1. Introduction
There is a large body of research showing that the academically related activities children do at home are associated with their educational development, although much of the research has used samples composed of preschoolers and children at the start of elementary school [
1,
2,
3,
4].
For example, the frequency of reading different types of books during this time period is positively associated with the development of children’s reading skills [
3,
5]. A similar relation holds for mathematics activities and skill development ([
6,
7]; for review, see [
8]) although the relation is less clear cut for mathematics than literacy and varies, to a degree, across demographic groups [
9,
10]. Research also shows that parents’ beliefs about their role in their children’s learning and how to foster academic skills is positively and significantly related to the frequency of children’s engagement in relevant reading and mathematics activities (e.g., [
11]).
Research on the home learning environment has generally focused on reading, and more recently, mathematics. There has been less focus on science or written language, two other important components of children’s schooling [
2]. Such research has also mainly focused on young children’s home learning activities and not those of children in elementary school.
How children acquire academically related skills depends upon what occurs at home and school, and communication between the two settings [
12,
13,
14]. We focus here primarily on home-based activities in mathematics, reading, written language, and science in elementary school-age children while acknowledging the importance of both settings for children’s development. Although the competencies developed in the preschool years and those before the start of formal schooling are very important (e.g., [
15,
16,
17]), we need a better understanding of what occurs at home as children go through school. As Serpell et al. (2005) [
3] found, children’s early reading trajectories can be improved by increased home-based literacy activities once children start formal schooling. Such findings provide further evidence for the need to study the home learning environment as children proceed through school.
This paper expands upon prior research by documenting the amount and type of mathematics, reading, written language and science activities children in first through sixth grade engage in at home. It is not sufficient to document just the frequency of activities; we also need to consider parents’ beliefs about how children learn and their role in such learning. Accordingly, we document various components of parents’ beliefs about children’s learning and their role in it. All of the data are based on parents’ reports. In what follows, we first present the theoretical framework that guides our conceptualization, we next provide a summary of relevant research on children’s home-based engagement in the four subjects and relevant beliefs about such engagement.
2. Theoretical Framework
This research reflects academic socialization theory [
18] and Bronfenbrenner’s (1979) [
12] ecological model. Parents’ academic socialization includes parents’ attitudes, values, goals, expectations, and beliefs about education as well as the opportunities and activities they make available to their children [
18,
19]. Hoover-Dempsey et al. (2005) [
20] discussed that parents must believe they have the relevant skills with which to assist their children, and the time to do so, to be involved in their children’s education. Relatedly, Pomerantz and Moorman (2010) [
21] reviewed how parents socialize their children’s skill development and motivation to succeed at a task. This implies that parents must believe they have the relevant skills with which to assist their children. However, we know very little about how confident parents feel about their ability to socialize their children’s academic skills (cf. [
22,
23,
24]).
Parents’ socialization of their children’s academic development is associated with their development of such skills [
18,
25,
26]. For example, children’s exposure to various literacy-related activities is associated with their literacy skills [
3]. How parents socialize their children’s academic development is grounded in cultural models shared by members of a cultural group (e.g., Blacks and Latinx, [
27,
28]) but also may reflect family income, parents’ educational level and their knowledge of children’s development [
11,
29].
The ecological model notes that children’s development occurs in several overlapping contexts (e.g., microsystems) and stresses that these contexts need to work well together (mesosystems) to optimize children’s development [
12]. For example, Epstein (2001) [
13] talked about overlapping spheres of influence in which parents and educators together exert an influence on children’s learning. Relatedly, Hoover-Dempseyet al. (2005) [
20] talked about the importance of home and school factors in predicting parents’ involvement in their children’s education.
2.1. Parents’ Socialization of Children’s Academic Skills
We begin by reviewing research on children’s home-based activities and then present a brief review of parents’ pertinent beliefs.
2.1.1. Reading
Research on parents’ socialization of children’s early literacy development has included documentation of parents’ beliefs, children’s relevant activities, and related development. Participation in reading-related activities at home is positively related to the development of early reading skills (e.g., [
3,
4,
5,
6,
11,
16,
24,
26,
29,
30,
31,
32,
33,
34,
35,
36]). Most studies that have documented the frequency of children’s engagement in activities have considered the number of times a child does an activity but not for how long. The number of minutes a child engages in an activity gives a more nuanced measure than just focusing on the number of times [
24].
Research on early reading has also considered parents’ beliefs about their role in socializing their children’s educational skills. Serpell et al. (2005) showed the importance of an approach that engaged children’s interest and tried to make it fun for the child [
3]. Such an approach was positively and significantly related to children’s engagement in activities and reading development as children progressed from preschool through third grade. Sonnenschein and colleagues found that most parents in their sample of low-income families (
n = 70) strongly believed that children should engage in reading activities at home, and they should assist their children with these activities [
11]. However, children engaged in reading activities only once or twice a week, less than others find optimal (see also [
3,
37]). These data beg the question of what other factors might play a role in whether parents and children engage in reading-related activities? Two such factors are [
38] whether parents have confidence in their knowledge of what to do; and [
8] whether teachers are requesting that children engage in reading-related activities.
2.1.2. Mathematics
As with research on literacy development, research on parents’ socialization of young children’s mathematics skills has taken two general approaches, sometimes in the same study. One is to document children’s activities and parents’ beliefs. The other is to investigate relations between home activities, parents’ beliefs, and children’s outcomes. Research shows that, as with reading, parents support the importance of children engaging in mathematics activities at home and the need to assist them with such activities [
11]. Although one would expect to find positive relations between engaging in mathematics activities at home and children’s skills, the research evidence is equivocal ([
23]; for a review, see [
8]). This lack of consistent relation may reflect inconsistencies between the types of mathematics activities children engage in and the specific mathematics skills assessed [
8]. As with research on reading, research on mathematics also considered the number of times and not the amount of time children engaged in activities. Research also did not generally consider how confident parents were to assist their children (cf., [
24]).
We are aware of only three studies that considered the confidence that parents had to assist their children with mathematics. All used parents of preschoolers. Cannon and Ginsburg (2008) asked 37 middle-income parents of preschoolers how confident they felt to assist their children with mathematics [
22]. Most of the parents reported not feeling very confident. Missall et al. (2015) asked a similar set of questions to 70 parents of preschoolers [
23]. Their sample was more diverse. Most reported feeling moderately comfortable. Sonnenschein et al. (2021) reported data with 105 parents of preschoolers [
24]. These parents reported feeling moderately comfortable assisting their children with mathematics. Thus, their results were similar to that of Missall et al. (2015) but the sample was mainly well educated and affluent [
23].
2.1.3. Written Language
Most of the research on this topic has considered how children learn written language in school (e.g., [
38,
39,
40]). We have not been able to find a body of research on parents’ socialization of their children’s written language. In fact, we found only two studies [
2,
3] and neither considered the factors that we are considering in this paper nor documented engagement in focal activities.
2.1.4. Science
There are also limited studies focusing on how parents socialize their children’s science development at home. However, recent research has indicated that parents who are interested in science are more likely to engage in science learning at home with their children [
41]. The authors of the aforementioned study also reported that parents with higher levels of education and income are more likely to engage in science-based activities with their children. These findings are consistent with what several researchers have suggested: that is, findings from research on parent involvement at home for reading or mathematics should be applicable to the domain of science (e.g., [
42,
43]).
2.1.5. Comparison between Reading and Mathematics Socialization
Given how limited the research on parents’ socialization of written language and science is, we do not include those domains in this section. The fairly limited research comparing parents’ socialization of children’s reading and mathematics skill shows that parents view reading as more important than mathematics [
44]. Overall, parents see it as more important for their children to engage in reading than mathematics at home and assist their children with reading more frequently than with mathematics [
11,
44]. This trend has continued even as there has been more of a societal emphasis on STEM fields. For example, Sonnenschein et al. (2021) recently surveyed 105 parents of preschoolers about home-based reading and mathematics-related practices [
24]. They used an online survey distributed on social media sites. Participants were primarily middle to upper income and highly educated. As with the other data reported here, parents viewed reading engagement as more important than mathematics engagement. Relatedly, children engaged in reading activities more days per week and minutes per day than in mathematics activities. And parents were more confident in assisting their children with reading than mathematics. The difference in confidence is particularly interesting given the nature of the sample, which was mainly highly educated.
2.2. Parents’ Beliefs about Their Role in Children’s Learning
Most of the research on this topic has been based on preschool-age children. For example, Sonnenschein et al. (2016) [
11] found that parents from low-income backgrounds emphasized the importance of preschool children engaging in reading and mathematics activities and the need for parental assistance. However, parents emphasized reading more highly. As reviewed in Elliott et al. (2021) [
10], some studies have found positive associations between parents’ ratings of importance and children’s engagement in reading and mathematics activities [
33], whereas others have not found such associations between parents’ ratings of importance and children’s academic skills [
44].
The present study expands consideration of parents’ beliefs and attitudes to focus on older children with the four focal academic subjects, mathematics, reading, written language, and science. We consider emotionally valanced beliefs by documenting parents’ ratings for how much they and their children enjoy engaging in these four academic domains. We also consider cognitive beliefs by asking parents to rate how good they and their children are at the four academic domains.
There is a growing body of research that has explored parents’ anxiety when engaging in mathematics interactions (e.g., [
45]). Anxiety displayed by parents during interactions with their children interferes with children’s learning. Another way of considering the topic is by considering how confident parents feel to teach their children various content. This is not a heavily researched topic. In fact, we are aware of only three studies which we reviewed in a prior section. For example, Sonnenschein et al. (2021) [
24] found that educated and affluent parents of preschoolers reported being more confident to assist their preschool children with reading than mathematics.
Who Initiates Activities at Home
There is little research on this topic. We know that parents are dissatisfied with information that teachers of preschoolers send home [
24]. And children start getting homework from teachers as they enter and proceed through elementary school. Accordingly, we expect that parents will report that teachers are more likely to initiate the activities that children do at home as they get older.
2.3. The Present Study
Despite there being a large and growing body of research on children’s home learning environments, as reviewed above, there are still some significant limitations to our knowledge. Most of the research has focused on reading or mathematics activities or both but has not considered how written language and science are socialized at home. However, written language and science are important components of schooling, especially as children get older. Therefore, we need to understand how these four subjects, not just one or two, are socialized at home. Considering just reading or just reading and mathematics may give an incorrect picture.
Most of the research uses preschool children with little attention devoted to children in elementary school. However, it is important to understand the home learning environment as children go through elementary school. Are there differences in how much time is devoted to mathematics, reading, written language, and science? What are parents’ beliefs about the importance of each and their confidence in assisting their children with each? Although it may be reasonable to assume that activities during the preschool years are initiated by the family, little research has been conducted on this topic (cf., [
24]). And, who initiates activities may well vary by academic domain or by children’s grade (as homework demands initiated by teachers may play a larger role as children get older).
This study addressed five questions. One, how much time do children spend on mathematics, reading, written language, and science at home? Does the amount of time spent differ by subject or grade of child or both? Most of the research that quantifies children’s engagement in activities considers the number of days per week but not the number of minutes per day (cf., [
24]). It is important to get a more nuanced metric. Consistent with prior research, we expect that children will spend more time engaged in reading than mathematics activities or the other two subjects. Two, how important do parents think it is that children engage in home learning activities in each of the four subjects and assist their children with these activities? Does it vary by subject and/or by children’s grade? Prior research suggests that parents will rate highly the importance of these activities [
11]. Three, how confident are parents to support their children’s learning in these four academic subjects and does it differ by subject or children’s grade? Again, consistent with research with parents of preschoolers, we expect that parents will be more confident in supporting their children’s reading activities. Four, how do parents rate their and their children’s enjoyment of engagement in these activities and their aptitude for them. Does it differ by subject or grade? Five, who (parents, child, or teacher) prompts engagement in home learning activities in each subject and does it differ with grade? Although we are unaware of other research on this topic, we expect that teachers will increasingly initiate activities as children get older.
4. Results
Prior research shows differences regarding children’s ethnicity, income, and gender in their engagement with academically related activities [
11]. Therefore, we initially included income, ethnicity, and child gender as covariates. However, because there were no significant associations found in analyses which included these covariates (
p > 0.10), for parsimony, we did not include them further in this paper.
In what follows, we present data addressing each of the five overarching research questions. We first present descriptive information followed by inferential statistics, usually analyses of variance (ANOVA). Unless noted differently, we conducted mixed 4 (subject) × 3 (grade level: first/second grade, third/fourth grade, fifth/sixth grade) ANOVAs with appropriate follow-up tests (Bonferroni for domain-wise comparisons and Tukey’s b for the grade-level comparisons).
RQ1. How Much Time Do Children Spend on Mathematics, Reading, Written Language, and Science at Home? Does The Amount of Time Spent Differ by Subject or Grade of Child or Both?
Consistent with our expectations and as shown in
Table 3, parents reported that their children spent more time at home each week engaged in reading followed by mathematics, writing, and science activities. The overall subject effect was significant,
F(3, 178) = 78.53,
p < 0.001,
ηp2 = 306. More specifically, the amount of time spent doing each activity at home differed significantly from each of the others (
p < than at least 0.039). Neither the main effect for grade level (
p = 0.405) nor the interaction between grade level and subject was statistically significant (
p = 0.489).
We also asked parents what kinds of mathematics, reading, written language and science activities their children engaged in at home. We made a listing of all activities for each subject. These were then reliably categorized by two raters. In general, parents reported that their children engaged in a wide variety of activities. The most common activities engaged in for mathematics were doing homework, practicing skill development (flashcards and worksheets) and playing online games (video and computer games). They also engaged in daily living activities such as cooking. The most common activities engaged in for reading were reading for pleasure and doing homework. Children reportedly read a wide range of genres (e.g., novels/chapter books, non-fiction, graphic novels, comic books, and magazines). They read e-books, audiobooks and printed matter). For science, children most frequently did experiments and exploratory activities (trying to figure out how things worked). For written language, the most common activities children engaged in were academic activities (doing homework, practicing writing letters and words), free/creative writing (writing poems, plays, and stories), and writing cards/letters.
We asked parents two questions about importance of the four subjects. One, how important do parents think it is for their child to do reading, mathematics, written language, and science activities at home? The means (SDs) are reported in
Table 3. Consistent with prior research on the importance of children doing reading and mathematics activities at home [
4], the majority of parents thought that engaging in reading, mathematics, written language, and science at home was important to very important (reading 79% of parents, mathematics 73%, written language, 72%, science 63%). Nevertheless, there was a significant main effect for subject [
F(2, 180) = 33.98,
p < 0.001,
ηp2 = 0.159]. Engaging in reading was viewed as significantly more important than engaging in mathematics, science, or written language activities (
p < 0.001, respectively). None of the other comparisons differed significantly (
p > 0.10).
There also was a main effect of grade level,
F(2, 180) = 4.01,
p = 0.020,
ηp2 = 0.043. Parents of children in fifth/sixth grades were less likely to view their children’s home learning as important than parents of children in third/fourth grade (
p = 0.018, see
Table 3). None of the other contrasts were statistically significant (
p > than at least 0.10). The interaction between grade level and subject was not significant (
p = 0.267).
Two, we asked parents how important it was for them to assist their children with reading, mathematics, science, and written language activities at home. There was a main effect for subject,
F(3167) = 8.63,
p < 0.001,
ηp2 = 0.049. As shown in
Table 3, assisting with written language or mathematics was rated as significantly more important than assisting with science (
p < 0.001, respectively), but not significantly more important than reading (
p = 0.114). None of the other differences were statistically significant (
p > 0.10).
There also was a main effect for grade level,
F(2, 167) = 11.35,
p < 0.001,
ηp2 = 0.120. As shown in
Table 3, parents believed it is significantly more important to help children in first/second grade and third/fourth grade than in fifth/sixth grade (
p < 0.001, 0.004, respectively). The interaction between subject and grade level was not significant, (
p = 0.132).
Sixty-two percent of the participating parents reported they were confident/very confident in supporting their children’s reading activities. Slightly more than half of the parents (59%) parents reported being confident to very confident to help their children with written language activities. Similarly, 56% reported feeling confident/to very confident to support their children’s mathematics activities, and 56% of parents reported feeling confident in assisting their children’s science activities. There was a main effect for subject, F (3, 166) = 35.84, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.178. Parents were significantly more confident in assisting their children’s reading than written language (p = 0.01), mathematics (p < 0.001), or science (p < 0.001) at home. In contrast, parents were less confident to assist their children with science learning at home than with reading (p < 0.001) or writing (p < 0.001). However, there were no significant differences between parents’ confidence level in supporting science and mathematics activities at home (p = 0.35).
Although there was no significant main effect for grade level (p = 0.68), the interaction between grade level and parents’ confidence in assisting their children’s learning at home was significant, F (6, 166) = 4.07, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.047. Parents’ confidence in helping with reading, written language, and science activities did not differ by grade level (p> 0.20); in contrast, parents’ confidence in helping their children with mathematics did vary by their children’s grade level. Parents were more confident to support mathematics home learning for their first/second graders (M = 4.02, SD = 0.15) or their third/fourth (M = 4.01, SD = 0.14) than their fifth/sixth graders (M = 3.35, SD = 0.19), (p = 0.019, 0.022, respectively).
4.1. Parents’ Reports of Their Enjoyment and Abilities in the Four Subjects
Parents’ ratings of their enjoyment of reading, mathematics, science, and written language are shown in
Table 3. Parents rated reading as the subject they enjoyed the most followed by science, then written language, and then mathematics. There was a significant main effect for subject,
F(3, 186) = 59.417,
p < 0.001,
ηp2 = 0.246. More specifically, parents enjoyed reading significantly more than mathematics (
p < 0.001), science (
p < 0.001), and written language (
p < 0.001). Differences between written language and science and mathematics were not statistically significant (
p > 0.10, respectively). Neither the main effect for grade level (
p = 0.107) nor the interaction between grade level and parents’ enjoyment was statistically significant (
p = 0.984).
Parents also reported their perceptions of how good they were in each subject. They rated themselves as the best at reading activities followed by written language, then science, and then mathematics. There was a main effect of subject, F(3, 185) = 50.85, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.216. Parents’ perceptions of how good they were at each subject differed significantly (p < 0.001 across all pairwise comparisons), except for the mean differences between mathematics and science (p = 0.086).
There was a main effect of grade level, F(2, 185) = 6.53, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.066. Parents of third/fourth graders viewed themselves as significantly better than parents of first/second graders’ in terms of their abilities in each subject (p = 0.001), but not significantly better than fifth/sixth graders’ parents (p = 0.343). The interaction between grade level and parents’ abilities was not significant (p = 0.823).
Zero-order correlations were calculated between parents’ enjoyment of the four subjects and perceptions of how good they were at each subject. There were positive and significant moderate to strong associations among parents’ enjoyments and skills for mathematics r(187) = 0.726, p < 0.001, reading r(187) = 0.578, p < 0.001, science r(188) = 0.670, p < 0.001, and written language r(186) = 0.630, p < 0.001.
4.2. Parents’ Reports of Children’s Enjoyment and Abilities in the Four Subjects
We also asked parents to report their children’s perceived enjoyment and abilities in each subject. Parents reported that their children enjoyed science activities the most followed by reading, mathematics, and written language. There was a main effect of subject for children’s enjoyment, F(3, 182) = 34.54, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.160. Parents reported that their children enjoyed science significantly more than reading (p < 0.001) and reading significantly more than mathematics (p < 0.001) and written language (p = 0.001). Differences between the enjoyment of written language and mathematics were not statistically significant (p > 0.13). Neither the main effect for grade level (p = 0.887) nor the interaction between grade level and subject was significant (p = 0.222).
Parents also rated how good they thought their children were at these four subjects. There was a main effect of subject, F(3, 181) = 19.73, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.098. Parents’ ratings of children’s abilities significantly differed only in terms of how good they were at written language. Parents viewed their children as not as good at written language as mathematics (p < 0.001), reading (p < 0.001), and science (p < 0.001). Differences in ratings between mathematics, reading, and science did not differ significantly among themselves (p > 0.70). Neither the main effect for grade level (p = 0.194) nor the interaction between grade level and subject was statistically significant, p = 0.426.
We also computed zero-order correlations for children’s enjoyment and skills. The results revealed positive and significant moderate relations between children’s enjoyment and skills for mathematics [r(185) = 0.723, p < 0.001], reading [r(185) =0.689, p < 0.001], science [r(185) = 0.606, p < 0.001], and written language [r(184) = 0.687, p < 0.001].
We explored who prompts activities at home for each subject and whether it changed with children’s grade level. Parents reported that mathematics activities (
M = 63.34,
SD = 31.14) were initiated by teachers more than written language (
M = 54.10,
SD = 33.15) and reading activities (
M = 40.52,
SD = 29.65), while science activities (
M = 33.63,
SD = 38.05) were initiated by teachers the least. There was a main effect for subject for teacher-initiated activities,
F(3, 155) = 51.737,
p < 0.001,
ηp2 = 0.250. Teachers reportedly initiated mathematics activities more frequently than home-based activities in any of the other subjects (
p < 0.001, respectively). As shown in
Table 4 and at the beginning of this paragraph, all of the pairwise differences were statistically significant (
p < 0.001 across all pairwise comparisons).
There was a main effect of grade level, F(2, 155) = 14.249, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.155, which revealed that teachers prompted home learning activities significantly more for fifth/sixth graders (M = 62.31, SD = 3.92) than third/fourth (M = 50.19, SD = 3.11) and first/second graders (M = 35.54, SD = 3.25), (p = 0.05, p < 0.001, respectively). The mean differences for teacher-led activities between first/second and third/fourth graders were also significant (p = 0.004).
There was also a significant interaction between grade level and subject, F(6, 155) = 3.609, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.044. Overall, teacher-initiated activities significantly increased by children’s grade level for mathematics (M = 64.73, SD = 2.37), science (M = 36.15, SD = 2.85), and written language (M = 55.64, SD = 2.54) activities (p < than at least 0.015). However, teacher led reading activities did not significantly differ by children’s grade level (p > than at least 0.217).
Parents reported that their children prompted science learning (M = 43.80, SD = 33.63) and reading (M = 44.25, SD = 32.54) the most and prompted less written language activities (M = 29.52, SD = 30.63) followed by mathematics activities (M = 20.13, SD = 24.74) at home. There was a main effect of subject, F(3155) = 32.503, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.173. Differences between the various activities were all statistically significant (p < than at least 0.002), except for the mean differences between reading and science, (p = 0.76).
The overall grade-level effect was also significant, F(2, 155) = 5.864, p = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.070. Parents of first/second graders (M = 41.86, SD = 2.73) reported a significantly higher number of child-led activities than third/fourth (M = 30.73, SD = 2.61) and fifth/sixth (M = 29.43, SD = 3.29) graders, (p = 0.011, 0.013, respectively). The interaction between subject and grade level was not significant, (p = 0.255).
Finally, parents reported how parents themselves initiated home learning activities with their children. As illustrated in
Table 4, there was no significant effect for subject,
F(3, 155) = 1.573,
p = 0.195,
ηp2 = 0.010. However, there was a main effect of grade level,
F(2, 155) = 11.415,
p < 0.001,
ηp2 = 0.128. Parents of children in first/second (
M = 29.59,
SD = 2.35) and third/fourth (
M = 23.46,
SD = 2.25) grades initiated significantly more activities than parents of children in fifth/sixth grades (
M = 12.02,
SD = 2.84;
p = 0.01). There were no significant differences between first/second and third/fourth graders’ initiation of activities. (
p = 0.184). The interaction between subject and grade level was not significant, (
p = 0.503).
5. Discussion
This study significantly expanded our knowledge of elementary school-age children’s home learning environments by considering their home-based engagement in mathematics, reading, science, and writing activities, four important school subjects. Other research has typically considered children’s engagement in reading and/or mathematics activities and focused on preschool children not elementary school-age children (e.g., [
11]. Considering each subject in isolation or with only one other may give a misleading picture of the home learning environment. We also increased the precision of measuring frequency by not only considering how many days a week but also how many minutes each time children engage in an activity. Aspects of parents’ beliefs and attitudes about children engaging in these subjects [e.g., how important parents consider it to be, how confident they felt to assist, and how good they thought they were) were also considered. And, the focal children were elementary school-age children, an older cohort than is typically examined in research (e.g., [
24]). Although there is extensive research on children’s engagement in reading and, to a degree, mathematics activities at home (e.g., [
3,
44]), we know far less about written language and science activities. Moreover, we know fairly little about how parents’ beliefs about their role or capabilities, or their attitudes (e.g., confidence, enjoyment) vary across subjects.
We begin this section by briefly answering our five overarching research questions. We then present some important themes that come from combining aspects of the findings across the five research questions. As our results show, in order to understand children’s home learning environments, it is necessary to consider their frequency of engagement in activities as well as parents’ various beliefs about what is important, how children learn and parents’ role in their children’s learning as well as the interface between the home and school [
11,
13,
18,
20].
The elementary school-age children in our study were clearly spending time at home engaged in reading, mathematics, science, and written language activities. However, they by far reportedly spend the most time doing reading activities. They also engaged in different genres of reading activities. The relative frequency of reading engagement is consistent with what others have reported with preschool children (e.g., [
24]) and was what we expected. It begs the questions of why they are spending so much more time reading? We believe the answer needs to include other aspects of parents’ academic socialization (e.g., parents’ confidence to support learning in a subject) and will be discussed in a subsequent section.
Research with preschoolers has found that parents highly rate the importance of their children doing reading and mathematics activities at home and the need for parental assistance [
11,
24,
33]. Our results show that parents with older children also believe that engagement in academically related activities is important and that they should assist with such engagement. Our study extended prior findings to include elementary school-age children and four subjects not the typical two (reading and mathematics). As with other research (e.g., [
33]), parents emphasized reading more highly than other subjects. We also found that parents’ ratings differed by their children’s grade. Parents of younger children rated their involvement in their children’s home activities as more important than did parents of older children. This may reflect that parents think as children get older, they do more of this work in school and are able to complete schoolwork independently [
34].
A person’s confidence to assist others with academic tasks is an understudied topic. The few studies conducted used parents of preschoolers (e.g., [
22,
23,
24]). In the present study, confidence levels varied across those three studies but generally parents were more confident assisting with reading and written language than mathematics or science. This was true across grade levels. We find this interesting because the parents, as in other research, were highly educated. We turn in a later section to how confidence levels may play a role in what activities children engage in at home.
How much people enjoy academically related activities and how good they believe they are at such activities are interesting topics because they may be related. We found strong associations between how much parents said they enjoyed each subject and how skilled at the subject they believed they were. Parents rated themselves as significantly better at reading than the other subjects. Their lowest ratings were in mathematics and science. A similar but not identical pattern occurred with how parents rated enjoyment. They rated reading the highest and mathematics the lowest. Such feelings about one’s skills and how much one enjoys an activity also may be related to what activities children engage in at home.
Parents also rated their children’s skills and enjoyment across the four subjects. Again, how much children reportedly enjoyed a subject was moderately related to their skills in that subject. Mathematics was not a highly enjoyed activity. Interestingly, however, unlike with how parents rated their enjoyment of science, they rated their children as enjoying science more than the other subjects.
There were two findings of note. One, as predicted, teachers initiated more activities for children to do at home as children got older. And older children initiated fewer activities than younger children. Two, teachers were more likely to initiate mathematics activities for children to do at home than activities in other subjects. And, interestingly, children were more likely to initiate reading and science activities than other activities.
We turn next to themes that combine information from several of the research questions.
Children spent less time engaged in mathematics activities per week than reading. They spent, on average, two hours a week engaged in mathematics at home which may not be sufficient for them to acquire the necessary skills. How one acquires skills goes beyond exposure to relevant activities; the beliefs parents have about children’s learning and their own aptitude or perceived aptitude also matter [
18,
20]. Consistent with what others have found [
11], although parents viewed it as important for their children to do mathematics-related activities at home, these were not viewed as important as reading. Furthermore, parents rated themselves as less confident to assist their children with mathematics than with other subjects and they believed they were less skilled in mathematics than in other subjects. This picture of parents’ beliefs about their own mathematics skills and their confidence to assist their children in such activities coupled with parents’ ratings of their children as not particularly enjoying mathematics may explain why children less frequently engage in mathematics activities at home. Other research has shown a positive relation between frequency of engagement in activities and children’s development of mathematics skills [
3,
11].
Theme 2: Who Initiates Engagement in Mathematics, Reading, Science, and Writing Activities at Home and Why?
Children initiate activities that they think are interesting. Parents initiate activities with children for several reasons: they think their children will like the activity, they themselves like these activities, they think the activity is important for skill development. Teachers also prompt or initiate activities typically as homework assignments as part of the school curriculum or suggestions to parents as ways to develop children’s skills.
Teachers increasingly initiated activities, particularly mathematics, as the children got older. Children displayed the opposite pattern. In thinking about skills for children to develop, can educators and parents take advantage of children’s interests and suggest activities in keeping with those interests? For example, children reportedly liked to do science activities at home. Science skill development is an important part of STEM competencies; therefore, we should encourage children’s skill development in that subject. However, parents did not rate the importance of assisting their children with science at home as high. In fact, it was the lowest rated of the four topics. Similarly, they rated their skills, confidence, and enjoyment of science as relatively low. Therefore, it is not surprising that science activities were the least frequently occurring of the four.
If parents are not going to encourage children to do science activities at home, can teachers play a more active role in encouraging children to engage in science tasks at home? And if so, can they provide more assistance to parents so that parents feel more comfortable engaging in science activities with their children?
Limitations, Future Directions, and Conclusions
This study had four limitations that need to be considered when interpreting the findings. One, the generalizability of our findings is limited because our sample was a highly educated, affluent, White sample [
46,
47,
48]. Therefore, the findings may not necessarily apply to less educated parents or low-income families. For example, low-income families may have less time to assist their children at home or may believe they have less skill for doing so [
20]. Unfortunately, as described in the Method section, our attempts to recruit a more diverse sample were not successful. Future research should seek to increase the diversity of the sample or focus more exclusively on groups not well represented in this study. In addition, the sample size in this study was relatively small (
n = 177). Therefore, this study should be considered exploratory and replicated with a larger, more diverse sample.
Two, our data are based on parents’ (typically mothers’) reports, not children’s observed behaviors. Asking parents to report on children’s engagement in activities is typical (e.g., [
3,
11]) and we have no reason to doubt the validity of these data. However, it is important to confirm the validity of the findings with actual observations.
Three, our inquiry of the home learning environment was limited to interactions with parents. Children often have siblings who may interact with them when engaging in mathematics, reading, written language, and science activities. Future research should consider the nature of such interactions and expand inquiry into the role that children play in initiating engagement in relevant activities. Four, our study was conducted prior to the outbreak of the COVID-19 pandemic during which parents of preschool and school-age children often engaged in distance learning with their children because of the suspension of in-school classes. It is possible that parents who spent significant time engaging in distance learning with their children might now feel more confident in their ability to assist their children. On the other hand, the limited research on this topic suggests that many parents reported struggling (e.g., [
49]). Five, unfortunately, we could not assess children’s actual reading, mathematics, written language, or science skills. However, other research shows relations between parents’ beliefs, aspects of the home environment and the development of children’s academic skills (e.g., [
3,
11,
33]).
In conclusion, while the importance of the home learning environment is widely accepted and children naturally gain an understanding in core subjects as they develop, more attention is needed to leverage the two. Researchers and practitioners alike need to focus on supporting families of school-age children in socializing academic skills. Parents will need support from teachers, especially if their confidence and interest in a subject is low. We know from this exploratory study that parents often report low levels of confidence and interest, particularly in mathematics, which may keep them from engaging in mathematical activities with their children, especially as the difficulty of the content increases. As we continue to make progress in the field, we need to ensure we are considering children’s full learning environments which include both the home and the school.