Digital Devices Use and Chinese-Canadian First Graders’ Early English Literacy Development: A Mixed-Methods Study
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Literature Review
2.1. Digital Use and Early Literacy Development
2.2. Home Digital Use and Its Possible Impacts on Literacy Development
2.3. Digital Exposure and Early English Literacy Development for Bilingual Children
- What are the patterns in the frequency of Chinese-Canadian first graders’ digital devices use and their early English literacy achievements?
- What is the relationship between the children’s digital devices use and their English literacy skills?
- What are the contexts (e.g., purposes and practices) of the children’s digital devices use at home for English and what are their parents’ perceptions of its impact on the children’s English language development?
3. Methods
3.1. Participants
3.2. Measures and Procedures
3.2.1. English Receptive Vocabulary
3.2.2. English Reading Comprehension and Decoding Skills
3.2.3. Frequency of Digital Devices Use
3.2.4. Interview Procedures
3.3. Data Analysis
3.3.1. Quantitative Data Analyses
3.3.2. Qualitative Data Analysis
4. Findings
4.1. Patterns of Digital Devices Use and Achievements in English Literacy Skills
4.2. The Relationship Between Children’s Digital Devices Use and Their Achievements in Early English Literacy Skills
4.3. Purposes of Multiple Digital Devices at Home
From Monday to Friday, the teacher said that from 9:30 to 10:00, there will be story-telling. So they (the students) listen in front of the computer. After that, they take a 20 min break. Then they start class again. They have three breaks of 15 to 20 min… No (class) in the afternoon. Many of the assignments are done online… For those that need to be printed, we print them out and he (the child) completes them, and then sends them to the teacher on the computer… If [the assignments are] less, they can be completed in half an hour. If more, it may take him an hour.(F159, boy)
I found that watching cartoons was actually the fastest way to learn a language. Because I was like that before. When I was a child, I moved to Shenzhen… I was not local [Cantonese speakers]… then we were only exposed to television stations in Hong Kong, so I picked up Cantonese very fast as we watched [Cantonese] cartoons at that time… I often found my daughter repeating the sentences from those [English] cartoons… so they were learning the language.(F140, girl)
4.4. Diversity in Digital Resources for English Learning
4.5. Parental Attitudes Toward the Role of Digital Devices
5. Discussion
6. Conclusions and Implications
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Alamri, M., Alrehaili, M., Albariqi, W., Alshehri, M., Alotaibi, K., & Algethami, A. (2023). Relationship between speech delay and smart media in children: A systematic review. Cureus, 15(9), e45396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anderson, D. R., & Pempek, T. A. (2005). Television and very young children. American Behavioral Scientist, 48(5), 505–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Auxier, B., Anderson, M., Perrin, A., & Turner, E. (2020, July 28). Parenting children in the age of screens. Pew Research Center. Available online: https://www.pewresearch.org/internet/2020/07/28/parenting-children-in-the-age-of-screens/ (accessed on 1 June 2024).
- Babayiğit, S., Hitch, G. J., Kandru-Pothineni, S., Clarke, A., & Warmington, M. (2022). Vocabulary limitations undermine bilingual children’s reading comprehension despite bilingual cognitive strengths. Reading and Writing, 35(7), 1651–1673. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2008). Lexical access in bilinguals: Effects of vocabulary size and executive control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21(6), 522–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K. F., & Yang, S. (2010). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(4), 525–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Blankson, A. N., O’Brien, M., Leerkes, E. M., Calkins, S. D., & Marcovitch, S. (2015). Do hours spent viewing television at ages 3 and 4 predict vocabulary and executive functioning at age 5? Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 61(2), 264–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2012). Thematic analysis. In H. Cooper, P. M. Camic, D. L. Long, A. T. Panter, D. Rindskopf, & K. J. Sher (Eds.), APA handbook of research methods in psychology, Vol. 2. Research designs: Quantitative, qualitative, neuropsychological, and biological (pp. 57–71). American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buckingham, D. (2003). Media literacies. In Media education: Literacy, learning and contemporary culture (pp. 39–45). Polity. [Google Scholar]
- Chapelle, C. A. (2009). The relationship between second language acquisition theory and computer-assisted language learning. The Modern Language Journal, 93, 741–753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cycyk, L. M., & Anda, S. D. (2021). Media exposure and language experience: Examining associations from home observations in Mexican immigrant families in the US. Infant Behavior and Development, 63, 101554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dixon, L. Q., Zhao, J., Quiroz, B. G., & Shin, J.-Y. (2012). Home and community factors influencing bilingual children’s ethnic language vocabulary development. International Journal of Bilingualism, 16(4), 541–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dore, R. A., Logan, J., Lin, T.-J., Purtell, K. M., & Justice, L. (2020a). Characteristics of children’s media use and gains in language and literacy skills. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 2224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dore, R. A., Logan, J., Lin, T.-J., Purtell, K. M., & Justice, L. M. (2020b). Associations between children’s media use and language and literacy skills. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1734. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dunn, L. M. (2019). Peabody picture vocabulary test (5th ed.). Pearson. [Google Scholar]
- Elias, N., & Abdulaev, N. (2022). Immigrant children and media. In The Routledge international handbook of children, adolescents, and media (2nd ed., pp. 363–370). Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Evans, M. A., Shaw, D., & Bell, M. (2000). Home literacy activities and their influence on early literacy skills. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(2), 65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fitzpatrick, C., Binet, M.-A., Cristini, E., Almeida, M. L., Bégin, M., & Frizzo, G. B. (2023). Reducing harm and promoting positive media use strategies: New perspectives in understanding the impact of preschooler media use on health and development. Psicologia, Reflexao e Critica: Revista Semestral Do Departamento de Psicologia Da UFRGS, 36(1), 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Garrett, N. (1991). Technology in the service of language learning: Trends and issues. Modern Language Journal, 75, 74–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gath, M., McNeill, B., & Gillon, G. (2023). Preschoolers’ screen time and reduced opportunities for quality interaction: Associations with language development and parent-child closeness. Current Research in Behavioral Sciences, 5, 100140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7(1), 6–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoikkala, T., Rahkonen, O., Tigerstedt, C., & Tuormaa, J. (1987). Wait a minute, Mr Postman!-Some critical remarks on Neil Postman’s childhood theory. Acta Sociologica, 30(1), 87–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 2, 127–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hudon, T. M., Fennell, C. T., & Hoftyzer, M. (2013). Quality not quantity of television viewing is associated with bilingual toddlers’ vocabulary scores. Infant Behavior & Development, 36(2), 245–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jing, M., Ye, T., Kirkorian, H. L., & Mares, M. (2023). Screen media exposure and young children’s vocabulary learning and development: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 94(5), 1398–1418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kabali, H. K., Irigoyen, M. M., Nunez-Davis, R., Budacki, J. G., Mohanty, S. H., Leister, K. P., & Bonner, R. L. (2015). Exposure and use of mobile media devices by young children. Pediatrics, 136(6), 1044–1050. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2014). Kaufman test of educational achievement (3rd ed.). Pearson. [Google Scholar]
- Khan, K. S., Purtell, K. M., Logan, J., Ansari, A., & Justice, L. M. (2017). Association between television viewing and parent-child reading in the early home environment. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 38(7), 521–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Korat, O., & Falk, Y. (2019). Ten years after: Revisiting the question of e-book quality as early language and literacy support. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 19(2), 206–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kostyrka-Allchorne, K., Cooper, N. R., & Simpson, A. (2017). The relationship between television exposure and children’s cognition and behaviour: A systematic review. Developmental Review, 44, 19–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krashen, S. (1982). Principles and practice in second language acquisition. Pergamon. [Google Scholar]
- Kucirkova, N., & Flewitt, R. (2022). Understanding parents’ conflicting beliefs about children’s digital book reading. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 22(2), 157–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lauricella, A. R., Wartella, E., & Rideout, V. J. (2015). Young children’s screen time: The complex role of parent and child factors. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 36, 11–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, G., Mei, Z., & Zhen, F. (2024). Friend or foe? A mixed-methods study on the impact of digital device use on Chinese–Canadian children’s heritage language learning. Digital Studies in Language and Literature, 1(1–2), 55–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, G., Sun, Z., Zhen, F., Ji, X. R., & Gunderson, L. (2022). Home literacy environment and Chinese-Canadian first graders’ bilingual vocabulary profiles: A mixed methods analysis. Sustainability, 14(23), 15788. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, G., Zhen, F., Lin, Z., & Gunderson, L. (2023). Bilingual home literacy experiences and early biliteracy development among Chinese–Canadian first graders. Education Sciences, 13(8), 808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lin, L. Y., Cherng, R. J., Chen, Y. J., Chen, Y. J., & Yang, H. M. (2015). Effects of television exposure on developmental skills among young children. Infant Behavior & Development, 38, 20–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Masur, E. F., Flynn, V., & Olson, J. (2016). Infants’ background television exposure during play: Negative relations to the quantity and quality of mothers’ speech and infants’ vocabulary acquisition. First Language, 36(2), 109–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moursad, F., Utriainen, S., Lamminpää, R., & Katri, V.-J. (2023). Parents’ expectations regarding media use among children up to 6 years of age. Women and Children Nursing, 1(1), 2–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Neuman, S. B. (1991). Literacy in the television age: The myth of the TV effect. Greenwood Publishing Group. [Google Scholar]
- Neumann, M. M., & Neumann, D. L. (2014). Touch screen tablets and emergent literacy. Early Childhood Education Journal, 42, 231–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Okwir, S., & Nuur, C. (2022). Reflections on Neil Postman’s “Amusing ourselves to death: Public discourse in the age of show business” (1985) in the context of today’s digital social media platforms. Journal of Applied Economics and Business Research, 12(2), 71–79. [Google Scholar]
- Operto, F. F., Pastorino, G. M. G., Marciano, J., De Simone, V., Volini, A. P., Olivieri, M., Buonaiuto, R., Vetri, L., Viggiano, A., & Coppola, G. (2020). Digital devices use and language skills in children between 8 and 36 month. Brain Sciences, 10(9), 656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Papastefanou, T., Marinis, T., & Powell, D. (2021). Development of reading comprehension in bilingual and monolingual children—Effects of language exposure. Languages, 6(4), 166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paradis, J., Emmerzael, K., & Duncan, T. S. (2010). Assessment of English language learners: Using parent report on first language development. Journal of Communication Disorders, 43(6), 474–497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peters, E., & Webb, S. (2018). incidental vocabulary acquisition through viewing l2 television and factors that affect learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40(3), 551–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Postman, N. (1985). Chapter X: Media and Technology as Educators. Teachers College Record, 86(5), 183–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reich, S. M., Yau, J. C., Xu, Y., Muskat, T., Uvalle, J., & Cannata, D. (2019). Digital or print? A comparison of preschoolers’ comprehension, vocabulary, and engagement from a print book and an e-book. AERA Open, 5(3), 2332858419878389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rose, E., Lehrl, S., Ebert, S., & Weinert, S. (2018). Long-term relations between children’s language, the home literacy environment, and socioemotional development from ages 3 to 8. Early Education and Development, 29(3), 342–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sailer, M., & Homner, L. (2020). The gamification of learning: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 32(1), 77–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Saleem, A. N., Noori, N. M., & Ozdamli, F. (2022). Gamification applications in e-learning: A literature review. Technology, Knowledge and Learning, 27(1), 139–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Segers, E., & Kleemans, T. (2020). The impact of the digital home environment on kindergartners’ language and early literacy. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 538584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. A. (2014). Continuity and change in the home literacy environment as predictors of growth in vocabulary and reading. Child Development, 85(4), 1552–1568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Skwarchuk, S. L., Sowinski, C., & LeFevre, J. A. (2014). Formal and informal home learning activities in relation to children’s early numeracy and literacy skills: The development of a home numeracy model. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 121, 63–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sonnenschein, S., Stites, M. L., Gursoy, H., & Khorsandian, J. (2023). Elementary-school students’ use of digital devices at home to support learning pre- and post-COVID-19. Education Sciences, 13(2), 117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Statistics Canada. (2022, August 17). Mandarin is the main non-official language spoken predominantly at home in Toronto and Vancouver, while in Montréal, it is Spanish and Arabic. Statistics Canada. Available online: https://www150.statcan.gc.ca/n1/daily-quotidien/220817/g-a007-eng.htm (accessed on 2 January 2023).
- Sun, H., Tan, J., & Chen, W. (2023). COVID-19 and bilingual children’s home language environment: Digital media, socioeconomic status, and language status. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1115108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sun, H., & Yin, B. (2020). Multimedia input and bilingual children’s language learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 2023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taylor, G., Monaghan, P., & Westermann, G. (2018). Investigating the association between children’s screen media exposure and vocabulary size in the UK. Journal of Children and Media, 12(1), 51–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Teale, W. H., & Sulzby, E. (1987). Literacy acquisition in early childhood: The roles of access and mediation in storybook reading. The future of literacy in a changing world, 1987, 111–130. [Google Scholar]
- Teng, (Mark) Feng. (2022). Vocabulary learning through videos: Captions, advance-organizer strategy, and their combination. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 35(3), 518–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomasello, M. (2005). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Tomopoulos, S., Dreyer, B. P., Berkule, S., Fierman, A. H., Brockmeyer, C., & Mendelsohn, A. L. (2010). Infant media exposure and toddler development. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 164(12), 1105–1111. [Google Scholar]
- Wong, K. M., Flynn, R. M., & Neuman, S. B. (2021). L2 vocabulary learning from educational media: The influence of screen-based scaffolds on the incidental–intentional continuum. TESOL Journal, 12(4), e641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhao, S., & Flewitt, R. (2020). Young Chinese immigrant children’s language and literacy practices on social media: A translanguaging perspective. Language and Education, 34(3), 267–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zimmerman, F. J., Christakis, D. A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). Associations between media viewing and language development in children under age 2 years. The Journal of Pediatrics, 15, 364–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
N | Percent | Mean | SD | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Total | 121 | 100 | ||
Age (in months) | 79.50 | 4.04 | ||
Gender | ||||
Male | 58 | 47.93 | ||
Female | 63 | 52.07 | ||
Family SES | ||||
Low-SES | 27 | 22.31 | ||
Non-low-SES | 94 | 77.69 | ||
Immigration status | ||||
Immigrant | 26 | 21.49 | ||
Born in Canada | 95 | 78.51 |
N | Percent | Mean | SD | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Total | 66 | 100 | ||
Age (in months) | 78.24 | 3.65 | ||
Gender | ||||
Male | 28 | 42.42 | ||
Female | 38 | 57.58 | ||
Family SES | ||||
Low-SES | 14 | 21.21 | ||
Non-low-SES | 52 | 78.79 | ||
Immigration status | ||||
Immigrant | 11 | 16.67 | ||
Born in Canada | 55 | 83.33 |
Laptops | TV Programs or Movies | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | Mean | SD | t | Cohen’s d | Mean | SD | t | Cohen’s d | |
Total | 121 | 1.36 | 0.79 | 1.19 | 0.75 | ||||
Gender | |||||||||
Male | 58 | 1.33 | 0.78 | −0.37 | 0.80 | 1.25 | 0.76 | 0.73 | 0.75 |
Female | 63 | 1.38 | 0.81 | 1.14 | 0.74 | ||||
Family SES | |||||||||
Low-SES | 27 | 1.19 | 0.83 | −1.27 | 0.79 | 0.93 | 0.78 | −2.12 * | 0.74 |
Non-low-SES | 94 | 1.40 | 0.78 | 1.27 | 0.72 | ||||
Immigration status | |||||||||
Immigrant | 26 | 1.31 | 0.84 | −0.34 | 0.8 | 1.35 | 0.75 | 1.21 | 0.74 |
Born in Canada | 95 | 1.37 | 0.79 | 1.15 | 0.74 |
Receptive Vocabulary | Letter and Word Recognition | Reading Comprehension | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | t | Cohen’s d | Mean | SD | t | Cohen’s d | Mean | SD | t | Cohen’s d | |
Total | 91.33 | 14.12 | 106.22 | 18.93 | 105.26 | 17.61 | ||||||
Gender | ||||||||||||
Male | 88.93 | 13.84 | −1.81 | 13.99 | 105.68 | 18.74 | −0.21 | 19.07 | 102.68 | 17.64 | −1.10 | 17.58 |
Female | 93.54 | 14.13 | 106.66 | 19.33 | 107.37 | 17.53 | ||||||
Family SES | ||||||||||||
Low-SES | 86.89 | 17.91 | −1.87 | 13.98 | 104.13 | 17.03 | −0.48 | 19.04 | 100.20 | 17.56 | −1.26 | 17.53 |
Non-low-SES | 92.61 | 12.67 | 106.80 | 19.54 | 106.67 | 17.52 | ||||||
Immigration status | ||||||||||||
Immigrant | 91.85 | 20.96 | 0.21 | 14.18 | 112.91 | 17.07 | 1.29 | 18.84 | 103.55 | 22.43 | −0.35 | 17.72 |
Born in Canada | 91.19 | 11.74 | 104.95 | 19.14 | 105.59 | 16.76 |
Independent Variables | Receptive Vocabulary | Letter and Word Recognition | Reading Comprehension | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Regression 1 | Regression 2 | Regression 1 | Regression 2 | Regression 1 | Regression 2 | |
Demographic variables | ||||||
Gender | 0.12 | 0.13 | 0.05 | 0.04 | 0.10 | 0.10 |
SES | 0.20 * | 0.19 * | 0.34 ** | 0.34 ** | 0.34 ** | 0.34 ** |
Immigration status | −0.01 | 0.01 | −0.19 | −0.19 | −0.03 | −0.03 |
Model fit | ||||||
0.06 | 0.06 | 0.13 | 0.13 | 0.13 | 0.13 | |
F | 2.68 * | 2.68 * | 3.20 * | 3.20 * | 3.36 * | 3.36 * |
Key variables | ||||||
Computers | −0.02 | −0.09 | −0.04 | |||
Model fit | ||||||
∆ | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.01 | |||
∆F | 0.03 | 0.61 | 0.11 | |||
TV programs | 0.18 * | 0.10 | 0.01 | |||
Model fit | ||||||
∆ | 0.03 | 0.01 | 0.00 | |||
∆F | 4.11 * | 0.68 | 0.01 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Li, G.; Zhen, F.; Mei, Z. Digital Devices Use and Chinese-Canadian First Graders’ Early English Literacy Development: A Mixed-Methods Study. Educ. Sci. 2025, 15, 48. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15010048
Li G, Zhen F, Mei Z. Digital Devices Use and Chinese-Canadian First Graders’ Early English Literacy Development: A Mixed-Methods Study. Education Sciences. 2025; 15(1):48. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15010048
Chicago/Turabian StyleLi, Guofang, Fubiao Zhen, and Ziwen Mei. 2025. "Digital Devices Use and Chinese-Canadian First Graders’ Early English Literacy Development: A Mixed-Methods Study" Education Sciences 15, no. 1: 48. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15010048
APA StyleLi, G., Zhen, F., & Mei, Z. (2025). Digital Devices Use and Chinese-Canadian First Graders’ Early English Literacy Development: A Mixed-Methods Study. Education Sciences, 15(1), 48. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15010048