Monitoring the Own Spatial Thinking in Second Grade of Primary Education in a Spanish School: Preliminary Study Analyzing Gender Differences
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Metacognition, Spatial Reasoning and Education
1.2. Metacognitive Processes in Primary School
1.3. The Gender Issue in Spatial Tasks
1.4. Applying the Accesibility Model to Spatial Metacognition at Primary School
- It was hypothesized that boys and girls of the Second Year of Primary Education will be able to evaluate their own performance in a spatial ability test, as directly examined in other cognitive tasks for this age group (as mathematical performance, [9]).
- It is expected to replicate the results of previous studies [33,34,35,36] in this early childhood sample and with the applied spatial task, hypothesizing a dissociation of accessibility and Accuracy Indexes. The confidence indexes of each item of the applied spatial test were obtained to analyze their relationship with the probability of receiving a correct answer (OBA) and with the probability that they receive any response (ACC). It is expected to replicate Ruiz and Contreras’ [33] results, and following Koriat’s [35] model, a greater relationship of the Confidence judgments with accessibility than with precision was hypothesized.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Materials
- -
- Accessibility Index (ACC): which reflects the amount of information that comes to the participant’s mind once the question is raised. In order to calculate this index, the percentage of participants who have given a response is taken into account, regardless of its accuracy;
- -
- Precision Index (OBA): which reflects the probability that the information that comes to the participant′s mind is correct. It is calculated from the percentage of correct answers among the total answers given.
2.3. Procedure
3. Results
3.1. Individual and Gender Analyses
3.2. Items Analyses
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Ethics Approval and Consent to Participate
Availability of Data and Materials
References
- Santamaría, P.; Arribas, D.; Pereña, J.; Seisdedos, N. EFAI, Evaluación Factorial de las Aptitudes Intelectuales; TEA Ediciones; Departamento I+D: Madrid, Spain, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- O’Leary, A.P.; Sloutsky, V.M. Components of metacognition can function independently across development. Dev. Psychol. 2018, 55, 315–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Koriat, A.; Goldsmith, M. Monitoring and control processes in the strategic regulation of memory accuracy. Psychol. Rev. 1996, 103, 490–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Du Puis, D.; Ram, N.; Willner, C.J.; Karalunas, S.; Segalowitz, S.J.; Gatzke-Kopp, L.M. Implications of ongoing neural development for the measurement of the error-related negativity in childhood. Dev. Sci. 2015, 18, 452–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Santesso, D.L.; Segalowitz, S.J.; Schmidt, L.A. Error-related electrocortical responses in 10-year-old children and young adults. Dev. Sci. 2006, 9, 473–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zelazo, P.D. The development of conscious control in childhood. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2004, 8, 12–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chevalier, N.; Blaye, A. Metacognitive monitoring of executive control engagement during childhood. Child Dev. 2016, 87, 1264–1276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wall, J.L.; Thompson, C.A.; Dunlosky, J.D.; Merriman, W.E. Children can accurately monitor and control their number lined estimation performance. Dev. Psychol. 2016, 52, 1493–1502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Neys, W.; Feremans, V. Development of Heuristic Bias Detection in Elementary School. Dev. Psychol. 2013, 49, 258–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Estes, Z.; Felker, S. Confidence Mediates the Sex Difference in Mental Rotation Performance. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2012, 41, 557–570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Neys, W.; Verschueren, N. Working memory capacity and a notorious brain teaser: The case of the Monty Hall Dilemma. Exp. Psychol. 2006, 53, 123–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newstead, S.E.; Handley, S.J.; Harley, C.; Wright, H.; Farelly, D. Individual differences in deductive reasoning. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A Hum. Exp. Psychol. 2004, 57, 33–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stanovich, K.E.; West, R.F. Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behav. Brain Sci. 2000, 23, 645–665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cheng, Y.-L.; Mix, K.S. Spatial training improves children’s mathematics ability. J. Cogn. Dev. 2014, 15, 2–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mix, K.S.; Levine, S.C.; Cheng, Y.-L.; Young, C.; Hambrick, D.Z.; Ping, R.; Konstantopoulos, S. Separate but correlated: The latent structure of space and mathematics across development. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2016, 145, 1206–1227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gunderson, E.A.; Ramirez, R.; Beilock, S.L.; Levine, S.C. The relation between spatial skills and early number knowledge: The role of the linear number line. Dev. Psychol. 2012, 48, 1229–1241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Olkun, S.; Altun, A.; Smith, G. Computers and 2D geometric learning of Turkish fourth and fifth graders. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 2005, 36, 317–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Voyer, D.; Voyer, S.; Bryden, M.P. Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychol. Bull. 1995, 117, 250–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sorby, S. Developing spatial cognitive skills among middle school students. Cogn. Process. 2009, 10, 312–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frick, A. Spatial transformation abilities and their relation to later mathematics performance. Psychol. Res. 2019, 83, 1465–1484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Uttal, D.H.; Meadow, N.G.; Tipton, E.; Hand, L.L.; Alden, A.R.; Warren, C.; Newcombe, N.S. The malleability of spatial skills: A meta-analysis of training studies. Psychol. Bull. 2013, 139, 352–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vandenberg, S.G.; Kuse, A.R. Mental rotations, a group test of three-dimensional spatial visualization. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1978, 47, 599–604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Halpern, D. Sex Differences in Cognitive Abilities, 4th ed.; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Linn, M.C.; Petersen, A.C. Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Dev. 1985, 56, 1479–1498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Maccoby, E.E.; Jacklin, C.N. The Psychology of Sex Differences; Stanford University Press: Stanford, CA, USA, 1974. [Google Scholar]
- Lippa, R.A.; Collaer, M.L.; Peters, M. Sex differences in mental rotation and line angle judgments are positively associated with gender equality and economic development across 53 nations. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2010, 39, 990–997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cooke-Simpson, A.; Voyer, D. Confidence and gender differences on the Mental Rotations Test. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2007, 17, 181–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Estes, Z. Confidence and gradedness in semantic categorization: Definitely somewhat artifactual, maybe absolutely natural. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2004, 11, 1041–1047. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rahe, M.; Ruthsatz, V.; Jansen, P.; Quaiser-Pohl, C. Different practice effects for males and females by psychometric and chronometric mental-rotation tests. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2019, 31, 92–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shepard, R.N.; Metzler, J. Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science 1971, 171, 701–703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Moè, A.; Pazzaglia, F. Following the instructions! Effects of gender beliefs in mental rotation. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2006, 16, 369–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moè, A.; Meneghetti, C.; Cadinu, M. Women and mental rotation: Incremental theory and spatial strategy use enhance performance. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2009, 46, 187–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ruiz, M.; Contreras, M.J. Juicios de Metamemoria: Contraste del Modelo de Accesibilidad mediante 300 preguntas de cultura general. In Lecturas de Psicología. Experimental; Contreras, M.J., Botella, J., Cabestrero, R., de Liaño, B., Eds.; UNED: Madrid, Spain, 2006; pp. 37–46. [Google Scholar]
- Koriat, A. Dissociating knowing and the feeling of knowing: Further evidence for the accessibility model. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 1995, 124, 311–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koriat, A. How do we know that we know? The accessibility model of the feeling of knowing. Psychol. Rev. 1993, 100, 609–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Koriat, A. Memory’s knowledge of its own knowledge: The accessibility account of the feeling of knowing. In Metacognition: Knowing about Knowing; Metcalfe, J., Shimamura, A.P., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1994; pp. 115–135. [Google Scholar]
- Raven, J.; Court, J.H.; Raven, J.C. Standard Progressive Matrices; Oxford Psychologists Press: Oxford, UK, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Koriat, A.; Goldsmith, M. Memory in naturalistic and laboratory contexts: Distinguishing the accuracy-oriented and quantity-oriented approaches to memory assessment. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 1994, 123, 297–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kruger, J.; Dunning, D. Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficulties in recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 77, 1121–1134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koriat, A.; Lichtenstein, S.; Fischhoff, B. Reasons for Confidence. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. Mem. 1980, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fernández-Méndez, L.M.; Contreras, M.J.; Elosúa, M.R. From what age is mental rotation training effective? Differences in Preschool age but not in sex. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fernández-Méndez, L.M.; Contreras, M.J.; Elosúa, M.R. Developmental differences between 1st and 3rd year of early Childhood Education (Preschool) in Mental Rotation and its Training. Psychol. Res. 2020, 84, 1056–1064. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rodán, A.; Contreras, M.J.; Elosúa, M.R.; Gimeno, P. Experimental but not sex differences of a mental rotation training program on adolescents. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rodán, A.; Gimeno, P.; Elosúa, M.R.; Montoro, P.R.; Contreras, M.J. Boys and girls gain in spatial, but not in mathematical ability after mental rotation training in primary education. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2019, 70, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geiser, C.; Lehmann, W.; Corth, M.; Eid, M. Quantitative and qualitative change in children’s mental rotation performance. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2008, 18, 419–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peters, M.; Chisholm, P.; Laeng, B. Spatial Ability, Student Gender and Academic Performance. J. Eng. Educ. 1995, 84, 69–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Titze, C.; Jansen, P.; Heil, M. Mental rotation performance and the effect of gender in fourth graders and adults. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2010, 7, 432–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brosnan, M.J. Spatial ability in children’s play with Lego blocks. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1998, 87, 19–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chien, S.-C. The Effectiveness of Animated and Interactive Microcomputer Graphics on Children’s Development of Spatial Visualization Ability/mental Rotation Skills. Ph.D. Thesis, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Shavalier, M. The effects of CAD-like software on the spatial ability of middle school students. J. Educ. Comput. Res. 2004, 31, 37–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Terlecki, M.S.; Brown, J.; Harner-Steciw, L.; Irvin-Hannum, J.; Marchetto-Ryan, N.; Ruhl, L.; Wiggins, J. Sex Differences and Similarities in Video Game Experience, Preferences, and Self-Efficacy: Implications for the Gaming Industry. Curr. Psychol. 2011, 30, 22–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, C.S.; Uttal, D.H. Cultural values, parents beliefs, and children achievement in the United-States and China. Hum. Dev. 1988, 31, 335–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Total (n = 33) | Boys (n = 18) | Girls (n = 15) | F | p | Cohen’s d | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
EFAI-E | 12.33 (5.02) | 12.61 (6.25) | 12 (3.16) | 0.12 | 0.73 | 0.12 |
Raven | 25.97 (9.42) | 25.94 (10.16) | 26 (8.79) | 0.00 | 0.98 | 0.00 |
CJ | 3.33 (0.58) | 3.3 (0.69) | 3.36 (0.57) | 0.06 | 0.80 | 0.09 |
Mean | S.D. | MEDIAN | |
---|---|---|---|
ACC (%) | 68.8 | 30.23 | 80.5 |
OBA (%) | 57.5 | 21.91 | 55.5 |
CJ | 3.47 | 0.27 | 3.47 |
HIGH OBA ≥55.5% | LOW OBA ≤55.5% | |
---|---|---|
HIGH ACC ≥80.5% | MC = 3.53 (N = 9) | MC = 3.49 (N = 6) |
LOW ACC ≤80.5% | MC = 3.58 (N = 6) | MC = 3.33 (N = 9) |
OBA | CJ | |
---|---|---|
ACC | 0.22 | 0.06 |
OBA | - | 0.42 * |
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Contreras, M.J.; Meneghetti, C.; Uttal, D.H.; Fernández-Méndez, L.M.; Rodán, A.; Montoro, P.R. Monitoring the Own Spatial Thinking in Second Grade of Primary Education in a Spanish School: Preliminary Study Analyzing Gender Differences. Educ. Sci. 2020, 10, 237. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci10090237
Contreras MJ, Meneghetti C, Uttal DH, Fernández-Méndez LM, Rodán A, Montoro PR. Monitoring the Own Spatial Thinking in Second Grade of Primary Education in a Spanish School: Preliminary Study Analyzing Gender Differences. Education Sciences. 2020; 10(9):237. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci10090237
Chicago/Turabian StyleContreras, María José, Chiara Meneghetti, David H. Uttal, Laura M. Fernández-Méndez, Antonio Rodán, and Pedro R. Montoro. 2020. "Monitoring the Own Spatial Thinking in Second Grade of Primary Education in a Spanish School: Preliminary Study Analyzing Gender Differences" Education Sciences 10, no. 9: 237. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci10090237
APA StyleContreras, M. J., Meneghetti, C., Uttal, D. H., Fernández-Méndez, L. M., Rodán, A., & Montoro, P. R. (2020). Monitoring the Own Spatial Thinking in Second Grade of Primary Education in a Spanish School: Preliminary Study Analyzing Gender Differences. Education Sciences, 10(9), 237. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci10090237