Home Musical Activities Boost Premature Infants’ Language Development
Abstract
:1. Introduction
Aims and Hypotheses of the Current Study
- (i)
- Test the associations of crucial infant demographic information with language development, such as gestational age and weight at birth, as well as the corrected age (corrected age, or adjusted age, is a premature baby’s chronological age minus the number of weeks or months s/he was born early) at the time when completing the study. Gestational age and corrected age are expected to be positively correlated, and birth weight to be negatively correlated with language development.
- (ii)
- Test the associations of parental variables with language development, such as perinatal stress [71] and parental education. Based on the literature reviewed above, parental education is expected to be positively correlated with infant language outcomes. The effects of perinatal stress are expected to be negatively (e.g., [72]) associated with the dependent measures.
- (iii)
- Investigate the effects of two types of self-reported enrichment on language development, namely (i) home musical activities and, to control for the influence of non-musical enrichment and (ii) book readings and play stimulation. Language development was measured using the MacArthur–Bates Communicative Development Inventories: CDI-UK adaptation Words & Gestures form [73], suitable for the first phase of language acquisition. Specifically, the dependent variables derived from the the CDI were the Comprehension and Gestures scales (e.g., [74]). Based on previous research [48,49,50], we expect that home musical activities will be associated with language outcomes over and above the effect of other enrichment activities.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Design
2.2. Participants
2.3. Materials
- (i)
- The adaptation of the MacArthur–Bates CDI Questionnaire to British English [73], Words & Gestures form, suitable for infants aged 8–18 months. (Cronbach’s α = 0.99 for Comprehension and Production of Words and Cronbach’s α = 0.98 for Gesture). Example item: ‘Things children understand. In the list below, please mark the phrases that your child seems to understand. Are you hungry?’
- (ii)
- Music@Home infant version [75], which is a psychometrically robust questionnaire for the assessment of the home musical environment, comprising 18 items in total, scored on a 7-point agreement scale (Cronbach’s α = 0.87). Items cover activities such as singing and playing with sound and instruments, including toys and parental beliefs. The questionnaire yields an overall Music@Home score (ranging from 18 to 126), with higher scores indicating higher levels of home musical activities. Example item: ‘I make music with my child (including toy instruments) almost every day’.
- (iii)
- STIM-Q infant version [76] (Cronbach’s α = 0.89) was used to measure families’ general enrichment. Specifically, two scales were used that appear particularly relevant for language development: the ‘Reading’ scale (12 questions) referring to reading activities in the home environment, and ‘Parental Involvement in Developmental Advance’ (PIDA: 7 questions) measuring the number of different interactional activities occurring between caregiver and infant. Example item: ‘Do you have the opportunity to point to things around the house and name them for your child?’
- (iv)
- The Perinatal PTSD Questionnaire (PPQ; Cronbach’s α = 0.86) [71] was used to investigate the level of stress and the presence of PTSD symptoms in caregivers. PPQ comprises 14 items scored on a five-point scale, ranging from 0 = not at all to 4 = often, more than a month (e.g., ‘Did you have bad dreams of giving birth or of your baby’s hospital stay?’). Higher scores are indicative of higher levels of stress/PTSD symptomatology. There are three subscales measuring intrusion, avoidance and hyperarousal symptoms.
2.4. Procedure
2.5. Data Analysis
3. Results
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
- (i)
- This study used a simultaneous design, i.e., measures of home musicality and language development were taken at the same point in time. However, it would be important that further research adopts a longitudinal design [120], which could help with identifying crucial time windows for intervention. For example, caregivers of pre-term infants could provide data on home musical and other activities at 6, 14, and 20 months of age of their children, and the relationship of these measures could be studied in association with their early developing language. Indeed, it has been suggested that parental singing at 6 months predicts early language outcomes at 14 months in full-term infants [46]. Initial work could be conducted using online surveys (quantitative) and parental diaries (qualitative), but remote observational methods could be also employed.
- (ii)
- The participants in this study were predominantly highly educated and representative of the middle social class. Therefore, it would be interesting to conduct a similar study, recruiting mainly participants from lower social classes, in order to gain a deeper understanding of the impact that social class has on language development.
- (iii)
- An important implication of the present study is the need to create a partnership with relevant institutions, such as NICUs, baby groups and early childcare settings. This partnership could aim to develop workshops, dedicated to caregivers of preterm infants for developing musicality at home, as well as to provide parents with the information required to facilitate access to those activities (e.g., baby music groups).
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- WHO. Preterm Birth. Available online: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/preterm-birth (accessed on 29 November 2021).
- Foster-Cohen, S.; Edgin, J.O.; Champion, P.R.; Woodward, L.J. Early Delayed Language Development in Very Preterm Infants: Evidence from the MacArthur-Bates CDI. J. Child Lang. 2007, 34, 655–675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Guarini, A.; Sansavini, A.; Fabbri, C.; Alessandroni, R.; Faldella, G.; Karmiloff-Smith, A. Reconsidering the Impact of Preterm Birth on Language Outcome. Early Hum. Dev. 2009, 85, 639–645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jansson-Verkasalo, E.; Valkama, M.; Vainionpää, L.; Pääkkö, E.; Ilkko, E.; Lehtihalmes, M. Language Development in Very Low Birth Weight Preterm Children: A Follow-up Study. Folia Phoniatr. Logop. 2004, 56, 108–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luoma, L.; MD, E.H.; MD, A.M. Speech and Language Development of Children Born at 32 Weeks’ Gestation: A 5-year Prospective Follow-up Study. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 1998, 40, 380–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nguyen, T.-N.-N.; Spencer-Smith, M.; Zannino, D.; Burnett, A.; Scratch, S.E.; Pascoe, L.; Ellis, R.; Cheong, J.; Thompson, D.; Inder, T. Developmental Trajectory of Language from 2 to 13 Years in Children Born Very Preterm. Pediatrics 2018, 141, e20172831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sansavini, A.; Guarini, A.; Alessandroni, R.; Faldella, G.; Giovanelli, G.; Salvioli, G. Early Relations between Lexical and Grammatical Development in Very Immature Italian Preterms. J. Child Lang. 2006, 33, 199–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hirvonen, M.; Ojala, R.; Korhonen, P.; Haataja, P.; Eriksson, K.; Gissler, M.; Luukkaala, T.; Tammela, O. Visual and Hearing Impairments after Preterm Birth. Pediatrics 2018, 142, e20173888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stolt, S.; Klippi, A.; Launonen, K.; Munck, P.; Lehtonen, L.; Lapinleimu, H.; Haataja, L. Size and Composition of the Lexicon in Prematurely Born Very-Low-Birth-Weight and Full-Term Finnish Children at Two Years of Age. J. Child Lang. 2007, 34, 283–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stipdonk, L.W.; Weisglas-Kuperus, N.; Franken, M.J.; Nasserinejad, K.; Dudink, J.; Goedegebure, A. Auditory Brainstem Maturation in Normal-hearing Infants Born Preterm: A Meta-analysis. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2016, 58, 1009–1015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zimmerman, E.; Lahav, A. Ototoxicity in Preterm Infants: Effects of Genetics, Aminoglycosides, and Loud Environmental Noise. J. Perinatol. 2013, 33, 3–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cusson, R.M. Factors Influencing Language Development in Preterm Infants. J. Obstet. Gynecol. Neonatal Nurs. 2003, 32, 402–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wilson, S.; Pietsch, M.; Cordero-Grande, L.; Price, A.N.; Hutter, J.; Xiao, J.; McCabe, L.; Rutherford, M.A.; Hughes, E.J.; Counsell, S.J. Development of Human White Matter Pathways in Utero over the Second and Third Trimester. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2021, 118, e2023598118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stene-Larsen, K.; Brandlistuen, R.E.; Lang, A.M.; Landolt, M.A.; Latal, B.; Vollrath, M.E. Communication Impairments in Early Term and Late Preterm Children: A Prospective Cohort Study Following Children to Age 36 Months. J. Pediatr. 2014, 165, 1123–1128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- D’Odorico, L.; Majorano, M.; Fasolo, M.; Salerni, N.; Suttora, C. Characteristics of Phonological Development as a Risk Factor for Language Development in Italian-Speaking Pre-Term Children: A Longitudinal Study. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 2011, 25, 53–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gayraud, F.; Kern, S. Influence of Preterm Birth on Early Lexical and Grammatical Acquisition. First Lang. 2007, 27, 159–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luu, T.M.; Vohr, B.R.; Allan, W.; Schneider, K.C.; Ment, L.R. Evidence for Catch-up in Cognition and Receptive Vocabulary among Adolescents Born Very Preterm. Pediatrics 2011, 128, 313–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Luu, T.M.; Vohr, B.R.; Schneider, K.C.; Katz, K.H.; Tucker, R.; Allan, W.C.; Ment, L.R. Trajectories of Receptive Language Development from 3 to 12 Years of Age for Very Preterm Children. Pediatrics 2009, 124, 333–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- De Jong, M.; Verhoeven, M.; van Baar, A.L. School Outcome, Cognitive Functioning, and Behaviour Problems in Moderate and Late Preterm Children and Adults: A Review. In Seminars in Fetal and Neonatal Medicine; WB Saunders: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2012; Volume 17, pp. 163–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moon, C. Prenatal Experience with the Maternal Voice. In Early Vocal Contact and Preterm Infant Brain Development; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2017; pp. 25–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Filippa, M.; Panza, C.; Ferrari, F.; Frassoldati, R.; Kuhn, P.; Balduzzi, S.; D’Amico, R. Systematic Review of Maternal Voice Interventions Demonstrates Increased Stability in Preterm Infants. Acta Paediatr. 2017, 106, 1220–1229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Saliba, S.; Esseily, R.; Filippa, M.; Kuhn, P.; Gratier, M. Exposure to Human Voices Has Beneficial Effects on Preterm Infants in the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit. Acta Paediatr. 2018, 107, 1122–1130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Webb, A.R.; Heller, H.T.; Benson, C.B.; Lahav, A. Mother’s Voice and Heartbeat Sounds Elicit Auditory Plasticity in the Human Brain before Full Gestation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2015, 112, 3152–3157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chifa, M.; Hadar, T.; Politimou, N.; Reynolds, G.; Franco, F. The Soundscape of Neonatal Intensive Care: A Mixed-Methods Study of the Parents’ Experience. Children 2021, 8, 644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yue, W.; Han, X.; Luo, J.; Zeng, Z.; Yang, M. Effect of Music Therapy on Preterm Infants in Neonatal Intensive Care Unit: Systematic Review and Meta-analysis of Randomized Controlled Trials. J. Adv. Nurs. 2021, 77, 635–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- VandenBerg, K.A. Individualized Developmental Care for High Risk Newborns in the NICU: A Practice Guideline. Early Hum. Dev. 2007, 83, 433–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gray, L.; Philbin, M.K. Effects of the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit on Auditory Attention and Distraction. Clin. Perinatol. 2004, 31, 243–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Smith, S.W.; Ortmann, A.J.; Clark, W.W. Noise in the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit: A New Approach to Examining Acoustic Events. Noise Health 2018, 20, 121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scala, M.; Seo, S.; Lee-Park, J.; McClure, C.; Scala, M.; Palafoutas, J.J.; Abubakar, K. Effect of Reading to Preterm Infants on Measures of Cardiorespiratory Stability in the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit. J. Perinatol. 2018, 38, 1536–1541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scala, M.L.; Marchman, V.A.; Godenzi, C.; Gao, C.; Travis, K.E. Assessing Speech Exposure in the NICU: Implications for Speech Enrichment for Preterm Infants. J. Perinatol. 2020, 40, 1537–1545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Filippa, M.; Monaci, M.G.; Spagnuolo, C.; Serravalle, P.; Daniele, R.; Grandjean, D. Maternal Speech Decreases Pain Scores and Increases Oxytocin Levels in Preterm Infants during Painful Procedures. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 17301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Restin, T.; Gaspar, M.; Bassler, D.; Kurtcuoglu, V.; Scholkmann, F.; Haslbeck, F.B. Newborn Incubators Do Not Protect from High Noise Levels in the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit and Are Relevant Noise Sources by Themselves. Children 2021, 8, 704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Suttora, C.; Spinelli, M.; Monzani, D. From Prematurity to Parenting Stress: The Mediating Role of Perinatal Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2014, 11, 478–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lam-Cassettari, C.; Kohlhoff, J. Effect of Maternal Depression on Infant-Directed Speech to Prelinguistic Infants: Implications for Language Development. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0236787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- D’Odorico, L.; Jacob, V. Prosodic and Lexical Aspects of Maternal Linguistic Input to Late-talking Toddlers. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2006, 41, 293–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ramírez, N.F.; Lytle, S.R.; Kuhl, P.K. Parent Coaching Increases Conversational Turns and Advances Infant Language Development. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2020, 117, 3484–3491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ramírez-Esparza, N.; García-Sierra, A.; Kuhl, P.K. The Impact of Early Social Interactions on Later Language Development in Spanish–English Bilingual Infants. Child Dev. 2017, 88, 1216–1234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Filippa, M.; Gratier, M.; Devouche, E.; Grandjean, D. Changes in Infant-Directed Speech and Song Are Related to Preterm Infant Facial Expression in the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit. Interact. Stud. 2018, 19, 427–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oyetunji, A.; Chandra, P. Postpartum Stress and Infant Outcome: A Review of Current Literature. Psychiatry Res. 2020, 284, 112769. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Slomian, J.; Honvo, G.; Emonts, P.; Reginster, J.-Y.; Bruyère, O. Consequences of Maternal Postpartum Depression: A Systematic Review of Maternal and Infant Outcomes. Womens Health 2019, 15, 1745506519844044. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gueron-Sela, N.; Atzaba-Poria, N.; Meiri, G.; Marks, K. The Caregiving Environment and Developmental Outcomes of Preterm Infants: Diathesis Stress or Differential Susceptibility Effects? Child Dev. 2015, 86, 1014–1030. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wild, K.T.; Betancourt, L.M.; Brodsky, N.L.; Hurt, H. The Effect of Socioeconomic Status on the Language Outcome of Preterm Infants at Toddler Age. Early Hum. Dev. 2013, 89, 743–746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Patra, K.; Greene, M.M.; Patel, A.L.; Meier, P. Maternal Education Level Predicts Cognitive, Language, and Motor Outcome in Preterm Infants in the Second Year of Life. Am. J. Perinatol. 2016, 33, 738–744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sentenac, M.; Johnson, S.; Charkaluk, M.-L.; Sëppanen, A.-V.; Aden, U.; Cuttini, M.; Maier, R.; Mannamaa, M.; Zeitlin, J. Maternal Education and Language Development at 2 Years Corrected Age in Children Born Very Preterm: Results from a European Population-Based Cohort Study. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2020, 74, 346–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Neuhauser, A.; Ramseier, E.; Schaub, S.; Burkhardt, S.C.; Lanfranchi, A. Mediating Role of Maternal Sensitivity: Enhancing Language Development in At-risk Families. Infant Ment. Health J. 2018, 39, 522–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Franco, F.; Suttora, C.; Spinelli, M.; Kozar, I.; Fasolo, M. Singing to Infants Matters: Early Singing Interactions Affect Musical Preferences and Facilitate Vocabulary Building. J. Child Lang. 2022, 49, 552–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gerry, D.; Unrau, A.; Trainor, L.J. Active Music Classes in Infancy Enhance Musical, Communicative and Social Development. Dev. Sci. 2012, 15, 398–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Papadimitriou, A.; Smyth, C.; Politimou, N.; Franco, F.; Stewart, L. The Impact of the Home Musical Environment on Infants’ Language Development. Infant Behav. Dev. 2021, 65, 101651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Politimou, N.; Dalla Bella, S.; Farrugia, N.; Franco, F. Born to Speak and Sing: Musical Predictors of Language Development in Pre-Schoolers. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 450640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pino, M.C.; Giancola, M.; D’Amico, S. The Association between Music and Language in Children: A State-of-the-Art Review. Children 2023, 10, 801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- D’Odorico, L.; Franco, F.; Vidotto, G. Temporal Characteristics in Infant Cry and Non-Cry Vocalizations. Lang. Speech 1985, 28, 29–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Locke, J.L.; Bogin, B. Language and Life History: A New Perspective on the Development and Evolution of Human Language. Behav. Brain Sci. 2006, 29, 259–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- D’Odorico, L.; Franco, F. Selective Production of Vocalization Types in Different Communication Contexts. J. Child Lang. 1991, 18, 475–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Snow, D. Regression and Reorganization of Intonation between 6 and 23 Months. Child Dev. 2006, 77, 281–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wermke, K.; Mende, W. Melody as a Primordial Legacy from Early Roots of Language. Behav. Brain Sci. 2006, 29, 300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldstein, M.H.; King, A.P.; West, M.J. Social Interaction Shapes Babbling: Testing Parallels between Birdsong and Speech. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2003, 100, 8030–8035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuhl, P.K. Early Language Acquisition: Cracking the Speech Code. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2004, 5, 831–843. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Falk, S.; Fasolo, M.; Genovese, G.; Romero-Lauro, L.; Franco, F. Sing for Me, Mama! Infants’ Discrimination of Novel Vowels in Song. Infancy 2021, 26, 248–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- François, C.; Teixidó, M.; Takerkart, S.; Agut, T.; Bosch, L.; Rodriguez-Fornells, A. Enhanced Neonatal Brain Responses to Sung Streams Predict Vocabulary Outcomes by Age 18 Months. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 12451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fujioka, T.; Ross, B.; Kakigi, R.; Pantev, C.; Trainor, L.J. One Year of Musical Training Affects Development of Auditory Cortical-Evoked Fields in Young Children. Brain 2006, 129, 2593–2608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schön, D.; Boyer, M.; Moreno, S.; Besson, M.; Peretz, I.; Kolinsky, R. Songs as an Aid for Language Acquisition. Cognition 2008, 106, 975–983. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sénéchal, M.; Pagan, S.; Lever, R.; Ouellette, G.P. Relations among the Frequency of Shared Reading and 4-Year-Old Children’s Vocabulary, Morphological and Syntax Comprehension, and Narrative Skills. Early Educ. Dev. 2008, 19, 27–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baker, C.E.; Cameron, C.E.; Rimm-Kaufman, S.E.; Grissmer, D. Family and Sociodemographic Predictors of School Readiness among African American Boys in Kindergarten. Early Educ. Dev. 2012, 23, 833–854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chazan-Cohen, R.; Raikes, H.; Brooks-Gunn, J.; Ayoub, C.; Pan, B.A.; Kisker, E.E.; Roggman, L.; Fuligni, A.S. Low-Income Children’s School Readiness: Parent Contributions over the First Five Years. Early Educ. Dev. 2009, 20, 958–977. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bus, A.G.; Van Ijzendoorn, M.H.; Pellegrini, A.D. Joint Book Reading Makes for Success in Learning to Read: A Meta-Analysis on Intergenerational Transmission of Literacy. Rev. Educ. Res. 1995, 65, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mol, S.E.; Bus, A.G.; De Jong, M.T.; Smeets, D.J. Added Value of Dialogic Parent–Child Book Readings: A Meta-Analysis. Early Educ. Dev. 2008, 19, 7–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flack, Z.M.; Field, A.P.; Horst, J.S. The Effects of Shared Storybook Reading on Word Learning: A Meta-Analysis. Dev. Psychol. 2018, 54, 1334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Niklas, F.; Cohrssen, C.; Tayler, C. The Sooner, the Better: Early Reading to Children. Sage Open 2016, 6, 2158244016672715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duursma, A.E. The Effects of Fathers’ and Mothers’ Reading to Their Children on Language Outcomes of Children Participating in Early Head Start in the United States. J. Theory Res. About Men Parents. 2014, 12, 283–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Neri, E.; De Pascalis, L.; Agostini, F.; Genova, F.; Biasini, A.; Stella, M.; Trombini, E. Parental Book-Reading to Preterm Born Infants in NICU: The Effects on Language Development in the First Two Years. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health 2021, 18, 11361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Callahan, J.; Borja, S.; Hynan, M. Modification of the Perinatal PTSD Questionnaire to Enhance Clinical Utility. J. Perinatol. 2006, 26, 533–539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goodman, S.H.; Rouse, M.H.; Connell, A.M.; Broth, M.R.; Hall, C.M.; Heyward, D. Maternal Depression and Child Psychopathology: A Meta-Analytic Review. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 2011, 14, 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alcock, K.; Meints, K.; Rowland, C. The UK Communicative Development Inventories: Words and Gestures; J&R Press: Havant, UK, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Duff, F.J.; Reen, G.; Plunkett, K.; Nation, K. Do Infant Vocabulary Skills Predict School-age Language and Literacy Outcomes? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2015, 56, 848–856. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Politimou, N.; Stewart, L.; Müllensiefen, D.; Franco, F. Music@ Home: A Novel Instrument to Assess the Home Musical Environment in the Early Years. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0193819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dreyer, B.P.; Mendelsohn, A.L.; Tamis-LeMonda, C.S. Assessing the Child’s Cognitive Home Environment through Parental Report; Reliability and Validity. Early Dev. Parent. Int. J. Res. Pract. 1996, 5, 271–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Qualtrics: Online Survey Software. Available online: https://www.qualtrics.com (accessed on 20 October 2020).
- Kirkpatrick, L.A. A Simple Guide to IBM SPSS Statistics-Version 23.0; Cengage Learning: Boston, MA, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Kang, H. Sample Size Determination and Power Analysis Using the G* Power Software. J. Educ. Eval. Health Prof. 2021, 18, 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Suresh, K.; Chandrashekara, S. Sample Size Estimation and Power Analysis for Clinical Research Studies. J. Hum. Reprod. Sci. 2012, 5, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pérez-Pereira, M. Prevalence of Language Delay among Healthy Preterm Children, Language Outcomes and Predictive Factors. Children 2021, 8, 282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pérez-Pereira, M.; Fernández, P.; Gómez-Taibo, M.L.; Resches, M. Language Development of Low Risk Preterm Infants up to the Age of 30 Months. Early Hum. Dev. 2014, 90, 649–656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Suttora, C.; Guarini, A.; Zuccarini, M.; Aceti, A.; Corvaglia, L.; Sansavini, A. Speech and Language Skills of Low-Risk Preterm and Full-Term Late Talkers: The Role of Child Factors and Parent Input. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health 2020, 17, 7684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schaal, N.K.; Politimou, N.; Franco, F.; Stewart, L.; Müllensiefen, D. The German Music@ Home: Validation of a Questionnaire Measuring at Home Musical Exposure and Interaction of Young Children. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0235923. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Williams, K.E.; Barrett, M.S.; Welch, G.F.; Abad, V.; Broughton, M. Associations between Early Shared Music Activities in the Home and Later Child Outcomes: Findings from the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children. Early Child. Res. Q. 2015, 31, 113–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benassi, E.; Savini, S.; Iverson, J.M.; Guarini, A.; Caselli, M.C.; Alessandroni, R.; Faldella, G.; Sansavini, A. Early Communicative Behaviors and Their Relationship to Motor Skills in Extremely Preterm Infants. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2016, 48, 132–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ortiz-Mantilla, S.; Choudhury, N.; Leevers, H.; Benasich, A.A. Understanding Language and Cognitive Deficits in Very Low Birth Weight Children. Dev. Psychobiol. J. Int. Soc. Dev. Psychobiol. 2008, 50, 107–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ross, G.; Demaria, R.; Yap, V. The Relationship between Motor Delays and Language Development in Very Low Birthweight Premature Children at 18 Months Corrected Age. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2018, 61, 114–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- D’Agostino, J.A. An Evidentiary Review Regarding the Use of Chronological and Adjusted Age in the Assessment of Preterm Infants. J. Spec. Pediatr. Nurs. 2010, 15, 26–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harel-Gadassi, A.; Friedlander, E.; Yaari, M.; Bar-Oz, B.; Eventov-Friedman, S.; Mankuta, D.; Yirmiya, N. Developmental Assessment of Preterm Infants: Chronological or Corrected Age? Res. Dev. Disabil. 2018, 80, 35–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fasolo, M.; D’odorico, L.; Costantini, A.; Cassibba, R. The Influence of Biological, Social, and Developmental Factors on Language Acquisition in Pre-Term Born Children. Int. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 2010, 12, 461–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Suttora, C.; Salerni, N. Gestural Development and Its Relation to Language Acquisition in Very Preterm Children. Infant Behav. Dev. 2012, 35, 429–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sansavini, A.; Guarini, A.; Savini, S.; Broccoli, S.; Justice, L.; Alessandroni, R.; Faldella, G. Longitudinal Trajectories of Gestural and Linguistic Abilities in Very Preterm Infants in the Second Year of Life. Neuropsychologia 2011, 49, 3677–3688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cattani, A.; Bonifacio, S.; Fertz, M.; Iverson, J.M.; Zocconi, E.; Caselli, M.C. Communicative and Linguistic Development in Preterm Children: A Longitudinal Study from 12 to 24 Months. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2010, 45, 162–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caselli, M.C.; Casadio, P. Il Primo Vocabolario Del Bambino: Guida All’uso Del Questionario MacArthur per La Valutazione Della Comunicazione e Del Linguaggio Nei Primi Anni Di Vita; FrancoAngeli: Milan, Italy, 1995; Volume 5. [Google Scholar]
- Lefèvre, J.; Germanaud, D.; Dubois, J.; Rousseau, F.; de Macedo Santos, I.; Angleys, H.; Mangin, J.-F.; Hüppi, P.S.; Girard, N.; De Guio, F. Are Developmental Trajectories of Cortical Folding Comparable between Cross-Sectional Datasets of Fetuses and Preterm Newborns? Cereb. Cortex 2015, 26, 3023–3035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nino, G.; Govindan, R.B.; Al-Shargabi, T.; Metzler, M.; Massaro, A.N.; Perez, G.F.; McCarter, R.; Hunt, C.E.; du Plessis, A.J. Premature Infants Rehospitalized Because of an Apparent Life-Threatening Event Had Distinctive Autonomic Developmental Trajectories. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 2016, 194, 379–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thomas, M.S.; Annaz, D.; Ansari, D.; Scerif, G.; Jarrold, C.; Karmiloff-Smith, A. Using Developmental Trajectories to Understand Developmental Disorders. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2009, 52, 336–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McMahon, C.A.; Boivin, J.; Gibson, F.L.; Hammarberg, K.; Wynter, K.; Fisher, J.R. Older maternal age and major depressive episodes in the first two years after birth: Findings from the Parental Age and Transition to Parenthood Australia (PATPA) study. J. Affect. Disord. 2015, 175, 454–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ngum Chi Watts, M.C.; Liamputtong, P.; Mcmichael, C. Early Motherhood: A Qualitative Study Exploring the Experiences of African Australian Teenage Mothers in Greater Melbourne, Australia. BMC Public Health 2015, 15, 873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mirowsky, J.; Ross, C.E. Depression, Parenthood, and Age at First Birth. Soc. Sci. Med. 2002, 54, 1281–1298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mangeli, M.; Rayyani, M.; Cheraghi, M.A.; Tirgari, B. Exploring the Challenges of Adolescent Mothers from Their Life Experiences in the Transition to Motherhood: A Qualitative Study. J. Fam. Reprod. Health 2017, 11, 165. [Google Scholar]
- Nyström, K.; Axelsson, K. Mothers’ Experience of Being Separated from Their Newborns. J. Obstet. Gynecol. Neonatal Nurs. 2002, 31, 275–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mensah, F.K.; Kiernan, K.E. Parents’ Mental Health and Children’s Cognitive and Social Development. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2010, 45, 1023–1035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Madigan, S.; Browne, D.; Racine, N.; Mori, C.; Tough, S. Association between Screen Time and Children’s Performance on a Developmental Screening Test. JAMA Pediatr. 2019, 173, 244–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Foster-Cohen, S.H.; Friesen, M.D.; Champion, P.R.; Woodward, L.J. High Prevalence/Low Severity Language Delay in Preschool Children Born Very Preterm. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2010, 31, 658–667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brady, N.; Warren, S.F.; Sterling, A. Interventions Aimed at Improving Child Language by Improving Maternal Responsivity. Int. Rev. Res. Ment. Retard. 2009, 37, 333–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trehub, S.E.; Hill, D.S.; Kamenetsky, S.B. Parents’ Sung Performances for Infants. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. Can. Psychol. Expérimentale 1997, 51, 385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Abad, V.; Edwards, J. Strengthening Families: A Role for Music Therapy in Contributing to Family Centred Care. Aust. J. Music Ther. 2004, 15, 3–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ettenberger, M.; Ardila, Y.M.B. Music Therapy Song Writing with Mothers of Preterm Babies in the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit (NICU)–A Mixed-Methods Pilot Study. Arts Psychother. 2018, 58, 42–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Loewy, J. NICU Music Therapy: Song of Kin as Critical Lullaby in Research and Practice. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2015, 1337, 178–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lordier, L.; Meskaldji, D.-E.; Grouiller, F.; Pittet, M.P.; Vollenweider, A.; Vasung, L.; Borradori-Tolsa, C.; Lazeyras, F.; Grandjean, D.; Van De Ville, D. Music in Premature Infants Enhances High-Level Cognitive Brain Networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2019, 116, 12103–12108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stamou, L.; Evaggelou, F.; Stamou, V.; Diamanti, E.; Loewy, J.V. The Effects of Live Singing on the Biophysiological Functions of Preterm Infants Hospitalized in a Neonatal Intensive Care Unit in Greece: A Pilot Study. Music Med. 2020, 12, 109–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kehl, S.M.; Marca-Ghaemmaghami, L.; Haller, M.; Pichler-Stachl, E.; Bucher, H.U.; Bassler, D.; Haslbeck, F.B. Creative Music Therapy with Premature Infants and Their Parents: A Mixed-Method Pilot Study on Parents’ Anxiety, Stress and Depressive Symptoms and Parent–Infant Attachment. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health 2021, 18, 265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Caitlin Van Ness, M. Effects of Music on Physiological and Behavioral Indices of Acute Pain and Stress in Premature Infants: Clinical Trial and Literature Review. Music Med. 2011, 3, 72–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Virtala, P.; Partanen, E. Can Very Early Music Interventions Promote At-risk Infants’ Development? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2018, 1423, 92–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Costantini, A.; Cassibba, R.; Coppola, G.; Castoro, G. Attachment Security and Language Development in an Italian Sample: The Role of Premature Birth and Maternal Language. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2012, 36, 85–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Day, C. Attachment and Early Language Development: Implications for Early Intervention. NHSA Dialog 2007, 10, 143–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Norman, H.; Elliot, M.; Fagan, C. Which Fathers Are the Most Involved in Taking Care of Their Toddlers in the UK? An Investigation of the Predictors of Paternal Involvement. Community Work Fam. 2014, 17, 163–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sansavini, A.; Savini, S.; Guarini, A.; Broccoli, S.; Alessandroni, R.; Faldella, G. The Effect of Gestational Age on Developmental Outcomes: A Longitudinal Study in the First 2 Years of Life. Child Care Health Dev. 2011, 37, 26–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
M | SD | Min | Max | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Maternal age (years) | 31.03 | 5.65 | 20 | 44 |
Gestational age at birth of the children (weeks) | 30.60 | 3.54 | 22 | 36 |
Days spent in the NICU (N) | 47.71 | 49.89 | 0 | 242 |
Infant birth weight (g) | 1542.85 | 639.60 | 368 | 3040 |
Corrected age of the infants when the survey was completed (months) | 13.38 | 3.39 | 8 | 18 |
Gender of the Children | Siblings | Categories of Prematurity | Categories of Birth Weight | Status |
---|---|---|---|---|
55 (47.0%) Female | 43 (36.8%) Yes | 31 (26.5%) Extremely preterm | 31 (26.5%) ELBW | 95 (81.2%) Singleton |
62 (53.0) Male | 74 (63.2%) No | 39 (33.3) Very preterm | 27 (23.1) Very low birth weight | 21 (17.9%) Twins |
47 (40.2) Moderate to late preterm | 53 (45.3) Low birth weight | 1 (0.9%) Triplets | ||
6 (5.1) Normal birth weight |
Gender of the Respondents | Nationality of Participants | Highest Level of Education Achieved | Years Spent in Education | Occupational Groups of Respondents |
---|---|---|---|---|
117 (100%) Female | 81 (69.2 %) British | 3 (2.6%) Post-graduate (PhD or doctorate) | 50 (42.7%) 14–18 | 34 (29.1%) Intermediate managerial/professional |
18 (15.4%) American | 28 (23.9%) Post-graduate (Master’s degree or equivalent) | 29 (24.8%) Over 18 | 31 (26.5%) Supervisory or clerical/junior managerial | |
7 (6%) Caucasian | 61 (52.1%) College or University | 17 (14.5%) 12–14 | 18 (15.4%) Skilled manual worker | |
5 (4.3%) Australian | 17 (14.5%) Higher or secondary education | 16 (13.7%) 9–12 | 10 (8.5%) Higher managerial/administrative | |
6 (5.1%) Canadian | 8 (6.9%) Secondary school up to 16 years of age | 4 (3.4%) 6–9 | 9 (7.7%) Homemaker | |
1 (0.9%) Less than 6 | 15 (12.8%) Other |
Mean | Median | Std. Dev | Range | |
---|---|---|---|---|
CDI-UK Comprehension | 162.18 | 129.00 | 123.11 | 2.00–429.00 |
CDI-UK Gesture | 32.18 | 32.00 | 19.03 | 2.00–75.00 |
CDI-UK Production | 39.35 | 10.50 | 72.99 | 0–397.00 |
Music@Home general factor | 98.77 | 99.00 | 15.10 | 66.00–126.00 |
STIMQ: reading scores | 12.31 | 12.00 | 2.81 | 3.00–19.00 |
STIMQ: PIDA scores | 5.52 | 6.00 | 1.41 | 1.00–7.00 |
PPQ: overall scores | 25.71 | 28.00 | 12.18 | 0–50.00 |
PPQ: intrusiveness | 5.68 | 5.00 | 3.29 | 0–12.00 |
PPQ: avoidance | 9.06 | 9.00 | 4.86 | 0–19.00 |
PPQ: arousal | 10.96 | 11.00 | 5.77 | 0–20.00 |
Gestational age (weeks) | 30.60 | 31.00 | 3.54 | 22.00–36.00 |
Birth weight (grams) | 1542.85 | 1559.00 | 639.60 | 368.00–3040.00 |
Days spent in the NICU | 47.71 | 30.00 | 49.89 | 0–242.00 |
Maternal education (levels 1) | 4.69 | 5.00 | 1.14 | 1.00–6.00 |
Corrected infants’ age (months) | 13.15 | 14.00 | 3.43 | 8.00–18.00 |
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | 13 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Gestational age | |||||||||||||
2. Birth weight | 0.85 ** | ||||||||||||
3. Infants’ age (corrected) | 0.13 | 0.08 | |||||||||||
4. Days spent in the NICU | −0.81 ** | −0.67 ** | −0.06 | ||||||||||
5. Maternal age | 0.07 | 0.07 | −0.12 | −0.04 | |||||||||
6. CDI-UK Comprehension | 0.09 | 0.05 | 0.61 ** | −0.06 | −0.28 ** | ||||||||
7. CDI-UK Production | 0.11 | 0.08 | 0.51 ** | −0.09 | −0.18 | 0.66 ** | |||||||
8. CDI-UK Gesture | 0.26 ** | 0.21 * | 0.78 ** | −0.14 | −0.11 | 0.82 ** | 62 ** | ||||||
9. M@H general factor | −0.01 | 0.05 | 0.17 | 0.08 | 0.04 | 0.25 ** | 0.20 * | 0.33 ** | |||||
10. STIMQ-reading | 0.05 | −0.01 | 0.13 | 0.04 | −0.02 | 0.08 | −0.07 | 0.17 | 0.00 | ||||
11. STIMQ-PIDA | 0.12 | 0.08 | 0.39 ** | −0.11 | −0.07 | 0.27 ** | 0.17 | 0.38 ** | 0.21 * | 0.38 ** | |||
12. PPQ-overall score | −0.01 | 0.03 | −0.05 | 0.04 | −0.32 ** | 0.01 | −0.11 | −0.12 | 0.00 | 0.11 | 0.16 | ||
13. Maternal education | 0.11 | 0.09 | −0.07 | −0.07 | 0.35 ** | 0.08 | 0.05 | 0.13 | 0.21 * | 0.04 | −0.07 | −0.06 |
β | t | p | R2 | F | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1 | 0.39 | 14.28 | 0.000 | |||
Music@Home general factor | 0.15 | 1.99 | 0.04 | |||
Infants’ age (corrected) | 0.57 | 7.53 | 0.00 | |||
Gestational age | 0.07 | 0.49 | 0.62 | |||
Birth weight STIMQ-PIDA | −0.06 −0.01 | −0.45 −0.20 | 0.65 0.84 | |||
Model 4 | 0.39 | 36.41 | 0.000 | |||
Music@Home general factor | 0.15 | 2.06 | 0.04 | |||
Infants’ age (corrected) | 0.58 | 7.80 | 0.00 |
β | t | p | R2 | F | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1 | 0.68 | 48.26 | 0.000 | |||
Music@Home general factor | 0.21 | 3.87 | 0.00 | |||
Infants’ age (corrected) | 0.72 | 13.15 | 0.00 | |||
Gestational age | 0.18 | 1.81 | 0.07 | |||
Birth weight STIMQ-PIDA | −0.02 0.02 | −0.19 0.40 | 0.84 0.68 | |||
Model 3 | 0.68 | 81.67 | 0.000 | |||
Music@Home general factor | 0.21 | 3.91 | 0.00 | |||
Infants’ age (corrected) | 0.72 | 13.45 | 0.00 | |||
Gestational age | 0.16 | 3.13 | 0.00 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Franco, F.; Chifa, M.; Politimou, N. Home Musical Activities Boost Premature Infants’ Language Development. Children 2024, 11, 542. https://doi.org/10.3390/children11050542
Franco F, Chifa M, Politimou N. Home Musical Activities Boost Premature Infants’ Language Development. Children. 2024; 11(5):542. https://doi.org/10.3390/children11050542
Chicago/Turabian StyleFranco, Fabia, Maria Chifa, and Nina Politimou. 2024. "Home Musical Activities Boost Premature Infants’ Language Development" Children 11, no. 5: 542. https://doi.org/10.3390/children11050542
APA StyleFranco, F., Chifa, M., & Politimou, N. (2024). Home Musical Activities Boost Premature Infants’ Language Development. Children, 11(5), 542. https://doi.org/10.3390/children11050542