Next Article in Journal
Universities and Epistemology: From a Dissolution of Knowledge to the Emergence of a New Thinking
Next Article in Special Issue
Techno-Mathematical Discourse: A Conceptual Framework for Analyzing Classroom Discussions
Previous Article in Journal
Learning and Living Overseas: Exploring Factors that Influence Meaningful Learning and Assimilation: How International Students Adjust to Studying in the UK from a Socio-Cultural Perspective
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Variations of Reasoning in Equal Sharing of Children Who Experience Low Achievement in Mathematics: Competence in Context

by
Jessica Hunt
1,*,
Arla Westenskow
2 and
Patricia S. Moyer-Packenham
2
1
Teacher Education and Learning Sciences, North Carolina State University, 2310 Stinson Drive, Raleigh, NC 27695-6698, USA
2
Emma Eccles Jones College of Education and Human Services, Utah State University, 2605 Old Main Hill, Logan, UT 84322-2605, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Educ. Sci. 2017, 7(1), 37; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci7010037
Submission received: 10 January 2017 / Accepted: 28 February 2017 / Published: 3 March 2017
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Critical Issues in Mathematics Education)

Abstract

:
For children with persistent mathematics difficulties, research and practice espouses that an altered kind of mathematics instruction is necessary due to sustained performance differences. Yet, a critical issue in mathematics education rests in the question of why research locates the problem within these children. In this paper, we challenge a longstanding assumption about the type of mathematics children with low achievement in mathematics “need” along with how these children are positioned in terms of mathematical thinking and reasoning. Our aim in this work is to identify ways of reasoning evident in the partitioning activity of 43 fifth-grade children as they solved equal sharing situations independent of instruction over ten sessions. Results reveal three themes of reasoning that show a resemblance between these children’s reasoning and existing frameworks of reasoning in equal sharing problems found in prior research among children who did not show low achievement in mathematics. We discuss the results in terms of the problem of a continued conceptualization of low achieving students’ need for specific kinds of teaching and learning experiences and/or detached instructional experiences in school. We advocate for an increase in research that examines how teachers can support participation of these children in mathematics classrooms such that children might develop powerful mathematics conceptions.

1. Introduction

This paper illustrates variations of mathematical reasoning of 43 elementary school children identified as having low achievement in mathematics as evidenced through their problem solving in equal sharing tasks. Addressing this problem is important because illustrating the ways of reasoning these children do possess (as opposed to describing what they do not know or a level of performance they do not have) can inform the field about the potentially rich mathematics in which these children can engage. Such information can serve as a beginning to conversations concerning how instruction might be leveraged to support these children’s participation and engagement in important mathematics content and practices. As participation in mathematical practices hold implications for children’s later mathematics performance [1], beginning conversations on how researchers and teachers might support such participation seems essential.
Unfortunately, these conversations stand in sharp contrast to much of the current special education literature base and recommendations for mathematics instruction for children who experience sustained low achievement in mathematics (e.g., see [2]). Despite the fact that some researchers have long supported the view that children may be, in actuality, disabled by curriculum and school structures (e.g., [3,4]) research clearly illustrates that mathematics instruction in classrooms designated for these children has been dominated by explicit instruction and practice computing basic facts [5,6]. Moreover, a recent review [7] of articles researching the mathematics learning of Kindergarten through 12th grade students found significant differences between the mathematical teaching practices used with children with and without sustained low mathematics achievement. Mathematical teaching and learning is informed largely through constructivist and sociocultural perspectives with children without an achievement or learning difference. For children with low achievement or learning differences, mathematical teaching and learning is informed primarily by medical and behavioral perspectives. The research suggests two categories of mathematics learners who “need” different kinds of mathematics [7]. Consequently, rather than discuss how to increase the participation of these children in mathematics instruction that might work to build powerful mathematics conceptions, current research and policy suggests a replacement of participation with more directive teaching approaches [8].
We argue that attempts to remediate and “fix” children with procedural training is a poor replacement for supporting children to develop powerful mathematical conceptions [9]. In fact, if the goal is children’s development of mathematics competence, then such competence involves both procedural fluency and conceptual understanding, and concepts cannot be imposed onto children [10]. In this way, we argue that positioning some children as “normal” and others as “deficient” due to arbitrary cut-off scores and then delivering mathematics onto them does little to uncover the knowledge these children do possess and can build from in the classroom. Instead, we propose that researchers seeking to increase these children’s mathematics competence might begin by uncovering the conceptions that already exist and can be cultivated as children “solve problems that are within [their] reach [while] grappling with key mathematical ideas that are comprehendible but not yet well formed” [11] (p. 387).
In this study, we begin such a documentation by examining ways of reasoning for 43 children with low achievement as they solved fraction problems. Specifically, we present themes of reasoning evident in children’s partitioning activity in problem contexts not directly taught by the teacher (i.e., equal sharing contexts) over 10 sessions of instruction. Our aim in this work is to continue to challenge a longstanding assumption about the type of mathematics children with low achievement “need” [2,8] and their potential as mathematical learners. The following research questions guided our work:
(1)
What is the fractional reasoning of 43 children with low mathematical achievement as evidenced by their partitioning activity used in equal sharing problems over ten occurrences?
(2)
What differences, if any, seem apparent in the children’s reasoning from what we know about children without low mathematics achievement?

2. Conceptual Framework

Although there are many ways to capture children’s ways of reasoning with fractions, we used children’s partitioning activity when creating unit fractions, revealed through their problem solving, as a way to frame the current study. Specifically, children’s activity within equal sharing situations—equally sharing some number of the same-sized objects among some number of people, where the result is a fractional quantity—has provided observable evidence of children’s fractional reasoning in previous research [12,13,14,15,16]. Additionally, the partitioning activity that children use in these situations is well established as the root of children’s knowledge of fractions [15]. In this way, children’s work in equal sharing situations provides a window into the ways of reasoning that children do indeed possess. Moreover, equal sharing is an informal analogy not only for fractions, but also for the “big idea” of equal partitioning, which serves as the conceptual basis for partitive and measurement division, measurement, even numbers, and mean averages [17]. Below, we synthesize prior research on partitioning within equal sharing situations for children who do not experience low performance in mathematics and use it as a conceptual framework for the current study.

3. Children’s Reasoning Revealed in Equal Sharing

Children’s activity within equal sharing situations first appears as representations of acts of partitioning [12,13,14,15,18]. Literature suggests that in early experiences with fractions children may see the problem as unsolvable, possibly because they do not yet see wholes as divisible [14]. For example, when sharing five sticks of licorice equally among four people, some children may begin by adding more whole objects to obtain a whole number result (i.e., add three more sticks such that each person receives one whole stick of licorice) or they may create unequal shares (i.e., give one person two sticks of licorice, [13]). Empson and her colleagues [13] refer to this way of reasoning as “no coordination” because the children are attending to either the necessity to make shares equal or the need to share everything (unequally), but not both at the same time.
Children who accept the whole as divisible may begin using a rudimentary knowledge of whole number counting to partition the licorice stick(s) into some number of pieces [12,15]. This is to say that children determine the number of pieces they will create within their activity. Steffe and Olive [15] describe children who might consider the final stick by partitioning it into pieces that are likely unequal; these children may not be all that concerned with sharing the entire licorice stick. In other words, the children do not yet determine the number of parts needed to exhaust the wholes from the onset [14] and may be in the midst of developing their whole number understanding [15].
Other children may repeatedly halve the final stick until they have enough parts to deal out to each sharer [18]. When asked to show only one share, these children may work to make four equal parts but not use the entire stick or they may use the entire stick but not make equal parts [15]. In each case, children have two goals: to make four equal parts or to use the whole stick, yet they do not know how to coordinate the two goals to exhaust the whole with equal sized parts beyond repeated halving. Thus, they may not yet see the parts in relation to the whole. In fact, for these children, the parts may not be differentiated from the whole. Empson et al. [13] calls this way of reasoning “non-anticipatory” because, while the student is now attending to both the need to exhaust all items to be shared and make the shares equal, the partitioning and subsequent naming of the fractional quantity produced is not associated with a relation, or coordination, between the number of sharers and the amount being shared.
Over time, children begin to coordinate their two goals of making equal sized parts and exhausting the whole, and their partitioning becomes planned prior to activity. The child begins to use the number of sharers as an a priori plan to create a predetermined number of parts to exhaust the shared items [13]. Unitizing one whole, the child may eventually plan to cut each stick into four parts [14]. The mentally-planned partitioning may no longer be an act of counting and is plausibly supported by a developing notion of composite units (i.e., “four” as four units of one and one unit of four). Steffe and Olive [15] refer to this regular coordination of equal parts within the whole as the first true instance of partitioning. Yet, to the child, the value of the fractional parts remains tied to an empirical representation of a partitioned part out of some contextualized whole [14,15]. In other words, the child’s notion of fractions is not yet useful as a quantity.
Children’s notions of unit fractions begin to solidify as they continually coordinate making equal parts with exhausting the whole with larger numbers of sharers and begin to understand that they can repeat one of the parts they created to remake the whole [15] or other non-unit fractions [e.g., sharing four sticks among six people is (1/6) + (1/6) + (1/6) + (1/6), or (4/6)]. In fact, sustained work in equal sharing situations yields an anticipation of partitioning across wholes [13,15]. That is, children may use a developed or developing multiplicative reasoning to spread coordination of parts across the wholes (e.g., in a situation involving four items and six sharers, children might consider a subgroup of two sticks each cut into three parts, then repeat the action so that each of the six sharers receives two-thirds of a stick of licorice). That is, children’s partitioning activity becomes “distributive” [13].
Despite the depth of information contained in the mathematics education literature on the fractional reasoning of children without low achievement, there is a dearth of similar information on children with low mathematics achievement [19]. Hackenberg [19] suggests from her work with six children with low mathematics achievement that their fractional reasoning was consistent with prior research, although the children had not yet developed more sophisticated ways of reasoning that supported a robust knowledge. We hypothesize we may uncover similar results; further, we conjecture that a variance of reasoning will be documented, from early to more advanced ways of considering equal shares.
Our hypothesized result may be viewed by some as reifying what many may consider to be “common sense”. We assert, however, that such results might have important implications in terms of the problem of a continued conceptualization of low achieving students as “different” in terms of their need for specific kinds of teaching and learning experiences and/or detached instructional experiences in school. A description of the participants, data gathering, and analysis procedures follows.

4. Methods

4.1. Participant Selection and Setting

A total of 43 fifth-grade children (N = 43) from four suburban schools in a Western United States school district participated in the study. The selection process was as follows. First, a larger population of 182 children in the participating classrooms across the four schools completed a pretest dealing largely with equivalent fractions. Children’s scores ranged from 5% correct to 100% with a mean score of 51.1%. Next, children who both scored below 40% on the pretest and were identified by teachers as having low achievement in mathematics, were asked to participate in the study (44 children). At the request of several classroom teachers, eight additional children who had experienced low mathematics achievement in the past, yet who scored between 42% and 46% on the fraction pretest, were included in the sample. This increased the sample size to 52 children. Then, permission slips were distributed; 45 children received parental permission. Finally, the 45 children were assigned to intervention groups consisting of two to four children and one instructor. Forty-three of those children participated in both pre- and post-testing and constituted the final sample. There were two different instructors that taught the intervention; both had over 25 years of teaching experience.
A note is offered concerning the participants. Some of the more commonly used terms for children who demonstrate low achievement in mathematics are mathematical disabilities, mathematical learning disabilities, and dyscalculia [20]. These three terms are typically used to describe the same population: children who have been or could be identified as having a disability and qualify to receive special education services.
In contrast, the term mathematical learning difficulty encompasses children whose learning difficulties may be environmental and specific to certain topics as opposed to a broad difficulty with mathematics as a whole. The term is often used to describe all children below a certain percentile on a mathematical achievement test. It implies not necessarily a disability, but low mathematical performance in a domain or domains [21]. In Response to Intervention (RtI) literature, these children are conceptualized as those who have demonstrated low performance in whole class instruction, thus needing additional support in mathematics, yet not necessarily in need of individualized intervention [22]. This is the definition utilized in this study. That is, children involved in the current study experienced two prior years of instruction in fractions largely based in (a) shading pre-partitioned circular and square models of wholes; (b) attaching relevant mathematics vocabulary; and (c) employing mathematical procedures, yet these children showed sustained low performance on a pretest of fraction equivalence. None of the students had qualified for, or were receiving, special education services.

4.2. Data Source

Background. The data analyzed and reported on in this paper were taken from a larger project investigating equivalent fraction learning variations of children who experienced low achievement in mathematics when using virtual and physical manipulatives [23]. The intervention used as a basis for the larger study consisted of 10 lessons from the Rational Number Project: Initial Fraction Ideas Lessons [24] with minor adaptations to accommodate differences for the use of physical or virtual manipulatives during the lesson [23]. Lessons lasted one hour each and were delivered for 10 consecutive school days. The lessons were selected because they were designed and tested for elementary and middle grades children and have been used successfully in both regular and intervention school settings. The focus of the ten lessons was on naming and comparing fractions and the development of equivalent fractions using region and set models.
The first two intervention lessons addressed naming of fractions by having children name parts of region (square and circle) models and to develop and name fractional amounts of the strips. In lessons three and four, children compared wholes of models (square, circle, and paper strip) already partitioned into different amounts. In lessons five, six, and seven, children worked with equivalence concepts by comparing fraction models (squares, circles, and fraction strips) with the same model already partitioned into different sizes (1/2 = 2/4). In the final three lessons, children named fractions and created equivalent fractions using a set model representation (two-colored counters). For each lesson, students participated as a small group in inquiry-based instruction and activities. During the final 15 min, students worked individually and used either virtual or physical manipulatives to practice concepts (see [25] for a detailed description of the lessons, manipulatives, and activities).
Primary data source. To track students’ progress, researchers administered a daily fraction assessment. The daily assessments were used only as a research and assessment tool and not a learning tool. Although none of the lesson activities included instruction based in equal sharing, each daily assessment included a question asking students to engage in equal sharing. The question involved asking students to consider the quantity that would result if n items were shared among m people. In this study, we analyze student responses to the equal share question. The students did not receive any feedback on the correctness of their responses and results were not used to modify instruction.
All equal sharing questions used the same format with only the amounts being changed (i.e., You have n pizzas which you want to share with friends. Including yourself there are m people. How much pizza will each person receive? Draw your work.). Although included as a question on the daily assessment, the equal sharing problem tasks or the context of fractions as equal sharing division was not included in the intervention in any part of any lesson (see description of intervention above). Children never partitioned quantities themselves during the lessons nor did they take part in equal sharing situations; the context of fractions as a result of equal sharing an item or items was not a part of the intervention lessons.

4.3. Data Analysis Procedure

To analyze the data, researchers employed a constant comparative approach to delineate the ways of reasoning children used to solve the equal sharing problems through examining two indicators evident in the children’s work: the employed partitioning strategy and associated representation. First, the second author gathered and organized all daily assessments from all 43 children taken across the 10 intervention sessions. The first and second author met as a team and examined the full set of the children’s responses. Then, researchers began with a batch of approximately 40 assessments and independently labeled each response with a descriptive code that encapsulated the employed partitioning strategy and representation. The codes were based on the synopsis of prior research on the ways of reasoning children without low mathematics achievement displayed in equal sharing [26].
Researchers then compared their codes using peer debriefing [27]. That is, researchers met to compare assigned codes for each assessment coded. Disagreements in coding were handled using collaborative work [26]. That is, when coders’ scores differed, a discussion took place and a decision on coding was reached. Through this process, the researchers resolved all disagreements. Researchers compared each new problem solution and its code with previously coded data to ensure consistency [26]. As the researchers worked to code all remaining assessments, major categories of reasoning were established through the constant comparison approach [27].
We also considered overall trends in reference to partitioning across all children’s work. To that end, a content analysis was used to determine the number of times each way of reasoning was evident both as a function of the total sample and as a function of each equal sharing problem given in each daily assessment (i.e., per session). This descriptive information about the data also complemented the constant comparative analysis used earlier [26]. Researchers developed thematic codes for the analysis. For the name given to each theme, researchers counted how many occurrences comprised each grouping. Percentages were calculated by dividing the total number of occurrences of each grouping by the total sample.
Lastly, researchers prepared heat maps of children’s partitioning and summarized descriptive statistics. Heat maps are graphical representations of data where individual data values contained in a set are represented as colors [28]. Variances in ways of thinking can be represented by looking at the color changes. To produce the heat map, researchers created a matrix of codes in Microsoft Excel (Redmond, Washington, DC, USA). Next, we clicked on “Conditional Formatting” in the “Styles” section and selected/customized a color scheme. Finally, we sorted the participants from one to 40 so that trends across each student’s problem solving could be analyzed graphically. The next section of the paper presents the results of the study as they emerged through the analysis.

5. Results

We sought to identify the ways of reasoning apparent in 43 children’s partitioning strategies in equal sharing tasks. Generally, we found three broad categories, or themes: (a) “No Linking between the Number of Objects and/or Parts to the Question Context”; (b) “Partitioning of all Objects into Halves”; and (c) “Partitioning all Objects into the Number of Sharers”. Each way of reasoning is described below.

5.1. No Linking between Number of Objects and/or Parts to Question Context

“No Linking between the Number of Objects and/or Parts to the Question Context” was defined as a response that did not show a link between the numerical value in the question posed and the children’s partitioning. Put differently, the work suggested that the child aimed to partition the items into some number of pieces yet did not necessarily link his or her partitioning to the information given in the problem. We observed three ways of reasoning that were later included in this overall theme. The first way of reasoning shown in Figure 1a depicts a child who only drew one pizza and typically partitioned it, not into the number of sharers, but instead, the number of pizzas. This way of reasoning was evident in 36.5% of the total responses across the ten sessions. The second way of reasoning (Figure 1b) is depicted through responses that showed the amount of pizzas drawn was somewhat arbitrary and not linked to the quantity of pizzas given in the question. In the third way of reasoning, depicted (Figure 1c), children drew the correct number of pizzas, but either did not partition any of the pizzas or partitioned them into a number that was not linked to the number of sharers in the problem. For instance, Figure 1c shows the pizzas partitioned into eight sections and shaded to indicate that each person would receive 1/8 of the pizza; this does not reflect the four sharers that were posed in the question.
The responses evident under “No Linking between the Number of Objects and/or Parts to the Question Context” may be reflective of children who experienced difficulty relating the concept of equal sharing to the elements at play in the problem. That is, these children may not yet link the number of sharers to the objects shared. In other words, children are showing the activity of partitioning yet seem to attend to either the number of sharers or the number of items to be shared, but not both at once. An alternate explanation is that children may have attended to the context of the problem in an informal way or used an inappropriate operation or way of representing the situation. For instance, in Figure 1c, the child partitioning each item into eight parts, as opposed four, may be reflective of the fact that pizzas are typically cut into eight parts. The child could have attended to the context in a way that led her away from linking her partitioning to the number of sharers. Another example (i.e., Figure 1b) might be explained as a child multiplying the number in the problem and then making a number of parts to correspond to that number (i.e., 6 × 4 is 24, so that child makes 24 parts using three pizzas and eight parts). In either event, this way of reasoning seems distinct from what Empson et al. [13] describes as “no coordination” because the responses do not indicate a need to add to the situation (e.g., add more items) or create unequal shares. Conversely, this way of reasoning may be connected with what Steffe and Olive [15] refer to as “a rudimentary use of” whole number counting to partition, meaning that it is plausible that the children determined the number of parts they were going to create as they were partitioning (see Example b).

5.2. Partitioning of All Objects into Halves

“Partitioning of all Objects into Halves” was defined as a response that indicated that children drew the correct number of objects to be partitioned and then partitioned all objects into halves. As shown in the child’s work in Figure 2, children often attempted to deal out the halves they created, and seemed to work through their halving to create a number of parts that would result in an equal number of parts for the number of sharers described in the situation. However, in problems such as 5 pizzas shared by 3 people, children who used this form of reasoning halved all of the pizzas yet their work suggests that they may have had some difficulty determining how to further partition and deal out the remaining halves. This way of reasoning was evident in 8.75% of the total responses across the ten sessions and seemed to be more evident in questions that could not correctly be solved using the halving strategy, such as eight pizzas shared by three people.
The ways of reasoning evident from the children’s responses that were classified as “Partitioning all Objects into Halves” may be reflective of rudimentary ways of partitioning used to cut the wholes into some number of pieces as opposed to a plan to make a number of parts that relates to the number of sharers [13,15]. Such moves are likely based in children’s informal sharing notions and linked to a non-association with a relation, or coordination, between the number of sharers and the amount being shared. Moreover, it is possible that this way of reasoning is linked to children’s use of “two” to attend to their goal of making equal parts. Yet, they may not yet link making equal parts with exhausting the whole beyond their sense of “half” or “two” [15].

5.3. Partitioning All Objects into the Number of Sharers

A third way of reasoning was defined as children “Partitioning all Objects into the Number of Sharer”. As shown in Example a of Figure 3, children who evidenced this way of reasoning in their work drew the number of pizzas named in the problem (i.e., five pizzas) and partitioned each pizza into one section for each of the people sharing the pizza (i.e., three sections). This way of reasoning was evident in 54.75% of the total responses across the ten sessions.
This way of reasoning is reflective of children’s use of the “Partitioning of all Objects into the Correct Number of Sharers/Partitioned Leftovers into the Number of Shares” in the problem to partition the wholes [14] and may (but not necessarily) reflect an “Emerging Coordination of the Creation of Equal Parts” and “Exhausting the Whole” [13,15]. For instance, the work in Figure 3a may be reflective of the child’s coordination of the parts with exhausting the whole in this particular problem (e.g., the plan to cut each pizza into three parts).

5.4. Distributed Wholes and Partitioned Leftovers into the Number of Shares

In some cases, children seemed to determine the number of whole pizzas each sharer should receive and partitioned the remaining pizzas to reflect the number of sharers. As shown in Figure 3b, the child drew six pizzas, but partitioned only the two pizzas that were left after dealing out one pizza to each sharer. The remaining pizzas were partitioned into the number of sharers. As with partitioning each item in a number of parts equal to that of the number of sharers, this way of reasoning could be reflective of the child’s emerging coordination of the creation of equal parts and exhausting the whole [13,15].

5.5. Trends in Children’s Ways of Reasoning Evidenced in Partitioning

To create a visual image for analysis, a heat map of progressions from green (“No Linking” between the number of objects and/or parts to the question context) to yellow (“Partitioning of all Objects into Halves”) to red “Partitioning of all Objects into the Correct Number of Sharers/Partitioned Leftovers into the Number of Shares”) for partitioning was developed (see Figure 4). This map was analyzed for trends in the children’s ways of reasoning evidenced in partitioning by problem.
The heat map suggests that greater numbers of responses coded as “No Linking between the Number of Objects and/or Parts to the Question Context” between the number of objects and or parts to the question context took place in the beginning sessions (i.e., sessions one through three). As the sessions progressed, the number of responses that signified this way of reasoning diminished. The problem that was coded most heavily as “No Linking between the Number of Objects and/or Parts to the Question Context” was six objects shared by four people used after the first session. The problem coded for this method of reasoning the least was three people sharing five objects in session nine. “Partitioning of all Objects into Halves” was coded sporadically throughout the sessions. Problems coded most heavily for this way of reasoning include three people sharing eight objects in session two, three people sharing four objects in session five, and three people sharing five objects in session nine. “Partitioning of all Objects into the Correct Number of Sharers/Partitioned Leftovers into the Number of Shares” took place in smaller numbers in the beginning sessions (i.e., sessions one through three). However, as the sessions progressed, the number of responses that signified this way of reasoning increased and became the most coded way of reasoning after the third session. Many problems were coded heavily for this way of reasoning, including three people sharing five objects, four people sharing five objects, and four people sharing seven objects. “Partitioning of all Objects into the Correct Number of Sharers/Partitioned Leftovers into the Number of Shares” was coded the least amount of times for three people sharing eight objects.

6. Discussion

Results of this study revealed three ways of reasoning evidenced in children’s partitioning in equal sharing problems (i.e., “No Linking Between the Number of Objects and/or Parts to the Question Context”, “Partitioning of all Objects into Halves”, “Partitioning of all Objects into the Correct Number of Sharers/Partitioned Leftovers into the Number of Shares”) and evident in children who experience low achievement in mathematics over ten weeks of participation in lessons that did not directly address equal sharing. Trend analyses of the children’s partitioning suggest that children’s ways of thinking through equal sharing tasks varied, showed some change over the course of the sessions, and seemed dependent on the numbers used in the equal sharing problems. Thus, our data do not suggest sequential “levels” with respect to partitioning when looked at as indicators of reasoning, rather, just ways of knowing what exists in the children’s reasoning and that they utilized in equal sharing problems.
Children completed equal sharing problems as part of a daily assessment at the end of each intervention session. Their ways of reasoning provide a window into what conceptions children with low performance in mathematics do have when immersed in solving problems that supports the active use of partitioning and subsequent reasoning about the resulting quantity. These were ways of reasoning that the children already possessed, not a result of a teacher-modeled strategy or thought process. Arguably, the improvement over the sessions was the result of the children inventing more efficient strategies. Future research with more children who experience low achievement in mathematics solving equal sharing tasks as the basis for instruction would expand and extend the current findings. Such research might relate fraction symbolism to an equal-sharing analogy (division meaning) of fractions (e.g., 2/3 = two wholes shared equally among three), building on children’s informal knowledge, and naturally connecting to part-whole meanings of fractions [29]. Furthermore, the research might include a pretest-posttest design with random assignment and a comparison group to test for significant effects of different types of instruction.
The following paragraphs present two main contributions of the current study to the literature: (a) the aspects of the children’s ways of reasoning as “same” and “different” in terms of the children’s label of “low achieving” and (b) the implications of the children’s ways of reasoning in terms of “difference” and the subsequent need for “different” kinds of teaching and learning experiences in school.

6.1. What Did We Learn About Ways of Reasoning for Children Labeled as “Low Achieving”?

Findings from the current study support the notion that most of the children who experienced low performance in fractions in the study used partitioning within equal sharing situations, which is not unlike the performance reported in existing frameworks documenting conceptions of children who were characterized as “typically achieving” [12,13,15,18] (that is, we add to an existing body of evidence [30,31] that, given an opportunity to engage in solving problems and to reason about the results of their own activity, children labeled as “low achieving” evidenced ways of reasoning about fractions and used partitioning in similar ways as children without the label. As noted in past research [13,15,18], the numbers used in the problems seemed to have some effect on children’s partitioning. That is, problems that resulted in making three or five parts across one or more wholes seemed to bring about more difficulty in partitioning than those that did not for many of the assessments. However, as children’s experience with the equal sharing problems increased, ways of reasoning documented in previous research as somewhat more sophisticated [13,15] forms of partitioning, were evident. This is evidence that the children’s reasoning in equal sharing problems is not “different” from what we might expect from children who have not been labeled as low achieving.
This is not to say that no differences emerged compared to prior research with children not labeled as low achieving. Namely, some ways of reasoning evident in the children’s partitioning showed that some children did not link the number of sharers in the situation to their plan for partitioning the objects. Children drew an item and partitioned it into the number of items (as opposed the number of sharers) or a seemingly arbitrary number. This finding may be indicative of children who, in their early experiences with sharing situations, pictorially represent the situations (e.g., representations that show only the visual appearance of a problem element) instead of schematically (e.g., representations that show spatial relations among problem parts) [32]. Yet, these same children evidenced a variety of ways of reasoning over the sessions. Thus, even if we interpret some of the children’s initial depictions of the problem as qualitatively different, the children’s activity and reasoning, leveraged by the equal sharing situations, likely supported advancements in their partitioning plans. Moreover, these data were taken from daily assessments (e.g., static measures of knowing) and not from situations where children interacted with others about the problem situation and their own reasoning. If the children were engaged in instruction that leveraged what they knew and gave opportunities for children to unpack the problem context and engage in discourse about their ways of reasoning, it is likely their thinking would be extended [33].

6.2. Do Performance Differences Warrant Different Instructional Experiences?

The results of the study hold implications for the type of instruction children labeled as low achieving “need” due to their low mathematical achievement. When working with children, there is often a tendency to use black and white categories to view “knowing” as performance; children’s learning and subsequent conceptions seem to be labeled as either right or wrong. Based on this assessment, children with sustained low achievement are often times given labels of “deficient”, “not ready”, and “unable” [7,34,35] Mathematical knowledge, then, also takes on an altered form and is interpreted for these children as something that needs to be poured in rather than something that already exists [36], can grow and change, and can be supported and extended in the midst of instruction. In other words, the more children are placed into instructional situations that remove the responsibility for reasoning from the child and place it onto the teacher, the more these children experience an altered means of knowing and learning mathematics in school [3]. Arguably, such an experience might work to further marginalize and separate these children (e.g., [37,38]).
Knowing that children who are labeled as low achieving—a perceived “difference”—evidence ways of reasoning that are similar to those documented among their peers provides a platform from which to explore how to structure mathematics instruction for these children in ways that are currently underutilized. Particularly, children involved in the current study showed ways of knowing and reasoning within their own problem-solving activity that varied with the problem context and in terms of sophistication, just as we would expect from children without low achievement. In this way, the current work challenges the notion that low achieving students simply cannot engage in such problems that leverage their own activity and, thus, need only direct, systematic instruction to learn [39].
The children involved in the study had two years of regular classroom instruction in fractions that utilized already-partitioned shaded models, vocabulary instruction, and procedural training from which they did not benefit. Arguably, these children were defined as low achieving because they did not perform in an expected way or at an expected rate [2,8] from a curriculum that may have been limited in its support of deep fractional understanding [40]. Although the tradition has shifted in recent years to include concepts along with procedures, the tradition of measuring low achieving children’s “responsiveness” to such models of instruction continues [2,8,33]. Thus, current intervention research in special education focuses on children’s responsiveness to teacher-modeled strategies and not conceptual development within children’s thinking.
The equal share situations children encountered in this study were never directly addressed during the intervention instruction, and children were not given feedback on their solutions to equal sharing problems on the daily assessments. Yet, for the majority of children in the study, a variety of ways of reasoning evidenced through their partitioning were documented. The results of this study suggest that instructional experiences that are based in the child’s activity (e.g., partitioning) are not only beneficial but paramount to children with low achievement to develop their reasoning. We argue that instruction for any child, including children with low achievement, should focus on what the child is able to understand and do mindfully within their own activity and reasoning. Future research might document instruction in fractions for children labeled as low achieving from a problem-solving stance situated within classroom instruction with their peers, documenting how conceptions emerge and can be supported and extended through the children’s activity and reasoning alongside their peers.

7. Conclusions

Framing instruction in a disability studies perspective (e.g., [3]) that widens instruction from the start so that it is inclusive of diverse ways of knowing might be a way to place competence of all students in context. That is, when someone does not meet an expectation or an assumed “normal way” of doing, we tend to place the problem within the individual and move on. Yet, we question (a) why research and practice defines different ways of reasoning as a problem; and (b) why this “problem” is located within children. When researchers’ and educators’ notions of “normal” widen, we create spaces for and access to opportunities for diverse perspectives and ideas to be considered, shared, compared, contrasted, and, ultimately, valued. Children with different ways of navigating a mathematics problem may bring forth ways of knowing that, when viewed though a widened conception of “normal”, offers not only a form of access to the mathematics for the child, but also an additional way of viewing the mathematics for other children. Arguably, the mathematics improves, as mathematical knowing and learning becomes enriched and diversified. In this way, we advocate for an increase in research that positions children who experience low performance in mathematics as capable and decreases in research that positions children as failures and segregates their experiences in school. Instruction should utilize children’s activity as a platform from which to support and extend understanding.

Author Contributions

Jessica Hunt, Arla Westenskow and Patricia S. Moyer-Packenham conceived of and designed the experiments; Arla Westenskow performed the experiments; Jessica Hunt and Arla Westenskow analyzed the data; Jessica Hunt, Arla Westenskow and Patricia S. Moyer-Packenham contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools; Jessica Hunt and Arla Westenskow wrote the paper.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Bodovski, K.; Farkas, G. Mathematics growth in early elementary school: The roles of beginning knowledge, student engagement, and instruction. Elem. Sch. J. 2007, 108, 115–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Gersten, R.; Beckmann, S.; Clarke, B.; Foegen, A.; Marsh, L.; Star, J.R.; Witzel, B. Assisting Students Struggling with Mathematics: Response to Intervention (RtI) for Elementary and Middle Schools; NCEE 2009-4060; What Works Clearinghouse: Washington, DC, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  3. Baglieri, S.; Valie, J.; Connor, D.; Gallagher, D. Disability studies in education: The need for a plurality of perspectives on disability. Remedial Spec. Educ. 2010, 31, 267–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Elkind, D. Viewpoint: The curriculum-disabled child. Top. Learn. Learn. Disabil. 1983, 3, 71–79. [Google Scholar]
  5. Lewis, K.E.; Fisher, M.B. Taking stock of 40 years of research on mathematical learning disability: Methodological issues and future directions. J. Res. Math. Educ. 2016, 47, 338–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Kurz, A.; Elliott, S.N.; Wehby, J.H.; Smithson, J.L. Alignment of the intended, planned, and enacted curriculum in general and special education and its relation to student achievement. J. Spec. Educ. 2010, 44, 131–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Lambert, R.; Tan, P. Dis/ability and mathematics: Theorizing the research divide between special education and mathematics. In Proceedings of the 38th Annual Meeting of the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Tucson, AZ, USA, 3–6 November 2016; Wood, M.B., Turner, E.E., Civil, M., Eli, J.A., Eds.; The University of Arizona: Tucson, AZ, USA, 2016; pp. 1057–1063. [Google Scholar]
  8. Gersten, R.; Chard, D.J.; Jayanthi, M.; Baker, S.K.; Morphy, P.; Flojo, J. Mathematics instruction for students with learning disabilities: A meta-analysis of instructional components. Rev. Educ. Res. 2009, 79, 1202–1242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Hunt, J.H.; Empson, S.B. Exploratory study of informal strategies for equal sharing problems of students with learning disabilities. Learn. Disabil. Q. 2015, 38, 208–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Baroody, A.J. Learning: A framework. In Achieving Fluency: Special Education and Mathematics; Fennell, F.M., National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, Eds.; National Council of Teachers of Mathematics: Reston, VA, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  11. Hiebert, J.; Grouws, D.A. The effects of classroom mathematics teaching on students’ learning. In Second Handbook of Research on Mathematics Teaching and Learning; Lester, F.K., Ed.; Information Age: Greenwich, CT, USA, 2007; pp. 371–404. [Google Scholar]
  12. Charles, K.; Nason, R. Young children’s partitioning strategies. Educ. Stud. Math. 2000, 43, 191–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Empson, S.B.; Junk, D.; Dominguez, H.; Turner, E. Fractions as the coordination of multiplicatively related quantities: A cross sectional study of student’s thinking. Educ. Stud. Math. 2005, 63, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Piaget, J.; Inhelder, B.; Szeminska, A. The Child’s Conception of Geometry; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1960. [Google Scholar]
  15. Steffe, L.P.; Olive, J. Children’s Fractional Knowledge; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  16. Streefland, L. Fractions: A realistic approach. In Rational Numbers: An Integration of Research; Carpenter, T.P., Fennema, E., Romberg, T.A., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1993; pp. 289–326. [Google Scholar]
  17. Baroody, A.J.; Feil, Y.; Johnson, A.R. An alternative reconceptualization of procedural and conceptual knowledge. J. Res. Math. Educ. 2007, 38, 115–131. [Google Scholar]
  18. Pothier, Y.; Sawada, D. Partitioning: The emergence of rational number ideas in young children. J. Res. Math. Educ. 1983, 14, 307–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Hackenburg, A. The fraction knowledge and algebraic reasoning of students with the first multiplicative concept. J. Math. Behav. 2013, 32, 538–563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Mazzocco, M.M.M. Defining and differentiating mathematical learning disabilities and difficulties. In Why Is Math So Hard for Some Children? The Nature and Origins of Mathematical Learning Difficulties and Disabilities; Berch, D.B., Mazzocco, M.M.M., Eds.; Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.: Baltimore, MD, USA, 2007; pp. 7–27. [Google Scholar]
  21. Gersten, R.; Jordan, N.C.; Flojo, J.R. Early identification and interventions for students with mathematical difficulties. J. Learn. Disord. 2005, 38, 293–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Fuchs, L.S. Prevention research in mathematics: Improving outcomes, building identification models and understanding disability. J. Learn. Disord. 2005, 38, 350–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Westenskow, A.; Moyer-Packenham, P. Using an iceberg intervention model to understand equivalent fraction learning when students with mathematical learning difficulties use different manipulatives. Int. J. Technol. Math. Educ. 2016, 23, 45–63. [Google Scholar]
  24. Cramer, K.; Behr, M.; Post, T.; Lesh, R. Rational Number Project: Initial Fraction Ideas; Original Work Published 1997; Kendall/Hunt: Dubuque, IA, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  25. Westenskow, A. Equivalent Fraction Learning Trajectories for Students with Mathematical Learning Difficulties When Using Manipulatives. Ph.D. Dissertation, Utah State University, Logan, UT, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  26. Leech, N.L.; Onwuegbuzie, A.J. An array of qualitative data analysis tools: A call for data analysis triangulation. Sch. Psychol. Q. 2007, 22, 557–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Miles, M.B.; Huberman, A.M. An Expanded Sourcebook: Qualitative Data Analysis, 2nd ed.; Sage Publications, Inc.: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  28. Ward, M.O.; Grinstein, G.; Keim, D. Interactive Data Visualization: Foundations, Techniques, and Applications; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  29. Baroody, A.; Coslick, R.T. Fostering Children’s Mathematical Power: An Investigative Approach to K-8 Mathematics Instruction; Routledge: Abingdon, UK, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  30. Hunt, J.H.; Westenskow, A.; Silva, J.; Welch-Ptak, J. Levels of participatory conception of fractional quantity along a purposefully sequenced series of equal sharing tasks: Stu’s trajectory. J. Math. Behav. 2016, 41, 45–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Empson, S.B. Low-performing students and teaching fractions for understanding: An interactional analysis. J. Res. Math. Educ. 2003, 34, 305–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Van Garderen, D.; Montague, M. Visual-spatial representation, mathematical problem solving, and students of varying abilities. Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 2003, 18, 246–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Empson, S. Equal sharing and shared meaning: The development of fraction concepts in a first-grade classroom. Cognit. Instr. 1999, 17, 283–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Kroesbergen, E.H.; Van Luit, J.E. Mathematics interventions for children with special educational needs a meta-analysis. Remedial Spec. Educ. 2003, 24, 97–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. McDermott, R.; Goldman, S.; Varenne, H. The cultural work of learning disabilities. Educ. Res. 2006, 35, 12–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Piaget, J. The Development of Thought: Equilibration of Cognitive Structures; Rosin, A., Translator; Viking: New York, NY, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
  37. Compton, D.L.; Gilbert, J.K.; Jenkins, J.R.; Fuchs, D.; Fuchs, L.S.; Cho, E.; Barquero, L.A.; Bouton, B. Accelerating chronically unresponsive children to tier 3 instruction: What level of data is necessary to ensure selection accuracy? J. Learn. Disabil. 2012, 45, 204–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Balu, R.; Zhu, P.; Doolittle, F.; Schiller, E.; Jenkins, J.; Gersten, R. Evaluation of Response to Intervention Practices for Elementary School Reading; NCEE 2016-4000; National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences; U.S. Department of Education: Washington, DC, USA, 2015.
  39. Fuchs, L.S.; Schumacher, R.F.; Sterba, S.K.; Long, J.; Namkung, J.; Malone, A.; Hamlett, C.L.; Jordan, N.C.; Gersten, R.; Siegler, R.S.; et al. Does working memory moderate the effects of fraction intervention? An aptitude-treatment interaction. J. Educ. Psychol. 2014, 106, 499–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Siegler, R.; Carpenter, T.; Fennell, F.; Geary, D.; Lewis, J.; Okamoto, Y.; Thompson, L.; Wray, J. Developing Effective Fractions Instruction for Kindergarten through 8th Grade: A Practice Guide; NCEE #2010-4039; National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences; U.S. Department of Education: Washington, DC, USA, 2010.
Figure 1. Level 1 Partitioning: (a) only one pizza; (b) arbitrary number of pizzas; and (c) arbitrary number of parts.
Figure 1. Level 1 Partitioning: (a) only one pizza; (b) arbitrary number of pizzas; and (c) arbitrary number of parts.
Education 07 00037 g001
Figure 2. Level 2 partitioning: Partitioned number of pizzas into halves.
Figure 2. Level 2 partitioning: Partitioned number of pizzas into halves.
Education 07 00037 g002
Figure 3. Levels 3 and 4 partitioning: (a) partitioned all wholes; and (b) distributed wholes and partitioned parts.
Figure 3. Levels 3 and 4 partitioning: (a) partitioned all wholes; and (b) distributed wholes and partitioned parts.
Education 07 00037 g003
Figure 4. Heat maps showing ways of partitioning over ten sessions. Note: Participants 27, 36, 38, and 40 scored 3 on all session assessments.
Figure 4. Heat maps showing ways of partitioning over ten sessions. Note: Participants 27, 36, 38, and 40 scored 3 on all session assessments.
Education 07 00037 g004

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Hunt, J.; Westenskow, A.; Moyer-Packenham, P.S. Variations of Reasoning in Equal Sharing of Children Who Experience Low Achievement in Mathematics: Competence in Context. Educ. Sci. 2017, 7, 37. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci7010037

AMA Style

Hunt J, Westenskow A, Moyer-Packenham PS. Variations of Reasoning in Equal Sharing of Children Who Experience Low Achievement in Mathematics: Competence in Context. Education Sciences. 2017; 7(1):37. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci7010037

Chicago/Turabian Style

Hunt, Jessica, Arla Westenskow, and Patricia S. Moyer-Packenham. 2017. "Variations of Reasoning in Equal Sharing of Children Who Experience Low Achievement in Mathematics: Competence in Context" Education Sciences 7, no. 1: 37. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci7010037

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop