Next Article in Journal
Regional Socioeconomic Deprivation in Germany and Adherence to the 24-h Movement Guidelines among Children and Adolescents
Previous Article in Journal
The Influence of Emotional Regulation and Cognitive Flexibility on Sleep Habits in Spanish Children and Adolescents through the Lens of Parents
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Systematic Review

The Relationship between Narrative Skills and Executive Functions across Childhood: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis

Department of Psychology, University of Milan-Bicocca, 20126 Milan, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Children 2023, 10(8), 1391; https://doi.org/10.3390/children10081391
Submission received: 14 July 2023 / Revised: 5 August 2023 / Accepted: 8 August 2023 / Published: 15 August 2023

Abstract

:
Executive functions (EFs) and narrative competence (NC) are two important predictors of many outcomes in human development. To date, however, it is unclear whether these skills develop synergistically—supporting or opposing each other—or whether they are independent of each other. The purpose of this meta-analysis is to understand if these skills are related to over development and if the magnitude of their association changes over time; differs in typical and atypical development; and changes with EF (inhibition, working memory, flexibility, planning) and NC (oral, written; micro and macrostructural level). For this purpose, 30 studies containing 285 effect sizes were selected and combined. The results show that EFs and NC are weakly associated with each other (r = 0.236, p < 0.001) and that this association decreases with age (b(267) = −0.0144, p = 0.001). They are more associated in preschool and early elementary school grades, becoming more independent after seven years old. Between 3 and 7 years of age, the association seems stronger in atypically developing children and for macrostructural NC. Additionally, before 7 years old, the various EF domains seem to associate indistinctly with NC, and only later specific links between EFs and NC would be observed.

1. Introduction

Narratives represent an essential device for human communication and are a vehicle for cultural transmission.
The onset of the use of narratives represents a critical step in studies of language development, where storytelling represents a real and contextualized request for children. Therefore, it is seen by many authors as a “naturalistic” approach to studying language development [1]. Evaluation of children’s oral narratives is of significant interest to researchers and practitioners, as being a proficient narrator is an important skill in the life of children, and in adults. Oral narrative skills are a key component of most school curricula, and several studies support the importance of narrative abilities to academic and social success for both typically developing children and children with language and learning disabilities [2,3]. Extant research reports that good narrative skills are positively associated with structural language, literacy, and social skills [4,5,6].
Telling stories is a multi-componential complex competence. It requires the child to be able to plan and execute their production of the story’s plotline by using appropriate vocabulary, grammar, and syntax. Studies on the development of narrative skills have identified that stories have a typical structure, or story grammar [7], following a “schema” that children and adults use to understand, interpret, and produce stories. According to Stein and Glenn’s [7] story-grammar model, stories must include a setting and an episode system at a minimum. An episode consists of an introduction, a provision of the setting and description of the characters in the story, a problematic situation that shapes the protagonist’s goal, attempts to solve the problem, and a conclusion (e.g., [7,8,9]). Stories may also include multiple episodes organized in a linear or a hierarchical manner, resulting in more complexity (e.g., stories with multiple embedded episodes within a particular story arc). Developmental studies reveal that the acquisition of narrative proficiency is a slow process, which emerges in the preschool years and is not fully developed until adulthood [10]. In early childhood, there is a disproportionate emphasis on characters’ actions in narratives without a link to the plot line [11]. At 2 years, narratives are descriptions of character actions, and labels posited without a link to a central theme. Between 3 and 4 years, narratives generally include some local connections between adjacent story events and simple inferences across the story episode. At 4 years, children begin to use structural components of narratives, which generally include causal connections between events. However, until 5, children still show difficulty conceiving an overall plot or overarching goal.
It is not until 6–7 years old that children are able to produce “true narratives”. At this age, their narratives follow a logical progression of events, including sub-plots and understanding of time frames. After 7 years old, narratives are generally well-structured. Progress in literacy acquisition seems to play a significant role in this passage. The narrative generation process is thought to draw critically on reading skills. For example, Abbott and Berninger [12] found that reading contributes significantly to the quality of narrative composition for children in the first three grades.
Empirical findings suggest that reading and writing draw on shared knowledge yet are separate skills with distinct developmental trajectories [13,14]. In a study with 120 third-grade children, Olinghouse [15] found that reading skills directly influenced compositional quality. There are aspects of continuity and discontinuity in the transition from oral to written narrative composition during this period. Studies on typically developing children provide evidence that children who master writing preserve their narrative skills in the transition between the codes [16]. However, for those children who do not master it efficiently (e.g., children with learning disabilities and other neurodevelopmental disorders), written narrative composition becomes an obstacle.
At 8–10 years, children generally manage structural components correctly and demonstrate that they know how to tell a story to another person. After 10 years, narratives are more complex, detailed, and structurally coherent. Children use various linking devices (e.g., prepositions, conjunctions, and adverbs) and demonstrate more effort to engage the listener’s attention and adapt to different audiences.
Across development, oral and written narratives can be studied at the macro- and microstructure levels. Microstructure refers to specific features of the language used to convey ideas, including the use of decontextualized language and grammatical complexity (e.g., [17,18]). In contrast, macrostructure refers to global narrative features, particularly the ability to produce a story that is overall well structured, coherent, and cohesive. During development, a remarkable increase involves the macrostructural level (e.g., [19]), particularly in the transition from preschool- to school-age (e.g., [20,21]). This period is characterized by the rapid qualitative increase in executive functions (EFs).
EF refers to a broad set of neurocognitive processes underlying goal-directed control of thought, behaviour, and emotion that allow for adaptation to environmental demands [22]. Like narrative skills, EFs are predictors of great relevance to many developmental outcomes. A large body of research has demonstrated substantial links between EFs and academic achievement, literacy, health, wealth, and criminality [23] in children of various ages with and without neurodevelopmental disorders (see [24,25] for reviews).
There is no unanimous agreement on which domains include the construct of EFs.
Scholars studying EFs deal with the problem that EFs are initially unitary or undistinguishable (e.g., [26]), but they differentiate across development. To date, when and how they differentiate is still unclear. In the adult population, three specific core domains were identified: inhibition, updating of working memory, and shifting [27]. This finding was replicated in research with 8- to 13-years-old children [28]. However, research with younger children usually yields a smaller number of factors. Especially for preschool age, the debate on the structure of EFs is still open. This period is the most critical for the rapid changes occurring in child neurodevelopment. So far, some studies have found a single factor for all EFs [26], and other studies have proposed a two-factor model instead [29,30,31].
Furthermore, studies on children differ from studies on adults in broader processes of defining EFs. For instance, Diamond [32] includes working memory and cognitive flexibility instead of updating and set-shifting, which are more specific processes. Indeed, working memory here refers to a domain-general system that can store and process information simultaneously. It shows a linear increase from ages 4 to 14 and a levelling off between ages 14 and 15 [33]. In contrast, updating is the specific ability to change temporarily stored information in the light of incoming information and is mainly investigated in studies with adults and older school-aged children. Developmental studies have shown that updating increases with age along with upgrading of inhibition efficiency, and stabilizes by the age of 15 years [34]. Cognitive flexibility refers to a tendency to perform in ways that are not fixed or routine, to “think outside the box”, or to adapt to changes in the environment; instead, shifting refers to the ability to switch between conflicting operations or different task sets. Shifting is a more specific dimension than “cognitive flexibility”. However, some authors have pointed out that there is no evidence that cognitive flexibility can be considered a general, coherent construct usable in individual difference research with children [35]. Very often, the term “cognitive flexibility” in developmental studies is actually used with the meaning of “shifting” (e.g., [29]). The development of successful shifting seems to depend on inhibition and working memory. As Garon et al. [36] noted, before children can successfully shift between response sets, they must be able to maintain a response set in working memory and then be able to inhibit the activation of a response set to activate an alternative one. Developmental studies have revealed that shifting improves from age 4 to adolescence, reaching adult-like levels around 15 [37].
Other authors have included different types of inhibition in their definitions of EFs, distinguishing inhibition on a behavioural level (response inhibition or behavioural inhibition) and a cognitive/attention level (interference suppression or interference control), both sharing the need to suppress an action or a thought in order to control impulses and stay focused [32,38]. Studies on their development reveal that, at 4 years, these two inhibition processes are already distinguishable [39]. Improved behavioural inhibition tends to stabilize by the early school years (i.e., from 5 to 8 years; [28]), whereas a sensitive increase in interference control occurs during elementary school and is followed by slower improvement during early adolescence [33].
Furthermore, with increasing age, complex high-order EFs such as planning and problem solving become relevant to be included in the construct of EFs [32]. They develop particularly late in childhood and undergo a final growth spurt during the beginning of adolescence [40,41]. Research on these processes has examined chiefly the development of performance at Tower-like tasks across different age groups and found age effects only for the more complex problems [42].

1.1. NC and EFs: Are They Linked?

There are different reasons to expect that EFs and NC are related across development.
In general, the literature frequently reports significant relationships between EFs and different aspects of language skills. Especially during the preschool years, language skills undergo rapid development: vocabulary overgrows, the use of syntactic rules becomes more adult-like, and the ability to use language in narratives improves (e.g., [43,44,45]). At the same time, the preschool years are characterized by a substantial improvement in EFs that are commonly impaired in children with language disorders (e.g., [46]).
The fact that developments in NC emerge in concert with developments in EFs suggests a potential developmental relationship between these abilities. Evidence from imaging studies indicates that these skills depend upon overlapping neural substrates, mainly frontal lobe function, and deficits across these skill sets are observed in adults with traumatic brain injuries [47,48]. However, it is possible to find specific brain regions associated with narrative competence such as temporal poles, the posterior cingulate, and the left superior temporal gyrus [49]. On the other hand, cognitive executive functions are more associated with the bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [32].
Telling a good story requires the individual to set the goal of linking all of the story elements in a coherent manner, retrieving the appropriate semantic information, syntactic structures, and morphological features that would express the causal links between various story elements, and also indicate the characters’ motivations and reactions, and monitor the narrative while it is being produced. In order to tell a coherent story, children need to set up a hierarchical goal and plan and monitor the organization of the narrative events, and this seems to engage EFs [50]:
  • shifting may be involved in the generation of complete episodes within a narrative discourse, in the selection of informative words, and in the ability to monitor the communicative flow;
  • updating of working memory may be required to generate and understand sentences as well as recall episodic contents for an accurate organization of a story;
  • inhibition processes may be critical for monitoring the production of extraneous comments and derailments while telling a story and for the ability to inhibit the semantic competitors while producing words;
  • planning and more complex EFs may be recruited to the extent of coordinating all the processes involved, as well as for the planning and goal setting of the story (e.g., retelling a narrative containing all of the story elements in the correct sequence [51]).
In the same way, NC development may support the performance on EF tasks. This seems especially plausible on tasks with long and complex instructions and linguistic stimuli to be processed or producing oral responses [52].
However, both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies are inconsistent regarding the association, and potential causal relation, between EFs and NC. For instance [51], in a study on children between 3 and 6 years old, results showed that narrative production is best predicted by high-level EFs, measured with planning and shifting tasks. In contrast, other studies investigating the relationship between these domains in 4–5- and 7–8-years-old Turkish children found that narrative production, especially plot complexity, is related to these EFs only in the older group, not in the younger age band [53]. Moreover, other studies report no association between planning skills and the quality of written narratives in fourth-grade children [54].
A significant relationship between microstructural competence, such as lexical variety and syntax used in narratives, and shifting ability, addressed by the performance at card sorting task, is found in a sample of 47 four- to six-year-old Swedish children. In the same way, EFs accounts for 7% of the variation in syntactic complexity in Turkish-speaking preschoolers [53]. Longitudinal research on school-aged Dutch children reveals that the development of syntactic complexity in narratives between fourth and sixth grade is also predicted by planning and behavioural inhibition in the fourth grade [54]. The relationship between syntactic complexity and inhibitory skills is not found at preschool age in typically developing Swedish children [52].
Research on the role of working memory in narratives appears more consistent. A study on children aged 5 to 8 shows that the ability to update working memory is moderately associated with referential adequacy, the macrostructural competence to introduce and maintain a reference to story characters in narratives [55]. Studies on children aged 8 to 11 reveal that working memory and shifting significantly account for plot complexity variance, another macrostructural NC indicator, in written narratives [56]. Even when controlling for vocabulary, working memory correlated with text generation at the word, sentence, and text level in a sample of 10 years old children [57] and adolescents [58]. According to the authors, it may be involved in translating ideas in the memory into linguistic representation, organizing thoughts into temporally sequenced discourse, and revising text.
In general, studies on narrative writing show that children with higher updating and inhibitory skills produce longer, coherent narratives. The authors [58] explain the involvement of these processes with the need to suppress inappropriate lexical representations, select the relevant ones, and actively hold and update the representations in WM during writing composition. However, some studies on 5- and 6-year-old children with SLI found a significant correlation between narrative retelling skills and working memory, but not with inhibitory processes [59,60].
Furthermore, some studies fail to find a direct relationship between NC and inhibitory and WM updating skills, showing that the influence of these EF domains on NC may totally depend on handwriting skills [61]. Indeed, studies reported that children with poor handwriting skills tend to use the first linguistic expression that occurs to them to frame their ideas without being concerned about shaping the linguistic expression in response to narrative demands or the reader’s needs [62,63,64]. They must devote most or all of their cognitive effort to spelling and handwriting, leaving little resources available for other writing processes. This may limit the amount and quality of text they can generate.
In sum, there is conflicting evidence about the developmental stages at which EFs relates to NC. Inconsistent results suggest that the development of these skills can be heterochronous with ones that are deeply conceptually related and developing on different timescales. Even though they develop across the preschool period, it seems they do not do in lockstep. Some aspects of EFs may develop before others, and the relationship between these aspects and NC may be such that there is specificity in predictive relations over developmental time for microstructural and macrostructural elements [65]. Research with atypically developing populations presenting deficit in both EFs and NC show similar inconsistent results. For instance, in children with a diagnosis of ADHD and language impairment, Fernandez et al. [66] found a significant correlation between macrostructural elements produced in the narration (e.g., episodic structure) and planning skills, but not with phonological working memory. Some studies conducted in children with SLI, instead, found a significant association between plot structure and phonological working memory [59,67].
To date, our understanding of how and when different aspects of NC relate to EFs—or which part of EF they relate to—is limited. Integration of divergent findings has become a necessary and important task. The present study takes up this task using a meta-analytic approach in order to examine and explain the variability across findings. Larger sample approaches may indeed improve our knowledge on the relationship between EFs and NC over developmental time and orient future research on this topic. Currently, to our knowledge, there are no systematic reviews or meta-analyses addressing this issue.
The understanding of how different aspects of NC relate to EFs—or which part of EF they relate to—is also clinically relevant since both the skills predict important life outcomes (i.e., academic and social success) and are trainable [68,69,70,71]. Studies show that children—especially those at risk (e.g., children from backgrounds of poverty, children whose first language is not the one spoken in the country where they live, or children with psychopathological traits)—often exhibit less-well-developed language and executive skills, facing greater risks to academic success than do their typically developing or more privileged classmates [68]. The disadvantages attributed to a lagging NC and EF development increase as children progress through school [71]. Early interventions that support the development of narrative skills in young children have been shown to be effective at promoting NC and academic success at the preschool level (e.g., [72]). Furthermore, these interventions appear to have positive and substantial long-term effects. Evidence on EF training at preschool age also showed that cognitive training to improve these skills early could be effective [69,70].

1.2. Aims of the Study

The goals of the present meta-analysis are the following:
  • Determine the overall strength of the relationship between narrative competence (NC) and executive functions (EFs) across childhood and adolescence (3–18 years)
  • Determine if the strength of this relationship changes across childhood and when it changes across development.
  • Examine potential moderators to understand if the strength of the relation changes:
    • between typically vs. atypically developing children (e.g., attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), autism spectrum disorder (ASD), specific language impairment (SLI)).
    • by different EF domains (working memory capacity and updating, behavioural inhibition, interference control, shifting, planning, and problem-solving);
    • by different narrative types (oral vs. written) and levels (micro vs. macrostructural levels).

2. Methods

2.1. Operational Definitions

We categorized NC based on the characteristics of narratives: written or oral. Both types of narratives included the ability to retell or tell a story in written or oral form. Moreover, we classified measures related to NC by dividing them into micro-structural and macro-structural competence. Micro-structural components were collapsed into one dimension, including lexical (e.g., number and variety of words produced) and syntactic skills (e.g., indices of number and type of utterance and subordinate sentences produced; the mean length of utterance) in narration. Macro-structural components were collapsed into one dimension, including the richness of content of the narrative (e.g., the amount of information reported in the narrative), the presence of the key passages in the story (e.g., the ability to structure a coherent story), and the cohesion of the story (e.g., anaphoric use of the article and correct referencing across the narration).
Executive domains were differentiated according to which primary executive process the tasks assessed, based on the EFs assessment literature [32,38,42,73]. For instance, tasks requiring keeping in mind and actively manipulating auditory or visual information (e.g., backward digit; word or spatial span tasks) were coded as working memory capacity measures. These were distinguished from tasks that mainly required updating of working memory (e.g., n-back), defined as “the ability to monitor and code incoming information, and to update the content of memory by replacing old items with newer, more relevant, information” ([74] p. 428). Forward span-like tests were considered to measure short-term memory since they did not require working memory processes [75]; therefore, we did not include them in the meta-analysis.
We considered those tests that required children to suppress a dominant but inappropriate response or to prevent impulsive motor response (e.g., knock and tap task; go/no-go; Head Toes Knees Shoulders) as a measure of “behavioural inhibition” [38]. Instead, tasks requiring the ability to prevent interference due to resource or stimulus competition and filter out irrelevant information within the stimuli that contain both relevant and distracting information (e.g., Stroop-like, local to global and Flanker paradigm) were categorized as “interference control” task [38].
We categorized tests requiring shifting among different response sets and flexibly adjusting the response according to new rules (e.g., verbal fluency, five-point test, Trail Making Test and Wisconsin Change Card Sort) as measures of “shifting”.
We classified tests that required the ordering of events mentally in advance and planning of actions [76], such as Tower-like tasks or non-narrative sequences, as measures of planning abilities.
If a study collapsed different tasks in a single general dimension, we included it as a general measure of EF for the purpose of the main analysis (e.g., [31,46]. However, in such cases, we could not be able to discern between the various EF domains implied. For this reason, we could not consider such outcomes for the analysis of moderation by EF domains.

2.2. Search Strategy

In accordance with the PRISMA statement [77] we used a systematic search strategy to find the pertinent studies. Using different combinations of the terms “executive functions”, “narrative”, and their synonyms (see Appendix A for the detailed search strings), we searched on PubMed, PsycINFO, Linguistics and Language Behaviors, Proquest Dissertations and Theses Global, e-thesis online service (Ethos), DART-Europe E-theses Portal to identify all potential journal articles, unpublished studies, and doctoral dissertations that reported data on the relationship between EFs and NC in children and adolescents. This is the first meta-analysis on narrative competence and executive function in children and adolescents. Despite our extensive search of the grey literature, we found only a small amount of unpublished studies (overall, 5 studies and 46 different effect sizes). Preliminary analyses ruled out the presence of publication bias: the size of the relationship was similar in the published and unpublished studies. Therefore, we also included these studies in the main analysis.
After excluding duplicates, 885 records remained. The first author screened all of them based on title and abstract and according to inclusion and exclusion criteria. As a secondary search, the references of the selected studies (n = 15), in addition to relevant systematic reviews, were checked to find other eligible studies. The full text of the identified papers was reviewed by the first author and EB. Disagreements were solved through discussion. The agreement rate between the two raters was high (81%). Finally, as shown in the flow chart, we identified 25 articles (30 studies) with 287 effects that were eligible for the present meta-analytic review. Details concerning the literature search method and criteria for inclusion and exclusion of studies are shown in Figure 1.

2.3. Inclusion Criteria

The included studies had to meet the following criteria:
  • at least one performance-based test related to EFs and one related to the micro- or macrostructural level of NC;
  • correlational study with a cross-sectional or longitudinal design;
  • monolingual participants aged between 3 and 18 years old;
  • paper is written in English, Italian, or Spanish.

2.4. Exclusion Criteria

We excluded all the studies where participants were bilingual and older than 18.
All outcomes were based on correlations between one or more EF and NC tasks. Where available, we included correlation with accuracy and reaction times on EF tasks. We did not accept measures of EF aspects collected through teacher and parent reports (e.g., BRIEF) because these measures seem to capture different aspects from tasks [78]. At the same time, we did not accept measures of narrative comprehension measured through questions. The only kind of NC tasks included required the child to produce a personal story or to retell a story they heard, in oral or written form. We included the studies only if they reported at least one score of a neurocognitive EF measure and at least one micro- or macrostructural competence score for an NC task.

2.5. Coding

During the coding phase, the first author coded each record according to a predefined coding schema, collecting information about bibliographic information (i.e., title, author(s), and year of publication), sample characteristics (i.e., sample size, mean age and standard deviation of each group, clinical risk status of the sample), characteristics of the narrative tasks (i.e., written versus oral form; microstructural versus macrostructural level) and the kind of EF measure (i.e., working memory capacity, updating of working memory, behavioural inhibition, interference control, shifting, and planning) and the correlation indices between the NC and EF tasks.
All the correlation indices between the tasks were included if there were two or more eligible NC and EF measures. We applied the same procedure when multiple groups were suitable for the aims of the meta-analysis, like typically and atypically developing children in the same study (i.e., [60,79,80]) or preschoolers and school-aged children (i.e., [53,55]).

2.6. Meta-Analytic Procedures

We used R version 4.1.2 [81], RStudio version 1.4.1103 [82], and the Metafor package [83,84] to conduct the analyses. R code and data are openly available in Supplementary Materials.
Pearson product-moment correlation was used as the effect size to examine the relationship between NC and EFs. The magnitude of the correlation was interpreted using Cohen’s [85] conventions:
  • r ≈ 0.10 [z ≈ 0.10]: small effect;
  • r ≈ 0.30 [z ≈ 0.31]: moderate effect;
  • r ≈ 0.50 [z ≈ 0.54]: large effect.
Since correlations are restricted in their range (i.e., they can take values between −1 and 1), it can introduce bias when we estimate the standard error for studies with small sample size. Thus, the correlation coefficients collected from the selected studies were transformed into Fisher’s z. This transformation entails using the natural logarithm function to remove the range restriction and ensure that the sampling distribution is approximately normal. Fisher’s z and the standard error of Fisher’s z were calculated directly in R using the cor and log functions.
A positive z value reflected a positive association between NC and EFs, while a negative effect indicated that when the EF competence increased, NC decreased. We computed Z Fisher transformation using Olkin and Finn’s [86] formula. The summary statistics required for each outcome were the number of participants and the correlation coefficients between NC and EF measures. For one study based on regression analysis (i.e., [51]), the correlation coefficient was converted from the β coefficient, according to Peterson and Brown’s [87] procedure.
As discussed, many studies in the dataset reported several correlated relevant outcomes, and some studies comprised multiple groups of individuals (e.g., with typical and atypical development). This caused dependencies in the data. So far, several solutions have been introduced to avoid dependency [84,88]: analysing the outcomes as if they were independent (i.e., ignoring the dependency), averaging the dependent outcomes into a single effect size, selecting only one outcome for each study, and multilevel meta-analysis. Ignoring the dependency might bias the results; averaging or eliminating effect sizes, on the other hand, would decrease the power of the analysis and limit the research questions that we could ask, as we would not be able to compare moderation effects by EF and NC domains. We therefore conducted a three-level meta-analytic analysis, following Assink and Wibbelink [84]. The meta-analytic model considered three different sources of variance: the participants at level 1, the outcomes at level 2, and the studies at level 3.
We used the rma.mv function of the Metafor package and set the tdist parameter as TRUE. Therefore, we based the test statistics and confidence intervals on the t distribution, applied the Knapp and Hartung [89] adjustment, and used the Restricted Maximum Likelihood estimation method (REML) for estimating the parameters. Tau2, the Q-test for heterogeneity [90] and the I2 statistic were reported.
Studentized residuals and Cook’s distances were used to examine whether studies may be outliers or influential in the model context. Studies with a Studentized residual larger than the 100 × [1 − 0.05/(2 × k)] th percentile of a standard normal distribution were considered potential outliers (i.e., using a Bonferroni correction with two-sided alpha = 0.05 for k studies included in the meta-analysis). Studies with a Cook’s distance larger than the median plus six times the interquartile range of the Cook’s distances were considered influential.

3. Results

3.1. Selected Studies

Thirty studies were eligible for inclusion, for a total of 287 different outcomes, with 3250 participants with typical development (Mage = 8.18) and 346 participants (Mage = 8.02) with atypical development (i.e., diagnosis of learning disorder, autism spectrum disorder, language impairment, deafness).

3.2. Inspection for Publication Bias

We explored the funnel plot to investigate potential publication bias and checked for differences in effect sizes between published and unpublished studies. The Egger’s regression test, using the standard error of the observed outcomes as a moderator, was used to check for funnel plot asymmetry. The funnel plot is presented in Supplementary Materials Figure S1.
No evidence of publication bias emerged, (Egger’s t = 1.116, p = 0.266). A visual inspection showed that only a few studies fall outside the pseudo-confidence interval’s triangular region. Next, we compared the effect sizes of published and unpublished studies, as higher effects for published studies might be an important indication of publication bias. We could locate only five unpublished studies, with a total of 46 different outcomes. No evidence of publication bias emerged, F(1, 285) = 0.96, p = 0.325. On the contrary, the size of the effect was slightly bigger for the five unpublished studies than for the published studies: for the unpublished studies the effect was z = 0.283, SE = 0.041, 95% CI = (0.199, 0.367) and for the published studies the effect was z = 0.233, SE = 0.020, 95% CI = (0.193, 0.273). Since this difference was negligible, we decided to include the five unpublished studies in the main analysis.
Subsequent analysis indicated that the size of the effect was related neither to the year of publication of the study, F(1, 285) = 0.187, p = 0.665, nor to languages spoken by the sample of participants involved in the studies, F(7, 296) = 0.193, p = 0.986. Moreover, a sample size moderator analysis was performed, which resulted in a non-significant effect (p = 0.109), suggesting that differences in sample size are not a source of the heterogeneity of the results.
An examination of the Studentized residuals revealed that one study [91] had a value larger than ±3.7537 and may be a potential outlier in the context of this model. According to Cook’s distances, four studies [79,92,93,94] could be overly influential.

3.3. Research Question 1: Exploring the Overall Association between EFs and NC

A total of k = 287 effects were included in the analysis. The observed Fisher r-to-z-transformed correlation coefficients ranged from −0.0601 to 1.2111), with the total estimates being positive. The estimated average Fisher r-to-z-transformed correlation coefficient based on the random-effects model was z = 0.241, r = 0.236, (95% CI: 0.2053 to 0.2776). Therefore, the average outcome differed significantly from zero (t = 13.134, p < 0.0001), indicating a positive, small association between EFs and NC over development. According to the Q-test, the true outcomes appear heterogeneous (Q(286) = 597.25, p < 0.0001. The estimated variance components were tau2(level 3) = 0.005 and tau2(level 2) = 0.006. This means that I2(level 3) = 22.95% of the total variation can be attributed to between-study and I2(level 2) = 29.95% to within-study heterogeneity. We found that the three-level model provided a significantly better fit compared to a two-level model, with level 3 constrained to zero (χ2 = 33.39, p < 0.001).
The 75% rule (Hunter and Schmidt, 1990 [95]) suggests that we should inspect heterogeneity if <75% of the total amount of variance can be attributed to within-study sampling variance. Therefore, we proceeded to investigate potential moderators, following the research questions outlined above.

3.4. Research Question 2: Exploring If and When the Association between EFs and NC Changes over Development

We investigated the impact of age on the relationship between EFs and NC through meta-regression to understand if and when the relationship between NC and EFs changes over time (see Table 1). The mean age of the sample ranged between 4 and 15 years and significantly influenced the effect size so that as age increases, the overall effect size decreases, F(1, 265) = 6.744, p = 0.009.
The unstandardized regression coefficient and significance for the slope are reported in Table 1, which indicates the impact of each unitary change (i.e., one year) in the moderator on the effect size of the relationship between EFs and NC.
Looking at the trend in effect size over development (see Figure 2), the relationship’s turning point appears to be around 7–8 years old. Thus, we performed moderation analysis by dividing the sample into two-time windows (i.e., mean age < 7 years; mean age > 8 years). Results show that this variable significantly impact on the effect size, so that after 7 years old the magnitude of the relationship between EFs and NC decreases from z = 0.274 to z = 0.212, F(1, 265) = 3.908, p = 0.049. According to these results, we decided to conduct separate meta-analyses to investigate the influence of potential moderators in these two developmental windows (4–7 years; 8–15 years, see Table 2).
Table 3 and Table 4 summarized the characteristics of the studies included in the first and second meta-analysis, respectively. In particular, in Table 3 we reported the correlations between EFs and NC of participants aged 4–7 years old; in Table 4, we reported the correlations between EFs and NC of participants aged 8–15.

3.5. Research Question 3: Potential Moderators of the Relationship between EFs and NC before and after 7 Years Old

As previously mentioned, Table 2 shows a summary of the impact of the following moderators on the relationship between EFs and NC in the two developmental windows considered.
  • Typically vs. atypically developing population. We categorized the sample in typically developing and atypically developing participants based on the presence of a diagnosis (i.e., deafness, SLI, learning disorders, ADHD, and ASD). The studies involving children younger than 7 years old (n = 795) indicated that the effect sizes differed between the groups, F(1, 83) = 4.400, p = 0.039. The association between EFs and NC was almost twice in atypically developing children (z = 0.436) than in typical peers (z = 0.249), unless both effects are significant.
    Conversely, in the subsample of studies involving children older than 8 years old (n = 2615), the analysis indicated that the effect size was the same for typically (z = 0.211) and atypically (z = 0.196) developing populations, F(1, 180) = 0.132, p = 0.715.
    The number of studies involving atypically developing populations of children, however, was relatively small in both subsamples: we found only four studies with a total of eight different effects and 143 atypically developing children younger than 7 years old; and only five studies with a total of seventy-six different effects and 203 atypically developing children older than 8 years old.
  • EF domains. Looking at EFs, we investigated if, before and after 7 years old, effect size differs on the type of EF domains taken into consideration (i.e., interference control, behavioural inhibition, working memory capacity, updating of working memory, shifting, planning). Results showed that before 7 years, the effect size did not statistically differ based on the type of EF domains, F(5, 77) = 2.069, p = 0.109. At this stage, EF domains are equally significantly associated with NC. However, in the subsample of studies involving participants older than 8 years old, variance in the effect size was significantly explained by EF domains, F(5, 162) = 3.399, p = 0.006. In line with the age effect previously discovered, the relationship between NC and the majority of the EF processes decreased, with the exception of behavioural inhibition. The effect size of the association between behavioural inhibition and more general NC was larger than those observed in younger children.
Additionally, the association of shifting and planning with NC remain significant in older children, even if it is lower. As regards working memory dimension, the measures addressing its capacity remains similarly associated with NC, whereas those addressing updating processes decreased significantly in older children.
  • Narrative Competence. Looking at the characteristics of NC, we next compared studies on children before and after 7 years old, analysing if micro versus macrostructural levels of narratives moderated the effect size of the relationship between EFs and NC. Results referring to studies on participants younger than 7 years old indicated that the effect size was higher for macrostructural (z = 0.329) than microstructural (z = 0.208) competences, F(1, 75) = 12.23, p < 0.001, unless both the effects were significant (p < 0.001). After 7 years old, however, no significant difference emerged for the comparison between micro and macrostructural aspects, F(1, 180) = 0.074, p = 0.784.
Next, we questioned if, in the subsample of studies with children older than 8 years old, the relationship between EFs and NC differed based on the type of narrative tasks (i.e., written versus oral form). Results indicated that the type of narrative task did not explain variance in the effect size, F(1, 180) = 1.36, p = 0.243.

4. Discussion

EFs and NC are two widely investigated dimensions of human cognitive development, but our understanding of their relationship is limited. For instance, we do not know if these dimensions are related over time or if this relationship changes across development. We do not know much about this relationship, especially in atypically developing children and adolescents, although we know that these areas are usually impaired in such populations. In general, few studies have investigated this relationship. Mostly, these studies involved small samples, used a cross-sectional design, and produced mixed results. The aim of this meta-analysis is not to answer these questions according to the studies published so far. It intends to raise some points that can guide future research on these topics, such as which age range needs further consideration by scientists. We claim, as of right now, that more studies in general—and specifically more longitudinal studies—are needed to shed light on the relationship between these dimensions over time in typical and atypically developing individuals.
The first purpose of the present meta-analysis was to establish if, overall, EFs and NC are transversally—not longitudinally—associated.
As expected, the collected studies showed great heterogeneity within and between themselves. However, the multilevel meta-analysis showed that—overall—a positive but small relationship between EFs and NC exists (r = 0.236). It means that the studies selected provide evidence that—in general—individuals who performed well at EF tasks are also good narrators and vice versa. The result obtained reflects the high variability between the studies included. Nine studies reported an average effect size below 0.20, but most reported moderate (0.30–0.49) effect sizes. Inspection for publication bias revealed that the results obtained are similar in the published and unpublished literature, so the probability of overestimating the magnitude of this relationship is remote.
The second purpose was to examine if the relationship between EFs and NC changes over time and at which point it starts to change significantly. In order to fulfil this aim, we considered the mean age of participants in the studies. Results showed that the relationship between EFs and NC changes over time and decreases over development. The plot of the association between NC and EFs across development (Figure 2) showed that the transversal association increases during the preschool years, when both NC and EFs dramatically develop, peaking in the early elementary school years and then starting to decrease significantly after 7 years old.
Different factors might explain the turning point we can observe at this age.
We speculate that a key role might be played by literacy acquisition to which the early years of elementary schools are dedicated. During these years, children develop effective decoding skills [98]. Specifically, children speaking languages with shallow syllabic complexity and orthographic depth (e.g., Italian, Spanish, German, Greek) become accurate and fluent in foundation reading before the end of the first school year. In contrast, children speaking languages characterized by deep orthographies (English, French, Danish, and Portuguese)—the majority of children involved in the studies selected for this work belong to this group—become fluent at nearly 8 years old [98].
Research on the development of reading and writing suggests that the development of these skills is deeply interrelated and that, especially during elementary school years, reading contributes significantly to the quality of narrative composition [12,15], especially from a macrostructural point of view (i.e., better structured and cohesive narrations). It is possible that, after literacy acquisition, the role of EFs in narrative production is downgraded by other factors that contribute to NC development, such as reading skills. Of course, this is a speculative interpretative hypothesis. To our knowledge, there are no studies that have taken into consideration the role of both EFs and reading skills on the development of NC.
Changes in exposure to narratives could also explain the decrease in the association between EFs and NC. The amount of this exposure may play a role in the development of NC and downgrade the association between EFs and NC. It is true that narratives are cross-culturally used in childrearing systems, and children are exposed to them from very early in life to a greater or lesser extent. However, during preschool and the first years of elementary school, children are exposed to narratives and narration is widely used as an educational strategy in school. Narratives create a pleasant and creative learning environment and a more general constructive and enjoyable atmosphere for the children [99]. Moreover, the use of narrative in education attracts the interest of the children and assists in the better understanding of the information obtained through this. Often, story grammar becomes part of the school curriculum and children are taught to become good narrators, so it is possible that when the development of good NC becomes formal learning, NC may progressively be less associated with or dependent on EFs.
It seems that the two dimensions are more associated early in childhood, the period in which EFs and NC—taken singly—dramatically undergo rapid and qualitative changes [10,100]. We have discussed the possibility that EFs may become less relevant for supporting NC over the course of development, but it is also possible that NC supports EF development across time, becoming less essential by nearly 8 years old. There is evidence that language skills support EF development, especially across preschool age, and narrative language could be considered a “naturalistic” way to investigate children’s language in connected speech [1]. Therefore, it is possible that the practice of constructing causally coherent true narratives could help children in initiating and regulating behaviour—as demonstrated in language research [101,102]—and that narrative language may have a mediating role in EF performance, as there is evidence that language skills have this role in both deaf and hearing children [103].
However, it is still possible that increasing cognitive demands associated with the transition to elementary school and the development of other competencies play a more significant role than NC in the development and reorganization of EFs. The role of NC—and language—in EF development can be progressively nuanced by the other increasing competencies in this period, which could be responsible for the decrease observed in their association. It should be pointed out that the argument that the magnitude of the relationship between NC and EFs seems to decrease over time applies only to the transversal relationship between them. One competence may relate longitudinally with the other and vice versa. For instance, NC and EFs may be weakly related at 9 years old, but EFs at 5 years old is significantly associated with NC observed at 9 years old. However, there is insufficient data in the literature to answer this question with a meta-analysis.
The third purpose of this work was to try to understand some moderators responsible for the heterogeneity observed between studies in the magnitude of the association between EFs and NC. Since the magnitude of the transversal relationship between EFs and NC changes over time, we analysed the role of these moderators in two different time windows: before and after 7 years old.
We found that, before 7 years, the association between EFs and NC is stronger in children with atypical development, such as ASD, ADHD or SLI. However, later in development, the strength of the association fades. After 7 years, results suggest the strength of the association appears similar in typical and atypical development unless only the latter is statistically significant.
As mentioned above, NC and EFs are skills that predict important life outcomes and are trainable [68,69]. They are frequently impaired in children with ASD, ADHD or SLI [46,104], and our results may suggest that in such populations the impairment on EFs could somewhat impair NC, or vice versa, between 3 and 7 years of age. In the literature, several training programs aimed at improving EFs or NC have been described (e.g., [72,105], showing promising results in preschoolers [70,71]. There is also evidence that the training effects are higher in children with developmental risks or psychopathological traits [70,106,107].
Establishing if two dimensions are associated across development is the first necessary step to hypothesize that training one could foster the development of the other. Currently, research aiming to study the effectiveness of EF or NC training did not take into consideration possible far transfer effects on them. In the same way, there are no studies that implement integrated interventions targeting both NC and EF or studies that verify their effectiveness.
The results of this meta-analysis could be read as a first step towards research on integrated interventions and plans to verify the effectiveness of single EF intervention on NC and vice versa. Based on our results, we could propose some speculative hypotheses related to the fact that—if a far transfer between NC and EFs is possible—the chance to observe it on the non-directly trained skills would reduce after 7 years. Following this reasoning, according to our results, only training programs aimed at improving single specific competencies might be effective in older children showing impairments both in NC and EFs. This is consistent with research that unanimously agrees that intervention is likely more effective and pervasive when provided earlier in life rather than later [108].
Moderation analyses also explained part of the heterogeneity in the effect size between and within different studies depending on different EF domains and NC levels assessed.
We found that before 7 years the association between EFs and NC is stronger if we considered the macrostructural level of NC, which includes several important story characteristics such as the quantity of information, story structure, and cohesion. This is not unsurprising, as in the transition from preschool- to school-age this competence shows a remarkable increase (e.g., [20,21]). For instance, analysing the stories produced by children aged 4 to 8, Schneider et al., (2006) [97] showed a significant increase in the quantity of relevant information included in the narrations as children’s age increased. In addition, as children grow and develop their NC, they gradually move from non-goal-directed sequences toward complete episodes. From preschool to elementary school, children go from producing stories that include few causal connections between events to being able to conceive an overall plot with most of the story grammar elements and following a logical progression of events in their stories, which make them appear more cohesive and well structured. It is possible that EFs play a significant role in this progress and that this progress may support EF development.
It seems that, later in development, the strength of the association between EFs and macrostructural NC fades. After 7 years, results suggest the strength of the association appears similar at the microstructural and macrostructural levels. However, children’s ability to tell stories continues to develop during primary and secondary school. Older children indeed include more events than do younger ones [7]; they correctly use a broader range of conjunctions [109] and more advanced anaphoric strategies (e.g., pronouns were used to maintain a reference to characters, whereas nominals were used to switch a reference) that make the stories appear more cohesive. Additionally, EFs show an increase in late childhood and adolescence, but its development may be less involved in NC and vice versa. As argued before, a more significant role in NC increase at this time may be played by reading skills consolidation or other competencies.
Heterogeneity in the effect size seemingly cannot be explained by the narrative form (oral vs. written) used in the articles collected. This is consistent with results found by Bigozzi and Vettori [16], who showed that, in the transition from oral to written code, typically developing children who master writing preserve their oral narrative skills. There is evidence that difficulties in written over oral narrative form may be observed in atypically developing children who struggle with handwriting. Unfortunately, our sample size was not adequate to investigate the interaction of the two moderators (i.e., population and narrative form) in the subgroup analysis. In the subgroup of studies involved in the second meta-analysis (children older than 8 years), atypically developing children represent only 8% of the sample.
As regards EF domains, we found that the strength of the association between EFs and NC appears similar for different EF domains before 7 years old. After 7 years, results showed a general decrease in the strength of the relation, even if some differences from medium overall effect size emerge by different EF domains.
More specifically, in preschoolers and first and second graders, the contribution of EFs to NC appears statistically equal across EF domains. This could be because, at this age, EFs tend to be more related and less differentiated from each other [26,29,30,31], so any attempts to connect the various tasks to one distinct EF domain at this age may be artificial. For this reason, specific patterns between EF domains and NC could be challenging to observe in this time window. Additionally, a technical consideration may explain the absence of evidence. Studies included in the first meta-analysis showed substantial between-study heterogeneity within the EF domains, which decreased the pooled effect’s precision (i.e., increased the standard error). Yet, when the EF domains effect estimates are imprecise, their confidence intervals will have a large overlap, as in some of our cases (e.g., working memory updating CI index: 0.057, 0.632). Consequentially, this might make it harder to find a significant difference between subgroups—even if this difference could exist.
Specific patterns in the relationship with NC may emerge after 7 years old, when EF domains are more differentiated and distinguishable [28].
In general, the contribution of all EF domains to NC seems to decrease after 7 years, with the notable exception of behavioural inhibition. This domain refers to the ability to suppress a dominant but inappropriate response or prevent impulsive motor response, according to Nigg’s definition [38]. Together with interference control, behavioural inhibition may be critically involved over development to monitor the production of extraneous comments and derailments while telling a story or inhibit semantic competitors while producing words. NC may also be involved in inhibition tasks. Narrative language may indeed be used to exert control over attention and inhibit inadequate response and interferent representation.
As with inhibition, working memory capacity, shifting, and planning also appear to be involved in NC at this age. Working memory capacity could be required to keep in mind ideas before translating them into linguistic representations, as well as to recall episodic contents for an accurate organization of temporal sequences in the story. Shifting could be required in the generation of complete episodes and in the ability to monitor the communicative flow. Instead, planning may play a coordinating role in story organization, e.g., putting all the story elements in the correct sequence [51]. These results are in line with studies reporting that working memory, shifting, and planning are correlated with text generation in older children [57,61] and adolescents [58]. Other domains seem to be significantly less associated in this period with NC than in the previous time window, such as updating of working memory. This is consistent with previous findings in Swedish [52] and Canadian preschoolers [55].

Study Limitations

Finally, we would like to discuss some limits of the present work. As claimed above, the current meta-analysis cannot respond definitively to some questions about the relationship between NC and EFs because of its limits. The first limit is related to the fact that few studies investigate this relationship with a longitudinal design. Therefore, even if our results clearly show that a relationship between NC and EFs is definitively positive, we know it is just transversal. We cannot say something about how and if these dimensions are related longitudinally across time if there is one point at which one predicts the other and vice versa because there is not enough research addressing this issue. Future research should investigate if these domains are predictive of each other and establish the direction(s) of their development. A second limitation concerns the time variable used in this meta-analysis to answer the question of whether the relationship changes over time: the mean age of participants. Some studies included in the present meta-analysis involved participants of a large range of ages (e.g., 7–12; 7–14, see Table 4), so it was hard to classify the studies by age stage (e.g., preschoolers; school-aged; adolescents). We preferred not to exclude these studies from the analysis and chose to consider the mean age of the participants collecting—where available—the effect size adjusted for the effect of age. The time effect is one of the most interesting issues for a developmental psychologist. Even if the praxis to analyse the impact of time/age over a phenomenon in meta-analytic developmental psychology research is consolidated, it should be kept in mind that using aggregate information—such as the mean age of participants—may produce ecologically biased results [110,111]. Therefore, any conclusion around the relationship between EFs and NC changes should be taken cautiously and considered just orientational. Aggregating data suggest that a turning point in this relationship occurs at around 7–8 years old, but studies covering this age range also include 6- and 9-years-old participants. Furthermore, studies covering this age range in the sample of articles selected from the meta-analysis are few (k = 4). Meta-analytic research led to summarizing results from different studies, which potentially may offer a comprehensive picture of a phenomenon. In this case, we can see that the relationship between EFs and NC seems to decrease over time, even if we cannot be sure of the exact time point at which it starts to drop, but it seems that it takes place around the first three grades of elementary school. Future studies should examine this period in more detail than preschool.
Another limitation concerns the intrinsic multidimensionality and complexity of EF construct examined and the large variety of instruments used to capture the construct across development. We based the instruments’ classification on the scientific literature [32,38,42,73] in order to clarify which task assesses which specific component, but we are aware of the “task impurity problem”, a phenomenon in which one task assesses various EFs components beyond the one it aims to evaluate, which is frequently in young children. So far, we invite the reader to take cautiously into consideration findings about the specific pattern of relationships between various EF domains and NC since this may depend on the classification we used.
Finally, the last limitation we mention is that NC and EFs are two dimensions that, in real life, are related to many other dimensions of human development that could mediate or explain their relationship. One of these is theory of mind, which is associated with both dimensions [60,112]. In certain circumstances, speculatively, these variables might be responsible for the presence or the lack of association between EFs and NC across the studies. Studies included in this meta-analysis consider the account of potentially confounding variables (e.g., age) on the correlation between EFs and NC, to various degrees and differently. They used to control their effects by reporting partialized correlation coefficients of the relationship between EFs and NC. Unless this operation is fundamental to provide a reliable measure of the association between EFs and NC, it increases the between-study heterogeneity. For this reason, another limitation in interpreting our results is that we cannot be sure that this relationship is direct. Further investigations are necessary for this scope.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, despite these limitations, this work suggests that, over time, the domains of EF and NC are associated and may depend on each other. This seems to be especially true in young, atypically developing children and for macrostructural elements of NC. However, in general, the relationship between EFs and NC that is stronger in early childhood is bound to decrease over development. Since these competencies are usually impaired in children with atypical development, but they can be effectively trainable, we stress that good practice might be to introduce small group interventions to support one or both competencies at the end of preschool and in the first two grades, i.e., at the time EFs and NC appear more related.
Furthermore, the results provided in this meta-analysis and their limitations suggests some orientational consideration for future research:
  • Previous research has focused more on these domains taken singly than on their relationship. However, to understand human development and support it with effective intervention, we should also focus on connecting its parts. NC and EFs are promising domains because they predict many life outcomes and seem trainable. We should know much about their relationship, especially in atypically developing people and in longitudinal ways. This is to understand when and how it is better to intervene to be effective.
  • Previous research on EFs and NC focused mainly on two age bands (i.e., 3–6 and 9–12) and considered large age ranges. This makes it hard to understand the development of the relationship between EFs and NC across time. Even if results provided by single studies are frequently controlled by age differences, it would be insightful to observe the correlation in more homogeneous age groups. Furthermore, since the strength of the relationship seems to decrease over time, and a turning point in this sense may be represented by the first two grades of elementary school, studies focused on this particular time window—which is been more neglected—should be encouraged to better understand what happens at this specific stage and if we can use it to support child development.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/children10081391/s1, Figure S1. Funnel plot of the meta-analysis of main outcomes of all studies. Each plotted point represents the standard error and the z coefficient of the association between NC and EF, for a single outcome. The white triangle represents the region where 95% of the data points are expected to lie in the absence of publication bias. The vertical line represents the estimated effect size, based on the meta-analysis. Table S1. Moderation analyses in the overall sample of studies included.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

  • Narrat* AND Executive Function [OR working memory OR Inhibit* OR flexibility OR shifting OR planning OR problem solving] (filtered by age: > 18 years excluded; by type of document: NOT review)
  • Storytelling AND Executive Function [OR working memory OR Inhibit* OR flexibility OR shifting OR planning OR problem solving] (filtered by age: > 18 years excluded; by type of document: NOT review)

References

  1. Schraeder, T.; Quinn, M.; Stockman, I.J.; Miller, J. Authentic assessment as an approach to preschool speech-language screening. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 1999, 8, 195–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Fazio, B.B.; Naremore, R.C.; Connell, P.J. Tracking children from poverty at risk for specific language impairment: A 3-year longitudinal study. J. Speech Hear. Res. 1996, 39, 611–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. O’Neill, D.K.; Pearce, M.J.; Pick, J.L. Preschool Children’s Narratives and Performance on the Peabody Individualized Achievement Test—Revised: Evidence of a Relation between Early Narrative and Later Mathematical Ability. First Lang. 2004, 24, 149–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Dickinson, D.K.; McCabe, A. Bringing it all together: The multiple origins, skills, and environmental supports of early literacy. Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 2001, 16, 186–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Griffin, T.M.; Hemphill, L.; Camp, L.; Wolf, D.P. Oral Discourse in the Preschool Years and Later Literacy Skills. First Lang. 2004, 24, 123–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Johnston, J.R. Narratives: Twenty-five years later. Top. Lang. Disord. 2008, 28, 93–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Stein, N.L.; Glenn, C.G. An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In New Directions in Discourse Processing; Frredle, R.O., Ed.; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
  8. Pinto, G. Il Suono, il Segno, il Significato. Psicologia dei Processi di Alfabetizzazione; Carocci Editore: Roma, Italy, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  9. Stein, N.L. The development of children’s storytelling skill. In Child Language; Franklin, M., Barten, S., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1988; pp. 282–287. [Google Scholar]
  10. Berman, R.A.; Slobin, D.I. (Eds.) Relating Events in Narrative: A Crosslinguistic Developmental Study; Psychology Press: London, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  11. Van den Broek, P.; Lorch, E.P.; Thurlow, R. Children’s and adult’s memory for television stories: The role of causal factors, story-grammar categories, and hierarchical level. Child Dev. 1996, 67, 3010–3028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Abbott, R.D.; Berninger, V.W. Structural equation modeling of relationships among developmental skills and writing skills in primary-and intermediate-grade writers. J. Educ. Psychol. 1993, 85, 478–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Berninger, V.; Abbott, R.D.; Abbott, S.P.; Graham, S.; Richards, T. Writing and reading connections between language by hand and language by eye. J. Learn. Disabil. 2002, 35, 39–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Fitzgerald, J.; Shanahan, T. Reading and writing relations and their development. Educ. Psychol. 2000, 35, 39–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Olinghouse, N.G. Student- and instruction-level predictors of narrative writing in third-grade students. Read. Writ. 2008, 21, 3–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Bigozzi, L.; Vettori, G. To tell a story, to write it: Developmental patterns of narrative skills from preschool to first grade. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 2016, 31, 461–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Justice, L.M.; Bowles, R.; Pence, K.; Gosse, C. A scalable tool for assessing children’s language abilities within a narrative context: The NAP (Narrative Assessment Protocol). Early Child. Res. Q. 2010, 25, 218–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Mäkinen, L.; Loukusa, S.; Nieminen, L.; Leinonen, E.; Kunnari, S. The development of narrative productivity, syntactic complexity, referential cohesion and event content in four-to eight-year-old Finnish children. First Lang. 2014, 34, 24–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Castilla-Earls, A.; Petersen, D.; Spencer, T.; Hammer, K. Narrative Development in Monolingual Spanish-Speaking Preschool Children. Early Educ. Dev. 2015, 26, 1166–1186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Roch, M.; Florit, E.; Levorato, C. Narrative competence of Italian–English bilingual children between 5 and 7 years. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2016, 37, 49–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Zanchi, P.; Zampini, L. The Narrative Competence task: A standardised test to assess children’s narrative skills. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2021, 37, 15–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Shonkoff, J.; Phillips, D. From Neurons to Neighbourhoods: The Science of Early Childhood Development; National Academy Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  23. Moffitt, T.E.; Arseneault, L.; Belsky, D.; Dickson, N.; Hancox, R.J.; Harrington, H.; Houts, R.; Poulton, R.; Roberts, B.W.; Ross, S.; et al. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2011, 108, 2693–2698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Best, J.R.; Miller, P.H.; Jones, L.L. Executive functions after age 5: Changes and correlates. Dev. Rev. 2009, 29, 180–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Müller, U.; Lieberman, D.; Frye, D.; Zelazo, P.D. Executive function, school readiness, and school achievement. In Applied Cognitive Research in K–3 Classrooms; Thurman, S.K., Fiorello, C.A., Eds.; Routledge; Taylor & Francis Group: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 41–83. [Google Scholar]
  26. Wiebe, S.A.; Sheffield, T.; Nelson, J.M.; Clark, C.A.; Chevalier, N.; Espy, K.A. The structure of executive function in 3-year-olds. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2011, 108, 436–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P.; Emerson, M.J.; Witzki, A.H.; Howerter, A.; Wager, T.D. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 2000, 41, 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  28. Lehto, J.E.; Juujärvi, P.; Kooistra, L.; Pulkkinen, L. Dimensions of executive functioning: Evidence from children. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2003, 21, 59–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Monette, S.; Bigras, M.; Lafrenière, M.A. Structure of executive functions in typically developing kindergarteners. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2015, 140, 120–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Scionti, N.; Marzocchi, G.M. The dimensionality of early executive functions in young preschoolers: Comparing unidimensional versus bidimensional models and their ecological validity. Child Neuropsychol. 2021, 27, 491–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Usai, M.C.; Viterbori, P.; Traverso, L.; De Franchis, V. Latent structure of executive function in five-and six-year-old children: A longitudinal study. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2014, 11, 447–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Best, J.R.; Miller, P.H. A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Dev. 2010, 81, 1641–1660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Carriedo, N.; Corral, A.; Montoro, P.R.; Herrero, L.; Rucián, M. Development of the updating executive function: From 7-year-olds to young adults. Dev. Psychol. 2016, 52, 666–678. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Morra, S.; Panesi, S.; Traverso, L.; Usai, M.C. Which tasks measure what? Reflections on executive function development and a commentary on Podjarny, Kamawar, and Andrews (2017). J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2018, 167, 246–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Garon, N.; Bryson, S.E.; Smith, I.M. Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychol. Bull. 2008, 134, 31–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Davidson, K.; Norrie, J.; Tyrer, P.; Gumley, A.; Tata, P.; Murray, H.; Palmer, S. The effectiveness of cognitive behavior therapy for borderline personality disorder: Results from the borderline personality disorder study of cognitive therapy (BOSCOT) trial. J. Personal. Disord. 2006, 20, 450–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Nigg, J.T. Annual Research Review: On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2017, 58, 361–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  39. Gandolfi, E.; Viterbori, P.; Traverso, L.; Usai, M.C. Inhibitory processes in toddlers: A latent-variable approach. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  40. Anderson, V.A.; Anderson, P.; Northam, E.; Jacobs, R.; Catroppa, C. Development of executive functions through late childhood and adolescence in an Australian sample. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2001, 20, 385–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Welsh, M.C.; Pennington, B.F. Assessing frontal lobe functioning in children: Views from developmental psychology. Dev. Neuropsychol. 1988, 4, 199–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. McCormack, T.; Atance, C.M. Planning in young children: A review and synthesis. Dev. Rev. 2011, 31, 1–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Odato, C.V. The development of children’s use of discourse like in peer interaction. Speech 2013, 88, 117–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Song, S.; Su, M.; Kang, C.; Liu, H.; Zhang, Y.; McBride-Chang, C.; Tardif, T.; Li, H.; Liang, W.; Zhang, Z. Tracing children’s vocabulary development from preschool through the school-age years: An 8-year longitudinal study. Dev. Sci. 2015, 18, 119–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Tomasello, M. Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition 2000, 74, 209–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Gooch, D.; Thompson, P.; Nash, H.M.; Snowling, M.J.; Hulme, C. The development of executive function and language skills in the early school years. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2016, 57, 180–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Mar, R.A. The neuropsychology of narrative: Story comprehension, story production and their interrelation. Neuropsychologia 2004, 42, 1414–1434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Coelho, C.; Liles, B.; Duffy, R. Impairments of discourse abilities and executive functions in traumatically brain-injured adults. Brain Inj. 1995, 9, 471–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Fletcher, P.C.; Happe, F.; Frith, U.; Baker, S.C.; Dolan, R.J.; Frackowiak, R.S.; Frith, C.D. Other minds in the brain: A functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension. Cognition 1995, 57, 109–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Mozeiko, J.; Le, K.; Coelho, C.; Krueger, F.; Grafman, J. The relationship of story grammar and executive function following TBI. Aphasiology 2011, 25, 826–835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Khan, K.S. The Relation between Cognitive Skills and Language Skills in Typically Developing 3 ½ to 6 Year Olds. Ph.D. Thesis, The Pennsylvania State University, Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  52. Tonér, S.; Nilsson Gerholm, T. Links between language and executive functions in Swedish preschool children: A pilot study. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2021, 42, 207–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Balaban, H.O.; Hohenberger, A. The development of narrative skills in Turkish-speaking children: A complexity approach. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0232579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Drijbooms, E.; Groen, M.A.; Verhoeven, L. How executive functions predict development in syntactic complexity of narrative writing in the upper elementary grades. Read. Writ. Interdiscip. J. 2017, 30, 209–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. McNiven, C.L. The Relation Between Cognitive Skills and Language Skills in Typically Developing 3 1/2 to 6 Year Olds. Master Thesis, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  56. Balioussis, C.; Johnson, J.; Pascual-Leone, J. Fluency and complexity in children’s writing: The role of mental attention and executive function. Riv. Psicolinguist. Appl. 2012, 12, 33–45. [Google Scholar]
  57. Puranik, C.S. Expository Writing Skills in Elementary School Children from Third Through Sixth Grades and Contributions of Short—Term and Working Memory. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  58. Swanson, H.L.; Berninger, V.W. Individual differences in children’s writing: A function of working memory or reading or both processes? Read. Writ. Interdiscip. J. 1996, 8, 357–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Dodwell, K.; Bavin, E.L. Children with specific language impairment: An investigation of their narratives and memory. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2008, 43, 201–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Ketelaars, M.P.; Jansonius, K.; Cuperus, J.; Verhoeven, L. Narrative competence and underlying mechanisms in children with pragmatic language impairment. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2012, 33, 281–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Drijbooms, E.; Groen, M.A.; Verhoeven, L. The contribution of executive functions to narrative writing in fourth grade children. Read. Writ. Interdiscip. J. 2015, 28, 989–1011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  62. Salas, N.; Silvente, S. The role of executive functions and transcription skills in writing: A cross-sectional study across 7 years of schooling. Read. Writ. 2020, 33, 877–905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Swanson, H.L.; Berninger, V.W. Individual differences in children’s working memory and writing skill. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1996, 63, 358–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Artico, M.; Penge, R. Uno studio trasversale sul contributo delle Funzioni Esecutive nella narrazione scritta in bambini con Disturbo Specifico di Apprendimento [A transversal study on the contribution of Executive Functions on written narratives in children with learning disabilities]. G. Neuropsichiatr. Dell’età Evol. 2016, 36, 14–23. [Google Scholar]
  65. Friend, M.; Phoenix-Bates, R. The union of narrative and executive function: Different but complementary. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  66. Fernández, A.Y.; Miranda, B.R.; Casas, A.M. Funciones ejecutivas, comprensión de historias y coherencia narrativa en niños con trastorno por déficit de atención con hiperactividad. Rev. Logop. Foniatría Audiol. 2010, 30, 151–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Duinmeijer, I.; de Jong, J.; Scheper, A. Narrative abilities, memory and attention in children with a specific language impairment. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2012, 47, 542–555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Abel, C.; Nerren, J.; Wilson, H. Leaping the language gap: Strategies for preschool and head start teachers. Int. J. Child Care Educ. Policy 2015, 9, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Diamond, A.; Lee, K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science 2011, 333, 959–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Scionti, N.; Cavallero, M.; Zogmaister, C.; Marzocchi, G.M. Is cognitive training effective for improving executive functions in preschoolers? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 2020, 10, 2812. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  71. Petersen, D.B. A systematic review of narrative-based language intervention with children who have language impairment. Commun. Disord. Q. 2011, 32, 207–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Spencer, T.D.; Weddle, S.A.; Petersen, D.B.; Adams, J.A. Multi-tiered narrative intervention for preschoolers: A Head Start implementation study. NHSA Dialog 2018, 20, 1–28. [Google Scholar]
  73. Henry, L.A.; Bettenay, C. The assessment of executive functioning in children. Child Adolesc. Ment. Health 2010, 15, 110–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Van der Sluis, S.; De Jong, P.F.; Van der Leij, A. Executive functioning in children, and its relations with reasoning, reading, and arithmetic. Intelligence 2007, 35, 427–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Alloway, T.P.; Gathercole, S.E.; Pickering, S.J. Verbal and visuospatial short-term and working memory in children: Are they separable? Child Dev. 2006, 77, 1698–1716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  76. McCormack, T.; Hanley, M. Children’s reasoning about the temporal order of past and future events. Cogn. Dev. 2011, 26, 299–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Page, M.J.; McKenzie, J.E.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. Syst. Rev. 2021, 372, n71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Toplak, M.E.; West, R.F.; Stanovich, K.E. Practitioner review: Do performance-based measures and ratings of executive function assess the same construct? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2013, 54, 131–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Peristeri, E.; Baldimtsi, E.; Andreou, M.; Tsimpli, I.M. The impact of bilingualism on the narrative ability and the executive functions of children with autism spectrum disorders. J. Commun. Disord. 2020, 85, 105999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Park, H. Language Skills, Oral Narrative Production, and Executive Functions of Children Who Are Deaf or Hard of Hearing. Ph.D. Thesis, The University of Tennessee Health Science Center, Memphis, TN, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2021; Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 8 April 2022).
  82. RStudio Team. RStudio: Integrated Development for R. Rstudio; PBC: Boston, MA, USA, 2020; Available online: http://www.rstudio.com/ (accessed on 8 April 2022).
  83. Viechtbauer, W.; Viechtbauer, M.W. Package ‘metafor’. The Comprehensive R Archive Network. Package ‘Metafor’. 2015. Available online: http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/metafor/metafor.Pdf (accessed on 6 April 2022).
  84. Assink, M.; Wibbelink, C.J. Fitting three-level meta-analytic models in R: A step-by-step tutorial. Quant. Methods Psychol. 2016, 12, 154–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  85. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Routledge Academic: New York, NY, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  86. Olkin, I.; Finn, J.D. Correlations redux. Psychol. Bull. 1995, 118, 155–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Peterson, R.A.; Brown, S.P. On the Use of Beta Coefficients in Meta-Analysis. J. Appl. Psychol. 2005, 90, 175–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  88. Borenstein, M. Effect sizes for continuous data. In The Handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis; Cooper, H., Hedges, L.V., Valentine, J.C., Eds.; Russell Sage Foundation: New York, NY, USA, 2009; pp. 221–235. [Google Scholar]
  89. Knapp, G.; Hartung, J. Improved tests for a random effects meta-regression with a single covariate. Stat. Med. 2003, 22, 2693–2710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Cochran, W.G. Some methods for strengthening the common χ2 tests. Biometrics 1954, 10, 417–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Sacchetti, S.A. Lo Sviluppo delle Competenze Narrative e Funzioni Esecutive in Età Prescolare [The Development of Narrative Competence and Executive Functions in Preschool Age]. Master Thesis, University of Milan Bicocca, Milano, Italy, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  92. Arán Filippetti, V.; Richaud, M.C. Do Executive Functions Predict Written Composition? Effects beyond Age, Verbal Intelligence and Reading Comprehension. Acta Neuropsychol. 2015, 13, 331–349. [Google Scholar]
  93. Veraksa, A.; Bukhalenkova, D.; Kartushina, N.; Oshchepkova, E. The relationship between executive functions and language production in 5–6-year-old children: Insights from working memory and storytelling. Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  94. Vanderberg, R.; Lee Swanson, H. Which components of working memory are important in the writing process? Read. Writ. 2007, 20, 721–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Hunter, J.E.; Schmidt, F.L. Methods of Meta-Analysis: Correcting Error and Bias in Research Findings; Sage Publications, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
  96. Marini, A.; Piccolo, B.; Taverna, L.; Berginc, M.; Ozbič, M. The complex relation between executive functions and language in preschoolers with developmental language disorders. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 1772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  97. Schneider, P.; Hayward, D.; Vis Dubé, R. Storytelling from pictures using the Edmonton narrative norms instrument. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. Audiol. 2006, 30, 224–238. [Google Scholar]
  98. Seymour, P.H.; Aro, M.; Erskine, J.M. Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. Br. J. Psychol. 2003, 94, 143–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  99. Nanson, A. Storytelling and Ecology: Empathy, Enchantment and Emergence in the Use of Oral Narratives; Bloomsbury Publishing: London, UK, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  100. Anderson, P.J.; Reidy, N. Assessing executive function in preschoolers. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2012, 22, 345–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Vallotton, C.; Ayoub, C. Use your words: The role of language in the development of toddlers’ self-regulation. Early Child. Res. Q. 2011, 26, 169–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  102. Zelazo, P.D.; Jacques, S. Children’s Rule Use: Representation, Reflection, and Cognitive Control. In Annals of Child Development: A Research Annual; Jessica Kingsley Publishers: London, UK, 1996; Volume 12, pp. 119–176. [Google Scholar]
  103. Botting, N.; Morgan, G.; Jones, A.; Marshall, C.; Denmark, T.; Atkinson, J. Nonverbal executive function is mediated by language: A study of deaf and hearing children. Child Dev. 2017, 88, 1689–1700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  104. Craig, F.; Margari, F.; Legrottaglie, A.R.; Palumbi, R.; De Giambattista, C.; Margari, L. A review of executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2016, 12, 1191–1202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  105. Thorell, L.B.; Lindqvist, S.; Bergman Nutley, S.; Bohlin, G.; Klingberg, T. Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Dev. Sci. 2009, 12, 106–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Melby-Lervåg, M.; Hulme, C. Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Dev. Psychol. 2013, 49, 270–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  107. Wass, S.V.; Scerif, G.; Johnson, M.H. Training attentional control and working memory–Is younger, better? Dev. Rev. 2012, 32, 360–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  108. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Why Act Early if You’re Concerned about Development? Available online: https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/actearly/whyActEarly.html (accessed on 11 April 2022).
  109. Shapiro, L.R.; Hudson, J.A. Tell me a make-believe story: Coherence and cohesion in young children’s picture-elicited narratives. Dev. Psychol. 1991, 27, 960–974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Piantadosi, S.; Byar, D.P.; Green, S.B. The ecological fallacy. Am. J. Epidemiol. 1988, 127, 893–904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Thompson, S.G.; Higgins, J.P. How should meta-regression analyses be undertaken and interpreted? Stat. Med. 2002, 21, 1559–1573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  112. Perner, J.; Lang, B. Development of Theory of Mind and Executive Control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1999, 9, 337–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Prisma Diagram. Source: [77]. For more information, visit: http://www.prisma-statement.org/ (accessed on 19 April 2022).
Figure 1. Prisma Diagram. Source: [77]. For more information, visit: http://www.prisma-statement.org/ (accessed on 19 April 2022).
Children 10 01391 g001
Figure 2. The relationship between EFs and NC over development. Note: The solid line represents the trend of Fisher’s Z coefficient over time. Point of the solid line are averaged effect size of the relationship between EFs and NC in the five time-intervals considered. The dotted line is the trend line of the relationship between NC and EFs over time. The angular coefficient of the dotted line is negative, indicating that the association between NC and EFs decreases over time.
Figure 2. The relationship between EFs and NC over development. Note: The solid line represents the trend of Fisher’s Z coefficient over time. Point of the solid line are averaged effect size of the relationship between EFs and NC in the five time-intervals considered. The dotted line is the trend line of the relationship between NC and EFs over time. The angular coefficient of the dotted line is negative, indicating that the association between NC and EFs decreases over time.
Children 10 01391 g002
Table 1. Age effect on the relationship between EFs and NC.
Table 1. Age effect on the relationship between EFs and NC.
EffectNo. OutcomesNo. StudiesNo.
Participants
Estimated zSE95% CIp-Value
Children’s age (years)267293410−0.0140.005−0.025−0.0030.009
Developmental time windows
Before 7 years85137950.2740.0290.2160.333<0.001
After 7 years1821626150.2120.0210.1700.254<0.001
Note: Italic text indicates the levels of the categorical variables.
Table 2. Moderators of the relationship between NC and EFs before and after literacy acquisition.
Table 2. Moderators of the relationship between NC and EFs before and after literacy acquisition.
EffectNo. OutcomesNo. StudiesNo.
Participants
Estimated zSE95% CIp-Value
4–7 years: Population
Typically
developing
7796520.2480.2300.2020.294<0.001
Atypically
developing
841430.4360.0860.2640.607<0.001
8–18 years: Population
Typically
developing
1061124120.2210.0260.1690.273<0.001
Atypically
developing
7652030.1990.0400.1190.279<0.001
4–7 years: EF Domain
Working memory capacity3674590.2590.0350.1880.330<0.001
Working memory updating21370.3440.1440.0570.6320.019
Interference control82630.3090.0740.1600.458<0.001
Behavioural Inhibition1851850.1530.0490.0550.2510.002
Shifting1242110.2920.0540.1830.400<0.001
Planning721220.3720.0750.2220.522<0.001
8–18 years: EF Domain
Working memory capacity391122480.2320.0320.1680.297<0.001
Working memory updating121400.1350.087−0.0360.3070.120
Interference control52412950.2280.0440.1390.317<0.001
Behavioural Inhibition1442690.2920.0480.1970.387<0.001
Shifting3063390.2050.0430.1190.291<0.001
Planning1741770.2040.0520.1010.307<0.001
8–18 years: Narrative form
Oral8662660.2520.0440.1650.340<0.001
Written961023490.2000.0260.1480.252<0.001
4–7 years: Narrative Competence
Micro-structural4585780.2090.0230.1630.0255<0.001
Macro-structural3285270.3290.0250.2780.380<0.001
8–18 years: Narrative Competence
Micro-structural1051224760.2130.0240.1640.261<0.001
Macro-structural771412080.2160.0260.1640.268<0.001
Note: Italic text indicates the levels of the categorical variables.
Table 3. Studies including participants aged 4–7 years old.
Table 3. Studies including participants aged 4–7 years old.
ReferencesLocationClinical Risk Status of the SampleMean Age (Years)Age RangeEF DomainEF TaskNarrative FormNarrative CompetenceNC IndicatorFisher’s Z, [95% CI]SE
Balaban et al., 2020 [53]TurkieTypically developing (n = 18)4.424–5Behavioural InhibitionEmotional Stroop TaskOralMacro-structuralStory Content–plot complexity0.2554 [−0.2506, 0.7615]0.2583
Emotional Stroop Task Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.4847 [−0.0214, 0.9908]0.2583
Dodwell and Bavin, 2008 [59]AustraliaSpecific Language Impairment (n = 16)6.706–7Working Memory capacityDigit SpanOralMacro-structuralInformation0.182 [0.3616, 0.7256]0.2773
Working Memory capacityWord Span Information0.3205 [0.2231, 0.8641]0.2773
Working Memory capacityRecalling Sentences Information0.4059 [0.1377, 0.9495]0.2773
Duinmeijer et al., 2012 [67]The NetherlandsSpecific Language Impairment (n = 34)7.356–9Working Memory capacityDigit SpanOralMicro-structuralMean Length of Utterance0.6416 [0.2896, 0.9936]0.1797
Friend and Phoenix-Bates, 2014 [65]USATypically developing (n = 38)5.004–5ShiftingANT-executive attention subtestOral-Story content, lexicon and syntax0.2693 [−0.062, 0.6006]0.1691
ShiftingANT-executive attention subtest (latency) -Story content, lexicon and syntax0.3062 [−0.0251, 0.6375]0.1691
Behavioural InhibitionTapping -Story content, lexicon and syntax0.1861 [−0.1452, 0.5174]0.1691
Behavioural InhibitionTapping (latency) -Story content, lexicon and syntax0.2059 [−0.1254, 0.5372]0.1691
USATypically developing (n = 42)4.424–5Behavioural InhibitionTapping -Story content, lexicon and syntax0.1748 [−0.1391, 0.4886]0.1600
Behavioural InhibitionTapping (latency) -Story content, lexicon and syntax0.3172 [0.0034, 0.6311]0.1600
ShiftingANT-executive attention subtest -Story content, lexicon and syntax0.3406 [0.0267, 0.6544]0.1600
ShiftingANT-executive attention subtest (latency) -Story content, lexicon and syntax0.009 [−0.3048, 0.3228]0.1600
Ketelaars et al., 2011 [60]The NetherlandsSpecific Language Impairment (n = 77)5.604–6-Nepsy subtestsOralMicro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.3884 [0.1606, 0.6163]0.1162
The NetherlandsTypically developing (n = 77)5.604–6-Nepsy subtests Micro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.3095 [0.0817, 0.5374]0.1162
Khan, 2013 (dissertation) [51]USATypically developing (n = 84)4.503.5–5ShiftingVerbal FluencyOralMacro-structuralStory Content0.2132 [−0.0046, 0.4309]0.1109
PlanningTower of Hanoi Story Content0.2769 [0.0591, 0.4946]0.1109
ShiftingCard Sorting Story Content0.3316 [0.1139, 0.5494]0.1109
Marini et al., 2020 [96] ItalyDevelopmental Language Disorder (n = 16)5.175Working Memory capacityDigit SpanOralMacro-structuralInformation 0.3294 [−0.2142, 0.873]0.2773
Interference ControlSquare/Circle Micro-structuralNumber Of Utterance0.5101 [−0.335, 1.0537]0.2773
Square/Circle Macro-structuralInformation0.6169 [0.0734, 1.1605]0.2773
McNiven, 2010 [55]CanadaTypically developing (n = 37)6.955–8Updating of Working MemoryKeep TrackOralMacro-structuralCohesiveness-Referential accuracy0.3462 [0.0101, 0.6823]0.1715
Updating of Working MemoryN-back Cohesiveness-Referential accuracy0.362 [0.0259, 0.6982]0.1715
Updating of Working MemorySound monitoring task Cohesiveness-Referential accuracy0.4784 [0.1423, 0.8146]0.1715
Sacchetti, 2018 (dissertation) [91]ItalyTypically developing (n = 38–40)4.923–5PlanningNon-Narrative SequencesOralMicro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.4392 [0.1079, 0.7705]0.1691
Non-Narrative Sequences Micro-structuralLexical Variety0.1186 [−0.2127, 0.4498]0.1691
Non-Narrative Sequences Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.3417 [0.0104, 0.673]0.1691
Non-Narrative Sequences Micro-structuralMean Length of Utterance0.2247 [−0.1066, 0.556]0.1691
Non-Narrative Sequences Macro-structuralStory Content0.5037 [0.1724, 0.835]0.1691
Non-Narrative Sequences Macro-structuralCoherence of structure0.5191 [0.1878, 0.8504]0.1691
Behavioural InhibitionGo/NoGo Micro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.008 [−0.3142, 0.3302]0.1643
Go/NoGo Micro-structuralLexical Variety0.006 [−0.3162, 0.3282]0.1643
Go/NoGo Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.1034 [−0.2188, 0.4256]0.1643
Go/NoGo Micro-structuralMean Length of Utterance0.1409 [−0.1813, 0.4631]0.1643
Go/NoGo Macro-structuralStory Content0.1419 [−0.1803, 0.4642]0.1643
Go/NoGo Macro-structuralCoherence of structure0.044 [−0.2782, 0.3662]0.1643
Working Memory capacityVocal Span Micro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.1522 [−0.1701, 0.4744]0.1643
Vocal Span Micro-structuralLexical Variety0.1624 [−0.1598, 0.4846]0.1643
Vocal Span Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.051 [−0.2712, 0.3733]0.1643
Vocal Span Micro-structuralMean Length of Utterance0.043 [−0.2792, 0.3652]0.1643
Vocal Span Macro-structuralInformation and Story Content0.0832 [−0.239, 0.4054]0.1643
Vocal Span Macro-structuralCoherence of structure0.0852 [−0.237, 0.4074]0.1643
Tonér and Nilsson Gerholm, 2021 [52]SwedenTypically developing (n = 47)5.304–6Interference ControlFlankerOralMicro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.1409 [−0.1546, 0.4364]0.1507
Behavioural InhibitionHead-Toes-Knees-Shoulders Total Lexical Production0.0701 [−0.2254, 0.3656]0.1507
Working Memory capacityDigit Span Total Lexical Production0.01 [−0.2855, 0.3055]0.1507
ShiftingDimensional Change Card Sorting Total Lexical Production0.01 [−0.2855, 0.3055]0.1507
Interference ControlFlanker Micro-structuralLexical Variety0.3654 [0.0700, 0.6609]0.1507
Behavioural InhibitionHead-Toes-Knees-Shoulders Lexical Variety0.2132 [−0.0823, 0.5086]0.1507
Working Memory capacityDigit Span Lexical Variety0.2554 [−0.041, 0.5509]0.1507
ShiftingDimensional Change Card Sorting Lexical Variety0.4847 [0.1892, 0.7802]0.1507
Interference ControlFlanker Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Accuracy0.4356 [0.1401, 0.7311]0.1507
Behavioural InhibitionHead-Toes-Knees-Shoulders Morphosyntactic Accuracy0.1206 [−0.1749, 0.4161]0.1507
Working Memory capacityDigit Span Morphosyntactic Accuracy0.2877 [0.0078, 0.5832]0.1507
ShiftingDimensional Change Card Sorting Morphosyntactic Accuracy0.2554 [−0.0401, 0.5509]0.1507
Interference ControlFlanker Morphosyntactic Complexity0.1614 [−0.1341, 0.4569]0.1507
Behavioural InhibitionHead-Toes-Knees-Shoulders Morphosyntactic Complexity0.05 [−0.2454, 0.3455]0.1507
Working Memory capacityDigit Span Morphosyntactic Complexity0.2448 [−0.0507, 0.5402]0.1507
ShiftingDimensional Change Card Sorting Morphosyntactic Complexity0.3428 [0.0474, 0.6383]0.1507
Interference ControlFlanker Morphosyntactic Complexity–Unified predicates0.1717 [−0.1238, 0.4671]0.1507
Behavioural InhibitionHead-Toes-Knees-Shoulders Morphosyntactic Complexity–Unified predicates0.03 [−0.2655, 0.3255]0.1507
Working Memory capacityDigit Span Morphosyntactic Complexity–Unified predicates0.1206 [−0.1749, 0.4161]0.1507
ShiftingDimensional Change Card Sorting Morphosyntactic Complexity–Unified predicates0.3316 [0.0362, 0.6271]0.1507
Interference ControlFlanker Macro-structuralInformation 0.2877 [−0.0078, 0.5832]0.1507
Behavioural InhibitionHead-Toes-Knees-Shoulders Information 0.1104 [−0.185, 0.4059]0.1507
Working Memory capacityDigit Span Information 0.3095 [0.0140, 0.6050]0.1507
ShiftingDimensional Change Card Sorting Information 0.4722 [0.1768, 0.7677]0.1507
Veraksa et al., 2020 [93]RussiaTypically developing (n = 269)5.585–6Working Memory capacityMemory DesignOralMicro-structuralMorphosyntactic Accuracy0.1206 [0.0004, 0.2408]0.0616
Memory Design Micro-structuralNumber Of Syntagmas0.1511 [0.0310, 0.2713]0.0616
Memory Design Micro-structuralNumber Of Simple Utterance 0.1511 [0.0310, 0.2713]0.0616
Memory Design Macro-structuralCoherence–Semantic adequacy0.1614 [0.0412, 0.2816]0.0616
Memory Design Micro-structuralLexical Production0.1614 [0.412, 0.2816]0.0616
Memory Design Macro-structuralCoherence–programming0.182 [0.0618, 0.3022]0.0616
Working Memory capacitySentence Repetition Micro-structuralNumber Of Simple Utterance 0.2027 [0.0826, 0.3229]0.0616
Sentence Repetition Number Of Syntagmas0.2237 [0.1035, 0.3438]0.0616
Working Memory capacityMemory Design Macro-structuralCoherence–Semantic completeness0.2342 [0.114, 0.3544]0.0616
Memory Design Coherence of structure0.2554 [0.1352, 0.3756]0.0616
Working Memory capacitySentence Repetition Micro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.2554 [0.1352, 0.3756]0.0616
Working Memory capacityMemory Design Macro-structuralCoherence–narrative structure0.2661 [0.1459, 0.3863]0.0616
Working Memory capacitySentence Repetition Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Accuracy0.3205 [0.2004, 0.4407]0.0616
Sentence Repetition Macro-structuralCoherence–Semantic adequacy0.4356 [0.3154, 0.5558]0.0616
Sentence Repetition Coherence–narrative structure0.4599 [0.3397, 0.5801]0.0616
Sentence Repetition Coherence–programming0.4847 [0.3645, 0.6049]0.0616
Sentence Repetition Coherence–narrative type (complete, simplified, distorted)0.5361 [0.4159, 0.6562]0.0616
Sentence Repetition Coherence–Semantic completeness0.5493 [0.4291, 0.6695]0.0616
Table 4. Studies including participants aged 8–18 year old.
Table 4. Studies including participants aged 8–18 year old.
ReferencesLocationClinical Risk Status of the SampleMean Age (Years)Age RangeEF DomainEF TaskNarrative FormNarrative CompetenceNC IndicatorFisher’s Z [95% CI]SE
Artico and Penge, 2016 [64]ItalyDyslexia and Dysgraphia (n = 54)9.878–12ShiftingVerbal FluencyWrittenMicro-structuralLexical Variety0.1003 [0.1741, 0.3748]0.1400
Verbal Fluency Macro-structuralCohesiveness0.1003 [−0.1741, 0.3748]0.1400
PlanningTower of London Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.1307 [−0.1437, 0.4052]0.1400
ShiftingResponse set (NEPSY II) Micro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.1409 [−0.1335, 0.4154]0.1400
PlanningTower of London Total Lexical Production0.1409 [−0.1335, 0.4154]0.1400
ShiftingVerbal Fluency Total Lexical Production0.1717 [−0.1028, 0.4461]0.1400
PlanningTower of London Micro-structuralLexical Variety0.1820 [−0.0925, 0.4564]0.1400
ShiftingResponse set (NEPSY II) Macro-structuralCoherence0.1820 [−0.0925, 0.4564]0.1400
ShiftingSwitching NEPSY II Macro-structuralCohesiveness0.1923 [−0.0821, 0.4668]0.1400
PlanningTower of London Cohesiveness0.1923 [−0.0821, 0.4668]0.1400
ShiftingSwitching NEPSY II Micro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.2027 [−0.0717, 0.4772]0.1400
ShiftingResponse set (NEPSY II) Micro-structuralLexicalVariety0.2132 [−0.0613, 0.4876]0.1400
ShiftingVerbal Fluency Macro-structuralCoherence0.2132 [−0.0613, 0.4876]0.1400
PlanningTower of London Coherence0.2132 [−0.0613, 0.4876]0.1400
PlanningClocks Macro-structuralCohesiveness0.2342 [0.4030, 0.5086]0.1400
ShiftingSwitching NEPSY II Macro-structuralCoherence0.2342 [0.4030, 0.5086]0.1400
ShiftingResponse set (NEPSY II) Macro-structuralCohesiveness0.2448 [−0.0297, 0.5192]0.1400
Behavioural InhibitionGo/NoGo Macro-structuralCoherence0.2448 [−0.0297, 0.5192]0.1400
PlanningClocks Micro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.2877 [0.0132, 0.5621]0.1400
ShiftingSwitching NEPSY II Micro-structuralLexical Variety0.2986 [0.0241, 0.5730]0.1400
ShiftingResponse set (NEPSY II) Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.2986 [0.0241, 0.5730]0.1400
Behavioural InhibitionGo/NoGo Micro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.3428 [0.0684, 0.6173]0.1400
ShiftingVerbal Fluency Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.3541 [0.0796, 0.6285]0.1400
Behavioural InhibitionGo/NoGo Macro-structuralCohesiveness0.3541 [0.0796, 0.6285]0.1400
Go/NoGo Micro-structuralLexical Variety0.3654 [0.0910, 0.6399]0.1400
PlanningClocks Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.3884 [0.1140, 0.6629]0.1400
Clocks Macro-structuralCoherence0.4001 [0.1256, 0.6745]0.1400
Clocks Micro-structuralLexical Variety0.4236 [0.1492, 0.6981]0.1400
ShiftingSwitching NEPSY II Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.4599 [0.1854, 0.7343]0.1400
Behavioural InhibitionGo/NoGo Morphosyntactic Complexity0.5230 [0.2485, 0.7974]0.1400
Balaban et al., 2020 [53]TurkiaTypically Developing (n = 87)8.177–11Behavioural InhibitionEmotional Stroop TaskOralMicro-structuralSyntactic Complexity0.1717 [−0.0422, 0.3855]0.1091
Emotional Stroop Task Macro-structuralPlot Complexity0.3316 [0.1178, 0.5455]0.1091
Balioussis et al., 2012 [56]CanadaTypically Developing (n = 70)9.838–9Working Memory capacityLetter Memory TaskWrittenMicro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.3541 [0.1146, 0.5935]0.1221
ShiftingContingency Naming Task Micro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.4599 [0.2204, 0.6993]0.1221
Working Memory capacityLetter Memory Task Total Lexical Production0.3316 [0.0922, 0.5711]0.1221
ShiftingContingency Naming Task Micro-structuralSyntactic Complexity0.3428 [0.1034, 0.5823]0.1221
Drijbooms et al., 2017 [54]The NetherlandsTypically Developing (n = 93)11.08--Trail Making Test; Tower of LondonWrittenMicro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.03 [−0.1766, 0.2366]0.1054
-Trail Making Test; Tower of London Macro-structuralStory content0.03 [−0.1766, 0.2366]0.1054
-Digit Span; Letter Fluency; Ricerca visiva Story content0.0601 [−0.1465, 0.2667]0.1054
-Digit Span; Letter Fluency; Ricerca visiva Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.0701 [−1365, 0.2767]0.1054
-Digit Span; Letter Fluency; Ricerca visiva Micro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.0701 [−0.1365, 0.2767]0.1054
-Walk Don’t Walk; Opposite Worlds; Trail Making Test; Letter Digit Substitution Total Lexical Production0.1717 [−0.0349, 0.3783]0.1054
-Walk Don’t Walk; Opposite Worlds; Trail Making Test; Letter Digit Substitution Macro-structuralStory content0.2027 [−0.0039, 0.4093]0.1054
-Trail Making Test; Tower of London Micro-structuralMorphosintactic Complexity0.2237 [0.0171, 0.4303]0.1054
-Walk Don’t Walk; Opposite Worlds; Trail Making Test; Letter Digit Substitution Morphosintactic Complexity0.2554 [0.0488, 0.462]0.1054
Drijbooms et al., 2015 [61]The NetherlandsTypically Developing (n = 102)9.588–11Planning Tower of LondonWrittenMicro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.05 [−0.1469, 0.247]0.1005
ShiftingTrail Making Test Micro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.0701 [−0.1269, 0.2671]0.1005
Planning Tower of London Total Lexical Production0.0701 [−0.1269, 0.2671]0.1005
Behavioural InhibitionOpposite words Macro-structuralStory content0.1003 [−0.0966, 0.2973]0.1005
ShiftingTrail Making Test Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.1104 [−0.0865, 0.3074]0.1005
Working Memory capacityDigit Span Macro-structuralStoryContent0.1409 [−0.0561, 0.3379]0.1005
Digit Span Micro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.1511 [−0.0458, 0.3481]0.1005
Planning Tower of London Macro-structuralStory content0.1511 [−0.0458, 0.3481]0.1005
Behavioural InhibitionWalk don’t Walk Story content0.1717 [−0.0253, 0.3687]0.1005
ShiftingTrail Making Test Story content0.1717 [−0.0253, 0.3687]0.1005
Behavioural InhibitionWalk don’t Walk Micro-structuralMorphosintactic Complexity0.182 [−0.015, 0.379]0.1005
Behavioural InhibitionOpposite words Morphosyntactic Complexity0.2132 [0.0162, 0.4102]0.1005
Working Memory capacityDigit Span Morphosyntactic Complexity0.2237 [0.0267, 0.4206]0.1005
Behavioural InhibitionOpposite words Micro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.2448 [0.0478, 0.4418]0.1005
Behavioural InhibitionWalk don’t Walk Total Lexical Production0.2554 [0.0584, 0.4524]0.1005
Fisher et al., 2019 [97]USADyslexia (n = 92)9.25-ShiftingCard SortingOralMacro-structuralCoherence0.1206 [−0.0872, 0.3283]0.1058
Interference ControlStroop Coherence0.1614 [−0.0464, 0.3691]0.1058
ShiftingTrail Making Test Coherence0.1923 [−0.0154, 0.4001]0.1058
Working Memory capacityCorsi Coherence0.2877 [0.0799, 0.4954]0.1058
Park, 2014 (dissertation) [80]USATypically Developing (n = 10)10.009–11ShiftingTrail Making TestOralMacro-structuralGAO units0.4611 [−0.2797, 1.2019]0.3780
Trail Making Test Macro-structuralComplete GAO units (Integrity)0.1318 [−0.609, 0.8726]0.3780
PlanningTower of London Complete GAO units (Integrity)0.0993 [−0.6415, 0.8401]0.3780
Tower of London Macro-structuralGAO units–episodic structure0.038 [−0.7028, 0.7788]0.3780
ShiftingCard Sorting Macro-structuralComplete GAO units (Integrity)0.2079 [−0.5329, 0.9487]0.3780
Working Memory capacityDigit Span Backword Complete GAO units (Integrity)0.2586 [−0.4822, 0.9994]0.3780
Digit Span Backword Macro-structuralGAO units0.5682 [−0.1726, 1.3089]0.3780
ShiftingCard Sorting GAO units0.8053 [0.0645, 1.5461]0.3780
Deaf or hard to hearing (n = 11)10.009–11PlanningTower of LondonOralMacro-structuralGAO units0.5874 [−0.1056, 1.2803]0.3536
Working Memory capacityDigit Span Backword GAO units0.3451 [−0.3479, 1.038]0.3536
ShiftingCard Sorting GAO units0.2384 [−0.4545, 0.9314]0.3536
Working Memory capacityDigit Span Backword Macro-structuralComplete GAO units (Integrity)0.1145 [−0.5785, 0.8074]0.3536
PlanningTower of London Complete GAO units (Integrity)0.1155 [−0.5774, 0.8085]0.3536
ShiftingTrail Making Test Complete GAO units (Integrity)0.1348 [−0.5581, 0.8278]0.3536
Trail Making Test Macro-structuralGAO units0.231 [−0.4619, 0.924]0.3536
ShiftingCard Sorting Macro-structuralComplete GAO units (Integrity)0.4047 [−0.2882, 1.0977]0.3536
Peristeri et al., 2020 [79]GreeceAutism Spectrum Disorder (n = 20)9.807–12Updating of Working Memory2-backOralMicro-structuralLexical Variety0.1246 [−0.3507, 0.6]0.2425
2-back Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.1522 [−0.3232, 0.6275]0.2425
2-back Micro-structuralNumber of subordinated clauses0.2501 [−0.2253, 0.7254]0.2425
2-back Micro-structuralNumber of relative clauses0.046 [−0.4293, 0.5214]0.2425
2-back Macro-structuralStory Structure0.146 [−0.3293, 0.6214]0.2425
2-back Macro-structuralReferential Accuracy0.4153 [−0.06, 0.8907]0.2425
Interference ControlLocal-to-Global (Accuracy) Micro-structuralLexical Variety0.0993 [−0.376, 0.5747]0.2425
Local-to-Global (Accuracy) Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.3272 [0.1482, 0.8026]0.2425
Local-to-Global (Accuracy) Micro-structuralNumber of subordinated clauses0.031 [−0.4444, 0.5064]0.2425
Local-to-Global (Accuracy) Micro-structuralNumber of relative clauses0.047 [−0.4283, 0.5224]0.2425
Local-to-Global (Accuracy) Macro-structuralStory Structure0.0591 [−0.4163, 0.5344]0.2425
Local-to-Global (Accuracy) Macro-structuralReferential Accuracy0.353 [−0.1224, 0.8283]0.2425
Interference ControlGlobal-to-Local (Accuracy) Micro-structuralLexical Variety0.1206 [−0.3548, 0.5959]0.2425
Global-to-Local (Accuracy) Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.0621 [−0.4133, 0.5374]0.2425
Global-to-Local (Accuracy) Micro-structuralNumber of subordinated0.0902 [−0.3851, 0.5656]0.2425
Global-to-Local (Accuracy) Micro-structuralNumber of relatives0.019 [−0.4564, 0.4944]0.2425
Global-to-Local (Accuracy) Macro-structuralStory Structure0.4822 [0.0068, 0.9576]0.2425
Global-to-Local (Accuracy) Macro-structuralReferential Accuracy0.0661 [−0.4093, 0.5415]0.2425
Interference ControlLocal-to-Global (Reaction Time) Micro-structuralLexical Variety0.4562 [−0.0191, 0.9316]0.2425
Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.3598 [−0.1156, 0.8351]0.2425
Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) Micro-structuralNumber of subordinated clauses0.2942 [−0.1812, 0.7696]0.2425
Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) Micro-structuralNumber of relative clauses0.3372 [−0.1381, 0.8126]0.2425
Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) Macro-structuralStory Structure0.037 [−0.4383, 0.5124]0.2425
Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) Macro-structuralReferential Accuracy0.049 [−0.4263, 0.5244]0.2425
Interference ControlGlobal-to-Local (Reaction Time) Micro-structuralLexical Variety0.4648 [−0.0105, 0.9402]0.2425
Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.2715 [−0.2039, 0.7468]0.2425
Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) Micro-structuralNumber of subordinated clauses0.1013 [−0.374, 0.5767]0.2425
Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) Micro-structuralNumber of relative clauses0.045 [−0.4303, 0.5204]0.2425
Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) Macro-structuralStory Structure0.482 [0.0068, 0.9576]0.2425
Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) Macro-structuralReferential Accuracy0.0661 [−0.4093, 0.5415]0.2425
Peristeri et al., 2020 [79]GreeceTypically Developing (n = 20)9.807–12Updating of Working Memory2-backOralMicro-structuralLexical Variety0.1257 [−0.3497, 0.601]0.2425
2-back Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.0862 [−0.3891, 0.5616]0.2425
2-back Micro-structuralNumber of subordinated clauses0.2048 [−0.2705, 0.6802]0.2425
2-back Micro-structuralNumber of relative clauses0.146 [−0.3293, 0.6214]0.2425
2-back Macro-structuralStory Structure0.1064 [−0.369, 0.5818]0.2425
2-back Macro-structuralReferential Accuracy0.231 [−0.2443, 0.7064]0.2425
Interference ControlLocal-to-Global (Accuracy) Micro-structuralLexical Variety0.0621 [−0.4133, 0.5374]0.2425
Local-to-Global (Accuracy) Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.2779 [−0.1974, 0.7533]0.2425
Local-to-Global (Accuracy) Micro-structuralNumber of subordinated clauses0.045 [−0.4303, 0.5204]0.2425
Local-to-Global (Accuracy) Micro-structuralNumber of relative clauses0.9417 [0.4663, 1.4171]0.2425
Local-to-Global (Accuracy) Macro-structuralStory Structure0.2342 [−0.2412, 0.7096]0.2425
Local-to-Global (Accuracy) Macro-structuralReferential Accuracy0.1389 [−0.3365, 0.6142]0.2425
Interference ControlGlobal-to-Local (Accuracy) Micro-structuralLexical Variety0.041 [−0.4343, 0.5164]0.2425
Global-to-Local (Accuracy) Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.5139 [0.0386, 0.9893]0.2425
Global-to-Local (Accuracy) Micro-structuralNumber of subordinated clauses0.0923 [−0.3831, 0.5676]0.2425
Global-to-Local (Accuracy) Micro-structuralNumber of relative clauses0.7137 [0.2384, 1.1891]0.2425
Global-to-Local (Accuracy) Macro-structuralStory Structure0.3496 [−0.1258, 0.8249]0.2425
Global-to-Local (Accuracy) Macro-structuralReferential Accuracy0.0701 [−0.4052, 0.5455]0.2425
Interference ControlLocal-to-Global (Reaction Time) Micro-structuralLexical Variety1.211 [0.7357, 1.6864]0.2425
Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.5308 [0.0554, 1.0062]0.2425
Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) Micro-structuralNumber of subordinated clauses0.2877 [−0.1877, 0.763]0.2425
Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) Micro-structuralNumber of relative clauses0.3507 [−0.1247, 0.8261]0.2425
Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) Macro-structuralStory Structure0.7582 [0.2828, 1.2335]0.2425
Local-to-Global (Reaction Time) Macro-structuralReferential Accuracy0.2533 [−0.2221, 0.7286]0.2425
Interference ControlGlobal-to-Local (Reaction Time) Micro-structuralLexical Variety0.1206 [0.3548, 0.5959]0.2425
Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.0741 [−0.4012, 0.5495]0.2425
Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) Micro-structuralNumber of subordinated clauses0.1186 [−0.3568, 0.5939]0.2425
Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) Micro-structuralNumber of relative clauses0.3586 [−0.1167, 0.834]0.2425
Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) Macro-structuralStory Structure0.6155 [0.1402, 1.0909]0.2425
Global-to-Local (Reaction Time) Macro-structuralReferential Accuracy0.002 [−0.4734, 0.4774]0.2425
Puranik, 2006 (dissertation) [57]USATypically Developing (n = 90)10.228–12Working Memory capacityCompeting Language Processing TaskWrittenMicro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.4001 [0.1899, 0.6102]0.1072
Working Memory capacityDigit Ordering Total Lexical Production0.3316 [0.1215, 0.5418]0.1072
Working Memory capacityCompeting Language Processing Task Macro-structuralInformation0.4118 [0.2017, 0.6219]0.1072
Working Memory capacityDigit Ordering Information0.3884 [0.1783, 0.5986]0.1072
Working Memory capacityCompeting Language Processing Task Micro-structuralNumber of Utterance0.2986 [0.0884, 0.5087]0.1072
Working Memory capacityDigit Ordering Number of Utterance0.2661 [0.056, 0.4762]0.1072
Salas and Silvente, 2020SpainTypically Developing (n = 1337)10.177–14Interference ControlStroopWrittenMicro-structuralMean Length of Utterance0.0802 [0.0265, 0.1338]0.0265
Working Memory capacityDigit Span Micro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.2237 [0.17, 0.2773]0.0265
Working Memory capacityDigit Span Micro-structuralMean Length of Utterance0.0802 [0.0265, 0.1338]0.0265
Interference ControlStroop Micro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.2342 [0.1805, 0.2879]0.0265
Swanson and Berninger, 1996a [63]USATypically Developing (n = 300)11.099–12Working Memory capacityListening Recall, Listening Generate RecallWrittenMicro-structuralNumber of Utterance0.2769 [0.1631, 0.3906]0.0583
Listening Recall, Listening Generate Recall Macro-structuralContent and organization0.2554 [0.1417, 0.3691]0.0583
Working Memory capacityMatrix Micro-structuralNumber of Utterance0.0601 [−0.0537, 0.1738]0.0583
Matrix Macro-structuralContent and organization0.1206 [0.0069, 0.2343]0.0583
Swanson and Berninger, 1996b [58]USATypically Developing (n = 50)10.509–12Working Memory capacitySentence Span TestWrittenMacro-structuralContent0.3095 [0.0236, 0.5945]0.1459
Sentence Span Test Micro-structuralMean Length of Utterance0.2769 [−0.009, 0.5628]0.1459
Sentence Span Test Micro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.3654 [0.0796, 0.6513]0.1459
Vanderberg and Swanson, 2006 [94]USATypically Developing (n = 160)15.2114–15Working Memory capacityRhyming wordsWrittenMacro-structuralStructure0.182 [0.0256, 0.3384]0.0800
Rhyming words Micro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.1511 [−0.0053, 0.3076]0.0800
Rhyming words Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.0902 [−0.0662, 0.2467]0.0800
Working Memory capacitySentence Span Macro-structuralStructure0.1104 [−0.046, 0.2669]0.0800
Sentence Span Micro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.0701 [−0.0863, 0.2265]0.0800
Sentence Span Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.1409 [−0.0155, 0.2973]0.0800
Working Memory capacityVisual Matrix Macro-structuralStructure0.0902 [−0.0662, 0.2467]0.0800
Visual Matrix Micro-structuralTotal Lexical Production0.1409 [−0.0155, 0.2973]0.0800
Visual Matrix Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.0601 [−0.0964, 0.2165]0.0800
Working Memory capacityMapping Macro-structuralStructure0.01 [−0.1464, 0.1664]0.0800
Mapping Micro-structuralTotal Lexical Production−0.0601 [−0.2165, 0.0964]0.0800
Mapping Micro-structuralMorphosyntactic Complexity0.02 [−0.1364, 0.1764]0.0800
Fernandez et al., 2010 [66]SpainAttention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (n = 26)8.506–11Behavioural InhibitionMatching Familiar Figure TestOralMacro-structuralCoherence0.4236 [0.015, 0.8323]0.2086
Working Memory capacityDigit SpanOral Coherence0.1104 [−0.2982, 05191]0.2086
Interference ControlStroopOral Coherence0.2661 [−0.1426, 0.6748]0.2086
Working Memory capacityRey FigureOral Coherence0.4973 [0.0886, 0.906]0.2086
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Scionti, N.; Zampini, L.; Marzocchi, G.M. The Relationship between Narrative Skills and Executive Functions across Childhood: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Children 2023, 10, 1391. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10081391

AMA Style

Scionti N, Zampini L, Marzocchi GM. The Relationship between Narrative Skills and Executive Functions across Childhood: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Children. 2023; 10(8):1391. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10081391

Chicago/Turabian Style

Scionti, Nicoletta, Laura Zampini, and Gian Marco Marzocchi. 2023. "The Relationship between Narrative Skills and Executive Functions across Childhood: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis" Children 10, no. 8: 1391. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10081391

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop