1. Introduction
The 21st century has increasingly been characterized by uncertainty, as frequent and unpredictable events—such as economic crises, pandemics, and environmental disruptions—continue to reshape social and economic landscapes (
Ball, 2012;
Dishon & Gilead, 2021;
Pietrocola et al., 2025). In response to this instability, diversity and inclusion have gained prominence as essential values across sectors such as business, education, and public policy (
Chimakati & Kelemba, 2023;
Davis & Miller, 1996;
Saxena, 2014;
Stamps & Foley, 2023). A growing body of research suggests that environments fostering psychological safety and inclusion are associated with enhanced creativity, problem-solving, and overall well-being (
Carmeli et al., 2010;
Frazier et al., 2017). Within organizational studies, human resource diversity has been conceptualized in terms of four distinct states: inclusion (high uniqueness and high belonging), differentiation (high uniqueness and low belonging), assimilation (low uniqueness and high belonging), and exclusion (low uniqueness and low belonging) (
Shore et al.,
2011,
2018). While some scholars argue that homogeneity may enhance team cohesion and decision-making efficiency in small groups, others suggest the potential role of diversity in promoting collaboration and innovation within larger organizations and societies (
Distefano & Maznevski, 2000;
Mannix & Neale, 2005;
Modi et al., 2025). As global uncertainty deepens, understanding how individuals experience inclusion—and how this experience relates to their well-being—has become increasingly relevant for designing democratic and resilient societies. However, most existing research has focused on adults in workplaces and communities, leaving significant gaps in our understanding of how children experience diversity and inclusion in educational settings. Moreover, the rapid expansion of digital technologies (e.g., smartphones, tablets, online platforms) has begun to reshape children’s social environments in and beyond the classroom. While digital engagement offers new opportunities for connection and learning, it also raises questions about its influence on students’ sense of inclusion and psychological well-being. Against this backdrop, this study examines how elementary and junior high school students in rural Japan perceive classroom diversity and inclusion, how their use of digital devices may shape their social interactions, and how these factors relate to their subjective well-being (SWB). In addition to SWB, we consider related psychological outcomes such as inquisitiveness and generativity, along with the moderating role of adult responsiveness, as these interrelated dimensions provide a comprehensive understanding of children’s experiences in diverse and digitalized classrooms. By focusing on children’s perspectives, the study aims to contribute to the emerging discourse on inclusion and digitalization in educational contexts.
While numerous studies have examined diversity in workgroups, the concept of inclusion has recently gained significant attention in research on well-being within school settings (
Adams & Meyers, 2020;
Downey et al., 2015;
Juvonen et al., 2019;
Randel, 2025;
Versteegen & Adams, 2025). As scholarship on diversity and inclusion has progressed, researchers have increasingly explored how these factors contribute to organizational performance, as well as the mechanisms through which the potential benefits of diversity and inclusion can be realized (
Gomez & Bernet, 2019;
Stahl & Maznevski, 2021;
Trkulja et al., 2024). A growing body of work now emphasizes the importance of cultivating inclusive environments—whether in workplaces or schools—where individuals perceive themselves as valued members of a group (
Nishii & Leroy, 2022;
Sabharwal, 2014;
Shafaei et al., 2024;
Woods et al., 2024). To understand the psychological basis of such perceptions, Brewer’s Optimal Distinctiveness Theory (ODT) offers a useful framework (
Brewer,
1991,
1993;
Way et al., 2022;
Zhao & Glynn, 2022). According to ODT, individuals are motivated to find social contexts that satisfy two fundamental and often competing needs: the need for belongingness and the need for uniqueness. Supporting this proposition,
Pickett et al. (
2002) found that individuals preferentially select groups that allow them to balance these needs, especially in uncertain or dynamic environments. Building on this theoretical foundation, we define inclusion in this study as the extent to which individuals perceive themselves to be recognized as important group members (
Shore et al.,
2011,
2018). This perception arises from experiences that simultaneously fulfill both the need for belonging and the need for uniqueness (
Good et al., 2012;
Mor Barak et al., 2022;
Randel et al., 2018) and serves as the conceptual foundation for our analysis.
Children born and raised in environments where smartphones and other digital devices are ubiquitous are often referred to as digital natives (
Agárdi & Alt, 2024;
Bennett et al., 2008;
Helsper & Eynon, 2010). With the rapid advancement of technologies such as quantum computing and artificial intelligence (AI), digital innovation continues to accelerate at an unprecedented pace (
Coccia, 2024;
How & Cheah, 2024;
Taylor, 2025). These developments are transforming children’s daily lives and educational experiences and may also shape their future career paths and aspirations (
Akour & Alenezi, 2022;
Mhlanga, 2023;
Southworth et al., 2023). While the inclusivity or exclusivity of social environments—such as classrooms and workplaces—remains a key determinant of children’s well-being, these environments are undergoing significant change. The growing presence of internet technologies and AI suggests they increasingly involve non-human actors (e.g., AI chatbots, recommendation systems, and virtual assistants), alongside traditional human interactions (
Bobillier Chaumon, 2021;
Georgiou, 2023;
Kozyreva et al., 2020). In an empirical study involving 511 Japanese children,
Hirose (
2024) found that curiosity-driven questioning is positively associated with children’s subjective well-being (SWB) and that the quality of adult responses plays a critical role in nurturing children’s intrinsic curiosity. From this perspective, reciprocal interactions—whereby a child poses a question out of curiosity and receives an engaged response from an adult—are considered crucial for fostering children’s SWB (
Åkerman et al., 2024;
Eaude, 2009;
Jirout et al., 2024;
Park & Peterson, 2006). However, the potential influence of alternative sources of response—such as internet searches or AI-generated assistance—on children’s curiosity, well-being, and intergenerational relationships remains largely unexplored (
Kang et al., 2021;
Mhlanga, 2022;
Rubin et al., 2024). Overall, while adult engagement remains vital, it is also important to examine how emerging digital actors such as AI and smart devices shape children’s inquiry and well-being (
Banks et al., 2024;
Clemente-Suárez et al., 2024;
Ullrich et al., 2022).
Erikson (
1963) introduced the concept of generativity within the framework of life-course theory, defining it as a concern for and commitment to guiding and nurturing the next generation. Generativity can be expressed through a wide range of values and behaviors, including mentoring, community volunteering, or passing down family traditions and skills to younger generations (
McAdams & Logan, 2004;
Peterson, 2006;
Timilsina et al., 2019;
Wiktorowicz et al., 2022). To capture individual differences in generativity, several psychometric scales have been developed to assess its various dimensions (
Schoklitsch & Baumann, 2012). Among the most widely used is the Loyola Generativity Scale (LGS), which measures generative concern—that is, the emotional and motivational aspects of generativity—and has been commonly used in research (e.g.,
Jones & McAdams, 2013;
Lawford et al., 2005;
McAdams & de St. Aubin, 1992;
McAdams et al., 2001;
Peterson & Duncan, 1999). Another widely used instrument is the Generative Behavior Checklist (GBC), which evaluates generative behavior by assessing the frequency of relevant actions in the past two months (
McAdams et al., 1993;
Schoklitsch & Baumann, 2012). Studies using these two scales have consistently found a positive association between generative concern and generative behavior, suggesting a conceptual distinction between motivation and behavior, as well as an empirical link between them (
McAdams et al., 1993).
Recognizing the importance of cultural context,
Marushima and Arimitsu (
2007) developed the Revised Generative Concern Scale (r-GCS) for use in Japan. The r-GCS comprises three subscales—creativity, sustaining, and care offering—designed to reflect culturally relevant dimensions of generativity. More recently,
Hirose (
2024) employed the r-GCS in a questionnaire survey targeting Japanese children, adapting the scale to account for their developmental stage and sociocultural background. Across these studies, generativity has consistently emerged as a strong predictor of subjective well-being (SWB), even when controlling for prosocial tendencies and basic sociodemographic characteristics such as age and gender (
Jones & McAdams, 2013;
Lawford et al., 2005;
Peterson & Duncan, 1999;
Pratt et al., 2001;
Rittenour & Colaner, 2012;
Schoklitsch & Baumann, 2012;
Tabuchi et al., 2015). Findings based on the r-GCS further suggest that generative traits in children—such as kindness toward younger peers and concern for environmental sustainability—provide further evidence as significant predictors of their SWB (
Hirose, 2024). Overall, generativity correlates strongly with sociodemographic factors such as age, education, and income and is also associated with social sustainability indicators, including prosociality and SWB.
Maslow’s theory suggests that the fulfillment of psychological needs contributes to life satisfaction (
Maslow, 1954), which is widely regarded as a core aspect of well-being (
Diener, 2009). Various instruments have been developed to evaluate well-being, including the Subjective Happiness Scale (SHS), Ryff’s Psychological Well-Being Scales, and the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS), each capturing distinct dimensions of subjective and psychological well-being (see, e.g.,
Diener et al., 1985,
2003;
Lyubomirsky & Lepper, 1999;
Ryff, 1989). Well-being reflects not only material conditions but also emotional satisfaction, interpersonal relationships, and happiness. These factors are strongly associated with individuals’ overall quality of life (QOL), a broad concept that encompasses subjective well-being (SWB) as one of its key dimensions. Beyond economic conditions, happiness has been studied in relation to cultural norms, demographic factors, and psychological traits. Among these, age, gender, marital status, education, self-regard, and interpersonal ties have consistently been highlighted in the literature (
Diener et al.,
1998,
1999;
Chitchai et al., 2020;
Jan & Masood, 2008;
Kahneman et al., 1999;
Lee et al., 2000;
Oishi & Diener, 2001). While substantial research has focused on the determinants of well-being, increasing attention has been paid to its potential associations with beneficial outcomes, including higher engagement, optimism, and creativity (
Au et al., 2020;
Magnani & Zhu, 2018;
Meisenberg & Woodley, 2015). Factors such as age, economic status, social relationships, and personality traits are significant correlates of individuals’ well-being and life satisfaction. Furthermore, recent studies suggest that well-being is influenced by various factors but may also shape individuals’ cognitive patterns and behaviors. This underscores the reciprocal nature of well-being, highlighting its role as both an outcome and a contributing component within the broader context of QOL (
Bibi et al., 2015;
Hirose, 2024;
Hirose & Kotani, 2022;
Leung et al., 2011;
Welsch, 2006;
Zidansek, 2007).
People with an inquisitive mindset are more likely to exhibit curiosity about unfamiliar things or people and often initiate conversations by asking questions (
Bardone & Secchi, 2017;
Black, 2005;
Hirayama & Kusumi, 2004;
Watson, 2019). Building on the conceptualization of inquisitiveness as a component of critical thinking (
Facione et al., 1995;
Hirayama & Kusumi, 2004;
Hogan, 2009), subsequent research has increasingly explored how inquisitive individuals approach learning and social interaction across diverse contexts and how these behaviors may facilitate innovative solutions (
Bardone & Secchi, 2017;
Harris, 2011;
Hogan, 2009;
Kawashima & Petrini, 2004;
Watson, 2019). In a study involving 426 Japanese university students,
Hirayama and Kusumi (
2004) investigated how critical thinking dispositions influence the reasoning process. Their findings indicate that inquisitiveness is associated with forming conclusions that are not constrained by one’s preexisting beliefs. More recently,
Hirose and Kotani (
2022) and
Hirose (
2024) found that inquisitiveness is positively correlated with both generative concern and subjective well-being (SWB), based on surveys conducted with Japanese adults and children, respectively. Taken together, these findings suggest that inquisitiveness may be an important motivational factor associated with exploratory behaviors, dialogue initiation, and engagement with unfamiliar environments. Such behaviors, including initiating conversations, asking thoughtful questions, and seeking new experiences, may contribute to higher levels of SWB (
Baldwin & Moses, 1996;
Black, 2005;
Cluver et al., 2013;
Hirose, 2024;
Hirose & Kotani, 2022).
Despite increasing research on diversity and its associations with creativity and well-being, it remains underexplored how children in Japan experience inclusive or exclusive environments. Recent studies conducted in rural contexts, including India, South Africa, and Japan, have shown that children’s access to and use of ICT are significantly related to socio-demographic conditions (
Aruleba & Jere, 2022;
Jamil, 2021;
Kardam et al., 2024;
Kormos & Wisdom, 2021;
Nae, 2024). However, few studies have examined how these disparities intersect with children’s perceptions of inclusion and their well-being, particularly in developed countries like Japan, where urban–rural divides persist. This study examines how the cognitive, non-cognitive, and digital environments that children engage with may influence their subjective well-being (SWB). By investigating the extent to which diversity and inclusion are fostered within classroom and digital settings, this research aims to provide insights into how inclusive environments are associated with higher levels of inquisitiveness, creativity, and overall well-being.
The study focuses primarily on children’s subjective well-being (SWB) as the central outcome. It also incorporates inquisitiveness and generativity as related psychological dimensions that capture children’s curiosity-driven engagement and sense of future orientation. These constructs, along with digital device usage and adult responsiveness, are examined together because they interactively shape how inclusive or exclusive environments are experienced by children in contemporary classrooms. Specifically, the study classifies a sample of approximately 2150 elementary and junior high school students in Kochi, Japan, into five diversity-related categories: inclusion, assimilation, exclusion, differentiation, and an intermediate category. Using multinomial logistic regression and median regression analysis, the study explores how these categories are associated with digital device usage, inquisitiveness, generativity, and SWB. The results will elucidate the distribution of students across these categories and identify key factors that characterize each. This will contribute to a deeper understanding of how diverse classroom and digital environments are associated with children’s development and SWB.
Taken together, this study is guided by the following research questions:
How are Japanese school children distributed across diversity-related categories—such as inclusion, assimilation, exclusion, differentiation, and intermediate—and what factors are associated with their placement in these categories?
How is children’s SWB associated with their diversity-related categories, inquisitiveness, generativity, and digital device usage?
How is children’s generativity associated with their diversity-related categories, inquisitiveness, and digital device usage?
How is children’s inquisitiveness associated with their diversity-related categories, the responsiveness of adults to their questions, and digital device usage?
4. Results
4.1. Descriptive Statistics
Table 2 and
Table 3 present the variable definitions and summary statistics for the sample. The final sample includes 2158 elementary and junior high school students from Kochi Prefecture, with a mean age of
years (
), ranging from 8 to 15 years. Approximately
of participants were female. Regarding classroom diversity-related categories, the largest proportion of students were classified into the “Inclusion” category (
), followed by the “Intermediate” category (
), while the “Assimilation”, “Differentiation”, and “Exclusion” categories each accounted for between
and
of the sample. The mean household size was
persons. On average, students attended cram schools
days per week and participated in other extracurricular activities
days per week. In terms of digital device usage, the average frequency of tablet use for learning was
on a four-point scale, while PC use for learning averaged
. The mean score for expected future computer use for work was
on a four-point scale. In terms of family communication at home, children most often reported talking with their mother (
) and less frequently with their father (
). Families shared breakfast and dinner an average of
and
times per week, respectively. Psychological traits were measured using validated psychometric scales, with mean scores of
for generativity,
for inquisitiveness, and
for subjective well-being (SWB). Overall, the descriptive statistics indicate a well-balanced sample in terms of gender, school level, digital engagement, and psychological traits.
Table 4 presents the diversity classifications by gender. A higher proportion of girls (
) than boys (
) were classified into the “Inclusion” category, while proportionally more boys belonged to the “Intermediate” category.
4.2. Multinomial Logistic Regression Analysis
To empirically address research question (1), we employed the Multinomial Logit (MNL) model, specifying students’ perceived diversity status as the dependent variable. The dependent variable comprised five categories: inclusion, assimilation, differentiation, exclusion, and intermediate. This method estimates the probability of belonging to one of these categories. The independent variables included subjective well-being (SWB), generativity (r-GCS), compliance with digital device usage rules, relevant sociodemographic factors, and additional covariates outlined in Equation (1).
Table 5 presents the average marginal effects (AMEs, which show how a one-unit change in a predictor affects the probability of belonging to each category) from the model, examining predictors of classification into five diversity-related categories: inclusion, assimilation, differentiation, exclusion, and intermediate (reference group). Several significant patterns emerged, highlighting significant associations with age, digital engagement, psychological traits, and family communication. First, age was positively associated with the probability of being in the inclusion category (
) and negatively associated with assimilation and differentiation, indicating that older students are more likely to feel both unique and included in their classrooms. Although the effects of gender were comparatively modest, female students showed a marginally significant probability of being classified into the assimilation group (
) and were significantly less likely to be classified into the intermediate category (
).
Regarding digital learning tools, frequent PC use for learning was positively associated with inclusion () and negatively associated with both intermediate and einclusionxclusion, suggesting that structured digital engagement (i.e., purposeful, learning-oriented ICT use) is associated with a stronger sense of belonging and self-expression. In addition, adherence to digital device usage rules was positively associated with inclusion () and negatively with exclusion (), indicating that rule-based digital engagement (ICT use following family guidelines) is associated with differences in inclusive versus exclusive classroom experiences. In contrast, extended device usage time on high-use days (periods of unusually long screen use) was negatively associated with inclusion () and positively with intermediate, implying that unstructured screen time (excessive use without clear learning purposes) is associated with lower levels of classroom inclusion. Among psychological attributes, both generativity and inquisitiveness were positively associated with inclusion and negatively with intermediate (), reinforcing the idea that proactive and reflective dispositions (tendencies to act intentionally and think about one’s actions) are associated with inclusive experiences. Inquisitiveness was also negatively associated with exclusion (). In terms of family interactions, more frequent family dinners were modestly associated with inclusion (). Moreover, reporting one’s mother or father as the primary conversation partner at home was significantly and positively associated with inclusion ( and , respectively) and negatively with intermediate. In sum, these findings suggest that a combination of individual characteristics, learning-oriented ICT use with clear rules, and supportive family environments may contribute to fostering inclusive classroom experiences.
4.3. Median Regression Analysis of Subjective Well-Being (SWB)
To empirically address research question (2), we conducted a median regression analysis, a method less sensitive to outliers than ordinary least squares regression using the Subjective Happiness Scale (SHS) as the dependent variable to assess subjective well-being (SWB). Independent variables included diversity-related categories, generativity (r-GCS), inquisitiveness, adherence to digital device usage rules, relevant sociodemographic characteristics, and additional covariates specified in Equation (2). The results are summarized in
Table 6. Models were constructed sequentially to assess the incremental contributions of diversity-related categories, psychological attributes, interpersonal interactions, and demographic controls. Across all specifications, inclusion status was positively and significantly associated with SWB. Estimated effect sizes (regression coefficients) ranged from
to
, all significant at the
level, indicating a robust relationship between classroom inclusion and students’ well-being. By contrast, exclusion was consistently and negatively associated with SWB, with effect sizes ranging from
to
, statistically significant at the
or
level. Assimilation also showed a positive association, with effect sizes between
and
, significant at the
or
level. Differentiation, however, was not significantly associated with SWB. Notably, adherence to digital device usage rules was positively associated with SWB across all models. Effect sizes ranged from
to
, remaining significant at the
level in the fully adjusted specification.
Inquisitiveness and generativity were significant predictors of SWB. Specifically, inquisitiveness was positively associated with SWB, with effect sizes ranging from to , all statistically significant at the level (highly unlikely to have occurred by chance) across different model specifications. Generativity was similarly associated with higher levels of SWB, with effects ranging from to , statistically significant at the or level. Interpersonal interactions, particularly conversations with adults, were also significantly associated with SWB. Talking with adults about topics beyond schoolwork was positively associated with SWB, ranging from to , significant at the level. In contrast, peer interactions were not significantly associated with SWB. Although age initially showed a negative association with SWB, this effect became non-significant after adjustment for psychological traits and social factors. Among other variables, frequent participation in extracurricular activities was positively related to SWB (, ), whereas more frequent attendance at cram schools exhibited a weak negative association (, ). Additionally, sharing dinner more frequently with family members was positively associated with SWB (, ). Overall, these findings suggest that children’s SWB is associated not only with personal dispositions such as inquisitiveness and generativity but also with experiences of classroom inclusion, structured digital engagement, and supportive family communication.
4.4. Median Regression Analysis on Generativity
To empirically address research question (3), we conducted a median regression analysis with generativity as the dependent variable. The independent variables included diversity status, inquisitiveness, adherence to digital usage rules, and relevant sociodemographic controls, as specified in Equation (3). The results are summarized in
Table 7. Across all model specifications, Inclusion status was positively and significantly associated with generativity. The estimated effect sizes (i.e., regression coefficients) ranged from
to
, and all were statistically significant at the
level. These findings suggest that students who perceive themselves as both distinct and accepted within the classroom are more likely to exhibit stronger generative traits. Notably, exclusion also showed a consistently positive association with generativity, with estimated effects ranging from
to
, statistically significant at the
or
level. This result implies that even students who feel socially isolated may develop a sense of generative concern—possibly reflecting a compensatory or resilience-building response to exclusion. Inquisitiveness was strongly and positively associated with generativity across all models, with estimated effects ranging from
to
, all statistically significant at the
level. These findings emphasize inquisitiveness as a key cognitive disposition underlying students’ generative concern for others and future generations.
Similarly, adherence to digital usage rules was consistently associated with higher levels of generativity, with estimated effects ranging from to , all statistically significant at the level. This suggests that structured digital habits may support the development of more prosocial and future-oriented attitudes. Interpersonal variables also played a significant role. Talking with adults about topics beyond schoolwork was positively and significantly associated with generativity, with effects ranging from to , all significant at the level. Conversations with friends showed a similar positive association with generativity ( to , ). These findings suggest that open communication, both intergenerational and peer-based, may contribute to generative tendencies. Among the sociodemographic controls, gender emerged as a significant predictor: female participants exhibited higher generativity scores (, ). In addition, shared shopping experiences with family were positively associated with generativity (, ), suggesting that family-based interactions may foster empathy, care, and a sense of social responsibility.
4.5. Median Regression Analysis on Inquisitiveness
To empirically address research question (4), we conducted a median regression analysis with inquisitiveness as the dependent variable. The independent variables included diversity-related categories, intra- and intergenerational communication, adherence to digital device usage rules, and relevant sociodemographic characteristics, along with additional covariates specified in Equation (4).
Table 8 presents the results of this analysis. Across all models, students classified in the inclusion group were associated with significantly higher levels of inquisitiveness compared to those in the intermediate category. The estimated effect sizes (i.e., regression coefficients) ranged from
to
, all statistically significant at the
level. Similarly, differentiation was positively associated with inquisitiveness, suggesting that a strong sense of uniqueness—despite the absence of belonging—may be linked to heightened curiosity. The estimated effect sizes ranged from
to
, with all statistically significant at the
level. In contrast, neither exclusion nor assimilation was significantly associated with inquisitiveness.
Positive responses from adults were consistently and strongly associated with inquisitiveness across all models. The estimated effect sizes ranged from to , and all were statistically significant at the level. In contrast, negative responses showed only a modest positive association with inquisitiveness, and the effects were not statistically significant in Model 1. These findings suggest that although unfavorable interactions may sometimes coincide with increased curiosity, their overall contribution is relatively minor. Age was negatively associated with inquisitiveness, suggesting that younger students are more likely to report higher levels of curiosity. The estimated effect sizes ranged from to , with all statistically significant at the or level across all specifications. Digital engagement patterns were also significantly associated with inquisitiveness. Tablet use for learning ( to ), the presence of family rules regarding digital device usage ( to ), and adherence to those rules ( to ) were all positively associated with inquisitiveness, with significance levels ranging from or across Models 2 to 4. In addition, expected future use of computers for work was also a significant predictor of inquisitiveness ( to ). Interpersonal communication was likewise positively associated with inquisitiveness. Talking with adults about topics beyond schoolwork ( to ) and with peers ( to ) were both positively associated with inquisitiveness. These relationships were statistically significant across relevant model specifications. Among the sociodemographic variables, weekly attendance at cram schools (, ) and frequency of family shopping trips (, ) emerged as modest but statistically significant associations of inquisitiveness, though only in Model 4. In contrast, other factors such as gender and household size did not exhibit significant associations.
5. Discussion
5.1. Summary of Findings
We are now in a position to summarize the findings in relation to the four research questions posed at the end of the introduction. To address these questions, we conducted a cross-sectional questionnaire survey with 2158 Japanese elementary and junior high school students. Drawing on established frameworks, we examined how diversity experiences, inquisitiveness, generativity, and digital device usage relate to children’s well-being. We also considered interpersonal factors such as children’s communication with family members and the responsiveness of adults to their questions. These variables were treated as indicators of the quality of social interactions in home and school contexts.
The first research question asked “How are Japanese school children distributed across diversity-related categories—such as inclusion, assimilation, exclusion, differentiation, and intermediate—and what factors are associated with their placement in these categories?” Our results indicated that approximately two-thirds of the sample were classified as “Inclusion”, suggesting that many students perceive their classrooms as environments where both self-expression and a sense of belonging coexist. The remaining students were distributed across the “Assimilation”, “Differentiation”, “Exclusion”, and “Intermediate” categories. Age, gender, patterns and rules of digital device use; family communication; and psychological characteristics—namely inquisitiveness and generativity—were all significantly associated with category classification.
The second research question asked “How is children’s well-being associated with their diversity-related categories, inquisitiveness, generativity, and digital device usage?” Median regression analyses revealed that children in the inclusion group reported significantly higher levels of subjective well-being (SWB), even after adjusting for sociodemographic and contextual variables. Inquisitiveness and generativity were also positively and independently associated with SWB. The third research question asked “How is children’s generativity associated with their diversity-related categories, inquisitiveness, and digital device usage?” Our analysis found positive associations between generativity and inquisitiveness, as well as between generativity and both adherence to digital usage rules and open communication with adults and peers. These findings indicate potential links between generativity and both individual dispositions and structured social and digital environments.
The fourth research question asked:“How is children’s inquisitiveness associated with their diversity-related categories, the responsiveness of adults to their questions, and digital device usage?” Inquisitiveness was more likely to be higher among students in the inclusion and differentiation categories, and it showed positive associations with frequent interpersonal dialogue and the structured use of digital tools for learning. These findings suggest interconnected associations among inclusive classroom environments, individual psychological tendencies, and structured digital and interpersonal experiences, which may also be related to children’s SWB and generativity (creativity, sustaining, and care offering), especially in the context of increasingly diverse and digitally mediated learning environments.
5.2. Broader Reflections
As many sociologists and social psychologists have noted, rural communities have traditionally been characterized by low population mobility and long-standing interpersonal ties (
San Martin et al., 2019;
Thomson et al., 2018;
Yuki & Schug, 2012). In such environments, maintaining social harmony has often relied on a high degree of homogeneity within the community.
Yamagishi and Yamagishi (
1994) conceptualized this type of stable, cohesive environment as an “assurance society” in which individuals feel secure due to long-term, predictable relationships. In contrast, their studies described a “trust society” as one where individuals must evaluate whether unfamiliar others can be trusted, particularly in contexts marked by frequent social turnover (
Yamagishi, 2011;
Yamagishi & Yamagishi, 1994). Researchers have argued that contemporary Japanese society is often seen as transitioning from an assurance-based to a trust-based social structure (
San Martin et al., 2019;
Thomson et al., 2018;
Yamagishi, 2011;
Yamagishi & Yamagishi, 1994;
Yuki & Schug, 2012). In light of this framework, our findings suggest that inquisitiveness—defined as curiosity and the behavioral tendency to ask questions of adults and peers—may be an increasingly important trait, even within communities often regarded as relatively homogeneous, such as rural Japan. At the same time, fostering inclusive environments that embrace cognitive and identity-based diversity remains an ongoing cultural challenge, particularly in rural settings where traditional norms of conformity and harmony continue to prevail.
A possible mechanism behind the observed link between inclusion and inquisitiveness is the role of psychological safety. When children perceive their classrooms as places where their individuality is accepted and they belong, they may feel less concerned about negative evaluation and more willing to ask questions and explore new ideas. This mechanism may be particularly salient in Japan, where cultural norms emphasize reading the atmosphere, maintaining group harmony, and avoiding standing out. In such contexts, inclusive environments can provide important opportunities for children to express individuality without social costs, thereby fostering inquisitiveness (
Yamagishi, 2011;
Yamagishi & Yamagishi, 1994). At the same time, digitalization further complicates these dynamics: ICT can serve as a supportive tool that enables diverse forms of self-expression, particularly for children who perceive themselves as different from their peers. Structured and rule-based digital use appears to strengthen inclusion and inquisitiveness, whereas unregulated or excessive use may undermine social connections and well-being. Thus, the impact of digital technologies on learning environments is not uniform but depends heavily on the pedagogical and cultural conditions that shape how such tools are used.
In our study, children in the rural Kochi Prefecture were categorized into diversity-related categories based on their self-reported levels of self-expression (vertical axis) and sense of belonging (horizontal axis), as shown in
Figure 2. Approximately two-thirds of participants were classified into the inclusion category, indicating that a substantial proportion of children perceive themselves as both accepted and able to express their individuality in classroom settings. The presence of students in the differentiation category, however, suggests an alternative pattern, wherein self-expression occurs without a strong sense of group belonging. This pattern may partly reflect the use of information and communication technologies (ICT), which can provide individualized spaces for identity exploration beyond traditional peer-group dynamics. These observations imply that even in culturally cohesive and low-mobility rural communities, ICT may open new avenues for engagement with broader social networks and diverse perspectives. Future studies should further investigate how digital tools interact with processes of social integration and identity formation in such educational contexts.
As information and communication technologies (ICT) continue to evolve, their impact on children’s well-being depends not only on access but also on the guidance surrounding their use. Our findings suggest that learning-oriented use of personal computers and clearly defined family rules regarding device usage are more likely to show positive associations with children’s inclusion, inquisitiveness, generativity, and subjective well-being (SWB). In contrast, the lack of such structure appears to be associated with higher odds of exclusion. These results highlight the importance of cultivating digital literacy and promoting responsible usage behaviors in both home and educational settings. Given the importance of supportive family environments in guiding children’s digital engagement, one promising approach is Future Design (
Saijo,
2020,
2023;
Timilsina et al., 2020). This participatory framework promotes intergenerational dialogue, long-term thinking, and consensus-building within families. While it has been further developed in studies addressing sustainability challenges in low- and middle-income countries (
Mostafizur et al., 2025;
Pandit et al., 2021), it may also be well suited to fostering responsible digital habits at home and creating more inclusive learning environments in developed societies.
Recent international studies have examined how socio-demographic factors influence rural students’ access to and use of ICT (
Aruleba & Jere, 2022;
Jamil, 2021;
Kormos & Wisdom, 2021). For example,
Kardam et al. (
2024) investigated ICT gadget usage among rural secondary school students in Haryana, India, and found that while smartphones and televisions were widely used, their use varied considerably across schools and was positively correlated with education level but negatively with distance from urban centers. These findings highlight the strong influence of local socio-demographic conditions on ICT use. Similar challenges have also been noted in Japan. Research on the GIGA School Initiative indicates that, despite the nationwide distribution of digital devices, inequalities in actual use remain evident between urban and rural areas (
Nae, 2024). Together, these findings resonate with the focus of our study, which underscores the need for systematic and supportive digital practices in rural classrooms (
Afzal et al., 2023;
Oyanagi, 2024;
Twining et al., 2021).
Moreover, our findings highlight that fostering meaningful ICT engagement in rural classrooms requires more than securing physical access to devices; it also depends on the pedagogical and cultural conditions that shape children’s experiences (
Butler et al., 2018;
Dellagnelo, 2023). For instance, children classified under the “Differentiation” category—those who may not feel fully integrated into their classrooms but nevertheless use digital environments to express their individuality—did not report particularly low levels of well-being. This suggests that even in rural contexts, often regarded as socially homogeneous, ICT may open up new pathways for supporting diverse educational experiences (
Acilar & Sæbø, 2023;
Selwyn, 2023).
5.3. Limitations and Future Directions
This study has several limitations that warrant consideration in future research. First, as emphasized in the prior literature, the use of panel data—rather than cross-sectional data—would improve the robustness and generalizability of findings (
Cole & Maxwell, 2003;
Maxwell & Cole, 2007;
Maxwell et al., 2011). Future research should prioritize longitudinal or experimental designs to explore causal relationships among cognitive, non-cognitive, and sociodemographic factors more rigorously. Such approaches will be crucial for advancing our understanding of how subjective well-being (SWB) may shape, and be shaped by, preferences for different societal models.
Second, extending this research to diverse cultural and national contexts is essential for assessing the generalizability of the diversity-related categories—namely inclusion, assimilation, exclusion, differentiation, and intermediate. Comparative cross-cultural studies could yield valuable insights into whether these classifications and their psychological correlates are broadly applicable or shaped by sociocultural context. In addition, investigating how processes of globalization intersect with local diversity structures may contribute to a more nuanced understanding of the mechanisms underlying inclusion and identity development across settings.
Third, future research should employ more precise and quantitative methods to capture children’s digital device usage, including its purpose, frequency, duration, and the extent of adherence to family- or school-based guidelines. As smartphones and tablets become increasingly integrated into children’s daily routines, incorporating objective data sources—such as device usage logs or app-based tracking—may enhance the accuracy and granularity of analyses. Despite these limitations, the present study represents an important initial step in exploring how children’s preferences for inclusive environments are associated with digital engagement, inquisitiveness, and generativity. Future investigations should build on these findings to inform educational practices and policies aimed at enhancing subjective well-being (SWB) in increasingly digital and diverse learning contexts.