Next Article in Journal
Evaluating Two Brief Motivational Interventions for Excessive-Drinking University Students
Previous Article in Journal
“Take It One Dilation at a Time”: Caregiver Perspectives of Postoperative Anal Dilations in Pediatric Patients with Colorectal Conditions
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Developmental Profile in Children Aged 3–6 Years: Down Syndrome vs. Autism Spectrum Disorder

by
Patricia López Resa
1,* and
Esther Moraleda Sepúlveda
2
1
Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud, Universidad de Castilla La Mancha, 45600 Talavera de la Reina, Spain
2
Departamento de Psicología Experimental, Procesos Cognitivos y Logopedia, Facultad de Psicología y Logopedia, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, 28040 Madrid, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2024, 14(5), 380; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14050380
Submission received: 13 February 2024 / Revised: 26 April 2024 / Accepted: 29 April 2024 / Published: 30 April 2024

Abstract

:
This research aims to compare the developmental profiles of children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and children with Down Syndrome (DS) between the ages of 3 and 6 years. The study examines whether these developmental disorders share common developmental milestones or exhibit distinctive characteristics. A total of 43 children, 23 with DS and 20 with ASD, participated in the study. Cognitive and language skills were assessed using standardized tools, including the Battelle Developmental Inventory, Reynell Developmental Language Scales III, and NEPSY-II battery. The results indicated that children with ASD outperformed children with DS in the areas of fine motor skills, gross motor skills, and communication. Additionally, children with ASD demonstrated higher scores in language comprehension and expressive language, compared to children with DS. Significant correlations were found between motor skills and communication abilities. Neuropsychological evaluations revealed significant differences between the two groups in various tasks, such as the comprehension of instructions, body part naming and identification, and recognition of emotions. These findings contribute to our understanding of the similarities and differences between ASD and DS, shedding light on the dissociation between cognition and language and its impact on adaptive functioning in these populations.

1. Introduction

Currently, there is a growing body of evidence highlighting the significance of early childhood in human development, particularly in the domains of cognitive and linguistic development [1]. Given that the acquisition of these skills follows a predictable and common sequence in typically developing (TD) children, we can assert that, in certain instances, there are discernible regular patterns in the milestones of development [2,3,4,5,6]. However, this does not necessarily hold true in neurodevelopmental disorders [7,8], where we observe a range of deviations from the typical sequence of milestone acquisition. This can manifest as a temporary delay in the acquisition sequence, as seen in Down Syndrome [9,10], or as atypicalities in the pattern of acquisition, as observed in autism spectrum disorder [11,12]
Regarding Down Syndrome (hereafter referred to as DS), it is the most common identifiable genetic cause of intellectual disability [13,14]. It is a neurodevelopmental disorder that occurs in 1 in every 732 live births, although its prevalence has decreased over time, due to prenatal diagnosis [15]. Clinically, DS is characterized by its own phenotypic profile [16], variable intellectual disability [16], alterations in development and organic/systemic functioning [17], and impairments in the acquisition and development of communication and language [18]. However, despite a widespread consensus on the existence of developmental alterations in this population, not all domains appear to be equally affected [19]. In this regard, phonological memory, reasoning abilities, motor development, and prelinguistic elements acquisition are particularly compromised [20,21], leading to a cognitive profile that negatively impacts language [22] and adaptive functioning in the DS population [23]. Studies conducted to date have identified strengths in DS, such as socialization, motor skills, and activities of daily living [24,25], as well as weaknesses in communication [23].
On the other hand, autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is defined as a neurodevelopmental condition characterized by deficits in social communication, as well as the presence of restricted interests and repetitive behaviors [26]. It is a heterogeneous set of alterations that emerges in childhood and persists throughout the lifespan of the individual [27]. The worldwide prevalence is approximately 1%, being four times more common in males than females [28,29]. Regarding cognitive level, not all individuals with ASD present associated intellectual disability [30]. Furthermore, numerous studies have shown that most individuals with ASD, regardless of IQ, exhibit peak performance, for example, in block design tasks, and poorer performance in tasks involving verbal comprehension, revealing a discrepancy between verbal and non-verbal intelligence [31,32,33].
Regarding communication competence in ASD, a considerable number of researchers have focused their efforts on delineating the conceptions of “nonverbal” and “minimally verbal” children [34,35], thus demonstrating the existence of great heterogeneity in the overall development of children with ASD [36,37]. Furthermore, some authors report that between 30% and 40% of individuals with ASD do not acquire oral language as a form of communication [38], which negatively interferes with their social development and adaptive functioning [39]. Additionally, some studies have indicated that, while there is a significant correlation between linguistic and cognitive abilities, as well as their impact on adaptive functioning, higher levels of nonverbal intelligence are not always associated with linguistic level [40] or, consequently, better adaptive functioning [41]. On the other hand, Logan et al. [42] demonstrated that frequent delays in the acquisition and development of linguistic skills, such as word imitation, lack of protoconversations, vocabulary production, and responding to one’s own name, are the characteristics that most draw the attention of early age child interlocutors.
Therefore, the findings related to the childhood development of both populations suggest, to some extent at least, a dissociation between cognition and language that interferes with individuals’ adaptive functioning, which becomes particularly evident when comparing the performance of children with Down Syndrome (DS) and ASD [43]. For example, concerning cognition, a study conducted by Adamson et al. [44] found that children with ASD exhibited a deficit in establishing sustained joint attention, whereas, although the DS group could establish joint attention, difficulties arose in conveying desire or intention about a specific object. Regarding communication skills, a study by Sigman et al. [45] found that social interaction, communication, and joint attention skills improved as the chronological age increased, in groups composed of individuals with DS and ASD. Additionally, as the chronological age increased, children in both groups exhibited a greater number of initiations of play behaviors and a decrease in rejecting play requests. However, when the groups were matched for mental age, these differences disappeared, suggesting that cognitive capacity may have influenced these results.
Regarding the assessment of adaptive functioning profiles at early ages, studies such as that by Fidler and Nadel [46] have shown that when children with Down Syndrome (DS) are matched for mental age with other developmental disabilities, they obtain higher scores, demonstrating statistically significant differences compared to other disabilities. In this vein, the work of [47], after evaluating adaptive functioning in children under 36 months with ASD and other developmental disabilities, found lower socialization and communication skills in the group of children with ASD, as well as greater discrepancies between adaptive functioning and mental age.
On the other hand, it appears that individuals with DS may exhibit characteristics similar to those with ASD. For example, Kirchner and Walton [48] compared a group of 44 individuals with ASD, 78 with DS, and 46 with Williams Syndrome (WS), all aged between 6 and 18 years, who were assessed using the Social Communication Questionnaire. The results of the study showed that both individuals with DS and those diagnosed with WS obtained high scores in all symptomatic areas of ASD. Additionally, the DS group scored significantly lower on the subscales of unusual behaviors, social communication, and self-regulation, compared to individuals with ASD, while no statistically significant differences were found in the areas of language and atypical behaviors.
Therefore, the scientific literature has not yet clearly elucidated the similarities and differences between both developmental disorders. Given the limited understanding of the common characteristics of both disorders, the aim of the present research has been to compare the developmental profiles of the population with ASD and the population with DS, during the specific developmental stage of 3 to 6 years. This study aims to observe whether the profiles of both disorders follow common developmental milestones or whether DS and ASD exhibit distinctive characteristics from each other.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

A total of 43 children (29 girls and 14 boys) between the ages of 3 and 6 years, divided into two groups, constituted this study. The first group consisted of 23 children with Down Syndrome (14 girls and 9 boys), with a mean chronological age of 3.75 years (DS = 0.81). The second group consisted of 20 participants diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder Level 2 accompanied by associated intellectual disability (14 girls and 6 boys), with a mean age of 3.61 years (DS = 0.44).
Furthermore, since the participants’ intellectual capacity could explain differences in developmental profiles, the cognitive development level assessed using the Battelle Developmental Inventory was analyzed first. No significant differences were found between the two groups (p < 0.05). Additionally, we ensured that participants did not present comorbid diagnoses, by reviewing assessment and diagnostic reports provided by the parents.

2.2. Instruments

To conduct this study, cognitive and language skills were assessed using various standardized tools. Firstly, the Battelle Developmental Inventory [49] was used to assess the overall development of the children in the study. The inventory consists of 341 items, grouped into the domains of Personal/Social, Adaptive, Motor (gross and fine motor skills), Communication (Receptive and Expressive), and Cognitive. Its purpose is to gather data on strengths and weaknesses in children’s developmental areas (from 0 to 95 months of age), facilitating the creation of personalized intervention programs. Results are expressed in terms of Developmental Age Equivalents for each area, although they can also be converted into Developmental Quotients, to compare results obtained by individuals of different ages. Furthermore, it demonstrates reliability indices for various scores and age ranges, ranging from 0.88 to 0.99.
Secondly, the Reynell Developmental Language Scales III [50] were employed to evaluate language. These scales consist of 62 items divided into the following two scales: comprehension and expression. The comprehension scale assesses various aspects of language, such as preverbal and affective concepts, object naming, question interpretation, and verbal reasoning. On the other hand, the expressive language scale focuses on language structure, vocabulary, and content. This test is suitable for individuals between 18 months and 7 years old and is useful for evaluating both comprehension and expressive language development, facilitating the planning of necessary interventions. Additionally, it provides reliability measures across different score ranges and age brackets, ranging from 0.44 to 0.96.
Lastly, the NEPSY-II battery [51] was used, which allows for the assessment of neuropsychological functioning in children aged 3 to 16 years through 36 tests that are grouped into the following 6 domains: (1) Attention and Executive Function, (2) Language, (3) Memory and Learning, (4) Social Perception, (5) Visuospatial Processing, and (6) Sensorimotor. The NEPSY-II provides internal consistency coefficients and reliability scores for test scores across different age groups, with these coefficients ranging from 0.60 to 0.99. Additionally, inter-rater reliability studies yielded highly positive results, with agreement percentages ranging from 76% to 99%, depending on the subtest.

2.3. Procedure

To initiate the research, contact was initially made with the Down Syndrome Federation of Castilla-La Mancha, as well as various healthcare centers and private clinics in Castilla-La Mancha, Community of Madrid, and Castilla y León, to inform them of the project and to determine their interest in participating in the present study. All contacted centers expressed their willingness to participate in the study. After confirming the subjects for the sample, the participants were sent the informed consent document. Once signed, the work schedule was established, agreeing on the dates of the assessments. All participants were evaluated in four individual sessions of 45 min each. Questionnaires on Quality of Life and Adaptive Functioning were completed by the parents during the first session. The research was conducted with the explicit consent of the parents. All participants had either a diagnosis of Down Syndrome made by a doctor, or a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder previously made by a psychologist. The informed consent and the project were approved by the Ethics Committee in Social Research of the integrated area of Talavera de la Reina, with registration number CAU-683200-X6H7.

2.4. Data Analysis

IBM SPSS 24.0 Statistics Multilingual software was used as the tool for data analysis. To determine the degree of similarity between the sample distribution and the population distribution and, thus, to test the normality of the sample, a goodness-of-fit test was performed using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, obtaining parametric variables (>0.5) and non-parametric variables (<0.5). Additionally, to determine if there were significant differences between the different variables among the two groups within the age range of 3–6 years, Levene’s test for equality of variances and the t-test for equality of means were employed for parametric variables, as they are quantitative variables with a normal distribution. For non-parametric variables, which are quantitative variables without a normal distribution, the Kruskal–Wallis analysis of variance test was used. Spearman’s rho test was utilized to analyze the correlation between different areas for non-parametric variables.

3. Results

The most relevant results regarding the groups of children with DS and ASD are presented below.
Regarding the Battelle Developmental Inventory, as shown in Table 1, children with ASD obtained higher scores in the Adaptive, Fine Motor, Total Motor (Fine Motor + Gross Motor), Communication, Personal, and Cognitive domains, only scoring lower than the group of children with DS in the Gross Motor area. However, significant differences were found in favor of the group of children with ASD in the Gross Motor, Total Motor, and Communication domains (p < 0.05).
On the other hand, data obtained from the Reynell-III Scale (Table 2) illustrated how children with ASD outperformed the group of children with DS, in the areas of Total Score, Age Equivalent in years, Comprehension, and Expression. However, significant differences were found in the expressive domain (p < 0.05).
The results obtained from the correlation analysis between the different assessed areas of both tests (Table 3) revealed a significant correlation between the motor domain and the communication domain, as well as between the motor domain and the expressive level (p < 0.01).
Regarding the results of the neuropsychological evaluation conducted using the NEPSY-II tool, Table 4 presents the descriptive statistics.
We can observe a profile with statistically significant differences between the groups of children with DS and with ASD, respectively. In this regard, significant differences were found in the tasks of comprehension of instructions (F(1, 38) = 3.265; p < 0.01; h2 = 0.079), naming body parts (F(1, 38) = 48.173; p < 0.001; h2 = 0.559), identifying body parts (F(1, 38) = 40.741; p < 0.001; h2 = 0.517), recognition of emotions (F(1, 38) = 6.840; p < 0.01; h2 = 0.153), copying designs (F(1, 38) = 20.987; p < 0.01; h2 = 0.356), and imitation of hand configurations (F(1, 38) = 25.831; p < 0.01; h2 = 0.383), where the group of children with ASD obtained higher scores than the DS group, as well as in the tasks of pseudoword repetition (F(1, 38) = 28.148; p < 0.01; h2 = 0.426) and oromotor sequences (F(1, 38) = 17.255; p < 0.01; h2 = 0.404), where the DS group obtained higher scores. No significant differences were found in the remaining variables, except for theory of mind, which could not be considered because the scores were 0 in all cases.

4. Discussion

The main objective of this study was to investigate the differences that could be found in the developmental profiles between children diagnosed with ASD and DS in the 3–6 years age range, given the scarcity of research conducted in this specific age group. The results, in general, have revealed significant differences in the developmental profiles of both populations.
Specifically, the data obtained from the Batelle Developmental Inventory have demonstrated better motor and communicative skills in the group of children with ASD compared to the group with DS, in contrast to other studies [43]. Regarding the motor component, some studies have shown better motor skill performance in children with ASD, compared to children with DS [52,53,54], which is in line with our data. However, both populations seem to present motor difficulties that arise because of sensory processing difficulties [21,55], such as perceptual–motor coupling [56,57]. For example, authors like Simon et al. [58] found difficulties in performing tasks such as grasping, throwing, and catching, which seemed to affect the establishment of essential interaction formats in language acquisition in a group of young children with DS [59,60]. On the other hand, Tomasello [61] reported the existence of perceptual biases in infants that guide attention towards people and objects, which, along with motor development enabling exploration, lead to the emergence of secondary intersubjectivity towards the end of the first year of life. Therefore, we find some studies suggesting certain implications of a child’s motor development on their language acquisition [62,63,64,65,66], in line with the correlation between the motor component and the communicative–linguistic component found in this study.
Regarding language development specifically, the Reynell Language Development Scales III [50] and the NEPSY-II Battery [51] showed better comprehension of instructions and naming and identifying body parts skills in the group of children with ASD. On the other hand, our group of children with DS obtained higher scores in oromotor sequence tasks and pseudoword repetition. However, nowadays, we can assert that there is some consensus in the scientific literature that ASD does not imply a primary deficit at the phonological level [67]. In this regard, several studies have shown the existence of phonological and articulatory difficulties [68,69]. All these data seem to contradict our results because, while it would be expected based on the literature that children with ASD would obtain better scores in articulatory tasks and tasks involving the acoustic–phonological conversion mechanism, our data indicate that it is the children with DS who obtain higher scores. This can be explained, to some extent, by the difficulties in the acquisition and development of imitation skills characteristic of ASD [70,71]. As such, a study conducted by Stone et al. [72] found that, during the early years of life, motor imitation skills positively correlated with expressive language abilities in children with ASD. In this respect, the data obtained in this research have also reported a greater naming ability in the group of children with ASD compared to the group of children with DS. These data can be interpreted in line with studies reporting that, while the lexical–semantic component seems to be a strong point in children with DS [10,73], it is altered compared to typical development [74]. In the same direction, two meta-analyses, focused on the lexical–semantic skills of children with ASD, have highlighted a delay in vocabulary acquisition compared to typical development [75,76]. All these data suggest that, while both populations present linguistic difficulties compared to typical development, children with ASD seem to have fewer difficulties in decontextualized tasks that do not require the use of environmental information, as required in imitation and repetition tasks [77,78].
It is particularly striking that the group of children with ASD presents higher expressive communicative competence, especially in the 3–6-year-old stage, considering that communication difficulties are one of the distinctive features of this disorder [79]. These results contrast with those of Dardani et al. [80], who reported that children with DS have a higher level of communicative and linguistic skills compared to children with ASD. However, our data can be interpreted in line with studies that have shown a greater impairment in receptive skills compared to expressive skills, in preschool-aged children diagnosed with ASD [81,82]. In this respect, the meta-analysis conducted by Kwok et al. [77] revealed that, although there seems to be an expressive advantage over receptive skills, it is not a common profile within the ASD population, so this discrepancy in language should not be considered a marker of ASD [83,84]. All these findings could explain, at least to some extent, the significant expressive differences between the two groups, as there are several studies that have documented a significant delay in language acquisition in people with DS [85,86]. For example, a study conducted by Buckley and Oliver [87] found that the mean age for the consolidation of two-word combinations in children with DS occurs at around 37 months, presenting a delay of 18 months compared to normotypic development. On the other hand, studies on ASD show even greater discrepancies, specifically due to the variability in the symptomatology of ASD. In this respect, Rose et al. [88] found, in a sample of 246 children diagnosed with ASD, that approximately 36% were not using two-word phrases at 40 months, compared to 64% who were.
Some studies conducted between DS and ASD point to uneven development in different areas of language. For example, Roberts et al. [10] compared language and communication development in children with DS and ASD aged between 3-and-a-half years and 16 years, with similar cognitive abilities. The authors found that children with DS showed better performance in language comprehension and pragmatics, while children with ASD showed better performance in expressive language. On the other hand, a study by Laws, Bishop, and Adams [89] found that children with DS performed better in language comprehension and sentence production, while children with ASD performed better in passive vocabulary and receptive language in general. Similarly, de Falco et al. [90] found that, while children with DS had better pragmatic and linguistic comprehension skills, the sentence production and expressive language of children with ASD showed a higher performance, in line with the findings of this research.
Therefore, the current findings suggest an influence of the severity of prototypical symptomatology in ASD on the relative receptive and expressive language abilities [82,83,91]. In this regard, our esults seem to indicate that, when equating children with DS and children with ASD with intellectual disability (ID) based on cognitive development, children with ASD in the 3–6-year-old stage appear to have a better expressive language capacity. These new findings, taken into consideration along with the information found in the scientific literature on the developmental trajectories of both populations, emphasize the need to open lines of research that focus their efforts on comparing the clinical phenotype of ASD and genetic syndromes such as DS in order to obtain high-quality information that enables the creation of assessment protocols tailored to the needs of each population, as well as effective intervention programs.
However, it is important to highlight that the present study is not without limitations. Firstly, it should be noted that participants in the ASD group were recruited based on the researchers’ review of diagnostic reports. Therefore, the lack of confirmation of the diagnosis could be considered as a confounding variable that may interfere with the results. Secondly, the atypical gender distribution within the ASD group sample is noteworthy. In this regard, while the scientific literature reports a significantly higher prevalence of ASD in males than females, most participants in our study were female. This could potentially complicate the interpretation of the results.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, P.L.R. and E.M.S.; Methodology, P.L.R. and E.M.S.; Formal analysis, P.L.R. and E.M.S.; Writing—original draft, P.L.R. and E.M.S.; Writing—review & editing, P.L.R.; Supervision, E.M.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Social Science Research Ethics Committee with code CEIS-683200-X6H7 and approval date 4 July 2023 for human studies.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are contained within the article.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Britto, P.R.; Lye, S.J.; Proulx, K.; Yousafzai, A.K.; Matthews, S.G.; Vaivada, T.; Perez-Escamilla, R.; Rao, N.; Ip, P.; Fernald, L.C.H.; et al. Nurturing care: Promoting early childhood development. Lancet 2017, 389, 91–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Bishop, D.; Mogford, K. Language Development in Exceptional Circumstances; Psychology Press: Oxfordshire, UK, 2013; Available online: https://books.google.es/books?hl=es&lr=&id=zPp5tJlQxmoC&oi=fnd&pg=PP1&dq=Language+development+in+exceptional+circumstances&ots=wMMkBqYcYB&sig=8UEdVnPbX4FgqLrC0UnNsnW5IQU (accessed on 4 March 2023).
  3. Delis, D.; Ober, B. Cognitive neuropsychology. In Approaches to Cognition; Routledge: Oxfordshire, UK, 2016; pp. 243–266. Available online: https://www.taylorfrancis.com/chapters/edit/10.4324/9781315630816-11/cognitive-neuropsychology-dean-delis-beth-ober (accessed on 4 March 2023).
  4. Korkman, M. Introduction to the Special Issue on Normal Neuropsychological Development in the School-Age Years. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2001, 20, 325–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Wu, Z.; Gros-Louis, J. Infants’ prelinguistic communicative acts and maternal responses: Relations to linguistic development. First Lang. 2014, 34, 72–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Bishop, D.V.; Snowling, M.J.; Thompson, P.A.; Greenhalgh, T.; CATALISE-2 Consortium. Phase 2 of CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: Terminology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2017, 58, 1068–1080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Luyster, R.J.; Seery, A.; Talbott, M.R.; Tager-Flusberg, H. Identifying Early-Risk Markers and Developmental Trajectories for Language Impairment in Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2011, 17, 151–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Fowler, A.; Gelman, R.; Gleitman, L. The course of language learning in children with Down syndrome. In Constraints on Language Acquisition: Studies of Atypical Children; Psychology Press: Oxfordshire, UK, 2014; pp. 91–140. Available online: https://www.taylorfrancis.com/chapters/edit/10.4324/9781315807522-5/course-language-learning-children-syndrome-anne-fowler-rochel-gelman-lila-gleitman (accessed on 4 March 2023).
  9. Roberts, J.E.; Price, J.; Malkin, C. Language and communication development in down syndrome. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2007, 13, 26–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Leonard, H.C.; Bedford, R.; Pickles, A.; Hill, E.L. Predicting the rate of language development from early motor skills in at-risk infants who develop autism spectrum disorder. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2015, 13–14, 15–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Tek, S.; Mesite, L.; Fein, D.; Naigles, L. Longitudinal Analyses of Expressive Language Development Reveal Two Distinct Language Profiles Among Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2014, 44, 75–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Basile, H. Retraso mental y genética Síndrome de Down. Rev. Argent. Clínica Neuropsiquiátrica 2008, 15, 9–23. Available online: http://alcmeon.com.ar/15/57/04_basile.pdf (accessed on 4 March 2023).
  13. Ruparelia, A.; Pearn, M.L.; Mobley, W.C. Aging and intellectual disability: Insights from mouse models of down syndrome. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2013, 18, 43–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Alegre, G.; Anderlini, C.; Sadir, M.; Stehli, C.; Cuestas, E. Diagnóstico prenatal de anomalías cromosómicas. Methodo Investig. Apl. A Cienc. Biológicas 2019, 4, 148–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Crissman, B.G.; Worley, G.; Roizen, N.; Kishnani, P.S. Current perspectives on Down syndrome: Selected medical and social issues. Am. J. Med. Genet. Part C Semin. Med. Genet. 2006, 142, 127–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Hodapp, R.M.; Urbano, R.C. Demographics of African-American vs. European-Heritage Mothers of Newborns With Down Syndrome. Wiley Online Libr. 2008, 5, 187–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Grieco, J.; Pulsifer, M.; Seligsohn, K.; Skotko, B.; Schwartz, A. Down syndrome: Cognitive and behavioral functioning across the lifespan. Am. J. Med. Genet. Part C Semin. Med. Genet. 2015, 169, 135–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Vicari, S. Motor Development and Neuropsychological Patterns in Persons with Down Syndrome. Behav. Genet. 2006, 36, 355–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Abbeduto, L.; Pavetto, M.; Kesin, E.; Weissman, M.; Karadottir, S.; O’Brien, A.; Cawthon, S. The linguistic and cognitive profile of Down syndrome: Evidence from a comparison with fragile X syndrome. Down Syndr. Res. Pract. 2001, 7, 9–15. Available online: https://library.down-syndrome.org/en-gb/research-practice/07/1/linguistic-cognitive-profile-down-syndrome-evidence-from-comparison-fragile-x-syndrome (accessed on 4 March 2023). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Malak, R.; Kostiukow, A.; Krawczyk-Wasielewska, A.; Mojs, E.; Samborski, W. Delays in motor development in children with Down syndrome. Med. Sci. Monit. Int. Med. J. Exp. Clin. Res. 2015, 21, 1904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Ypsilanti, A.; Grouios, G. Linguistic Profile of Individuals with Down Syndrome: Comparing the Linguistic Performance of Three Developmental Disorders. Child Neuropsychol. 2008, 14, 148–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Will, E.A.; Caravella, K.E.; Hahn, L.J.; Fidler, D.J.; Roberts, J.E. Adaptive behavior in infants and toddlers with Down syndrome and fragile X syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. Part B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 2018, 177, 358–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Fidler, D.J.; Most, D.E.; Booth-LaForce, C.; Kelly, J.F. Emerging Social Strengths in Young Children with Down Syndrome. Infants Young-Child. 2008, 21, 207–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Matthews, T.J.; Allain, D.C.; Matthews, A.L.; Mitchell, A.; Santoro, S.L.; Cohen, L. An assessment of health, social, communication, and daily living skills of adults with Down syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. Part A 2018, 176, 1389–1397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. American Psychiatric Association. Manual Diagnóstico y Estadístico de los Trastornos Mentales DSM-V-TR, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  26. Wing, L. Language, social, and cognitive impairments in autism and severe mental retardation. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1981, 11, 31–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Ferri, S.L.; Abel, T.; Brodkin, E.S. Sex Differences in Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Review. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2018, 20, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Kirkovski, M.; Enticott, P.G.; Fitzgerald, P.B. A Review of the Role of Female Gender in Autism Spectrum Disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2013, 43, 2584–2603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Gil-Vera, V.; Amigo, C.U.C.L.; Quintero-Lopez, C.; Velez-Lopez, J.; Gomez-Muñoz, N. Capacidades de aprendizaje en niños con autismo: Un análisis relacional. Rev. Espac. 2020, 41, 341–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Deonna, T.; Ziegler, A.-L.; Maeder, M.-I.; Ansermet, F.; Roulet, E. Reversible behavioural autistic-like regression: A manifestation of a special (new?) epileptic syndrome in a 28-month-old child. A 2-year longitudinal study. Neurocase 1995, 1, 91–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Lai, M.-C.; Lombardo, M.V.; Suckling, J.; Ruigrok, A.N.V.; Chakrabarti, B.; Ecker, C.; Deoni, S.C.L.; Craig, M.C.; Murphy, D.G.M.; Bullmore, E.T.; et al. Biological sex affects the neurobiology of autism. Brain 2013, 136, 2799–2815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Lai, M.-C.; Lombardo, M.V.; Ruigrok, A.N.V.; Chakrabarti, B.; Wheelwright, S.J.; Auyeung, B.; Allison, C.; Baron-Cohen, S.; MRC AIMS Consortium. Cognition in Males and Females with Autism: Similarities and Differences. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e47198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Delehanty, A.D.; Stronach, S.; Guthrie, W.; Slate, E.; Wetherby, A.M. Verbal and nonverbal outcomes of toddlers with and without autism spectrum disorder, language delay, and global developmental delay. Autism Dev. Lang. Impair. 2018, 3, 2396941518764764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Koegel, L.; Bryan, K.; Su, P.; Vaidya, M.; Camarata, S. Definitions of nonverbal and minimally verbal in research for autism: A systematic review of the literature. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2020, 50, 2957–2972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Fusaroli, R.; Weed, E.; Fein, D.; Naigles, L. Hearing me hearing you: Reciprocal effects between child and parent language in autism and typical development. Cognition 2019, 183, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Startin, C.M.; D’souza, H.; Ball, G.; Hamburg, S.; Hithersay, R.; Hughes, K.M.O.; Massand, E.; Karmiloff-Smith, A.; Thomas, M.S.C.; Strydom, A.; et al. Health comorbidities and cognitive abilities across the lifespan in Down syndrome. J. Neurodev. Disord. 2020, 12, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Riva, V.; Caruso, A.; Apicella, F.; Valeri, G.; Vicari, S.; Molteni, M.; Scattoni, M.L. Early developmental trajectories of expressive vocabulary and gesture production in a longitudinal cohort of Italian infants at high-risk for Autism Spectrum Disorder. Autism Res. 2021, 14, 1421–1433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Estes, A.; Swain, D.M.; MacDuffie, K.E. The effects of early autism intervention on parents and family adaptive functioning. Pediatr. Med. 2019, 2, 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Howlin, P.; Savage, S.; Moss, P.; Tempier, A.; Rutter, M. Cognitive and language skills in adults with autism: A 40-year follow-up. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2014, 55, 49–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Kjellmer, L.; Hedvall, Å.; Fernell, E.; Gillberg, C.; Norrelgen, F. Language and communication skills in preschool children with autism spectrum disorders: Contribution of cognition, severity of autism symptoms, and adaptive functioning to the variability. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2012, 33, 172–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Logan, K.; Iacono, T.; Trembath, D. A systematic review of research into aided AAC to increase social-communication functions in children with autism spectrum disorder. Augment. Altern. Commun. 2017, 33, 51–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Hamner, T.; Hepburn, S.; Zhang, F.; Fidler, D.; Rosenberg, C.R.; Robins, D.L.; Lee, N.R. Cognitive Profiles and Autism Symptoms in Comorbid Down Syndrome and Autism Spectrum Disorder. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2020, 41, 172–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Adamson, L.B.; Bakeman, R.; Deckner, D.F.; Romski, M. Joint Engagement and the Emergence of Language in Children with Autism and Down Syndrome. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2009, 39, 84–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Sigman, M.; Ruskin, E.; Arbelle, S.; Corona, R. Continuity and Change in the Social Competence of Children with Autism, Down Syndrome, and Developmental Delays. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 1999, 64, 1–139. Available online: https://www.jstor.org/stable/3181510?casa_token=YvDUxIdLU5gAAAAA:7t890naY_k4JZle9be3HClqocDnOiy4wOVHyw3kw4Zv0OzcdqIEcHnMo929d-daHS-JrwdT1pGcmLNRey2scNAl8q5L13bNi_EEV3PspEOipu4aTPp0 (accessed on 4 March 2023). [CrossRef]
  45. Fidler, D.J.; Nadel, L. Education and children with Down syndrome: Neuroscience, development, and intervention. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2007, 13, 262–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Stone, W.L.; Coonrod, E.E.; Ousley, O.Y. Brief Report: Screening Tool for Autism in Two-Year-Olds (STAT): Development and Preliminary Data. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2000, 30, 607–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Kirchner, R.M.; Walton, K.M. Symptoms of Autism Spectrum Disorder in Children With Down Syndrome and Williams Syndrome. Am. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2021, 126, 58–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Sanz López, Y.; Guijarro Granados, T.; Sánchez Vázquez, V. Inventario de Desarrollo Battelle como instrumento de ayuda diagnóstica en el autismo. Rev. Asoc. Española Neuropsiquiatría 2007, 27, 31–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Reynell, J.; Gruber, C. Reynell Developmental Language Scales III; Western Psychological Services: Torrance, CA, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  50. Korkman, M.; Kirk, U.; Kemp, S. NEPSY-II, Batería Neuropsicológica Infantil: Manual de Aplicación y Corrección; Spanish Adaptation by Pearson Clinical; Pearson Clinical: Madrid, Spain, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  51. Adamson, L.B.; Deckner, D.F.; Bakeman, R. Early Interests and Joint Engagement in Typical Development, Autism, and Down Syndrome. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2010, 40, 665–676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Ozonoff, S.; Macari, S.; Young, G.S.; Goldring, S.; Thompson, M.; Rogers, S.J. Atypical object exploration at 12 months of age is associated with autism in a prospective sample. Autism 2008, 12, 457–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Rodrigue, J.R.; Morgan, S.B.; Geffken, G.R. A comparative evaluation of adaptive behavior in children and adolescents with autism, Down syndrome, and normal development. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1991, 21, 187–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Schoen, S.A.; Lane, S.J.; Mailloux, Z.; May-Benson, T.; Parham, L.D.; Roley, S.S.; Schaaf, R.C. A systematic review of ayres sensory integration intervention for children with autism. Autism Res. 2019, 12, 6–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. ElMaksoud, G.; AbdElmonem, A.; RezkAllah, S. Effects of individual and group Sensory- Perceptual Motor Training on Motor Proficiency and Quality of Life in Children with Down Syndrome. Int. J. Ther. Rehabilitation Res. 2016, 5, 37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Virji-Babul, N.; Kerns, K.; Zhou, E.; Kapur, A.; Shiffrar, M. Perceptual-motor deficits in children with Down syndrome: Implications for intervention. Down Syndr. Res. Pract. 2006, 10, 74–82. Available online: https://library.down-syndrome.org/en-gb/research-practice/10/2/perceptual-motor-deficits-down-syndrome-implications-intervention/ (accessed on 7 March 2023). [CrossRef]
  57. Simon, D.; Elliott, D.; Anson, J. Perceptual-motor behaviour in children with Down syndrome. In Development of Movement Coordination in Children: Applications in the Fields of Ergonomics, Health Sciences and Sport; Routledge: Oxfordshire, UK, 2003; pp. 133–155. Available online: https://books.google.es/books?hl=es&lr=&id=GlAqSYuLy0AC&oi=fnd&pg=PA133&dq=Charlton+et+al.+(2000+down+syndrome&ots=OmqLBDeZQ8&sig=IYDPbl1jlbkaHjo2UdOvaHfKDo4 (accessed on 4 March 2023).
  58. Bruner, J. De la comunicación al lenguaje: Una perspectiva psicológica. J. Study Educ. Dev. 1981, 4, 133–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Oyarbide, M.A. Jerome Seymour Bruner: De la percepción al lenguaje. Rev. Iberoam. Educ. 2004, 34, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Tomasello, M. How children come to understand false beliefs: A shared intentionality account. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2018, 115, 8491–8498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Alvarado, P.F.; Onandia-Hinchado, I. Perfil Cognitivo Del Trastorno Del Espectro Autista en Población Infantojuvenil: Una Revisión Sistemática. Rev. Psicol. Clínica Niños Adolesc. 2022, 9, 36–49. Available online: https://www.revistapcna.com/sites/default/files/2236_1_0_0.pdf (accessed on 8 March 2023).
  62. Martos, J.; Ayuda, R. Desarrollo temprano: Algunos datos procedentes del autismo y los trastornos del lenguaje. Rev. Neurol. 2004, 38, 39–46. Available online: https://sid-inico.usal.es/idocs/F8/ART13842/desarrollo_temprano,_autismoy_tel.pdf (accessed on 4 March 2023). [CrossRef]
  63. Mora, P.; Cano, S.; García, A. Retos y estrategias para la convivencia: El síndrome de asperger y el contexto escolar y familiar. Rev. Orientación Educ. 2013, 27, 51–62. Available online: https://dialnet.unirioja.es/servlet/articulo?codigo=5509188 (accessed on 7 March 2023).
  64. Suárez-Yepes, N.; Sourdis, M.; Harb, S.; Reyes-Aragón, C. Efecto de un programa de estimulación de la conciencia fonológica en niños preescolares: Sensibilidad a la rima ya la segmentación. Psicogente 2019, 22, 236–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Ternera, L. Importancia del desarrollo motor en relación con los procesos evolutivos del lenguaje y la cognición en niños de 3 a 7 años de la ciudad de Barranquilla. Salud Uninorte 2010, 26, 65–76. Available online: https://www.redalyc.org/pdf/817/81715089008.pdf (accessed on 7 March 2023).
  66. Cantwell, D.P.; Baker, L.; Rutter, M.; Mawhood, L. Infantile autism and developmental receptive dysphasia: A comparative follow-up into middle childhood. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1989, 19, 19–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Wolk, L.; Edwards, M.L.; Brennan, C. Phonological difficulties in children with autism: An overview. Speech Lang. Hear. 2016, 19, 121–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Kent, R.D.; Vorperian, H.K.; Quinn, E.D.; Kaiser, A.P.; Ledford, J.R.; Carl, M.; Kent, R.D.; Levy, E.S.; Whalen, D.H.; Wild, A.; et al. Speech Impairment in Down Syndrome: A Review. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2013, 56, 178–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Stoel-Gammon, C. Down syndrome phonology: Developmental patterns and intervention strategies. Down Syndr. Res. Pract. 2001, 7, 93–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Charman, T.; Baron-Cohen, S. Another look at imitation in autism. Dev. Psychopathol. 1994, 6, 403–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Vivanti, G.; Hamilton, A. Imitation in autism spectrum disorders. In Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders: Diagnosis, Development, and Brain Mechanisms; Volkmar, S., Rogers, R., Pelphrey, K., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons, Inc.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2014; pp. 278–301. [Google Scholar]
  72. Stone, W.L.; Ousley, O.Y.; Littleford, C.D. Motor imitation in young children with autism: What’s the object? J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 1997, 25, 475–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Deckers, A.; Muris, P.; Roelofs, J. Being on Your Own or Feeling Lonely? Loneliness and Other Social Variables in Youths with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2017, 48, 828–839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Zampini, L.; D’Odorico, L. Vocabulary development in children with Down syndrome: Longitudinal and cross-sectional data. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2013, 38, 310–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Belteki, Z.; Lumbreras, R.; Fico, K.; Haman, E.; Junge, C. The Vocabulary of Infants with an Elevated Likelihood and Diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Infant Language Studies Using the CDI and MSEL. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 1469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Kwok, E.Y.; Brown, H.M.; Smyth, R.E.; Cardy, J.O. Meta-analysis of receptive and expressive language skills in autism spectrum disorder. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2015, 9, 202–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Gillam, S.L.; Gillam, R.B.; Reece, K. Language Outcomes of Contextualized and Decontextualized Language Intervention: Results of an Early Efficacy Study. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2012, 43, 276–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. de Souza, J.M.; Veríssimo, M.d.L.R. Child development: Analysis of a new concept. Rev. Latino-Americana de Enferm. 2015, 23, 1097–1104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Lane, J.D.; Lieberman-Betz, R.; Gast, D.L. An Analysis of Naturalistic Interventions for Increasing Spontaneous Expressive Language in Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder. J. Spéc. Educ. 2016, 50, 49–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Dardani, C.; Schalbroeck, R.; Madley-Dowd, P.; Jones, H.J.; Strelchuk, D.; Hammerton, G.; Croft, J.; Sullivan, S.A.; Zammit, S.; Selten, J.-P.; et al. Childhood Trauma As a Mediator of the Association Between Autistic Traits and Psychotic Experiences: Evidence From the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children Cohort. Schizophr. Bull. 2023, 49, 364–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Hudry, K.; Leadbitter, K.; Temple, K.; Slonims, V.; McConachie, H.; Aldred, C.; Howlin, P.; Charman, T.; The PACT Consortium. Preschoolers with autism show greater impairment in receptive compared with expressive language abilities. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2010, 45, 681–690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Luyster, R.J.; Kadlec, M.B.; Carter, A.; Tager-Flusberg, H. Language Assessment and Development in Toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2008, 38, 1426–1438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Cohen, D.; Volkmar, F. Autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 1997, 10, 282–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Mitchell, S.; Cardy, J.O.; Zwaigenbaum, L. Differentiating Autism Spectrum Disorder from Other Developmental Delays In The First Two Years Of Life. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2011, 17, 130–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Abbeduto, L.; Warren, S.F.; Conners, F.A. Language development in Down syndrome: From the prelinguistic period to the acquisition of literacy. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2007, 13, 247–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Mundy, P. Joint attention and social-emotional approach behavior in children with autism. Dev. Psychopathol. 1995, 7, 63–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Buckley, S.; Oliver, B. The language development of children with Down syndrome: First words to two-word phrases. Down Syndr. Res. Pract. 1994, 2, 71–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Rose, V.; Trembath, D.; Keen, D.; Paynter, J. The proportion of minimally verbal children with autism spectrum disorder in a community-based early intervention programme. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2016, 60, 464–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Laws, G.; Bishop, D.V. A comparison of language abilities in adolescents with Down syndrome and children with specific language impairment. JSLHR 2003, 46, 1324–1339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. De Falco, S.; Esposito, G.; Venuti, P.; Bornstein, M.H. Fathers’ play with their Down Syndrome children. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2008, 52, 490–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Charman, T.; Baron-Cohen, S.; Swettenham, J.; Baird, G.; Drew, A.; Cox, A. Predicting language outcome in infants with autism and pervasive developmental disorder. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2003, 38, 265–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Descriptive statistics obtained in the Battelle Scale.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics obtained in the Battelle Scale.
DomainMeans for DS GroupMeans for ASD Groupp-Value
Adaptive domain68.74 (16.12)74.42 (7.44)0.251
Gross motor domain44.39 (33.47)33.84 (21.89)0.005 *
Fine motor domain27.48 (12.77)36.53 (3.61)0.113
Total motor domain52.22 (38.79)76.47 (22.94)0.011 *
Communication domain46.91 (17.32)56.05 (7.92)0.006 *
Personal domain110.13 (27.53)117.47 (9.79)0.276
Cognitive domain28.74 (21.29)41.05 (16.70)0.197
Note: Standard deviations in parentheses. * p > 0.005.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics obtained in the Reynell-III Scale.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics obtained in the Reynell-III Scale.
DomainMeans for DS GroupMeans for ASD Groupp-Value
Total score321.04 (107.12)372.05 (33.47)0.168
Age Equivalent in years2.57 (1.02)2.97 (0.33)0.187
Comprehension23.74 (11.81)34.74 (8.14)0.078
Expression6.48 (3.32)6.89 (2.82)0.02 *
Note: Standard deviations in parentheses. * p < 0.05.
Table 3. Correlations between the Battelle Developmental Inventory and the Reynell-III Scale.
Table 3. Correlations between the Battelle Developmental Inventory and the Reynell-III Scale.
Adaptative Domain (BDI)Gross Motor Domain (BDI)Fine Motor Domain (BDI)Total Motor Domain (BDI)Communication Domain (BDI)Personal Domain (BDI)Cognitive Domain (BDI)Comprehension (RS)Expression (RS)
DS group
   Adaptative domain (BDI)
   Gross motor domain (BDI) *
   Fine motor domain (BDI)
   Total motor domain (BDI) ** **
   Communication domain (BDI) * **
   Personal domain (BDI)
   Cognitive domain (BDI)
   Comprehension (RS)
   Expression (RS) * **
ASD group
   Adaptative domain (BDI)
   Gross motor domain (BDI) *
   Fine motor domain (BDI)
   Total motor domain (BDI) ** **
   Communication domain (BDI) * **
   Personal domain (BDI)
   Cognitive domain (BDI)
   Comprehension (RS)
   Expression (RS) * **
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
Table 4. Descriptive statistics obtained from the NEPSY-II assessment.
Table 4. Descriptive statistics obtained from the NEPSY-II assessment.
DomainMeans for DS GroupMeans for ASD Groupp-Value
Attention and Executive FunctionStatue8.85 (0.36)8.35 (1.18)0.213
Inhibition3.00 (0.75)4.90 (1.74)0.325
LanguageComprehension of Instructions3.00 (0.72)4.90 (5.00)<0.01 *
Naming Body Parts2.65 (2.76)7.50 (1.47)<0.001 *
Identifying Body Parts1.85 (2.03)5.00 (0.85)<0.001 *
Semantic Verbal Fluency2.90 (0.30)2.85 (0.67)0.065
Phonological Verbal Fluency1.05 (0.22)1.25 (0.55)0.140
Phonological Processing1.35 (1.60)1.20 (1.06)0.123
Pseudoword Repetition22.25 (2.63)7.10 (3.12)0.423
Oromotor Sequences4.55 (1.63)2.65 (1.22)0.185
Naming Speed1.50 (0.88)1.70 (0.65)0.303
Memory and learningFace Memory4.10 (1.16)6.90 (1.86)0.215
Design Memory6.50 (2.48)8.70 (2.25)0.314
Narrative Memory4.05 (0.22)3.55 (0.51)0.145
Word List Interference2.40 (0.82)3.50 (0.70)0.185
Phrase Repetition1.75 (1.16)2.50 (1.27)0.723
Social perceptionRecognition of Emotions0.20 (0.61)1.10 (1.41)<0.01 *
Theory of Mind0 (0)0 (0)-
Visuospatial ProcessingBlock Construction2.45 (1.27)2.10 (0.78)0.303
Design Copying3.60 (0.99)4.90 (0.78)<0.01 *
SensorimotorImitation of Hand Configurations4.00 (0)5.10 (0.96)<0.01 *
Visuomotor Precision2.80 (0.89)3.20 (1.70)0.359
Note: Standard deviations in parentheses. * p < 0.05.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

López Resa, P.; Moraleda Sepúlveda, E. Developmental Profile in Children Aged 3–6 Years: Down Syndrome vs. Autism Spectrum Disorder. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 380. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14050380

AMA Style

López Resa P, Moraleda Sepúlveda E. Developmental Profile in Children Aged 3–6 Years: Down Syndrome vs. Autism Spectrum Disorder. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(5):380. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14050380

Chicago/Turabian Style

López Resa, Patricia, and Esther Moraleda Sepúlveda. 2024. "Developmental Profile in Children Aged 3–6 Years: Down Syndrome vs. Autism Spectrum Disorder" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 5: 380. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14050380

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop