Next Article in Journal
RGB Color Model: Effect of Color Change on a User in a VR Art Gallery Using Polygraph
Previous Article in Journal
Chipless RFID Sensor for Measuring Time-Varying Electric Fields Using a Contactless Air-Filled Substrate-Integrated Waveguide Resonator
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

An Investigation into the Effects of Correlated Color Temperature and Illuminance of Urban Motor Vehicle Road Lighting on Driver Alertness

School of Mechanical Engineering, Southeast University, Suyuan Avenue 79, Nanjing 211189, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sensors 2024, 24(15), 4927; https://doi.org/10.3390/s24154927
Submission received: 17 June 2024 / Revised: 19 July 2024 / Accepted: 19 July 2024 / Published: 30 July 2024
(This article belongs to the Section Vehicular Sensing)

Abstract

:
Current international optical science research focuses on the non-visual effects of lighting on human cognition, mood, and biological rhythms to enhance overall well-being. Nocturnal roadway lighting, in particular, has a substantial impact on drivers’ physiological and psychological states, influencing behavior and safety. This study investigates the non-visual effects of correlated color temperature (CCT: 3000K vs. 4000K vs. 5000K) and illuminance levels (20 lx vs. 30 lx) of urban motor vehicle road lighting on driver alertness during various driving tasks. Conducted between 19:00 and 20:30, the experiments utilized a human-vehicle-light simulation platform. EEG (β waves), reaction time, and subjective evaluations using the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) were measured. The results indicated that the interaction between CCT and illuminance, as well as between CCT and task type, significantly influenced driver alertness. However, no significant effect of CCT and illuminance on reaction time was observed. The findings suggest that higher illuminance (30 lx) combined with medium CCT (4000K) effectively reduces reaction time. This investigation enriches related research, provides valuable reference for future studies, and enhances understanding of the mechanisms of lighting’s influence on driver alertness. Moreover, the findings have significant implications for optimizing the design of urban road lighting.

1. Introduction

Since the discovery of intrinsically photoreceptive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs) in mammalian eyes [1,2], there has been an escalating focus on investigating the non-visual effects of lighting. The research reveals that, upon the perception of light signals by ipRGCs, transmission occurs to the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN), orchestrating the regulation of the individual’s circadian rhythm system and hormone secretion. Concurrently, through the associative relationships between SCN and various regions of the cerebral cortex (such as raphe, amygdala, lateral hypothalamus, etc.), the light signals prompt a series of neural projection responses, thereby adjusting aspects of human physiology and psychology, including alertness, emotions, cognitive functions, and behavior [3,4,5]. Alertness is considered a cognitive state characterized by heightened preparedness and receptiveness to incoming sensory stimuli, resulting in swifter and more precise responses to external cues [6]. Maintaining a high level of alertness is essential for drivers, as it enables the prompt detection of subtle environmental changes, thereby significantly reducing the risk of accidents and allowing for swift and accurate reactions to potential hazards, thus enhancing overall road safety [7,8,9]. Within this context, this study aims to provide insights into the influence of the association between the non-visual effects of urban motor vehicle road lighting and driver alertness, an area where related research is relatively limited.
The impact of light on cognition is determined by several key factors, including optical parameters such as illuminance and spectral composition (CCT), temporal characteristics like the timing and duration of exposure, and the nature of the cognitive task [5]. Previous research has consistently demonstrated that short-wavelength blue light (approximately 460–480 nm) exerts a profound influence on non-visual effects related to alertness [4,5,10]. The majority of research findings suggest that high CCT, associated with short-wave blue light, or high illuminance can enhance human alertness [11,12,13,14]. However, there are also studies that yield completely opposite conclusions [15]. Additionally, some research indicates that altering illuminance or CCT may not have a significant impact on human alertness [16,17]. Consequently, consensus on the impact of light on alertness in current research remains elusive. Furthermore, beyond the inherent properties of light, there is growing evidence suggesting that the nature of cognitive tasks may also modulate the sensitivity of non-visual effects produced by light on individual cognition [17,18,19].
Despite significant advancements in comprehending the non-visual impacts of road lighting on driver alertness within the research field, limitations persist due to various factors. Firstly, current research predominantly focuses on the non-visual effects of tunnel lighting on drivers, with limited investigation into outdoor road segments such as urban motor vehicle roads [20,21,22]. This is because the lighting conditions and physical environment within tunnels are more easily simulated in laboratory settings, facilitating experimental studies while ensuring participant safety. However, urban motor vehicle roads present more complex conditions and a higher likelihood of unexpected incidents, necessitating higher driver alertness. Therefore, studying the non-visual effects of nighttime urban motor vehicle road lighting is crucial to enhance our understanding and improve road safety. Secondly, current studies often utilize melatonin levels and reaction time as primary indicators for assessing driver alertness [23]. While these measures provide valuable insights, they have notable limitations. Melatonin levels, though indicative of circadian rhythms, may not directly correlate with moment-to-moment fluctuations in alertness during driving tasks. Reaction time, while a useful metric, offers a limited scope, as it does not capture the underlying neural processes influencing driver alertness. To address these shortcomings, incorporating physiological indicators such as electroencephalography (EEG) can significantly enhance the reliability and precision of evaluations. EEG provides real-time data on brain activity, offering a more comprehensive understanding of cognitive states [24]. This integration allows for a nuanced analysis of how different lighting conditions impact neural mechanisms underlying alertness, leading to more robust and actionable research findings. Thirdly, relevant studies are carried out mostly during the daytime [21,23]. Nevertheless, the rate of traffic collisions at night is significantly higher than during the daytime [25] and the non-visual impacts of light on the human circadian rhythm system exhibit distinct variations throughout different time intervals within a day [26]. Therefore, it is necessary to carry out a special study on nighttime hours. Notably, few previous studies have explored the interactive effects of illuminance and CCT, both of which are pivotal determinants in characterizing the non-visual effects of light [21]. Therefore, this research endeavors to shed light on the combined influences of illuminance and CCT of road lighting on driver alertness during nighttime driving activities. This study constitutes one component of the research project focusing on the comprehensive evaluation criteria and methodologies for urban road lighting. The ultimate objective is to provide scientific assessment methods and design recommendations for the holistic illumination of urban roads, with the aim of rendering urban road lighting more pleasant, environmentally friendly, efficient, and artistic in the future. The findings of this research provide valuable recommendations for assessing the utility of illuminance and CCT of urban motor vehicle road lighting to enhance driver alertness and task performance, contributing to the optimization of design solutions for road lighting.

2. Methodology

2.1. Design

The experiment was conducted in a 3 × 2 × 3 within-subjects repeated measures design; experimental variables and specific parameter settings are shown in Table 1. To mitigate the potential influence of the sequence of lighting conditions on the experimental outcomes, we implemented two key strategies. Firstly, a washout period of one day was imposed between each experimental session, ensuring that participants were exposed to only one lighting environment per session. This arrangement necessitated each participant to complete six distinct experimental sessions, with a day of rest interposed between each session. Secondly, to balance the order effects, a Latin square design was employed to systematically vary the sequence of the different CCT and illuminance combinations across participants. This methodology guarantees a balanced exposure sequence, thereby minimizing the risk of systematic biases influencing our results. This laboratory investigation was performed from September to December 2022.

2.2. Participants

A total of 18 healthy volunteers (M ± SD = 34.6 ± 0.73, 15 males, 3 females) holding valid driving licenses were recruited from Nanjing Lighting Construction and Operation Group Co., Ltd for this research. On average, participants had 7.2 ± 0.82 years of driving experience. Sample size determination utilized G*power 3.1.9 with a significance level (α) set at 0.05 and a medium effect size (f) of 0.25. Analysis indicated a minimum sample size requirement of 10 individuals to achieve a statistical power level of 80%. A comprehensive interview assessment confirmed the absence of mood disorders and chronic ailments among all participants. All participants had either normal vision or vision corrected to normal, assessed through standard vision tests, including the Ishihara color test. In the week preceding the experiment, participants were instructed to maintain their regular daily routines, minimize prolonged exposure to unique lighting (e.g., monochromatic light), ensure adequate sleep (approximately 8 h), and abstain from stimulant beverages such as coffee and strong tea. These precautions were taken to ensure that sleep quality, light exposure, and caffeine consumption did not influence the accuracy of EEG measurements by affecting participants’ baseline neural activity. All participants signed an informed consent form and received monetary compensation of 270 RMB for their participation after the experiment.

2.3. CCT and Illuminance Setting Basis

2.3.1. Measurement of Actual Road Lighting Parameters

To faithfully replicate the lighting environment of urban motor vehicle road within a laboratory setting and control deviations between simulated and actual lighting conditions, this research employs measurements of static and dynamic illuminance values (including road surface illuminance and driver’s eye-level illuminance) obtained from actual road lighting. These measurements serve as the basis for configuring optical parameters in simulating the lighting environment for experiments. Specifically, eye-level illuminance refers to the illuminance measured at the horizontal position where the driver’s eyes are aligned when seated and looking forward (as illustrated in Figure 1). This parameter authentically reflects the illuminance values of light projected into the interior of the human eye, making it a critical efficacy-based visual metric for evaluating non-visual effects. Therefore, by assessing the relationship between the actual road surface illuminance provided by streetlights and the driver’s eye-level illuminance inside the vehicle, it is possible to scientifically control the simulation of the laboratory lighting environment, achieving a higher degree of fidelity.
This study adopted the measurement method of traditional road photometry indicators (central point method). The sampling points were selected on two road sections, namely Southeast University Road in Jiangning District and Shuanglong Avenue in Nanjing, both of which are four-lane two-way roads. The measurement methods are detailed in Figure 2.
The measurement of illuminance utilized an illuminance meter (Victor-VC1010D), with a sampling rate of 1 sample per second, capable of continuously collecting 50 data points. The experimenters conducted measurements during the evening from 19:00 to 20:30, capturing eye-level illuminance values of the drivers inside the vehicle (driver’s eye height from the ground is approximately 70 cm) and actual maintained average illuminance of road surface.
Specifically, the two roads under investigation are situated in the periphery of Southeast University, a location relatively secluded from the bustling city center. Consequently, during the period from 19:00 to 20:30, traffic volumes are comparatively low, rendering the impact of vehicle headlights on our measurements negligible. To further mitigate any potential interference, we ensured that static illuminance measurements were conducted in areas devoid of passing vehicles, thereby minimizing the disturbance from headlights. During the measurement process, if dynamic illuminance readings were recorded during vehicle passage (which typically exhibit elevated values), they were systematically identified and excluded from our analysis. This methodology ensures the accuracy of our research findings.
Static illuminance refers to the average illuminance measured at specified reference points on the road surface while the vehicle is stationary. Dynamic illuminance denotes the continuously measured average illuminance obtained at a frequency of 1 time/s as the vehicle travels at a speed of 60 km/h. Ultimately, A total of 428 data points were obtained for eye-level illuminance (400 dynamic illuminance, 28 static illuminance, with detailed statistical results provided in Table 2).

2.3.2. Illuminance Setting Basis

In accordance with optical principles, the relationship between the illuminance of an illuminated object surface and the distance from the light source is typically expressed through a proportional relationship, as indicated by Equation (1) [27]. Specifically, as the distance between the illuminated object and the light source increases, the illuminance on the surface of the illuminated object decreases. Hence, it can be deduced that under the condition of controlling other physical influencing variables (e.g., object occlusion, material of the illuminated object, etc. are consistent), there also exists a simple ratio relationship between the average illuminance of the road surface and the illuminance of the eye level (which is defined as K in this study; see Equation (2)), which serves as a crucial reference metric for guiding and examining simulated outdoor road lighting environments within the laboratory, ensuring that illuminance settings closely align with real-world scenarios to the greatest extent feasible. It is essential to note that, given the practical context, the majority of the time, drivers operate in dynamic driving conditions. Consequently, in this study, dynamic eye-level illuminance is selected for the computation of K. Here, E represents illuminance (lx), I represents the light intensity, defined as the luminous flux per solid angle, and D is the distance from the observation point to the light source (meters). E(eye-level) denotes the illuminance at the eye level of the driver inside the vehicle, while E(road surface) represents the average illuminance of the road surface.
E = I D 2
K = E ( e y e l e v e l ) E ( r o a d   s u r f a c e )
In this study, the parameter settings for illuminance conform to the Chinese urban road lighting design standard (China Academy of Building Research, 2016), with illuminance set at two levels: 20 lx (the minimum average illuminance standard for urban motor vehicle main roads) and 30 lx (the maximum average illuminance standard for urban motor vehicle main roads).
Substituting the measurement results from Table 2 into Equation (2), the calculated value of K is approximately 0.112 (K = 3.2/28.5). Therefore, when the average illuminance of the road surface is 30 lx, substituting this into Equation (2) (K ≈ 0.112), the corresponding eye-level illuminance value can be calculated to be approximately 3.4 lx. Similarly, when the average illuminance of the road surface is 20 lx, the calculated corresponding eye-level illuminance value is approximately 2.2 lx (values measured during the actual experiment were: 2.3 lx (eye level)—20 lx (road surface); 3.45 lx (eye level)—30 lx (road surface), compliance with experimental requirements).

2.3.3. CCT Setting Basis

The current design specifications for the CCT of urban motor vehicle roads in China (China Academy of Building Research, 2016) recommend a standard guideline that the CCT should not exceed 5000K. However, these specifications provide limited details. Therefore, in this study, the configuration of the CCT is, on one hand, based on the actual CCT distribution in urban motor vehicle road lighting (CCT range of urban motor vehicles in Nanjing: 2700K~6000K, data sourced from Nanjing Lighting Construction and Operation Group Co. Database). This provides a basis for delineating the approximate range of CCT settings (2500K~6500K). On the other hand, reference is made to relevant studies [28,29,30,31] and our research team’s related work [32,33], providing a basis for selecting which CCT parameters to focus on (2500K, 3000K, 3500K, 4000K, 4500K, 5000K, 5500K, 6000K, 6500K) and for setting the difference between the different CCT levels (usually set to 500K or 1000K). Consequently, in the experiments conducted in this study, three CCT levels were selected (3000K, 4000K, 5000K).

2.4. Setting

2.4.1. Physical Environment Settings

A darkroom (10 m × 20 m × 3.8 m) was constructed to replicate urban motor vehicle road lighting conditions. The windows were covered with blackout stickers to prevent outside distracting light from entering. Moreover, the walls and ceiling were enveloped in light-absorbing flocking fabric of photographic-grade quality, chosen for its capacity to minimize internal light reflections. In pursuit of simulating the diffuse reflective characteristics akin to asphalt concrete pavement, asphalt sheets were used to pave the ground.
The interior of the darkroom is equipped with a comprehensive driving simulation system, which encompasses a driving simulator (seat height: 70 cm) and a simulated driving scenario design program, underpinned by SACNeR Studio 2022.2 software. The integration of these components affords drivers an immersive simulated nighttime driving experience (the display offers a wide horizontal field of view of approximately 135°).
In addition, the laboratory is equipped with Brain Products (Munich, Germany) to comprehensively measure the physiological and psychological characteristic changes in the participants throughout the experiment. The related equipment and its functional description are shown in Figure 3. It should be noted that, in order to meet the illuminance requirements for simulating experimental conditions, a customized automotive enclosure was additionally installed (Figure 4). Figure 5 depicts the participants performing the experimental tasks in different simulated lighting during the experiment.

2.4.2. Light Environment Settings

The light environment in the laboratory consists of two main components: the primary light source provided by the LED streetlight dimming system and the virtual 3D nighttime driving environment (see Figure 6) presented on the screen. The LED streetlight dimming system comprises a control module, fixtures, lampshades, and mounting brackets. Utilizing the dimming control module (as shown in Figure 7), operators can remotely adjust the CCT (2000K~7000K) and illuminance (0%~100%) of the fixtures in real time.
In this research, the virtual light source model with RGB values and illuminance (see Table 3) equivalent to the actual luminaires was created using 3D Max to ensure that the light environment in the virtual driving scene provided an equivalent visual experience to the subjects as the actual fixtures in the darkroom, in order to control for the influence of visual effects on the results of the experiment.
In addition to the direct effects of correlated color temperature (CCT) and illuminance on drivers’ alertness and reaction time, it is essential to consider the potential influence of light pulsation and the power supply method of the light source on EEG brain activity. Previous studies [34] have demonstrated that the CCT and pulsation frequency of light emitted from fluorescent lamps and LEDs can significantly impact bioelectrical brain activity compared to traditional tungsten filament bulbs. Specifically, certain power supply methods and CCT values may introduce additional EEG emissions, notably in the 21–22 Hz range corresponding to Beta waves. However, it is important to note that not all supply methods and CCT values exhibit such effects. In our study, we employed a PWM power supply method, which we believe minimized potential interferences with EEG recordings while ensuring stable light flux emission.
To mitigate and control the impact of the display’s lighting, the screen illumination was adjusted to a level of 10% (screen illumination: 9.5 lx, eye level illumination: approx. 0.67 lx). The photometric measurements indicate that the spectral distribution characteristics of the combined light from the screen and the luminaires (referring to LED streetlights) closely approximate those of the light from the luminaires alone, within the range of approximately 440 nm to 630 nm. Hence, it can be inferred that both the non-visual effects induced by mixed light (from the screen and luminaires) and single light (from luminaires) exhibit similar characteristics, thereby validating the scientific rigor of the experimental setup. It is imperative to explicitly state that the SPD test point was positioned immediately adjacent to the subject’s eye, horizontally aligned with it, and situated 3.0 m away from the center of the screen and 0.9 m above the ground. This positioning ensures that the spectral distribution measured by the SPD accurately reflects the light entering the subject’s eye. Figure 8 illustrates the difference between mixed light and single light (using a 3500K as an example).

2.5. Experimental Tasks and Procedure

2.5.1. Experimental Tasks

In this study, three specific daily driving scenarios were selected as fundamental templates to design the simulated tasks (Figure 9). The aim was to replicate the real driving experience as closely as possible. During the simulated driving experiment, participants were instructed to drive along a straight road. It should be noted that with SCANeR Studio, the surrounding traffic density was set to ρ = 0.6, which represents a simulated value of the traffic flow on the urban motor vehicle road in the evening from 19:00 to 20:30.
(1)
Task 1 (baseline—monotonous driving): Task 1 phase (baseline) was devoid of any unusual stimuli (e.g., traffic lights, obstacles, etc.) to simulate a monotonous driving environment. Participants were instructed to sustain a naturally relaxed state, manage the virtual vehicle at 60 km/h, adhere to traffic regulations, and avoid changing lanes.
(2)
Task 2 (waiting for red lights and traffic jams): Task 2 simulated a driving scenario involving frequent waits at red lights and sporadic instances of random traffic congestion. Throughout this phase, the vehicle’s speed limit remained consistent at 60 km/h.
(3)
Task 3 (follow-up task and auditory PVT task): Task 3 employed a dual-task experimental paradigm (primary task: follow-up task, secondary task: auditory psychomotor vigilance task (aPVT)) [35] to amplify perceptual load and diminish the likelihood of slower or inaccurate responses due to reduced attention. In this phase, participants were tasked with controlling the simulated vehicle to trail a specified target vehicle (a red pickup truck) while maintaining a self-assessed safe distance. If the target vehicle changed lanes, participants were required to steer the vehicle accordingly without overtaking or tailgating, and no specific speed limit was imposed. Furthermore, concurrently managing the following task, participants were prompted to react to randomized acoustic stimuli (a simulated ringtone resembling an incoming call) by swiftly pressing a designated key (designated as the space bar).

2.5.2. Procedure

One week before the experiment, participants received operational training on the driving simulator to ensure proficient handling of the equipment. Prior to the formal experiment, investigators provided participants with a detailed explanation of the experimental procedures and task execution requirements. Following the practice phase, participants wore completely occlusive eyeshades, closed their eyes, and underwent a 15-min dark adaptation in a dark environment to eliminate the potential impact of prior lighting conditions on the experimental results. After the dark adaptation period, participants underwent a 5-min period of light adaptation to acclimate to the current experimental lighting environment. Upon the completion of the light adaptation phase, the formal experiment commenced, during which participants’ EEG signals were collected and recorded in real time. Each participant underwent the experimental tasks in the following sequential order: Task 1, Task 2, and Task 3. Additionally, after completing each task, participants were required to fill out the KSS questionnaire. Figure 10 illustrates the procedure of the experiment.

2.6. Measures

2.6.1. EEG Indicators (β)

Numerous studies have demonstrated a strong correlation between electroencephalogram (EEG) readings and assessments of alertness tasks [11,36,37,38]. The Beta (β) frequency band, ranging from approximately 14 Hz to 30 Hz, represents the EEG activity that is typically associated with states of nervousness, agitation, or arousal [37,39]. β power, measured in microvolts squared per hertz (μV²/Hz), serves as a highly sensitive indicator for detecting changes in driver alertness during driving sessions [39,40]. Specifically, in this research, the average power values of the β band, collected from electrode locations (P3, P4, PO3, PO4, O1, Oz, O2) [41,42,43] that follow the international 10–20 system, were employed as an assessment index to evaluate the influence of various lighting configurations on driving alertness. This assessment index was used to quantify the effect of different lighting setups on driver alertness.

2.6.2. Task Performance (RT)

The Auditory Psychomotor Vigilance Task (aPVT), a prevalent psychological experimental paradigm, primarily aims to assess an individual’s proficiency in maintaining sustained attention and executing rapid responses [44,45]. During this task, participants are instructed to respond promptly and precisely to auditory stimuli, such as sound signals, either by pressing a button or executing a designated bodily movement. This paradigm has been extensively utilized in investigating cognitive and behavioral performances, particularly under conditions of sleep deprivation, fatigue, and vigilance decrement. To facilitate the development of the aPVT’s procedure, the software E-prime 3.0 was employed. During Task 3, the average reaction time (RT) of participants in response to auditory stimuli was collected to assess the sustained attention and cognitive control of drivers. Each auditory stimulus lasted 3000 ms, a duration determined as optimal through pre-experiment measurements to ensure drivers could respond within this time, maintaining data validity. The interval between stimuli, referred to as the inter-stimulus interval (ISI), was randomly set between 50,000 and 70,000 ms to prevent participants from predicting stimulus occurrence, thereby ensuring accurate measurements of reaction to unexpected events. Reaction time was recorded from stimulus onset to the participant pressing a button. All participants had normal hearing, verified using a standard audiometric test. Stimuli were presented at a standardized loudness level. Figure 11 illustrates the procedure of the aPVT experimental paradigm adopted in this research.

2.6.3. Subjective Evaluation Scale (KSS)

Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) [46] (Table 4) is a valid scale for assessing subjective alertness responses to external environmental, circadian and pharmacological stimuli. In this research, the KSS was employed in conjunction with the EEG indicator β to evaluate alertness during various task phases. Notably, in the experiment, since the drivers were not excessively drowsy and could not realistically rate themselves as ‘completely unable to stay awake’, the 10th option, which is not part of the standard KSS, was omitted. Consequently, the KSS scores in this study range from 1 to 9 points. A higher KSS score corresponds to a lower level of individual alertness, signifying increased sleepiness.

3. Results

3.1. EEG (β)

The results of the three-factor repeated measures ANOVA showed that the main effects of CCT, Illuminance, and Task [F(2, 34) = 32.778, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.658; F(1, 17) = 6.387, p = 0.022, η2 = 0.273; F(2, 34) = 9.799, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.366, respectively] were significant.
The interaction effects of CCT × Illuminance was significant [F(2, 34) = 8.975, p = 0.001,η2 = 0.346] (see Figure 12). Post-hoc comparisons showed that with a fixed CCT of 3000K, β was significantly reduced for 20 lx (vs. 30 lx) (p = 0.002); Similarly, with a fixed CCT of 4000K, β was significantly reduced for 20 lx (vs. 30 lx) (p = 0.002). When the illuminance is fixed at 20 lx, β was significantly lower at 3000K (vs. 4000K; 5000K) (p = 0.001; p < 0.001) and 4000K compared to 5000K; β was significantly lower (p = 0.002). At a fixed illuminance of 30 lx, β was significantly lower at 3000K (vs.4000K; 5000K) (p < 0.001; p < 0.001).
The interaction effects of CCT × Task was significant [F(4, 68) = 2.781, p = 0.033, η2 = 0.141]. Post-hoc comparisons showed that in Task 1, β was significantly lower for 3000K (vs. 4000K; 5000K) (p < 0.001); 4000K vs. 5000K, β was significantly lower (p < 0.001). In Task 2, β was significantly lower at 3000K (vs. 4000K; 5000K) (p = 0.001; p < 0.001). During Task 3, β was significantly lower at 3000K (vs. 4000K; 5000K) (p = 0.011; p < 0.001).
The interaction effects of Illuminance × Task [F(2, 34) = 1.648, p = 0.207, η2 = 0.088] or CCT × Illuminance × Task [F(2.555, 43.429) = 0.479, p = 0.669, η2 = 0.027] was not significant (see Figure 12).

3.2. Reaction Time (RT)

Since RT was only measured at Task 3, a two-way measurement ANOVA was employed, revealing that the main effects of CCT, Illumination and their interaction effects (CCT × Illumination) were not significant [F(2, 34) = 1.432, p = 0.253, η2 = 0.082; F(1, 17) = 1.141, p = 0.300, η2 = 0.063; F(2.912, 58.246) = 0.714, p = 0.543, η2 = 0.034, respectively] (see Figure 13).

3.3. Subjective Evaluation Results (KSS)

The results of the three-factor repeated measures ANOVA showed that the main effect of CCT [F(2, 34) = 4.414, p = 0.020, η2 = 0.206] was significant, while the main effect of Illuminance or Task [F(1, 17) = 4.402, p = 0.051, η2 = 0.206; F(2, 34) = 0.400, p = 0.674, η2 = 0.023, respectively] was not significant.
The interaction effects of CCT × Task were significant [F(4, 68) = 0.357, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.242] (see Figure 14), and post-hoc comparisons showed that during the Task 2 phase, KSS scores were significantly higher in the 3000K (vs. 4000K) (p = 0.024); 4000K was significantly lower than 5000K (p = 0.04). During the Task 3 phase, KSS scores in the 3000K (vs. 4000K) were significantly higher (p = 0.001); and significantly lower in 4000K (vs. 5000K) (p = 0.003). Given that higher KSS scores reflect reduced levels of alertness, the aforementioned findings suggest that participants reported significantly higher subjective alertness for 4000K compared to the 3000K (during the Task 2 phase) and the 5000K (during the Task 3 phase).
The interaction effects of CCT × Illuminance [(F(2, 34) = 0.796, p = 0.459, η2 = 0.045), Illuminance × Task [F(2, 34) = 2.064, p = 0.143, η2 = 0.108], and CCT × Illuminance × Task [F(4, 68) = 1.108, p = 0.360, η2 = 0.061] were not significant (see Figure 14).

4. Discussion

The EEG evaluations revealed a significant interaction between CCT and Illuminance, as well as between CCT and Task. The results of the interaction of CCT × Illuminance showed that elevated illuminance levels (30 lx vs. 20 lx) significantly increased subjects’ alertness (β) at 3000K and 4000K. However, a parallel trend was not evident at 5000K. This discrepancy may be attributed to the fact that light sources with high CCT contain a higher component of short-wave blue light, which can drive ipRGCs with a comparable intensity to that of the illuminance (20 lx, 30 lx). Consequently, there was limited variation in the impact of fine-tuning illuminance (20 lx, 30 lx) on driver alertness under high CCT (5000K). Additionally, under low illuminance(20 lx), increasing CCT proved advantageous in enhancing driver alertness, with significant differences observed between 3000K vs. 4000K (p = 0.001), 3000K vs. 5000K (p < 0.001), and 4000K vs. 5000K (p = 0.002). Under high illuminance (30 lx), the medium CCT (4000K) also significantly increased driver alertness in comparison to the low CCT (3000K). It is noteworthy that there was no significant change in participants’ alertness when exposed to 4000K and 5000K, possibly due to the similar short-wave distribution characteristics of the 4000K and 5000K, which stimulated ipRGCs to a similar extent under high light conditions.
The results of the interaction between CCT and Illuminance showed that during Task 2 and Task 3, participants exhibited higher levels of alertness in 4000K and 5000K compared to 3000K. Figure 12 shows that participants’ alertness improved as CCT increased, aligning with prior research findings [20,47,48,49].
The subjective alertness ratings revealed that only the interaction between CCT and Task significantly impacted participants’ subjective alertness. Specifically, during Task 2 and Task 3, participants reported notably higher subjective alertness scores in both the 3000K and 5000K compared to the 4000K. This implies that maintaining a medium CCT (4000K) facilitated a more favorable subjective vigilance and attentive state during driving tasks involving frequent stops at red lights, traffic congestion, and car-following scenarios. A comprehensive comparative analysis of the EEG and subjective evaluations displayed a high level of alertness, demonstrating a general trend of heightened alertness among subjects with increasing CCT. However, no discernible modulation effect of CCT on subjective alertness was observed during the initial phase of the task. In this context, it is hypothesized in this paper that the impact of CCT on drivers’ subjective alertness might also be influenced by the duration of the light stimulus [17,50,51]. Characterized by limited-duration light exposure, no significant differences in subjective alertness among subjects in various CCTs were observed. To summarize, these findings align with prior studies concluding that appropriate increments in illuminance (30 lx) and CCT (4000K, 5000K) can enhance individual alertness [52,53]. Nonetheless, further exploration is required to determine whether additional increases in CCT or illuminance might disrupt drivers’ cognitive efficiency.
RT measurements showed that neither the interaction between CCT and Illuminance nor the main effect of CCT and Illuminance reached statistical significance. This suggests that drivers’ RT to random stimuli were not substantially influenced by external lighting conditions. Several factors may contribute to this observation. Firstly, it may relate to the specific nature of the experimental task. For instance, a study by Ru et al. (2019) noted significant non-visual effects of illumination on RT during Go/No-go and Flanker tasks, yet illumination did not notably affect RT during the PVT. Secondly, it might stem from the subjective attitudes of the participants towards the task. Participants might not have felt a significant sense of urgency when faced with sudden stimuli in the virtual driving scenario, resulting in minimal differences in their RT across various lighting conditions. Figure 14 reveals that, regardless of CCT variations, RT was higher under high illumination (30 lx) compared to low illumination (20 lx). This implies that elevating the illuminance of urban motor vehicle road lighting may not necessarily enhance drivers’ immediate responsiveness to unforeseen situations, aligning with findings from [53]. The mechanism behind this phenomenon remains unclear and warrants further investigation. In addition, as CCT increased, RT displayed a pattern of initially shortening and then lengthening. This trend suggests that a moderate CCT (around 4000K) might be most favorable for drivers to respond promptly when confronted with unexpected driving circumstances.

5. Conclusions

Previous studies have extensively explored and documented the immediate impacts of illuminance and CCT within indoor or tunnel interior lighting on individual alertness and task performance. However, limited research has delved into the repercussions of nighttime outdoor roadway lighting on non-visual effects on driver alertness. Moreover, the interactive effects of illuminance and CCT on individual cognitive function have rarely been discussed in previous studies. Therefore, this research focused on the mechanisms through which illuminance and CCT, along with their interactions, influence driver alertness in urban motor vehicle road lighting during nighttime (19:00~20:30). The main conclusions of this study are as follows:
(1)
In summary, appropriately elevated illuminance (30 lx vs. 20 lx) and CCT (4000K and 5000K vs. 3000K) are more conducive to improving driver alertness and positively impacting driving task performance.
(2)
EEG measurements revealed a significant impact of the interaction between CCT × Illuminance and CCT × Task on drivers’ alertness (β). Moreover, a significant interaction effect of CCT × Task on drivers’ subjective alertness was observed. However, no significant main effects of CCT and Illuminance on reaction time (RT) across driving tasks were identified. Nevertheless, observational data indicated that drivers exhibited the shortest reaction time under lighting conditions of 4000K CCT and 30 lx illuminance. This suggests that this specific combination of lighting may help reduce driver reaction time.

Author Contributions

Q.C.: Conceptualization, Methodology, Validation, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Resources, Writing—Original Draft Preparation, Writing—Review & Editing; Z.P.: Methodology, Software, Validation, Data curation, Writing—Original Draft, Visualization; J.W.: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Data curation, Writing—Original Draft Preparation, Writing—Review & Editing; C.X.: supervision, Project Administration, Funding Acquisition. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The presented study was jointly sponsored by the Equipment Pre-Research Foundation of China (Grant No: 41412040304), the National Key R&D Program of China (Grant No. 2022YFF0607000), and the National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant no: 72271053 and 71871056).

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge the great guidance and assistance of Professor Chengqi Xue, Haiyan Wang, and Yafeng Niu.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work.

References

  1. Berson, D.M.; Dunn, F.A.; Takao, M. Phototransduction by Retinal Ganglion Cells That Set the Circadian Clock. Science 2002, 295, 1070–1073. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Provencio, I.; Rodriguez, I.R.; Jiang, G.; Hayes, W.P.; Moreira, E.F.; Rollag, M.D. A Novel Human Opsin in the Inner Retina. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 2000, 20, 600–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Chellappa, S.; Steiner, R.; Blattner, P.; Oelhafen, P.; Götz, T.; Cajochen, C. Non-Visual Effects of Light on Melatonin, Alertness and Cognitive Performance: Can Blue-Enriched Light Keep Us Alert? PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e16429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Vandewalle, G.; Gais, S.; Schabus, M.; Balteau, E.; Carrier, J.; Darsaud, A.; Sterpenich, V.; Albouy, G.; Dijk, D.J.; Maquet, P. Wavelength-Dependent Modulation of Brain Responses to a Working Memory Task by Daytime Light Exposure. Cereb. Cortex 2007, 17, 2788–2795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Vandewalle, G.; Maquet, P.; Dijk, D.-J. Light as a Modulator of Cognitive Brain Function. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2009, 13, 429–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Chandrakumar, D.; Keage, H.A.D.; Gutteridge, D.; Dorrian, J.; Banks, S.; Loetscher, T. Interactions between Spatial Attention and Alertness in Healthy Adults: A Meta-Analysis. Cortex 2019, 119, 61–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Matthias, E.; Bublak, P.; Müller, H.J.; Schneider, W.X.; Krummenacher, J.; Finke, K. The Influence of Alertness on Spatial and Nonspatial Components of Visual Attention. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2010, 36, 38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Oken, B.S.; Salinsky, M.C.; Elsas, S.M. Vigilance, Alertness, or Sustained Attention: Physiological Basis and Measurement. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2006, 117, 1885–1901. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Verwey, W.B.; Zaidel, D.M. Preventing Drowsiness Accidents by an Alertness Maintenance Device. Accid. Anal. Prev. 1999, 31, 199–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Cajochen, C.; Münch, M.; Kobialka, S.; Kräuchi, K.; Steiner, R.; Oelhafen, P.; Orgül, S.; Wirz-Justice, A. High Sensitivity of Human Melatonin, Alertness, Thermoregulation, and Heart Rate to Short Wavelength Light. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2005, 90, 1311–1316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Łaszewska, K.; Goroncy, A.; Weber, P.; Pracki, T.; Tafil-Klawe, M.; Pracka, D.; Złomańczuk, P. Daytime Acute Non-Visual Alerting Response in Brain Activity Occurs as a Result of Short-and Long-Wavelengths of Light. J. Psychophysiol. 2018, 32, 202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Smolders, K.C.H.J.; de Kort, Y.A.W. Bright Light and Mental Fatigue: Effects on Alertness, Vitality, Performance and Physiological Arousal. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 39, 77–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Vetter, C.; Juda, M.; Lang, D.; Wojtysiak, A.; Roenneberg, T. Blue-Enriched Office Light Competes with Natural Light as a Zeitgeber. Scand. J. Work. Environ. Health 2011, 37, 437–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Viola, A.U.; James, L.M.; Schlangen, L.J.; Dijk, D.-J. Blue-Enriched White Light in the Workplace Improves Self-Reported Alertness, Performance and Sleep Quality. Scand. J. Work. Environ. Health 2008, 34, 297–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Young, C.R.; Jones, G.E.; Figueiro, M.G.; Soutière, S.E.; Rea, M.S. At-Sea Trial of 24-h-Based Submarine Watchstanding Schedules with High and Low Correlated Color Temperature Light Sources. J. Biol. Rhythm. 2015, 30, 144–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Huiberts, L.M.; Smolders, K.C.H.J.; de Kort, Y.A.W. Non-Image Forming Effects of Illuminance Level: Exploring Parallel Effects on Physiological Arousal and Task Performance. Physiol. Behav. 2016, 164, 129–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Ru, T.; de Kort, Y.A.; Smolders, K.C.; Chen, Q.; Zhou, G. Non-Image Forming Effects of Illuminance and Correlated Color Temperature of Office Light on Alertness, Mood, and Performance across Cognitive Domains. Build. Environ. 2019, 149, 253–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Gabel, V.; Maire, M.; Reichert, C.F.; Chellappa, S.L.; Schmidt, C.; Hommes, V.; Cajochen, C.; Viola, A.U. Dawn Simulation Light Impacts on Different Cognitive Domains under Sleep Restriction. Behav. Brain Res. 2015, 281, 258–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Huiberts, L.M.; Smolders, K.C.H.J.; de Kort, Y.A.W. Shining Light on Memory: Effects of Bright Light on Working Memory Performance. Behav. Brain Res. 2015, 294, 234–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Liang, B.; He, S.; Tähkämö, L.; Tetri, E.; Cui, L.; Dangol, R.; Halonen, L. Lighting for Road Tunnels: The Influence of CCT of Light Sources on Reaction Time. Displays 2020, 61, 101931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Li, X.; Ling, J.; Shen, Y.; Lu, T.; Feng, S.; Zhu, H. The Impact of CCT on Driving Safety in the Normal and Accident Situation: A VR-Based Experimental Study. Adv. Eng. Inform. 2021, 50, 101379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Peng, L.; Weng, J.; Yang, Y.; Wen, H. Impact of Light Environment on Driver’s Physiology and Psychology in Interior Zone of Long Tunnel. Front. Public Health 2022, 10, 842750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Bhagavathula, R.; Gibbons, R.; Hanifin, J.; Brainard, G. LED Roadway Lighting: Impact on Driver Sleep Health and Alertness; National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Jung, T.P.; Makeig, S.; Stensmo, M.; Sejnowski, T.J. Estimating Alertness from the EEG Power Spectrum. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 1997, 44, 60–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Portera, A.; Bassani, M. Examining the Impact of Different LED Road Stud Layouts on Driving Performance and Gaze Behaviour at Night-Time. Transp. Res. Part F Psychol. Behav. 2024, 103, 430–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Sharkey, K.M.; Carskadon, M.A.; Figueiro, M.G.; Zhu, Y.; Rea, M.S. Effects of an Advanced Sleep Schedule and Morning Short Wavelength Light Exposure on Circadian Phase in Young Adults with Late Sleep Schedules. Sleep Med. 2011, 12, 685–692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Smith, W.J. Modern Optical Engineering 4E (PB); McGraw Hill Professional: New York, NY, USA, 2007; ISBN 978-0-07-159375-5. [Google Scholar]
  28. Hu, J.; Gao, X.; Wang, R. Study on the Drivers’ Recognition Law under Different Lighting Environment along the Tunnel Exit Section at Nighttime. In Proceedings of the 2019 6th International Conference on Frontiers of Industrial Engineering (ICFIE), London, UK, 10–12 September 2019; pp. 56–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Shamsul, B.; Sia, C.C.; Ng, Y.; Karmegan, K. Effects of Light’s Colour Temperatures on Visual Comfort Level, Task Performances, and Alertness among Students. Am. J. Public Health Res. 2013, 1, 159–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Sheu, M.; Chang, L.; Hsia, S.-C.; Sun, C.-C. Intelligent System Design for Variable Color Temperature LED Street Light. In Proceedings of the 2016 IEEE International Conference on Consumer Electronics-Taiwan (ICCE-TW), Nantou, Taiwan, 27–29 May 2016; pp. 1–2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Pan, C.; Wu, J.; Pan, Z.; Chen, Q.; Xue, C. Non-Visual Effects of CCT and Illuminance and Driving Task on Driver’s Mood: Evidence from an EEG Study. In Proceedings of the Eighth International Conference on Electromechanical Control Technology and Transportation (ICECTT 2023), Hangzhou, China, 19–21 May 2023; Volume 12790, pp. 1399–1406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Pan, Z.; Wang, H.; Wu, J.; Chen, Q. Non-Visual Effects of CCT on Drivers, Evidence from EEG. In Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on Intelligent Human Systems Integration IHSI 2023 Integrating People Intelligent Systems, Venice, Italy, 22–24 February 2023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Niemierzycka, A.; Tomczuk, K.; Mikicin, M.; Zdrodowska, A.; Orzechowski, G.; Kowalczyk, M. Examinations of the Methods Used to Power Supply of Different Light Sources and Their Effect on Bioelectrical Brain Activity. Neurol. Neurochir. Pol. 2018, 52, 505–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Dinges, D.F.; Pack, F.; Williams, K.; Gillen, K.A.; Powell, J.W.; Ott, G.E.; Aptowicz, C.; Pack, A.I. Cumulative Sleepiness, Mood Disturbance, and Psychomotor Vigilance Performance Decrements during a Week of Sleep Restricted to 4–5 Hours per Night. Sleep 1997, 20, 267–277. [Google Scholar]
  35. Kubicki, S.; Herrmann, W.M.; Fichte, K.; Freund, G. Reflections on the Topics: EEG Frequency Bands and Regulation of Vigilance. Pharmakopsychiatr. Neuropsychopharmakol. 1979, 12, 237–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Liang, S.F.; Lin, C.T.; Wu, R.C.; Chen, Y.C.; Huang, T.Y.; Jung, T.P. Monitoring Driver’s Alertness Based on the Driving Performance Estimation and the EEG Power Spectrum Analysis. In Proceedings of the 2005 IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology 27th Annual Conference, Shanghai, China, 17–18 January 2005; pp. 5738–5741. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Makeig, S.; Inlow, M. Lapse in Alertness: Coherence of Fluctuations in Performance and EEG Spectrum. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1993, 86, 23–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Lal, S.K.L.; Craig, A. Driver Fatigue: Electroencephalography and Psychological Assessment. Psychophysiology 2002, 39, 313–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Morales, J.M.; Díaz-Piedra, C.; Rieiro, H.; Roca-González, J.; Romero, S.; Catena, A.; Fuentes, L.J.; Di Stasi, L.L. Monitoring Driver Fatigue Using a Single-Channel Electroencephalographic Device: A Validation Study by Gaze-Based, Driving Performance, and Subjective Data. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2017, 109, 62–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Jap, B.T.; Lai, S.; Fischer, P.; Bekiaris, E. Using EEG Spectral Components to Assess Algorithms for Detecting Fatigue. Expert Syst. Appl. Int. J. 2009, 36, 2352–2359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. da Silveira, T.L.T.; Kozakevicius, A.J.; Rodrigues, C.R. Automated Drowsiness Detection through Wavelet Packet Analysis of a Single EEG Channel. Expert Syst. Appl. 2016, 55, 559–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Lee, B.G.; Lee, B.-L.; Chung, W.-Y. Mobile Healthcare for Automatic Driving Sleep-Onset Detection Using Wavelet-Based EEG and Respiration Signals. Available online: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/25264954/ (accessed on 28 January 2024).
  43. Chang, A.M.; Scheer, F.A.; Czeisler, C.A.; Aeschbach, D. Direct Effects of Light on Alertness, Vigilance, and the Waking Electroencephalogram in Humans Depend on Prior Light History. Sleep 2013, 36, 1239–1246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Rosinvil, T.; Daneault, V.; Vanderwalle, G.; Desaultels, A.; Forcier, P.; Gaudet-Fex, B.; Apinis-Deshaies, A.; Aumont, T.; Constantin, I.M.; Matute, A. 280 Is Blue-Enriched Light More Effective than Red Light to Enhance Vigilance and Cognitive Performance in Parkinson’s Disease? Sleep 2021, 44, A112–A113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Akerstedt, T.; Gillberg, M. Subjective and Objective Sleepiness in the Active Individual. Int. J. Neurosci. 1990, 52, 29–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Bekdash, M.; Asirvadam, V.; Kamel, N.; Yanti, D.K. Estimation of Driver Alertness to Different Colors and Intensities Using Brain Visual Evoked Potentials. In Proceedings of the 2016 6th International Conference on Intelligent and Advanced Systems (ICIAS), Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia, 15–17 August 2016; pp. 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Keis, O.; Helbig, H.; Streb, J.; Hille, K. Influence of Blue-Enriched Classroom Lighting on Students’ Cognitive Performance. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2014, 3, 86–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. He, S.; Yan, Y. Impact of Advance Light Exposure on Assembly-Line Workers’ Subjective Work Alertness and Sleep Quality. Light. Res. Technol. 2022, 55, 105–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Li, Y.; Ru, T.; Chen, Q.-W.; Qian, L.; Luo, X.; Zhou, G. Effects of Illuminance and Correlated Color Temperature of Indoor Light on Emotion Perception. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 14351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Bao, J.; Song, X.; Li, Y.; Bai, Y.; Zhou, Q. Effect of Lighting Illuminance and Colour Temperature on Mental Workload in an Office Setting. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 15284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Kahaki, Z.R.; Jahangiri, H.; Smith, A.P.; Kazemi, R. Subjective and Objective Survey of Office Lighting: Effects on Alertness, Comfort, Satisfaction, and Safety. Med. Lav. 2022, 113, e2022024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Zhu, Y.; Yang, M.; Yao, Y.; Xiong, X.; Li, X.; Zhou, G.; Ma, N. Effects of Illuminance and Correlated Color Temperature on Daytime Cognitive Performance, Subjective Mood, and Alertness in Healthy Adults. Environ. Behav. 2019, 51, 199–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Min, B.-K.; Jung, Y.-C.; Kim, E.; Park, J.Y. Bright Illumination Reduces Parietal EEG Alpha Activity during a Sustained Attention Task. Brain Res. 2013, 1538, 83–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Diagram of eye-level illuminance measurement points.
Figure 1. Diagram of eye-level illuminance measurement points.
Sensors 24 04927 g001
Figure 2. Schematic diagram of the measurement method for road surface illuminance.
Figure 2. Schematic diagram of the measurement method for road surface illuminance.
Sensors 24 04927 g002
Figure 3. Top view of the layout of the simulation platform.
Figure 3. Top view of the layout of the simulation platform.
Sensors 24 04927 g003
Figure 4. Schematic diagram of the solution for the control of illumination in the laboratory.
Figure 4. Schematic diagram of the solution for the control of illumination in the laboratory.
Sensors 24 04927 g004
Figure 5. Photographs of scenes from experiments with different lighting schemes.
Figure 5. Photographs of scenes from experiments with different lighting schemes.
Sensors 24 04927 g005
Figure 6. 3D modeling of virtual driving scenes with different light environment scenarios.
Figure 6. 3D modeling of virtual driving scenes with different light environment scenarios.
Sensors 24 04927 g006
Figure 7. Software and hardware for intelligent dimming module sets.
Figure 7. Software and hardware for intelligent dimming module sets.
Sensors 24 04927 g007
Figure 8. Difference in spectral distribution between single light and mixed light.
Figure 8. Difference in spectral distribution between single light and mixed light.
Sensors 24 04927 g008
Figure 9. Task setup instructions (screenshot of SCANeR Studio interface).
Figure 9. Task setup instructions (screenshot of SCANeR Studio interface).
Sensors 24 04927 g009
Figure 10. The procedure of the experiment.
Figure 10. The procedure of the experiment.
Sensors 24 04927 g010
Figure 11. The procedure of the aPVT.
Figure 11. The procedure of the aPVT.
Sensors 24 04927 g011
Figure 12. β at different CCT and Illumination levels.
Figure 12. β at different CCT and Illumination levels.
Sensors 24 04927 g012
Figure 13. RT at different CCT and Illumination levels (Task3).
Figure 13. RT at different CCT and Illumination levels (Task3).
Sensors 24 04927 g013
Figure 14. KSS scores at different CCT and Illumination levels.
Figure 14. KSS scores at different CCT and Illumination levels.
Sensors 24 04927 g014
Table 1. Research variables and parameterization.
Table 1. Research variables and parameterization.
VariableLevelParameters
Independent variableCCT33000K, 4000K, 5000K
Illumination
(the average illuminance of the road surface)
220 lx, 30 lx
Task3Task 1 (baseline): Monotonous driving.
Task 2: Waiting for traffic lights and experiencing traffic congestion.
Task 3: Auditory Psychomotor Vigilance Task (aPVT; Dual-task experimental paradigm)
Implicit variableEEG (β waves)
Reaction time (RT)
Table 2. The measurement results of illuminance on actual roads.
Table 2. The measurement results of illuminance on actual roads.
Lane 1
L1
Lane 2
L2
Lane 3
L3
Lane 4
L4
The Average Illuminance (Eye-Level) of the Entire Road = (L1 + L2 + L3 + L4)/4
Dynamic average (eye-level) illuminance (lx)3.8572.7712.7062.9423.069
Static average (eye-level) illuminance (lx)3.2143.2293.1003.2293.193
Maintained average illuminance of road surface (lx)28.5
Table 3. Parameter settings for simulation of the virtual light source models.
Table 3. Parameter settings for simulation of the virtual light source models.
Setting ObjectsParameters
CCT (Actual Luminaires)3000k4000k5000k
RGB (3D Max models)(255, 161, 72)(255, 209, 163)(255, 228, 206)
Table 4. Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS).
Table 4. Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS).
ItemScore
1
Very alert2
Alert3
Rather alert4
Neither alert nor sleepy5
Some signs of sleepiness6
Sleepy, but no effort to keep awake7
Sleepy, but some effort to keep awake8
Very sleepy, great effort to keep awake, fighting sleep9
Extremely sleepy, can’t keep awake10
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Chen, Q.; Pan, Z.; Wu, J.; Xue, C. An Investigation into the Effects of Correlated Color Temperature and Illuminance of Urban Motor Vehicle Road Lighting on Driver Alertness. Sensors 2024, 24, 4927. https://doi.org/10.3390/s24154927

AMA Style

Chen Q, Pan Z, Wu J, Xue C. An Investigation into the Effects of Correlated Color Temperature and Illuminance of Urban Motor Vehicle Road Lighting on Driver Alertness. Sensors. 2024; 24(15):4927. https://doi.org/10.3390/s24154927

Chicago/Turabian Style

Chen, Quan, Zelei Pan, Jinchun Wu, and Chengqi Xue. 2024. "An Investigation into the Effects of Correlated Color Temperature and Illuminance of Urban Motor Vehicle Road Lighting on Driver Alertness" Sensors 24, no. 15: 4927. https://doi.org/10.3390/s24154927

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop