Orthographic knowledge is a crucial component of literacy [
1,
2]. Broadly defined, it is knowledge of and the ability to learn, store, and use information about the written forms of words and the conventions of writing and spelling [
3,
4]. Spelling skills are thus one important dimension of orthographic knowledge. Spelling also has strong ties to other literacy skills, including word recognition, phonological awareness, and both text comprehension and composition [
5,
6,
7]. Spelling and its development have been widely studied in children, especially in a first language (L1) context, and the effectiveness of various instructional interventions for improving spelling (many of them based on phonological skills) has also been demonstrated [
8,
9,
10]. However, comparatively little work has examined spelling in second language (L2) adult learners, particularly the effectiveness of instructional interventions for improving spelling knowledge. Therefore, the goal of the present research was to examine spelling knowledge in adult learners of English as a second language (ESL) and to demonstrate the effectiveness of a phonics-based instructional intervention for improving their English spelling knowledge.
1. Spelling Development
Orthographic knowledge is crucial for literacy development. During the early stages of literacy acquisition, children commonly use their knowledge of phonological forms, such as rhymes or shared first sounds, to learn about the structure of words [
11]. Words with phonological overlap also often share parts of their spellings, thus providing a way for children to learn about whole spelling patterns rather than memorizing spellings letter by letter [
12]. This familiarity can then be used as the basis for making analogies, helping children read or spell unfamiliar words that contain familiar spelling patterns [
13,
14].
Familiarity with the orthographic forms of words is also critical for the development of rapid, automatic word recognition, and thus reading fluency. Though phonological information is initially used for decoding words, increased exposure to print develops readers’ familiarity with orthographic sequences, facilitating their ability to rapidly extract lexical information from print [
15,
16,
17]. This is a reciprocal process: as readers develop more automatic text processing, they begin to rely relatively more on orthographic information than grapheme-by-grapheme phonological decoding [
18,
19], ultimately linking together the sounds, spellings, and meanings of words in a process termed orthographic mapping [
17]. This ability to recognize words as units, based on whole orthographic forms rather than by decoding a series of individual letters, is necessary for fluent reading because it frees up cognitive resources for higher-level text processing and comprehension [
2,
20,
21,
22].
Overall, greater orthographic knowledge is associated with better decoding skills, more accurate word reading, and greater reading comprehension [
23,
24,
25], and it is important for supporting literacy cross-linguistically [
26]. However, there is evidence that orthographic knowledge may be even more important for readers of non-alphabetic languages, such as Chinese, in which phonological information cannot be directly recovered from the written form, as well as readers of deeper alphabetic orthographies, such as English [
27,
28,
29]. Thus, a thorough understanding of orthographic knowledge, its development, and how it can best be supported, is crucial for supporting literacy development.
It is important to recognize that the term ‘orthographic knowledge’ can be used to refer to a broad range of skills and types of knowledge, which can be grouped into sub-lexical and lexical orthographic knowledge [
4,
30,
31]. At the lexical level, it is often used to refer to spelling ability, and it is this aspect of orthographic knowledge that is the focus of the current research. This spelling knowledge is crucial for general literacy acquisition because of its role in automatizing word recognition and serving as the basis for making analogies among different word forms. However, the value of spelling knowledge goes beyond this, as spelling ability also plays a critical role in social perceptions, with poor spelling often associated with poor education, carelessness, or even lower intelligence [
32,
33,
34]. Even in an age of increasing acceptance of multiple language varieties, correct spelling is still critical and cannot be fully compensated for by tools such as spellcheck [
35,
36,
37]. Spelling knowledge also directly translates to more applied skills, such as the ability to proofread one’s writing and recognize orthographic errors [
38,
39].
Much of the research on spelling has focused on either documenting the development of spelling ability in children gaining L1 literacy or assessing the effectiveness of instructional techniques for improving L1 spelling (and other literacy skills). For example, studies have consistently found that phonics-based instruction is effective for improving spelling skills as well as reading ability, and that these effects can persist for years [
8,
40,
41,
42]. However, the vast majority of this research has focused specifically on children learning to spell in their L1, most commonly English. Compared to this extensive body of L1 spelling research, there is much less work on spelling development in an L2, in either children or adults [
43,
44]. Here, too, most of the research has concentrated on learners of English, although most studies comprise qualitative error documentation rather than examining the impact of instruction or L1 background on spelling ability. For example, Figueredo [
43] provides a review of ESL spelling studies and reports that, of studies identified for the review, 59% (16 of 27) focused primarily on providing “descriptive analyses of ESL learners’ English spelling errors” (p. 874) from a single L1 group, and only 11% (3 of 27) compared the spelling performance of learners from different L1 groups, making the generalizability of such findings unclear.
There has been even less work examining instructional interventions for L2 learners; indeed, most intervention studies and reviews, including the National Reading Panel [
45], have specifically excluded learners of English as a second language (henceforth referred to as English learners or ELs) from their analyses. Thus, direct evidence of the efficacy of varying instructional interventions for L2 learners is limited, and as highlighted by Moore and Klingner [
46], interventions that are effective for native English speakers cannot necessarily be assumed to be effective for ELs [
47]. This is a critical issue to address, given that in the United States alone there are over 20 million individuals that speak English less than “very well”, over 11 million adults that are non-literate, and, of those who were or are adult English language learners, 39% are “below basic” in their prose literacy [
48,
49,
50,
51].
Despite this, a small number of studies do provide evidence for the effectiveness of interventions for L2 English spelling and suggest that similar instructional approaches are effective for EL children as well as L1 English-speaking children. Stuart [
52,
53] found that phoneme awareness and phonics training significantly improved 5-year-old ELs’ reading, spelling, and general phonological awareness skills, and that these differences were maintained across multiple years of schooling. Similarly, Siegel and colleagues [
54,
55] have found that systematic phonological awareness activities and explicit instruction were effective for improving literacy outcomes (including spelling) and the rate at which children were classified as reading-disabled among ESL children in kindergarten and grades 1 and 2 (ages 5–7).
Similar results have been found with somewhat older learners. For example, Van Staden [
56] found that explicit instruction on a range of reading skills, including word decoding, sight word recognition, and vocabulary knowledge, significantly improved word recognition, reading comprehension, and spelling outcomes for ELs in grades 4–6 (ages 10–14). Lovett et al. [
57] examined the effectiveness of phonologically-based Response to Intervention (RtI) programs focusing on word decoding and identification for both ELs and L1 English speakers in grades 2–8 (ages 6–14) who were classified as reading-disabled. They found that such interventions were equally effective for improving outcomes for reading and related skills (e.g., phonological processing, word identification) in both groups. Among a small sample of adult learners, Massengill Shaw [
58] found that learners who received Word Study [
59] improved in their developmental spelling scores significantly more than control learners who received spelling tutoring focusing on traditional methods, such as sight word memorization.
Thus, there are some positive findings for the effectiveness of phonics-based, direct instruction for improving literacy skills in general, and spelling knowledge in particular, in ELs as well as L1 English speakers. This is therefore an area that needs increased attention in ESL literacy research, especially because to date such studies are limited in number, focus almost exclusively on younger children [
60], and often examine individual or small-group supplemental instruction, rather than whole-classroom instruction [
46,
60]. Although this type of individualized instruction may be ideal, it is also resource-intensive and may not be feasible in all classroom environments, particularly with adult learners [
61].
2. L1 Influences on Spelling Knowledge
Another factor that must be considered when examining ESL literacy skills is learners’ L1 background. A growing amount of research demonstrates that L1, particularly the L1 writing system, influences the development of and reliance on literacy skills in text processing. For example, readers with an alphabetic L1 (in which each grapheme corresponds to a unit of spoken language), rely relatively more on phonological skills and information to read a text and spell words compared to readers from a non-alphabetic L1 background (in which there is a much less direct connection between written and spoken units of language) who rely relatively more on orthographic skills and information for the same tasks [
27,
29,
62].
Most relevant to the current research, these L1 reading processes are often transferred to L2 literacy, so that L2 readers from an alphabetic L1 also rely more on phonological information and L2 readers from a non-alphabetic L1 rely more on orthographic information in their L2 for literacy tasks including vocabulary learning, word recognition, pseudoword decoding, phonological awareness, and spelling knowledge [
63,
64,
65,
66,
67,
68]. For example, Wang and Geva [
69] compared spelling abilities in L1 Chinese-speaking children learning ESL and L1 English-speaking children. They found that the L1 Chinese speakers had lower performance than the L1 English speakers for spelling pseudowords to dictation, but that they had higher performance for a spelling task in which they had to rely on visual information over phonological information in order to perform accurately. Dixon, Zhao, and Joshi [
70] compared bilingual children from Singapore with Chinese, Tamil (syllabic writing system), or Malay (alphabetic writing system) as L1 and English as L2 on measures of English word reading and spelling. Their results also showed that the L1 Chinese speakers had the best spelling accuracy overall; however, their L1 Chinese speakers had relatively fewer misspellings that were phonologically plausible than their L1 Tamil or L1 Malay speakers. These findings are further supported by the work of Leong, Tan, Cheng, and Hau [
71], who used regression, principle component analysis, and structural equation modeling to demonstrate stronger relationships in L1 Chinese (Cantonese) speakers between English word spelling and orthographic and lexical information, compared to phonological skills.
As these studies demonstrate, L1 background can help explain specific patterns of ESL spelling performance. This is notable given that, to date, most research on L2 literacy interventions have aggregated learners from different L1s, making it impossible to determine whether the effectiveness of instructional techniques varies by language background [
46,
60,
72,
73,
74]. Thus, cross-linguistic research, including comparisons of students from different L1 types, is also needed to better understand L2 spelling development.
7. Discussion
The goal of the current research was to examine spelling knowledge in intermediate-level adult ESL learners and examine the impacts of word characteristics, learner L1 background, and a phonics-based instructional intervention on this spelling knowledge. The instructional intervention was adapted from the PHAST program by Lovett and colleagues [
111,
112]. The intervention in this study lasted 12 weeks and consisted of approximately 5.5–8 h of activities that were designed to improve participants’ knowledge of GPCs and spelling patterns and provide them with strategies for dealing with unfamiliar words in a text. Spelling knowledge was evaluated using a spelling verification task, in which participants saw a list of single lexical items and for each had to indicate whether or not they thought it was a correctly spelled English word. These items varied on their word frequency, number of syllables (monosyllabic or disyllabic), spelling consistency (consistent, inconsistent, or exception), and their spelling (correct, misspelled but with preserved pronunciation, or misspelled with altered pronunciation).
The results from the spelling knowledge pre-test, administered at the beginning of the semester, revealed that word frequency, number of syllables, and spelling consistency were all significant predictors of accuracy: participants were more likely to accurately judge the correctness of an item’s spelling if it was more frequent, monosyllabic rather than disyllabic, and had either a consistent or an inconsistent spelling pattern (rather than an exception spelling pattern). In addition, participants’ L1 writing system type was also a significant predictor of spelling knowledge. Unsurprisingly, all ESL groups were less accurate than the L1 English speakers. In addition, the non-native English-speaker groups with an alphabetic L1 were more accurate than the groups with a consonant-based (abjad or abugida) L1, and the Roman alphabet L1 group was more accurate than the morphosyllabary L1 group.
A similar post-test was administered at the end of the semester to determine whether or how spelling knowledge had changed over the course of one semester of intensive English study. Compared to participants in the two cohorts that received only the standard IEP reading instruction, the participants in the cohort that received the phonics-based instruction intervention were more likely to accurately judge the correctness of an item’s spelling. Further, after controlling for pre-existing differences at pre-test and instruction type, participants’ L1 writing system type was still a significant predictor of accuracy. In particular, participants with a Roman alphabet L1 were more likely to respond to a given item correctly than participants with a consonant-based (abjad or abugida) L1.
The importance of these results are three-fold. First, and perhaps most critical, the results demonstrate the effectiveness of the instructional intervention for improving adult English learners’ spelling knowledge. Although some previous studies have demonstrated the effectiveness of phonologically-based, direct-instruction teaching methods for improving ELs’ literacy skills [
52,
53,
54,
55,
56,
57,
58], there have been relatively few of these studies [
46,
60] and most of them have focused on elementary-age children rather than adults. Thus, the results of the current study both confirm the efficacy of phonics-based and direct-instruction methods for ELs and extend these findings to an under-studied population: adult learners. Given the large and growing number of adult ESL users with limited English proficiency and literacy skills, this will be a crucial area of focus for continued research.
Second, the significant impact of various lexical characteristics (word frequency, number of syllables, and spelling consistency) on participants’ spelling knowledge is consistent with previous research that has focused on monolingual L1 speakers [
122,
123,
124,
125] and extends these findings to adult L2 English learners. This finding is notable because the source of these lexical effects is often thought to derive from experience with words and texts and may thus differ for non-native speakers who have different levels and types of language exposure. Thus, the finding of a similar set of effects in this study helps to establish that adult ELs with intermediate English proficiency have also experienced sufficient language exposure to be affected by such lexical variables, and it is therefore crucial to control them in studies with L2 speakers as well.
Third, literacy researchers have begun to recognize the diversity of ELs and the importance of the variations in their language experiences [
46,
60,
74,
126,
127]. Despite this, relatively few studies have directly examined how these variable experiences actually impact learners’ literacy skills. The current research therefore contributes to the growing body of literature demonstrating that learners with different L1 writing systems may have different levels of or strengths in their literacy skills [
69,
128,
129,
130,
131]. Specifically, in the current study, learners with an alphabetic L1 generally had higher odds of accurately judging the correctness of an item’s spelling than learners with a consonant-based or morphosyllabary L1. This suggests that the experiences these learners have had with their L1 alphabetic writing system have prepared them for a similar experience in their alphabetic L2, English. This finding of positive transfer of bottom-up literacy skills in learners with similar L1 writing systems is also consistent with previous research [
132,
133,
134,
135], which has demonstrated that L1 literacy facilitates the development of similar skills in a second language, especially one with a similar writing system.
Considering learners with a non-alphabetic L1, research has generally found that learners with a consonant-based L1 may have particular difficulty with L2 spelling in English [
136,
137,
138]. On the other hand, studies with morphosyllabic L1 speakers have typically found that these learners have relatively stronger spelling skills than learners with an alphabetic L1 [
69,
139,
140]. The finding from the current study that alphabetic L1 learners performed better on the spelling task is therefore somewhat inconsistent with previous research. Although it is not possible to definitively determine the reason for this, methodological considerations may play a role. Many of the studies that have found stronger spelling skills in speakers of a morphosyllabic L1 have used tasks that either required learners to actually write out words [
69,
139] or to compare multiple possible spellings [
140], rather than respond to a single item (as in the current study). These different tasks may require the use of different dimensions of orthographic knowledge and thus result in differential advantages for learners with different types of L1 orthographic experiences; future research that investigates multiple dimensions of orthographic knowledge in the same samples of participants from different L1 backgrounds may help to illuminate this issue.
There are, of course, limitations to the current study. Given the importance of exploring the impact of diverse L1 backgrounds on ELs’ spelling knowledge, and literacy skills in general, additional research would benefit from larger sample sizes of participants from each L1. This would provide crucial corroborating evidence of the patterns identified in this study and would also eliminate the need to group together learners from multiple L1s based on broader writing system types. This would be particularly useful given that even within writing system types (e.g., alphabets), there are wide ranges in the level of consistency [
141,
142]. Future research would also benefit from the use of multiple spelling measures. The ability to correctly identify misspelled words is useful in tasks such as proofreading [
38,
143,
144], and does reflect underlying spelling ability, but is only a single way of operationalizing this knowledge. Finally, although the instructional intervention was found to be effective compared to two independent control groups, further replication would be beneficial to confirm this finding. Additional research looking at the efficacy of different components of the intervention, such as direction instruction of GPCs versus practice using keywords to make analogies to other words, would also be useful to refine and improve the intervention and identify which instructional components are most crucial [
145].
To conclude, the current study demonstrated the effectiveness of a phonics-based instructional intervention for improving adult EL students’ knowledge of English spelling. It also demonstrated that characteristics of both learners (their L1 background) and words (number of syllables, frequency, and spelling consistency) can impact learners’ knowledge of English word spellings. Thus, this work extends to adults the findings from previous research regarding the usefulness of phonics-based activities and direct instruction on L2 spelling knowledge. This is a promising area for future research to more specifically identify the most crucial components of instruction for adult learners and refine instructional approaches for ELs, as well as determine how various lexical characteristics impact literacy skills in these individuals.