Is There a Right Way? Productive Patterns of Interaction during Collaborative Problem Solving
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Mathematics Lessons
2.3. Data Collection
2.4. Selection of Pairs for Analysis
2.5. Coding of Student Participation
- Noemi:
- (Draws seven circles) Put 15 [in each circle].
- Melanie:
- Why are we putting 15 in it?
- Noemi:
- Because we are gonna split it in 5, 5, and 5.
- Melanie:
- Why are we gonna split it?
- Noemi:
- Because it’s easier to do fives instead of fifteens.
- Melanie:
- Isn’t it more efficient to do fifteen?
- Noemi:
- Well, in fives you can just keep on counting because fives are really easy.
- Melanie:
- But don’t you already know that 15 plus 15 is 30? So it’s more easier than that way (nods at Noemi’s paper).
- Mira:
- Can you explain what you are doing?
- Donatello:
- I’m counting by fifteens, seven times.
- Mira:
- So you are counting by fifteens?
- Donatello:
- 45…
- Mira:
- How do you know the next number is 30? (points to Donatello’s notebook)
2.6. Coding of Group Interaction
2.6.1. Number of Students Leading the Math
2.6.2. Continuity of the Group’s Interaction
2.6.3. Patterns of How Group Interaction Unfolded over Time
2.6.4. Coding Process for Group Interaction Variables
3. Results
3.1. Summary of Highly-Detailed Participation in the Pairs
3.2. Relationship between Leading the Math and Highly-Detailed Participation
3.3. Relationship between Continuity of the Group’s Interaction and Highly-Detailed Participation
3.4. How Interaction Unfolded in the Pairs
3.5. Emergence of Highly-Detailed Participation during Pair Interaction
4. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Mercer, N.; Hennessy, S.; Warwick, P. Dialogue, thinking together and digital technology in the classroom: Some educational implications of a continuing line of inquiry. Int. J. Educ. Res. 2019, 97, 187–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Michaels, S.; O’Connor, C.; Resnick, L.B. Deliberative discourse idealized and realized: Accountable talk in the classroom and in civic life. Stud. Educ. 2008, 27, 283–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Resnick, L.B.; Michaels, S.; O’Connor, M.C. How (well-structured) talk builds the mind. In Innovations in Educational Psychology: Perspectives on Learning, Teaching, and Human Development; Preiss, D.D., Sternberg, R.J., Eds.; Springer Publishing Company: New York, NY, USA, 2010; pp. 163–194. [Google Scholar]
- Berkowitz, M.W.; Gibbs, J.C. The process of moral conflict resolution and moral development. In Peer Conflict and Psychological Growth. New Directions for Child Development; Berkowitz, M.W., Ed.; Jossey-Bass: San Francisco, CA, USA, 1985; pp. 71–84. [Google Scholar]
- Kruger, A.C. Peer collaboration: Conflict, cooperation, or both? Soc. Dev. 1993, 2, 165–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goos, M.; Galbraith, P.; Renshaw, P. Socially mediated metacognition: Creating collaborative zones of proximal development in small group problem solving. Educ. Stud. Math. 2002, 49, 193–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Asterhan, C.S.C.; Schwarz, B.B. Argumentation for learning: Well-trodden paths and unexplored territories. Educ. Psychol. 2016, 51, 164–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuhn, D.; Crowell, A. Dialogic argumentation as a vehicle for developing young adolescents’ thinking. Psych. Sci. 2011, 22, 545–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kuhn, D.; Wang, Y.; Li, L. Why argue? Developing understanding of the purposes and values of argumentive discourse. Discourse Process. 2011, 48, 26–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Resnick, L.B.; Schantz, F. Talking to learn: The promise and challenge of dialogic teaching. In Socializing Intelligence through Talk and Dialogue; Resnick, L.B., Asterhan, C., Clarke, S.N., Eds.; American Educational Research Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2015; pp. 441–450. [Google Scholar]
- Waggoner, M.; Chinn, C.; Yi, H.; Anderson, R.C. Collaborative reasoning about stories. Lang. Arts 1995, 72, 582–589. [Google Scholar]
- Barron, B. Achieving coordination in collaborative problem-solving groups. J. Learn. Sci. 2000, 9, 403–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Azmitia, M.; Montgomery, R. Friendship, transactive dialogues, and the development of scientific reasoning. Soc. Dev. 1993, 2, 202–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Forman, E.A.; Kraker, M.J. The social origins of logic: The contributions of Piaget and Vygotsky. In Peer Conflict and Psychological Growth. New Directions for Child Development; Berkowitz, M.W., Ed.; Jossey-Bass: San Francisco, CA, USA, 1985; pp. 23–39. [Google Scholar]
- Hatano, G. Time to merge Vygotskian and constructivist conceptions of knowledge acquisition. In Contexts for Learning: Sociocultural Dynamics in Children’s Development; Forman, E.A., Minick, N., Stone, C.A., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1993; pp. 153–166. [Google Scholar]
- Hogan, K.; Nastasi, B.K.; Pressley, M. Discourse patterns and collaborative scientific reasoning in peer and teacher-guided discussions. Cognition Instruct. 1999, 17, 379–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwartz, D.L. The emergence of abstract representations in dyad problem solving. J. Learn. Sci. 1995, 4, 321–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Volet, S.; Summers, M.; Thurman, J. High-level co-regulation in collaborative learning: How does it emerge and how is it sustained? Learn. Instr. 2009, 19, 128–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Iiskala, T.; Vaurus, M.; Lehtinen, E.; Salonen, P. Socially shared metacognition of dyads of pupils in collaborative mathematical problem-solving processes. Learn. Instr. 2011, 21, 379–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roschelle, J.; Teasley, S. The construction of shared knowledge in collaborative problem solving. In Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning; O’Malley, C., Ed.; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 1995; pp. 69–197. [Google Scholar]
- Vaurus, M.; Iiskala, T.; Kajamies, A.; Kinnunen, R.; Lehtinen, E. Shared-regulation and motivation of collaborating peers: A case study. Psychologia 2003, 46, 19–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mercer, N. The quality of talk in children’s collaborative activity in the classroom. Learn. Instr. 1996, 6, 359–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bargh, J.A.; Schul, Y. On the cognitive benefit of teaching. J. Educ. Psychol. 1980, 72, 593–604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, A.L.; Campione, J.C.; Webber, L.S.; McGilly, K. Interactive learning environments: A new look at assessment and instruction. In Changing Assessments: Alternative Views of Aptitude, Achievement, and Instruction; Gifford, B., O’Connor, M.C., Eds.; Kluwer Academic Publishers: New York, NY, USA, 1992; pp. 121–211. [Google Scholar]
- Chi, M.T.H. Self-explaining expository texts: The dual processes of generating inferences and repairing mental models. In Advances in Instructional Psychology: Educational Design and Cognitive Science; Glaser, R., Ed.; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 2000; pp. 161–238. [Google Scholar]
- Forman, E.A.; Cazden, C.B. Exploring Vygotskian perspectives in education: The cognitive value of peer interaction. In Culture, Communication and Cognition: Vygotskian Perspective; Wertsch, J.V., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1985; pp. 323–347. [Google Scholar]
- Roschelle, J. Learning by collaborating: Convergent conceptual change. J. Learn. Sci. 1992, 2, 235–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Whitebread, D.; Bingham, S.; Grau, V.; Pino Pasternak, D.; Sangster, C. Development of metacognition and self-regulated learning in young children: The role of collaborative and peer-assisted learning. J. Cog. Educ. Psych. 2007, 6, 433–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wittrock, M.C. Generative processes of comprehension. Educ. Psychol. 1990, 24, 345–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Forman, E.A.; Ramirez-DelToro, V.; Brown, L.; Passmore, C. Discursive strategies that foster an epistemic community for argument in a biology classroom. Learn. Instr. 2017, 48, 32–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Howe, C. Advances in research on classroom dialogue: Commentary on the articles. Learn. Instr. 2017, 48, 61–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Howe, C.; Abedin, M. Classroom dialogue: A systematic review across four decades of research. Camb. J. Educ. 2013, 43, 325–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Resnick, L.B.C.; Asterhan, C.S.C.; Clarke, S.N. (Eds). Socializing Intelligence through Academic Talk and Dialogue; AERA: Washington, DC, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Mercer, N.; Dawes, R.; Wegerif, R.; Sams, C. Reasoning as a scientist: Ways of helping children to use language to learn science. Brit. Educ.Res. J. 2004, 30, 367–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mercer, N.; Wegerif, R.; Dawes, L. Children’s talk and the development of reasoning in the classroom. Brit. Educ. Res. J. 1999, 25, 95–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rojas-Drummod, S.; Pérez, V.; Vélez, M.; Gómez, L.; Mendoza, A. Talking for reasoning among Mexican primary school children. Learn. Instr. 2003, 13, 653–670. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veenman, S.; Denessen, E.; van den Akker, A.; van der Rijt, J. Effects of a cooperative learning program on the elaborations of students during help seeking and help giving. Am. Educ. Res. J. 2005, 42, 115–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wegerif, R.; Mercer, N.; Dawes, L. From social interaction to individual reasoning: An empirical investigation of a possible socio-cultural model of cognitive development. Learn. Instr. 1999, 9, 493–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, A.L.; Palincsar, A.S. Guided, cooperative learning and individual knowledge acquisition. In Knowing, Learning, and Instruction: Essays in Honor of Robert Glaser; Resnick, L.B., Ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1989; pp. 393–451. [Google Scholar]
- Chinn, C.A.; Anderson, R.C.; Waggoner, M.A. Patterns of discourse in two kinds of literature discussion. Read. Res. Q. 2001, 36, 378–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gillies, R.M.; Ashman, A.F. Teaching collaborative skills in primary school children in classroom-based work groups. Learn. Instr. 1996, 6, 187–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gillies, R.M.; Ashman, A. Behavior and interactions of children in cooperative groups in lower and middle elementary grades. J. Educ. Psychol. 1998, 90, 746–757. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gillies, R.M. Structuring cooperative group work in classrooms. Int. J. Educ. Res. 2003, 39, 35–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gillies, R.M. The effects of communication training on teachers’ and students’ verbal behaviours during cooperative learning. Int. J. Educ. Res. 2004, 41, 257–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gillies, R.M.; Haynes, M. Increasing explanatory behavior, problem-solving, and reasoning within classes using cooperative group work. Instr. Sci. 2011, 39, 349–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gillies, R.M.; Khan, A. The effects of teacher discourse on students’ discourse, problem-solving and reasoning during cooperative learning. Int. J. Educ. Res. 2008, 47, 323–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Connor, M.C.; Michaels, S.; Chapin, S.H. “Scaling down” to explore the role of talk in learning: From district intervention to controlled classroom study. In Socializing Intelligence through Talk and Dialogue; Resnick, L.B., Asterhan, C., Clarke, S.N., Eds.; American Educational Research Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2015; pp. 111–126. [Google Scholar]
- Palincsar, A.S.; Brown, A. Reciprocal teaching of comprehension-fostering and comprehension-monitoring activities. Cognition Instr. 1984, 1, 117–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Palincsar, A.S.; Herrenkohl, L.R. Designing collaborative contexts: Lessons from three research programs. In The Rutgers Invitational Symposium on Education Series. Cognitive Perspectives on Peer Learning; O’Donnell, A.M., King, A., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1999; pp. 151–177. [Google Scholar]
- Reznitskaya, A.; Anderson, R.C.; Kuo, L. Teaching and learning argumentation. Elem. School J. 2007, 107, 449–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chi, M.T.H.; Menekse, M. Dialogue patterns that promote learning. In Socializing Intelligence through Academic Talk and Dialogue; Resnick, L.B., Asterhan, C., Clark, S.N., Eds.; American Educational Research Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2015; pp. 263–274. [Google Scholar]
- Chinn, C.A.; O’Donnell, A.M.; Jinks, T.S. The structure of discourse in collaborative learning. J. Exp. Educ. 2000, 69, 77–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Howe, C.; Tolmie, A.; Thurston, A.; Topping, K.; Christie, D.; Livingston, K.; Jessiman, E.; Donaldson, C. Group work in elementary science: Towards organisational principles for supporting pupil learning. Learn. Instr. 2007, 17, 549–563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roscoe, R.D.; Chi, M.T.H. Tutor learning: The role of explaining and responding to questions. Instr. Sci. 2008, 36, 321–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Webb, N.M. Peer interaction and learning in small groups. Int. J. Educ. Res. 1989, 13, 21–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Webb, N.M. Task-related verbal interaction and mathematics learning in small groups. J. Res. Math. Educ. 1991, 22, 366–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Webb, N.M.; Franke, M.L.; Johnson, N.C.; Ing, M.; Zimerman, J. Learning through explaining and engaging with others’ mathematical ideas. Math. Think. Learn. in press.
- Webb, N.M.; Palincsar, A.S. Group processes in the classroom. In Handbook of educational psychology; Berliner, D., Calfee, R., Eds.; Macmillan: New York, NY, USA, 1996; pp. 841–873. [Google Scholar]
- Webb, N.M.; Mastergeorge, A.M. The development of students’ learning in peer-directed small groups. Cognition Instr. 2003, 21, 361–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Webb, N.M.; Franke, M.L.; Ing, M.; Chan, A.; De, T.; Freund, D.; Battey, D. The role of teacher instructional practices in student collaboration. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2008, 33, 360–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Webb, N.M.; Franke, M.L.; De, T.; Chan, A.G.; Freund, D.; Shein, P.; Melkonian, D.K. ‘Explain to your partner’: Teachers’ instructional practices and students’ dialogue in small groups. Camb. J. Educ. 2009, 39, 49–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Webb, N.M.; Franke, M.L.; Ing, M.; Wong, J.; Fernandez, C.H.; Shin, N.; Turrou, A.C. Engaging with others’ mathematical ideas: Interrelationships among student participation, teachers’ instructional practices and learning. Int. J. Educ. Res 2014, 63, 79–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Engle, R.A.; Langer-Osuna, J.M.; de Royston, M.M. Toward a model of influence in persuasive discussions: Negotiating quality, authority, privilege, and access within a student-led argument. J. Learn. Sci. 2014, 23, 245–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kurth, L.A.; Anderson, C.W.; Palincsar, A.S. The case of Carla: Dilemmas of helping all students to understand science. Sci. Educ. 2002, 86, 287–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shah, N.; Lewis, C.M. Amplifying and attenuating inequity in collaborative learning: Toward an analytical framework. Cognition Instr. 2019, 37, 423–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bishop, J.P. “She’s always been the smart one. I’ve always been the dumb one”: Identities in the mathematics classroom. J. Res. Math. Educ. 2012, 43, 34–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Esmonde, I. Ideas and identities: Supporting equity in cooperative mathematics learning. Rev. Educ. Res. 2009, 79, 1008–1043. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Langer-Osuna, J.M. How Brianna became bossy and Kofi came out smart: Understanding the trajectories of identity and engagement for two group leaders in a project-based mathematics classroom. Can. J. Sci. Math. Tech. Educ. 2011, 11, 207–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wood, M.B. Mathematical micro-identities: Moment-to-moment positioning and learning in a fourth-grade classroom. J. Res. Math. Educ. 2013, 44, 775–808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Esmonde, I.; Langer-Osuna, J.M. Power in numbers: Student participation in mathematical discussions in heterogeneous spaces. J. Res. Math. Educ. 2013, 44, 288–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barron, B. When smart groups fail. J. Learn. Sci. 2003, 12, 307–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shah, N.; Lewis, C.M.; Caires, R. Analyzing equity in collaborative learning situations: A comparative case study in elementary computer science. In Proceedings of the 11th International Conference of the Learning Sciences, Boulder, CO, USA, 23–27 June 2014; Polman, J.L., Kyza, E.A., O’Neill, D.K., Tabak, I., Penuel, W.R., Jurow, A.S., O’Connor, K., Lee, T., D’Amico, L., Eds.; International Society of the Learning Sciences: Boulder, CO, USA, 2014; pp. 495–502. [Google Scholar]
- National Governors Association Center for Best Practices, Council of Chief State School Officers. Common Core Standards Mathematics; National Governors Association Center for Best Practices, Council of Chief State School Officers: Washington, DC, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- California Department of Education. Data and Statistics; California Department of Education: Sacramento, CA, USA, 2021.
- Carpenter, T.P.; Fennema, E.; Franke, M.L.; Levi, L.; Empson, S.B. Children’s mathematics: Cognitively guided instruction, 2nd ed.; Heinemann: Portsmouth, NH, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Cascio, M.A.; Lee, E.; Vaudrin, N.; Freedman, D.A. A team-based approach to open coding: Considerations for creating intercoder consensus. Field Method. 2019, 31, 116–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guba, E.G. Criteria for assessing the trustworthiness of naturalistic inquiries. Educ. Tech. Res. Dev. 1981, 29, 75–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boyatzis, R.E. Transforming Qualitative Information: Thematic Analysis and Code Development; Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Routledge Academic: New York, NY, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Langer-Osuna, J.M.; Gargroetzi, E.; Munson, J.; Chavez, R. Exploring the role of off-task activity on students’ collaborative dynamics. J. Educ. Psychol. 2020, 112, 514–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jansen, A. An investigation of relationships between seventh-grade students’ beliefs and their participation during mathematics discussions in two classrooms. Math Think Learn. 2008, 10, 68–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Webb, N.M.; Franke, M.L.; Johnson, N.C.; Turrou, A.C.; Ing, M. Dude, don’t start without me! Fostering engagement with others’ mathematical ideas. In Promoting Spontaneous Use of Learning and Reasoning Strategies: Theory, Research, and Practice for Effective Transfer; Manalo, E., Uesaka, Y., Chinn, C.A., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2018; pp. 292–309. [Google Scholar]
- Chizhik, A.W. Equity and status in group collaboration: Learning through explanations depends on task characteristics. Soc Psychol. Educ. 2001, 5, 179–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, E.G.; Lotan, R.A. Designing Groupwork: Strategies for the Heterogeneous Classroom, 3rd ed.; Teachers College Press: New York, NY, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Featherstone, H.; Crespo, S.; Jilk, L.M.; Oslund, J.A.; Parks, A.N.; Wood, M.B. Smarter Together! Collaboration and Equity in the Elementary Math Classroom; National Council of Teachers of Mathematics: Reston, VA, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
Description | |
---|---|
Ongoing | The pair showed sustained, regular back and forth engagement with each other that occurred continuously through small-group time. |
Periodic | Students engaged with each other periodically, with long stretches of time between engagements during which they worked on their own papers without interacting with each other. |
Negotiate and Move On | Students negotiated which strategy to use, selected one strategy to use, and moved on without further attempts to engage with each other. |
1-Shot | The pair showed a limited, short-lived, burst of one-time engagement with each other. This interaction was over and done fairly quickly. |
None | Students did not interact around the math. |
Students Participated at a Highly-Detailed Level | Number of Pairs |
---|---|
Both students | 24 (44%) |
One student | 17 (31%) |
No student | 14 (25%) |
Students Participated at a Highly-Detailed Level | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Number of Students Leading the Math | Both Students | One Student | No Student | Total |
Both students led the math | 22 | 7 | 7 | 36 |
One student led the math | 2 | 10 | 4 | 16 |
No one led the math (no interaction around the math) | 0 | 0 | 3 | 3 |
Total | 24 | 17 | 14 | 55 |
Students Participated at a Highly-Detailed Level | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Degree of Continuity of the Pair’s Interaction | Both Students | One Student | No Student | Total |
Ongoing | 16 | 11 | 7 | 34 |
Periodic | 8 | 6 | 4 | 18 |
1-Shot/None | 0 | 0 | 3 | 3 |
Total | 24 | 17 | 14 | 55 |
Both Students in the Pair Participated at a Highly-Detailed Level | ||
---|---|---|
Pattern of How Interaction Unfolded in the Pair | Yes | No |
Students took turns initiating a strategy | ||
| 7 | 4 |
| 2 | 2 |
Students each generated their own strategies | ||
| 1 | 2 |
| 2 | 1 |
| 2 | 0 |
One student generated the strategies | ||
| 3 | 0 |
| 2 | 2 |
Students jointly produced the strategies and jointly carried them out | 3 | 2 |
Students first solved the problem independently 1 | ||
| 1 | 0 |
| 1 | 1 |
Pattern of How Interaction Unfolded in the Pair | Example of How Highly-Detailed Participation Emerged in the Pair |
---|---|
Students took turns initiating a strategy | |
| While working to carry out the strategies, the students described steps of the strategy out loud, regularly asked and answered each other’s questions, repeated steps that the other student had voiced, and expressed agreement or disagreement with what the other student had said. For each strategy, upon completion, the student who had initiated it offered an unsolicited fully-complete explanation of it. On one strategy, when the pair produced different final answers, the students carried out extensive discussion about their work, with one student challenging the other, asking her specific questions about particular steps she had performed, explaining why she thought her partner’s work was incorrect, describing what her partner should do instead, and generating multiple ways to convince her partner of the correct solution. |
| While working on the strategies, the students occasionally asked questions or made comments about what their partner was doing, or briefly expressed agreement or disagreement with what their partner had written on their paper. For one number set, after they each had finished carrying out the strategy on their own, one student asked his partner how he got his answer, and his partner explained his strategy. For another number set, when the other student finished first, he turned his attention to making sure that his partner was carrying out the strategy correctly. In doing so, he told his partner what to write, and explained how to carry out those steps correctly. |
Students each generated their own strategies | |
| After deciding on an approach, they used manipulative materials (blocks) to carry them out. As they worked, they monitored what the other was doing and frequently commented on each other’s progress. The student who finished first gave a complete explanation of her strategy and the other student then added to her idea. |
| They largely worked independently on their own strategies but occasionally reviewed and confirmed each other’s ideas before moving on to the next strategy or number set. During one check in, one student started to question the other’s nascent strategy and then added mathematical detail. Later, the student whose work was questioned gave a complete explanation of his strategy. |
| They worked on their own strategies independently and came together after completing each one to explain in detail what they each did. They explicitly voiced a norm that they should explain their strategies to each other (e.g., “Ok, so let’s explain to each other our strategies.”). While each student explained how she carried out her strategy, her partner asked a few questions to make sure she understood what her partner had done. |
One student generated the strategies | |
| One student initiated the strategies and both students worked on them, with long pauses while they worked independently. Periodically, the second student asked questions of the first student to clarify and justify what he had done, challenged the first student about steps he had carried out, and suggested alternatives. The first student explained and justified his strategies in response to the second student’s questions and challenges. |
| At the beginning of the pair’s interaction, one student gave a complete explanation of the strategy he intended to carry out. He continued to generate the strategies for the subsequent number sets while the other student made frequent comments and asks continual questions. In the discussion around one of those subsequent number sets, the other student noticed a mistake in his partner’s work and offered an alternative path to solving the problem. That student went on to explain the strategy in thorough detail. |
Students jointly produced the strategies and jointly carried them out | The pair consistently worked through the strategies jointly. For each number set, a student suggested a strategy that they could carry out together, one or both students suggested revisions to it, and they worked together to carry it out. Their interaction included voicing steps in unison, each student suggesting the next step in the strategy, each student suggesting alternatives, and jointly troubleshooting errors and discrepancies. |
Students first solved the problem independently 2 | |
| Upon convening in groups, after working independently to develop multiple strategies, one group member requested that they take turns explaining their ideas. One student provided a fully-detailed explanation of how she had solved the first problem while the others listened. Then, the other two members of the group fully explained their strategies for subsequent problems. |
| Upon convening in groups, one student immediately suggested that they “check with each other” about their strategies. This prompted her partner to explain her strategy in complete detail. After listening intently to her partner’s explanation, the first student then provided a full explanation of her own approach to solving the problem. |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Webb, N.M.; Ing, M.; Burnheimer, E.; Johnson, N.C.; Franke, M.L.; Zimmerman, J. Is There a Right Way? Productive Patterns of Interaction during Collaborative Problem Solving. Educ. Sci. 2021, 11, 214. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11050214
Webb NM, Ing M, Burnheimer E, Johnson NC, Franke ML, Zimmerman J. Is There a Right Way? Productive Patterns of Interaction during Collaborative Problem Solving. Education Sciences. 2021; 11(5):214. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11050214
Chicago/Turabian StyleWebb, Noreen M., Marsha Ing, Eric Burnheimer, Nicholas C. Johnson, Megan L. Franke, and Joy Zimmerman. 2021. "Is There a Right Way? Productive Patterns of Interaction during Collaborative Problem Solving" Education Sciences 11, no. 5: 214. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11050214
APA StyleWebb, N. M., Ing, M., Burnheimer, E., Johnson, N. C., Franke, M. L., & Zimmerman, J. (2021). Is There a Right Way? Productive Patterns of Interaction during Collaborative Problem Solving. Education Sciences, 11(5), 214. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11050214