1. Introduction
Disgust is a basic emotion [
1] that can be identified across cultures [
2,
3]. At first, disgust was associated with the repulsive feeling caused by the prospect of oral incorporation of an offensive object, such as rotten food or body waste products [
3]. However, later studies indicated that disgust is associated not only with food rejection but also with a wide range of elicitors, such as body products, death, body envelope violations, hygiene, sex, and socio-moral violations, etc. [
4]. More recently, it has been suggested that the emotion of disgust acts as a psychological mechanism that protects humans and other animals, by motivating behavioral avoidance of possible contaminants [
5,
6].
Disgust, as a basic emotion, has a distinct facial expression, which consists of three main components, the gape, the retraction of the upper lip, and the nose wrinkle [
7]. It is also accompanied with specific physiological reaction (nausea), subjective feeling state (revulsion), and a behavioral response (keeping distance from the object of disgust) [
3]. Recent neuroimaging studies suggest that the anterior insula is associated with the emotion of disgust as it was activated during the exposure of healthy participants to disgust relevant stimuli [
8,
9,
10].
The individual ease or tendency to experience disgust more quickly and intensively is termed as disgust propensity [
11]. This personality trait has been shown to predict lower willingness to approach disgust elicitors [
12] and potentiate classical conditioning of disgust [
13]. Moreover, disgust propensity has been shown to be positively correlated with activation of the anterior insula during exposure to disgusting stimuli [
14]. Heightened levels of disgust propensity have been linked with various psychiatric disorders [
15,
16]. For example, it has been shown that disgust propensity is associated with contamination-based obsessive compulsive disorder [
17,
18] and contamination concerns [
19], spider phobia [
20], blood-injection-injury phobia [
21], and eating disorders [
22], as measured by self-report methods. Elevated disgust propensity levels have also been observed in patients with schizophrenia and depression, relative to controls [
23,
24]. A recent study showed that disgust propensity is associated with a negative body image and this relationship was mediated by self-disgust [
25]. A widely used psychometric tool for measuring individual differences in disgust propensity is the Disgust Scale-Revised (DS-R) [
26]. The DS-R is derived from the Disgust Scale (DS) and it is an improved and more reliable version of the tool [
27,
28]. The DS was developed by Haidt et al. [
4], in order to study the emotional state of disgust, and focused on the stimuli that elicited disgust. It consists of 32 items and 8 domains of disgust elicitors, more specifically, animals, food, hygiene, magical thinking, sex, body products, death, and envelope violations. However, despite the fact that the internal consistency of the total scale was good (
α = 0.84) [
4], the eight subscales demonstrated a compromised reliability that could be attributed to the small number of items in each domain,
α’s = 0.34–0.64 [
4] and
α’s = 0.40–0.68 [
26].
Consequently, Olatunji et al. [
26], after a series of analyses, suggested the reduction of the eight subscales to three (Core, Animal Reminder, and Contamination Disgust), which led to the development of DS-R. Core disgust (CO) concerns the prospective of oral incorporation of offensive items, including rotten food, small animals, and body waste products. The animal reminder disgust (AR) is related to stimuli that remind us our animal nature and mortality, such as body envelope violations and dead bodies. Lastly, contamination disgust (CD) describes the perceived threat of contagion via body byproducts and microbes [
26]. The newly revised DS-R has 27 items measured on a 5-point Likert Scale (ranging from 0 to 4), 25 of them concern disgust and 2 of them are “catch” questions for identifying poor responders.
Studies that were conducted later gave additional evidence to the three factor structure [
19,
27,
28], which was shown to have a better fit than the one or two factor (animal reminder and core-contamination) models [
12,
26]. Further support for the three factor model was provided by Olatunji et al. [
1,
19] who found evidence for a bi-factor model of disgust, which suggested that although there is a general disgust propensity factor that underlay all the items, the three DS-R factors still captures unique variance.
The DS-R scale has been validated and its three factors confirmed internationally, including a Korean sample [
29], students in the Netherlands [
28], samples from the United States [
19,
26], an Israeli sample [
30], and samples from Brazil, Australia, Germany, Italy, Japan, and the Netherlands [
27]. The factor structure of the DS-R has also been examined in a sample of adolescent students from the United States [
31].
Previous studies have shown evidence of the influence of several demographics characteristics on DS-R scores. More specifically, disgust propensity has been reported to be higher in women than in men [
4,
19,
27,
30,
31], and this finding was recently supported by an extensive meta-analysis of 90 studies from different cultures [
32]. Disgust propensity has also been associated with religiosity [
4,
30]. A weak negative association with age has also been reported [
30,
33]. However, due to the restricted number of evidence, there is still need of further investigation on the influence of demographic characteristics on disgust propensity.
Given the association of disgust propensity with the etiology of some psychopathological conditions, a psychometrically sound tool measuring this construct, is of high clinical importance. In clinical practice, the tool can be used to get more insight on the symptoms, in several psychopathological conditions that have been shown to be associated with disgust (e.g., assessing whether symptoms of anxiety- and OCD-related disorders are mostly motivated by fear, distress, or disgust). As disgust has been shown to be more resistant in extinction and exposure therapy, than in fear [
34], and the reduction of disgust propensity has been associated with improvement in obsessive-compulsive symptoms, after therapy [
35,
36], a measure of disgust propensity could also be valuable for designing (e.g., interventions that might be effective in reducing disgust [
34]) and assessing treatments for anxiety and Obsessive Compulsive and related disorders. Consequently, the DS-R could be very useful in Greek clinical and research settings. However, there is no Greek version of the DS-R available yet. Therefore, the adaptation of the DS-R into Greek and the psychometric assessment of this version is very important.
The present study had four main purposes. The first aim was to assess the psychometric properties and to identify the factor structure of the translated version of the DS-R, on a sample of Greek participants. The second aim was to assess the goodness of fit of various factor structures, previously reported in the literature, including the one, two, and three factor models [
26,
28], and their corresponding bi-factor analogs [
19]. The third aim was to evaluate the measurement invariance of the scale, with respect to age and gender. The fourth aim was to examine the influence of the demographic characteristics of our Greek sample on the DS-R scores.
4. Discussion
In the present study, we evaluated the psychometric properties, the factor structure, and the measurement invariance (with respect to age and gender) of the Greek-version of the DS-R. Our results support the Olatunji et al. [
26] 3-factor solution.
The EFA method concluded to a 3-factor solution with similar structure as that of the original 3-factor model, presented by Olatunji et al. [
26]. Our F2 factor was almost identical to the AR factor of the 3-factor model, as all AR items loaded to this factor. However, two items cross-loaded to the other two factors. In particular, items 10 (“watch a person with a glass eye take the eye out of the socket”) and item 19 (“pick up dead cat with bare hands”) also loaded to the F3 factor (similar to the Contamination disgust factor), which might reflect the Greek sample’s association of a mutilated/deformed body with illness and a source of contamination. Moreover, there was a cross loading of item 8 (“see someone vomit”, originally in CD) with F1 (similar to CD) and F2. This finding might be attributed to the fact that in the Greek culture seeing someone vomit, is not only characterized by its offensive nature and inappropriateness for oral incorporation, but is also a reminder of the vulnerability and animal origin of humans. However, these speculations regarding Greek culture should be investigated further in future studies. Regarding the other two factors, items from both CO and CD loaded on the F1 and F3 factors, meaning that the two original factors were merged in our data. Another fact to consider is that, in this model, 6 items cross-loaded (similar or identical load) onto two factors, which might have affected the appropriateness of the model.
The 2-factor EFA solution also had an adequate fit to our data, and had fewer cross loadings than the previous solution. The present model seems to be almost identical with the two-factor model proposed by Rozin et al. in 2000 (cited in Olatunji et al. [
26]), in which CO and CD formed one factor. The only difference with the proposed model was that, item 19 (“pick up dead cat with bare hands”) loaded onto the CO and CD factors, instead of the AR factor, and item 8 (“see someone vomit”) loaded onto the AR factor, instead of the other factor.
CFA was used to evaluate and compare the unidimensional model, the 2-factor and the 3-factor models of disgust, as well as their corresponding bi-factor models. Contrary to previous studies [
26,
28] in which the three-factor model had a superior fit to the one and two-factor models, all models were shown to have a close fit to the data, with no substantial differences between them. The bi-factor models had a slightly better fit to the data, in accord with Olatunji et al. [
19].
In the bi-factor analyses, all loadings of the items on their specific factors were reduced, nine of them became non-significant after the addition of the general factor of disgust, and most of them were smaller than the loadings on the general factor. This finding might be explained by the general and broad nature of core disgust [
3,
7]. Nevertheless, four items either retained the magnitude of their significant loadings (item 3, CO, item 4, CD) or increased it (item 6, CD, item 7, AR), after the introduction of the general factor to the model, indicating that these items were specific to the corresponding domains of disgust. In conclusion, according to the recommendations of Reise et al. [
44], the bi-factor analyses suggests that there is no loss of information if the DS-R is used as a unidimensional measurement scale via its total score, but at the same time the three-factor solution has a good fit to the data and can also be applied. Our results are partially in line with the ones reported by Olatunji et al. [
19], but the importance of the general disgust factor might be more pronounced in the Greek population and this could be explained by its culture and the everyday practices for avoiding disgusting objects. As suggested by one of the reviewers, the scale would potentially be benefited by appropriately revising the set of items, to empower the multidimensionality aspect.
Our finding regarding the strength of the general factor of disgust underlines its importance in disgust propensity. A common characteristic of most disgust elicitors is their animal nature. They are of animal origin as they might be animals (small animals that elicit disgust), animal byproducts, or reminders of our animal nature and vulnerability [
3]. However, to the best of our knowledge, this is the first study showing the superiority of the general factor, over the three factor model. This finding was less pronounced in the first study of Olatunji et al. [
19], who also applied a bi-factor model to their data. As a result, no safe assumptions can be made regarding the unidimensionality of disgust, yet, as this finding could be unique in the Greek population. Future research could evaluate the bi-factor model of disgust in other countries, to examine how the importance of the general disgust factor to the total variance, differs across cultures
The measurement invariance testing, with respect to gender and age revealed that there are considerations of measurement bias. Specifically, the present study suggests gender non-invariance in 6 items. Items 4, 9, 22, 27 (2 CD, 2 CO) were more likely to be endorsed by women. Whereas, items 20 and 25 (CO) were stronger indicators for men. The reason for this was still unclear. However, all four items that women agreed on more, described possible contaminants, and both items that men agreed on more, described possible oral incorporation of inappropriate foods. Kim et al. [
31] also found gender non-invariance, though in different items. However, the items that adolescent boys scored higher on, described food products—a finding that is in partial agreement with the present study. Finally, the present study also revealed that item 9 (CD) and less profoundly items 15 and 4, were more likely to have larger scores, as age increased. To our knowledge, this was the first study to examine measurement invariance in the DS-R item, with respect to age. More research is needed to examine measurement invariance of the DS-R, regarding the different demographic factors.
All reliability indices of the scale were satisfactory. The test-retest reliability indices confirmed the stability of the scale. The Cronbach’s alpha of the total scale and the CO and AR subscales were high, showing a satisfactory internal consistency. However, the internal consistency of the CD subscale was relatively low, being in line with previous findings [
26,
28,
29], and this could be attributed to some extent to the fact that, there was a small number of items in this factor. This could be addressed by increasing the number of items in the contamination subscale or adopting fewer factor solutions. All inter-correlations between the three factors of the DS-R were strong and statistically significant.
The present study also assessed the influence of demographic characteristics (religiousness, education, employment, income, gender, and age) on DS-R scores. First, religiousness was associated with the mean scores of all disgust factors, indicating that more religious participants tended to report higher levels of disgust. To our knowledge, this was the first study in which disgust propensity was assessed in an eastern orthodox sample. This finding was in accord with previous studies in which religiousness was associated with heightened levels of disgust [
4,
52]. Moreover, there was a weak negative correlation between levels of disgust and years of education, which contradicted the finding of Berger et al. [
30], in which disgust level was independent of education. Income was also independent of disgust, showing that it was unaffected by the financial and social status.
The present study also showed that women scored significantly higher than men in all DS-R factors and on the total scale. More pronounced disgust propensity in females than in males was also found in previous studies [
27,
29,
53], while a recent one revealed a modest female superiority, over males, in recognizing facial disgust cues [
54]. Heightened levels of disgust in women might be explained by the compensatory behavioral prophylaxis hypothesis [
55]. This hypothesis states that heightened disgust was employed in response to the compromised immune system, during the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle (when the body was prepared for possible pregnancy) and the early stages of pregnancy [
55,
56]. A recent series of two studies had also suggested that this difference in disgust between the two genders might be explained by risk-taking differences, where males were more willing to take risks than women, to attain some goals, and this might include getting exposed to pathogens [
32]. These remain to be further investigated by future studies.
Finally, the present study revealed a weak but significant correlation between age and Contamination Disgust, but not with the other subscales or the total score. This finding contradicts previous research in which it was shown that disgust propensity was less pronounced in the elderly [
30], as emotion control seemed to be improved with age [
57]. More research is needed to evaluate the development of disgust propensity and reactivity, across the life span.
Consistent with the findings of Haidt et al. [
4], examining the original DS, the total DS-R score was positively correlated with the EPQ neuroticism subscale, and negatively correlated with the EPQ psychoticism subscale. The correlation coefficients, though weak, were of similar magnitude with those observed by Haidt et al. [
4] and Kang et al. [
29], providing evidence of concurrent validity of the Greek version, related to personality correlates. Τhe total score of the DS-R was also positively correlated with all subscales of SCL-90, a finding that is in line with previous research in which the DS [
4] was used [
58]. However, these correlations were all weak (varied between 0.2 and 0.3) but of similar magnitude to those reported previously [
58].
Regarding the discriminant validity, there was no correlation between the extraversion subscale and the total DS-R score, but there was a weak significant negative correlation between this subscale and the Animal Reminder mean score. However, there was an unpredicted but weak correlation between the Lie subscale and the total DS-R score, which was inconsistent with previous research [
4,
29]. This might suggest a possibly compromised discriminant validity of the Greek DS-R, as self-presentation concerns might have affected the responses. Nevertheless, given that the correlation was weak, this limitation was restricted. Future research could use additional measures, such as the Self-Monitoring Scale by Snyder (1997), as cited in [
4], in the Greek DS-R, to further assess discriminant validity.
The present study had several limitations. Due to the lack of appropriate standardized psychometric tools relevant to the construct of disgust and OCD symptomatology in the Greek population, the assessment of the convergent validity of the Greek version of the DS-R was not feasible at this time point. The DS-R was the first measurement of disgust that has been validated in the Greek population, no other psychometrically assessed measurement exists. Moreover, the association between the DS-R and the OCD symptomatology, which could be examined as an indication of convergent validity, as in previous studies, [
26], was assessed only by the OCD subscale of the SCL-90, which did not allow the discrimination of OCD symptoms types (e.g., Contamination Concerns versus Checking). However, there are no published validation of an OCD scale in the Greek population available for use, yet, so such a measurement could not be used in the present study.
The present results lay the foundation for further research. For example, future study could evaluate the predictive validity of the Greek DS-R and its domains, by using behavioral avoidance and physiological tasks, e.g., [
53]. Τhe DS-R could also be administered in Greek participants that have been diagnosed with disorders associated with an elevated disgust propensity, such as obsessive-compulsive disorder, blood-injection injury, and small animal special phobias [
15]. The DS-R scale, alongside other measures of disgust, could also be used to assess disgust propensity in patients with an acute onset of Obsessive-Compulsive symptoms, which could be explained by inflammatory, infective, and immunological conditions, such as ‘Pediatric Autoimmune Neuropsychiatric Disorders Associated with Streptococcal Infections’ (PANDAS) [
59]. The Greek version of the DS-R could also be used to adapt and validate other scales of disgust in the Greek population, which could be useful in clinical settings. An example of these scales is the Propensity and Sensitivity Scale (DPSS) [
60], which assesses—in addition to the disgust propensity—disgust sensitivity (negative appreciation of experiencing disgust), which have also been associated with psychopathology [
61,
62]. Finally, given that our results outlined the strength of the general factor of digust, future research could assess the dimensionality of the scale, by using the bi-factor model, in participants from various cultures, and further revising the scale to improve its structure.