3. Results
Addressing the first research objective,
Table 4 shows the descriptive analyses of the sample of mathematical problems differentiated by mother tongue (MT) and the language of instruction (LI).
When there were no language coincidences, all the previously mentioned statistically significant differences (t > 2.50, p < 0.05) were maintained in favor of the second-year subjects compared with the first-year subjects. However, when there was a language coincidence, these differences were diluted in some cases: Type–Change (t = 0.85, p > 0.05), Type—Equalization (t = 1.35, p > 0.05), Unknown—Medium (t = 1.83, p > 0.05) and Operation—Subtraction (t = 0.87, p > 0.05).
The results indicate a clear influence of the LI on problem solving when it aligns with the MT. That is the reason the difference in problem solving between the first and second grades is less significant when the languages do coincide, with only a few cases showing significant differences, such as in comparison and combination problems, where the grammatical structure is more complex.
The descriptive analyses of the sample of mathematical problems differentiated by language coincidence resulted in the data presented in
Table 5.
Therefore, we analyzed the possible statistically significant differences in the mathematical problems depending on whether MT coincided or not with the LI used in the mathematics teaching (see
Table 6).
As the previous test denotes, there are statistically significant differences in most of the factors evaluated, which are always with higher values in language coincidence compared with non-coincidence and with a small or moderate effect size. Specifically, language coincidence is superior in the categories of Type—Change, Type—Combination, Type—Comparison, Unknown—Medium, Unknown—Beginning, Operation—Addition, Operation—Subtraction, and Total Score.
The findings reveal noteworthy distinctions in almost all instances, with heightened academic proficiency observed among those students whose MT matches with the LI. This trend, however, is not consistently upheld in problems pertaining to equalization and those wherein the unknown variable corresponds to the outcome.
Concerning the second objective, apart from language coincidences, we analyzed the possible differences between the combinations of LI and MT (Spa–Spa, Eng–Eng, Spa–Eng, and Eng–Spa).
Table 7 shows the descriptive statistics:
For this reason, we analyzed the possible statistically significant differences in the mathematical problems based on MT and LI (see
Table 8 and
Table 9). It should be noted that in the post hoc contrasts, only those that show statistically significant differences are presented.
The analysis of bilingual programs across diverse countries reveals a consistent trend in the competence displayed by students whose MT differs from the LI, resulting in closely aligned mean scores. Conversely, deviations from this trend are apparent in the response patterns of students whose MT corresponds with the LI. This contrast is substantiated in the subsequent tables, illustrating the disparity in the mean scores.
Considering the results, in most of the factors analyzed, there are statistically significant differences, with a small or moderate effect (t > 2.50, p < 0.05, η2 > 0.035), and always with higher values in the Spa–Spa compared with the Eng–Spa (4 elements) and Spa–Eng (5 elements) groups; in some cases, being both together (3 elements). Specifically, Spa–Spa students obtained better scores than Spa–Eng in the categories of Type—Comparison, Unknown—Beginning, Operation—Subtraction and Total Score. Similarly, Spa–Spa showed statistically higher scores than Eng–Spa in the Type—Change, Type—Combination, Unknown—Beginning, Operation—Subtraction, and Total Score categories.
In order to address the second research objective, the descriptive analyses of the sample of mathematical problems differentiated by course resulted in the data presented in
Table 10.
Once these data were extracted in response to the first research objective, we analyzed the possible statistically significant differences in the mathematical problems depending on the course (see
Table 11).
Once the hypothesis contrast tests were carried out, statistically significant differences were found in all the dimensions analyzed, always with higher values in the measurements for the second-grade students compared to the first-grade students and with an effect size, according to the interpretation of López-Martín and Ardura-Martínez [
43], from small (
d > 0.200) to moderate (
d > 0.500). Specifically, they were in the following categories: Type—Change, Type—Equalization, Type—Combination, Type—Comparison, Unknown—Result, Unknown—Medium, Unknown—Beginning, Operation—Addition, Operation—Subtraction, and Total Score.
It is evident that the resolution patterns are identical in both the first and second grades. Mathematical competence is determined by the placement of the unknown, with the students in the second grade consistently demonstrating greater competence than those in the first grade. After analyzing these data, the sample was dichotomized between subjects who had a coincidence of LI and MT (Eng–Eng or Spa–Spa) or not (Eng–Spa or Spa–Eng). The descriptive statistics of the subsamples can be seen in
Table 12.
The presented averages indicate that first-year students have lower proficiency than second-year students. However, when considering the coincidence of LI with MT, students whose languages coincide perform better. The differences between first and second-year students tend to be greater when their languages do not coincide, but narrower when they do.
4. Discussion
One of the main objectives of the present research was to determine whether the language of instruction influences problem solving and whether or not the type of LI conditions these outcomes; that is, whether it has a greater effect when the MT is English and the students are being taught in Spanish or whether it is not relevant. We can see how competence does not depend so much on this aspect. In this respect, when dealing with problems of change and combination, the students whose MT is English and LI is Spanish, their LI shows greater performance. However, in the problems of equalization and comparison, it would be the other way around, meaning that performance would be higher in students whose MT is Spanish and whose LI is English.
If we focus on comparing students whose LI is the same or not, we see that in the problems of change, students whose MT and LI is Spanish show better resolution, followed by those whose MT is English and LI is Spanish. Significant differences are also established between the rest of the groups. In contrast, concerning combination, equalization, and comparison problems, we did not find significant differences between the groups in terms of their LI and MT. This may be due to the high variability of answers in the change problems and also because these are the least difficult problems for the youngest students. Moreover, we could observe that there were students with good understanding and planning capabilities compared with other structures, and that in comparison problems, students obtained a higher failure rate because they are more complex, and the differences diminish.
Nevertheless, we can also see that, depending on the place where the unknown is located, there are many more differences, particularly when it is located at the beginning. This can happen because only a small proportion of the students can understand the situations posed by this type of problem, as it requires a greater capacity for abstraction. On this occasion, those whose coincident language is Spanish would show greater resolution, followed by students with the LI in English and whose MT is Spanish.
One of the noteworthy findings pertains to the striking similarity observed in the problem-solving patterns among students whose MT does not correspond with their LI, regardless of whether the instructional language is English or Spanish. This underscores a clear influence of the language of instruction on problem resolution, irrespective of the specific language and academic course under consideration [
19,
44,
45,
46,
47,
48].
This leads us to propose that the language of instruction (LI) is a variable that influences the development of mathematical concepts and cognitive processes such as comprehension, planning, and execution, irrespective of the methodology employed. It is essential to note that teaching methodologies in the United States and in Spain differ; for example, methodologies in bilingual programs in the United States utilize peer discussion and hands-on manipulation, whereas Spanish programs do the same but on a minor scale. In light of the results, we can assert that language exerts a more substantial impact than the methodology itself, as evidenced by the analogous patterns observed among students enrolled in bilingual programs whose MT does not coincide with the LI, in contrast to those with congruent linguistic backgrounds.
The second main objective of this study aimed at discovering if there were significant differences between the first- and second-grade groups regarding problem solving. As we can see, second-grade students show greater competence in problem solving regardless of the type of semantic structure shown. This reaffirms those studies [
44,
49,
50] that confirm evolutionary learning; that is, the more often the students do a task, the greater mastery they will acquire. One can observe that second-year students show better reading comprehension compared with first-year students. However, considering that the presentation of the problems was orally carried out, this reduces the correlation between problem solving and reading comprehension.
We must consider that in early childhood education, the problems that students mainly tackle are those of change and combination with the unknown in the result, and it is not until the first grade of elementary education that they face other verbal structures, such as comparison and equalization. However, in these problems, the first- and second-grade students did not show similar performance either, so regardless of whether the exposure to those problems was greater or less in previous stages, we can see how the resolution improves as students move up through the grades.
This could happen because second-grade students tend to improve their planning processes regarding the resolution and leave behind the more immediate responses in which the child only plans an operation with the numbers that arise in the problem without knowing how to explain what and why he did it.
Regarding the place of the unknown, we observe the same tendency as that occurring in accordance with the type of problem in the sense that second-grade students show better competence. As we can see, the resolution pattern is similar in both courses, since the problems that present the unknown in the result are the ones that have been solved best compared with those that present the unknown in one of the terms of the equation. Additionally, the problems with the unknown at the beginning have been those that present worse resolution, thus confirming all previous studies [
19,
42,
51,
52,
53]. This allows us to conclude that problems with the unknown at the beginning are the ones that present the greatest difficulty compared with problems that have the unknown in the result, which students find the easiest of all to solve.
When focusing on the LI, we observe how the response pattern remains the same, with the second-grade students showing greater competence in problem solving, but the more we observe the problem, the grade, and the LI, the more differences we find, since students whose language (MT) is coincident have better competence compared with students whose language is not. It means that they can solve a higher number of problems. Moreover, it indicates that, although the presentation of the problems has been both oral and written, language proficiency will prevent the student from being able to carry out a process of decoding the language at the same time as transcoding the number at an early age, thus making resolution of the problems difficult.
If we observe the mean scores, we can see that in problems with a simpler structure, such as change problems, first-year students with a coincident language (MT) have an average of Ẋ = 6.55 compared with second-year students with a non-coincidence language (non-MT), where we find that Ẋ = 6.25. If we analyze the rest of the problems, the way they are solved is very similar. For example, in the most complex problems, such as comparison problems, we find an average of 2.37 for first-grade students using their MT and 2.87 for second-grade students using their non-MT. If we consider the differences that exist between first- and second-grade students in the change problems, we can observe that the younger students show a better resolution if the language is coincident; the average for MT = 6.55, while that for non-MT is 5.41. In the second grade, these differences are reduced; when they solve in their MT, the average is 6.79, while in their non-MT, it is 6.25. This makes us think that when the LI is not the same and the grade is lower, there exists a greater difference in the resolution of problems, and if the structure of the problem is simpler, the difference becomes increasingly smaller as students course through the upper grades. However, in problems where the structure is more complex, such as in comparison problems, the differences continue to remain. First-grade students (MT) show an average of 2.37 while those who solve in their non-MT have an average of 1.83. Secondly, we observe that the mean differences remain similar (MT, 3.40; non-MT, 2.87).
If we look at
Table 7, we will see that when the LI is different from the MT, there are significant differences with respect to all types of problems, regardless of the place the unknown occupies or whether they are addition or subtraction problems. However, when the LI is the same, the differences do not appear in all problems. We can observe that in the simplest problems, such as change or equalization, there are no significant differences. We do not even see differences when the unknown is found at the end or in one of the other terms, since these problems are less difficult than when the unknown is located at the beginning.
This allows us to conclude that the student needs more time to decode and understand a problem in a language other than the MT, but as he becomes familiar with the structure and understands the situation, the gap narrows. It implies that LI has an initial effect in a direct way, but after being exposed to it for longer, the student acquires sufficient strategies to compensate for the difficulty that is found when the LI does not coincide with the MT. This makes us think that the mathematical language will develop independently of the LI at the beginning, as stated by Van Rinsveld et al. [
47,
52].
Considering all the results obtained, it becomes evident that the language of instruction functions as a variable with an influence analogous to the developmental stage of the student. The patterns shown by second-grade students with a non-coincident language closely resemble those exhibited by first-grade students with coincident language. It can be concluded that the LI may result in a gap in mathematical competence compared with the level attained in the MT during the early years. This leads to the idea that teaching mathematics in a language other the mother tongue may have a comparable impact to that of age on problem-solving dynamics [
19]. Consequently, a new thorough investigation has been opened to ascertain whether these disparities tend to diminish over time and culminate in the attainment of comparable proficiency, mirroring the evolutionary trajectory of the student.
One of the limitations of this study is that the sample size of students whose MT is English and LI is English is very limited. This is because the students that were taught in English came from only one group, and the number of students with English as their MT was smaller. The rest of the groups analyzed were made up of a larger number of students, with close to eighty students each. Ideally, the sample should reflect the wider population; however, the reliance on a convenience sample from institutions with bilingual programs determined the number of students across the respective groups.
Additionally, we have also controlled different variables such as the homogeneity of the groups and the intervention of the teachers, considering that the groups belonged to different educational centers, and that the students analyzed in terms of their LI and MT did not come from the same center. Students whose MT and LI are Spanish came from both Spain and the USA, a methodological variable we have controlled. However, there are other variables that cannot be controlled because these are specific limitations of a non-experimental design.
At the same time, it is important to highlight that due to the distribution of the sample, which sometimes did not meet the requirements of normality, the analyses carried out have been obtained through non-parametric tests, thus limiting the robustness of the conclusions.
In regards to the educational impact related to the results obtained from this study, we can highlight two main facts related to teaching mathematics in bilingual programs. In the first place, we must recognize the influence it exerts on problem-solving processes at early ages. Therefore, it would be convenient to set teaching routines that allow the understanding of the situation proposed as developed by Bruner [
53], that is, applying various mathematical teaching methods such as the enactive, pictorial, and symbolic steps. The first two methods allow the child to have a better transcoding of the number and, therefore, a better understanding of the shown situation [
54].
Secondly, the relationship of teaching mathematics in an LI presents several aspects for analysis that are ignored. One of those generally neglected issues is that teaching in a language other than the MT must have specific aspects that would not be necessary in the MT, such as a systematic teaching of vocabulary. If we support early-years teaching with images that allow students to relate words to their signifiers and aid with the systematic teaching of key words and vocabulary they can use to decipher information, comparison situations such as “more than” or “less than” will help them to create strategies to solve these specific problems. An effective approach could involve the use of code-switching techniques that can effectively support bilingual students in the mathematics classroom, including translanguaging, code-switching for clarification, code-switching for scaffolding, and code-switching for vocabulary development [
55].