral gavage of NaHCO3 (0.25 g/kg body weight).
 Time after NaHCO3 Gavage
Parameters0 h1 h2 h
pH7.357.44 *7.47 **
HCO3, mM21.625.8 *25.4 **
BE, mM−3.83.4 *1.4 **
PCO2, mmHg39.833.234.8
BE: base excess, PCO2: partial CO2 pressure; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, n = 5.
Table 2. Blood chemistry before and after 6-day access to 0.3 M NaHCO3.
Table 2. Blood chemistry before and after 6-day access to 0.3 M NaHCO3.
 WT
Parameters−NaHCO3+NaHCO3
pH7.27.39 *
HCO3, mM17.729.4 *
PCO2, mmHg43.648.2
BE, mM−10.14.2 *
Na, mM151155
K, mM6.13.5 *
Cl, mM118114
BUN, mg/dL23.926.6
HCt, %PCV4847
Hb, g/dL16.316
BE: base excess, PCO2: partial CO2 pressure, BUN: blood urea nitrogen, HCt, hematocrit, PCV: packed cell volume, Hb: hemoglobin, * p < 0.05, n = 4.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Lin, J.; Rivadeneira, A.P.; Ye, Y.; Ryu, C.; Parvin, S.; Jang, K.; Garraway, S.M.; Choi, I. Sodium Bicarbonate Decreases Alcohol Consumption in Mice. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2024, 25, 5006. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms25095006

AMA Style

Lin J, Rivadeneira AP, Ye Y, Ryu C, Parvin S, Jang K, Garraway SM, Choi I. Sodium Bicarbonate Decreases Alcohol Consumption in Mice. International Journal of Molecular Sciences. 2024; 25(9):5006. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms25095006

Chicago/Turabian Style

Lin, Jason, Ana P. Rivadeneira, Yani Ye, Clara Ryu, Shangrila Parvin, Kyeongran Jang, Sandra M. Garraway, and Inyeong Choi. 2024. "Sodium Bicarbonate Decreases Alcohol Consumption in Mice" International Journal of Molecular Sciences 25, no. 9: 5006. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms25095006

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop