Next Article in Journal
Comparative Study of Acute Stress in Infants Undergoing Percutaneous Achilles Tenotomy for Clubfoot vs. Peripheral Line Placement
Next Article in Special Issue
Beyond Language Scores: How Language Exposure Informs Assessment of Nonword Repetition, Vocabulary and Narrative Macrostructure in Bilingual Turkish/Swedish Children with and without Developmental Language Disorder
Previous Article in Journal
Clinical Characteristics of Offspring Born to Parents with Type 2 Diabetes Diagnosed in Youth: Observations from TODAY
Previous Article in Special Issue
Indices of Narrative Language Associated with Disability
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Effects of Language Background and Parental Education on Measures of Cognitive Ability: An Analysis of the WPPSI-IV Cognitive Profiles of Monolingual, Simultaneous Bilingual, and Sequential Bilingual German Children Aged 4 to 7 Years

1
Department of Medicine, Medical School Hamburg, 20457 Hamburg, Germany
2
Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, Helmut-Schmidt-University/University of the Federal Armed Forces, 22043 Hamburg, Germany
3
Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Helmut-Schmidt-University/University of the Federal Armed Forces, 22043 Hamburg, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Children 2024, 11(6), 631; https://doi.org/10.3390/children11060631
Submission received: 4 May 2024 / Revised: 16 May 2024 / Accepted: 22 May 2024 / Published: 24 May 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Developmental Language Disorder in Children and Adolescents)

Abstract

:
Background: The present study investigated the possible effects of language background (monolinguals, simultaneous bilinguals, and sequential bilinguals) and parental education (no/low, medium, high, and highest parental education) on measures of cognitive ability provided by the Wechsler Primary and Preschool Scale—Fourth Edition (WPPSI-IV). Methods: Statistical analyses were based on a sample of 290 children (130 females, 160 males). Three multivariate variance analyses were conducted to identify possible effects. In cases of statistically significant main effects, post hoc analyses were additionally performed to identify group differences. Results: The results indicated that simultaneous bilinguals performed more similarly to monolinguals than sequential bilinguals. On average, sequential bilinguals achieved significantly lower scores on the Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI), the Vocabulary Acquisition Index (VAI), and the associated subtests than monolinguals and simultaneous bilinguals. Significantly lower average scores on VAI and the associated subtests were found for simultaneous bilinguals compared to monolinguals. Children with parents having no, a lower, or a medium educational level achieved significantly lower scores on VCI, VAI, and the FSIQ than children with parents having a high or highest educational level on average. Conclusions: The present findings suggest that the WPPSI-IV represents a suitable and reliable test battery for the assessment of cognitive skills in children with different language backgrounds and parental educational levels.

1. Introduction

In recent years, one of the most highly debated issues in research on bilingualism is whether bilingualism might have a beneficial or detrimental effect on the development of cognitive abilities [1]. To this very day, however, research focusing on this issue is characterized by rather inconsistent results, especially for children at young ages. Many of those inconsistencies across studies in this field may yield from the application of different methodological approaches, variations in sample selection, the use of different cognitive tests as well as the use of heterogeneous definitions of bilingualism, and the investigation of a variety of different cognitive abilities.

1.1. Bilingualism

In general, language users can be grouped into monolinguals, bilinguals, and multilinguals. Based on the prefixes of those terms, it can already be concluded that monolingual individuals acquire one language, bilingual ones acquire two languages, and multilingual individuals acquire more than two languages during language development. Bilinguals can be further divided into two groups, namely, those children who learn two languages simultaneously or those who learn two languages sequentially [2]. In particular, a simultaneous bilingual child can be described as an individual who acquires two languages at the same time and whose development of both languages starts in early childhood. By contrast, a sequential bilingual child can be described as an individual who learns the first language at home and is exposed to a second language later, for example, upon admittance to kindergarten or school [3].
Regarding the appropriate classification of bilinguals as either simultaneous or sequential language learners, previous studies have discussed and used age cut-offs for defining these two categories of bilingualism. For instance, McLaughlin [4] set the minimum cut-off point for the categorization into simultaneous and sequential bilinguals at age three due to the assumption that children who acquire a language before the age of three should have a considerable advantage in the according language acquisition. Others argued that the minimum cut-off point of age three may be too late for analyzing meaningful differences between the two categories. This is due to the fact that developmental differences may likely be suggested even between children who acquire two languages since birth and those who begin to learn one language between ages two and three [5,6]. Based on this, DeHouwer [7] proposed a more restricted categorization, suggesting that simultaneous bilinguals are those who were exposed to both languages from birth. A little less restrictive, Paradis, Genesee, and Crago [8] defined simultaneous bilinguals as those who learn both languages between birth and the age of one year.

1.2. Bilingualism and Verbal Abilities

Language acquisition is a complex process and its critical phase lies in the early lifespan of childhood [9]. Bilingual children often show reduced language abilities at the end of preschool [10,11] and regularly exhibit delayed language development as well as less pronounced language comprehension and a smaller receptive as well as expressive vocabulary [12]. On average, bilinguals show significantly smaller and more context-specific vocabularies in both languages than monolinguals. If a bilingual child speaks German at school only, for example, context-specific words associated with school, such as academic activities, objects, and subject matter, may only be known in the context-specific language (e.g., German). In this case, bilinguals show better performances in tests of expressive and receptive vocabulary in their language of instruction rather than their mother tongue because they have not built up a context-specific vocabulary in their native language [13].
In general, several studies postulate that simultaneous bilinguals perform more similarly to monolinguals than sequential bilinguals on linguistic measures, except when sequential bilinguals learn their second language in early childhood [14]. The language acquisition of children who grow up speaking two or more languages from birth is, therefore, very similar to that of monolinguals. Although bilingual children have two separate linguistic systems, those systems are closely linked [15,16,17,18].
Previous studies have already demonstrated that bilingual children often tend to score lower on receptive vocabulary tests than monolingual children of the same age in one or both languages [19,20,21]. Even though a relatively smaller vocabulary has often been found for bilingual children when comparing both languages separately with the vocabulary of monolinguals, bilinguals are suggested to not suffer from a substantial disadvantage in their language development [22,23].

1.3. Bilingualism and Non-Verbal Cognitive Abilities

Regarding non-verbal cognitive abilities, numerous studies have already investigated a variety of different performances of bilinguals on tasks requiring executive functioning, memory, and spatial reasoning. Even though research findings are somewhat mixed so far, it can be assumed that bilinguals may have an advantage over monolinguals, especially in solving non-verbal tasks requiring high levels of cognitive monitoring. In contrast, there are no findings that postulate that bilinguals have an advantage in tasks measuring different executive functions that require lower levels of cognitive monitoring, such as in tasks of impulse control [24,25].
Another cognitive domain of interest in research on bilingualism and non-verbal cognitive abilities is working memory. Given that research on visuospatial working memory and bilingualism has delivered consistent results [26] but the total number of studies is rather small so far, current findings on that topic still remain to be reproduced before any final conclusions can be drawn. For instance, Garcia and colleagues [27] found that bilingual children outperformed monolingual children on tasks measuring visuospatial working memory tasks, while there was no significant group difference on tasks measuring verbal working memory. The effects of bilingualism on verbal working memory even remained after controlling for demographic variables such as age, sex, socioeconomic status, and IQ. Furthermore, the authors investigated possible differences in working memory between monolingual and bilingual preschoolers with disruptive behavior disorders and concluded that bilingualism might likely be a protective factor for preschoolers with disruptive behavior disorders. In general, there are more comprehensive studies available on working memory capacity for verbal stimuli than for visual or visuospatial stimuli, but the findings on how bilingualism may affect verbal working memory are quite inconsistent. While some studies have reported an advantage for bilingual children when performing a task of working memory involving verbal stimuli [24], others have failed to demonstrate any difference between monolingual and bilingual children regarding their performances on similar tasks [28,29].

1.4. Bilingualism and Intelligence Testing

Besides studies suggesting an advantage or disadvantage for bilinguals in several cognitive domains such as executive functions, working memory, spatial reasoning, or verbal ability [26], there is only limited research focusing on the role of bilingualism on intelligence measures. One of the most frequently used and most accepted instruments for assessing intellectual abilities in young children [30] is the Wechsler Primary and Preschool Scale of Intelligence—Fourth Edition (WPPSI-IV; [31]). While studies investigating differences in intellectual abilities between monolingual and bilingual children have used comprehensive intelligence test batteries, such as the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children such as WISC-IV [32], such analyses were mostly conducted based on the data of children older than 6 years.
Bialystok and Majumder [33] analyzed differences between the performances of monolingual and bilingual 7- to 9-year-old children on the WISC-IV subtest Block Design and found that bilinguals achieved higher scores than monolinguals. In another study focusing on four WISC-IV subtests, (Block Design, Digit Span, Vocabulary, and Arithmetic) Lauchlan, Parisi, and Fadda [34] found an advantage for bilinguals in the subtests Block Design and Vocabulary, but no significant mean score differences for the subtests Digit Span and Arithmetic when compared to monolinguals. Karlsson et al. [35] examined the effect of language background on ten WISC-IV subtests in children aged 7 and 10 to 11 and found that monolingual children of the younger age group on average achieved a significantly higher score on the subtest Symbol Search than bilinguals. When analyzing verbal visual attention measured with the WISC-IV subtest Cancellation of WISC-IV, Calvo and Bialystok [36] demonstrated that there was no difference between the performances of monolingual and bilingual children.
Schweizer et al. [37] investigated differences among monolinguals, simultaneous bilinguals, and sequential bilinguals regarding general intelligence and seven factors with two subtests each provided by the Intelligence and Development Scales—2 (IDS—2, [38]). Based on their sample, including children and adolescents aged 5 to 20, the results indicated lower scores for sequential bilinguals on general intelligence, verbal reasoning, and verbal long-term memory when compared to monolinguals and simultaneous bilinguals. However, there were almost no significant differences found between the performances of simultaneous bilinguals and monolinguals on any IDS-2 measure under examination.
Given the lack of research on this topic, the present study sets out to investigate the potential differences between the performances of monolingual and bilingual children on intellectual measures provided by the German WPPSI-IV [39].

1.5. Parental Education and Cognitive Abilities

As described above, studies on the role of bilingualism in cognitive abilities have suggested effects of bilingualism vary in their direction depending on whether verbal or non-verbal abilities were investigated [40]. In contrast to these findings regarding bilingualism, studies on sociodemographic factors found relatively strong and consistent effects of parental education and socioeconomic status on cognitive abilities [10].
Parental education is considered one of the best predictors of intelligence in children as well as their school achievement [41]. Recent studies have demonstrated that a higher parental educational level is related to higher levels of intelligence in children and better school performance due to other family-related factors such as social and material resources [42]. Generally, the heritability of intelligence is high; the relationship between parents’ intelligence and children’s intelligence already shows in early childhood, then increases in adolescence, and displays its highest relationship in adulthood [43].
Eilertsen and colleagues [44] investigated the relationship between socioeconomic status (SES; including parental education) and cognitive functioning measured with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Third Edition (WISC-III, [45]) in a sample of Norwegian children aged 8 to 12 years. The results indicated a strong association between SES and the Verbal Comprehension Index, in which maternal education was the only significant predictor of SES. Lower significant relationships were also found between SES and Full-Scale IQ as well as SES and Working Memory IQ.

1.6. Bilingualism and Parental Education

It may be assumed that performances on cognitive tasks might also be affected by an interaction between bilingualism and socioeconomic status so that the main effects of one depend on certain levels of the other [36]. It is, thus, possible that only those bilingual children featuring a specific level of parental education, such as the highest educational level, appear to have an advantage in tasks measuring working memory capacity while bilinguals featuring other levels of parental education do not [36].
Since bilingualism and parental education both are suggested to be strongly correlated or interdependent, thus sharing a great portion of common variance, it is challenging to examine the possible effects of both on cognitive abilities separately. Besides examining possible interaction effects of bilingualism and parental education on cognitive abilities, however, it is also deemed necessary to distinguish between possible effects of each of those factors while controlling for the other.
Daseking et al. [46] investigated the role of migration background on intellectual abilities in a sample of German children and adolescents aged 6 to 16 using the German Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fifth Edition (WISC-V, [47]). The sample was divided into two different groups: a group of younger children attending primary school and a second group of older children or adolescents attending secondary school. In the group of younger children, the possible effects of parental education and migration background were analyzed on primary and ancillary index scores as well as on the Full-Scale IQ (FSIQ). In the group of older children and adolescents, the attended school type was additionally included in the analyses as an additional potentially contributing factor for intellectual measures. The results of the study indicated that differences in the corresponding WISC-V index scores between children and adolescents with and without migration background could be fully explained either by parental education (in the group of younger children) or by the attended school type (in the group of older children and adolescents). Depending on their migration background, however, children and adolescents with a migration background were also found to attend school types associated with lower levels of education more frequently than those without a migration background.

1.7. Research Questions

Since there is only a small body of research suggesting quite diverse results on the role of language background on cognitive profiles, the need to provide more extensive investigations on this topic is evident. For this purpose, using measures of intellectual abilities that are provided by the widely used Wechsler Scales is required to reliably reflect those intellectual abilities to be examined, thus enabling the estimation of valid cognitive profiles. In particular, studies specifically focusing on the differences between young monolingual, simultaneous bilingual, and sequential bilingual children regarding their intellectual abilities are still missing despite their relevant implications for clinical practice. Especially in borderline cases, for instance, clinical decisions based on measures of intellectual abilities might likely be flawed due to the failure to consider the possible impact of language background on test performances.
There has been little research to date, and the results of this research on the role of language background in cognitive profiles vary considerably. For a more comprehensive study on this topic, the intellectual abilities of young children should be measured using the widely used Wechsler scales, which reliably reflect the intellectual abilities to be analyzed and, thus, enable the creation of valid cognitive profiles. Particularly for borderline performance, clinical decisions based on measures of intellectual ability may be flawed because the possible influence of language background on test performance is not adequately taken into account. As a result, children may not receive the necessary support or therapeutic assistance.
In everyday life, not only for parents but also for educational professionals, the question repeatedly arises as to the order in which the mother tongue (first language) and other languages (here in particular the national language or language of education) are acquired most successfully and how children and families can be supported in this process, especially that the mastery of the language of education can be regarded as essential for educational success.
Even though intelligence tests such as the Wechsler Scales were not primarily developed to assess intellectual abilities associated with language background, the 15 WPPSI-IV subtests, the primary and ancillary indices, and the FSIQ can still provide suitable measures for the comparison between young monolingual and bilingual children. In that regard, the current study aims to clarify whether language background (monolingual, simultaneous bilingual, and sequential bilingual) and parental education (no/low educational level, medium educational level, high educational level, and the highest educational level) can be suggested to have a remarkable impact on the WPPSI-IV test performances of children aged 4 to 7 as reflected by their scores on the primary and ancillary index, the 15 subtests, and the FSIQ.
On the basis of these considerations, the present study aims to address the following research questions:
  • Do scores on the WPPSI-IV primary indices and Full-Scale IQ significantly differ between children aged 4 to 7 years depending on their language background (monolingual, simultaneous bilingual, sequential bilingual) and parental education (no/low educational level, medium educational level, high educational level, and the highest educational level)? Is there an interaction effect between language background and parental education on the performances on the WPPSI-IV primary indices in children aged 4 to 7 years?
  • Do scores on the WPPSI-IV ancillary indices significantly differ between children aged 4 to 7 years depending on their language background (monolingual, simultaneous bilingual, sequential bilingual) and parental education (no/low educational level, medium educational level, high educational level, and the highest educational level)? Is there an interaction effect between language background and parental education on the performances on the WPPSI-IV ancillary indices in children aged 4 to 7 years?
  • Do performances on the WPPSI-IV subtests significantly differ between children aged 4 to 7 years depending on their language background (monolingual, simultaneous bilingual, sequential bilingual) and parental education (no/low educational level, medium educational level, high educational level, and the highest educational level)? Is there an interaction effect between language background and parental education on the performances on the WPPSI-IV subtests in children aged 4 to 7 years?

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample

Subsequent analyses were based on the data of a sample of N = 290 children (130 females, 160 males), which was selected from the extended dataset of the German WPPSI-IV standardization sample (see Table 1 for an overview). One parent per child completed a questionnaire assessing sociodemographic variables such as sex, age, parental education, and language background. The data from monolinguals were matched to the data from the group of simultaneous bilinguals and sequential bilinguals for age, sex, and parental education. Children were recruited from different kindergartens and elementary schools in Bremen, Lower Saxony, Saxony, Baden-Wuerttemberg, North Rhine-Westphalia, and Hamburg, Germany. Prior to data collection and testing, parents and (kindergarten) teachers were informed about the main goals and procedures of the study as well as about data processing and protection. Parents were then required to give their approval by signing a written informed consent form.
The exclusion criteria for the sample were the following: the child does not speak/understand German; the child participated in another intelligence testing during the last six months; the child has significant limitations of the upper and lower extremities; the child takes medication that may affect test performances (e.g., anticonvulsants); or the child has a diagnosed physical, neurological, or mental disorder that could significantly influence test performances (e.g., stroke, brain tumor, epilepsy).
Since previous research has suggested that performances on cognitive tasks and the language use of multilingual children are not comparable to those of bilingual and monolingual children due to qualitative differences [48], multilingual children were excluded from the data analyses of the present study. The less restrictive definition for simultaneous bilinguals proposed by Paradis, Genesee, and Crago [8] was used in the present study: children who learn both languages between birth and the age of one year are classified as simultaneous bilinguals, as this classification reflects a compromise between the restrictive and less restrictive definition. During testing, only German was spoken to the participating children and all tasks were presented in German.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Language Background and Parental Education

Language background and parental education were measured using specific questions within a sociodemographic questionnaire that was completed by one parent (see Table 1 for an overview of the questions addressing language background). Language background is defined as the number of acquired languages and the way they were acquired during language development. Parental education is defined as the highest level of education achieved by either one parent or both: 1 = low educational level, 2 = medium educational level, 3 = high educational level, 4 = highest educational level.

2.2.2. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale—Fourth Edition (WPPSI-IV)

The German Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence—Fourth Edition (WPPSI-IV; [39]) provides different measures for evaluating the intellectual abilities of children aged 2 years and 6 months to 7 years and 7 months. The WPPSI-IV provides two age group versions: one for children aged 2 years and 6 months to 3 years and 11 months (2:6–3:11) and the other one for children aged 4 years to 7 years and 7 months (4:0–7:7).
The WPPSI version for ages 4:0–7:7 includes a total of 15 subtests, each measuring specific and relatively independent intellectual abilities: Information (IN), Receptive Vocabulary (RV), Picture Naming (PN), Block Design (BD), Object Assembly (OA), Picture Memory (PM), Zoo Locations (ZL), Similarities (SI), Vocabulary (VO), Comprehension (CO), Matrix Reasoning (MR), Picture Concepts (PC), Bug Search (BS), Cancellation (CA), and Animal Coding (AC). Each subtest produces a raw score, which is transformed into a scaled score that can range from 1 to 19 (standard scores; M = 10, SD = 3), with scores between 7 and 12 usually considered average. The Full-Scale IQ (FSIQ), representing general intelligence, and 5 primary index scores for the Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI), Visual Spatial Index (VSI), Fluid Reasoning Index (FRI), Working Memory Index (WMI), and Processing Speed Index (PSI), representing five broad cognitive abilities, can be derived from the subtest scaled scores. In addition, ancillary index scores can also be calculated for the Vocabulary Acquisition Index (VAI), Nonverbal Index (NVI), General Ability Index (GAI), and Cognitive Proficiency Index (CPI). The FSIQ and all index scores can range from 40 to 160 (standard scores; M = 100, SD = 15), with scores from 85 to 114 considered average. The five primary index scores include those factor-based composite scores representing the main cognitive domains that are typically obtained for a comprehensive evaluation of cognitive functioning. The four ancillary index scores are theory-driven and may be used to provide additional or supporting information regarding a child’s WPPSI–IV performances.
For the German adaptation of the WPPSI-IV, the primary indices VCI, VSI, FRI, and WMI and the ancillary indices VAI, NVI, GAI, and CPI have indicated excellent internal consistency coefficients across all ages, with Cronbach’s alpha ranging from 0.87 to 0.94. The test–retest reliability coefficients for the PSI subtests, ranging from 0.72 to 0.74, turned out to be lower in value but still with sufficiently good stability.

2.3. Data Analysis

All analyses of the present study were performed using the IBM SPSS Statistics software (Version 29), and an alpha level of 0.05 for statistical significance was set for all statistical tests. First, the total sample of N = 290 children aged 4 to 7 years was scanned for outliers, deviations from multivariate normality (e.g., skewness and kurtosis), and homogeneity of the covariance matrices. In order to answer the research questions of the present study, three multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVA) were performed to evaluate the statistical significance of multivariate effects using Wilk’s λ as well as univariate main and interaction effects using F statistics. A first multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA 1) was conducted to test for the hypothesized main and interaction effects of language background and parental education on the five WPPSI-IV primary indices and the FSIQ. Possible main and interaction effects of language background and parental education on two WPPSI-IV ancillary indices Vocabulary Acquisition Index (VAI) and Nonverbal Index (NVI) were then analyzed by performing a second multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA 2). A third multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA 3) was finally conducted to test for the hypothesized main and interaction effects of language background and parental education on the 15 WPPSI-IV subtests. Partial eta squared (η2par) was used to evaluate the effect sizes of statistically significant main and interaction effects, with η2par ≥ 0.01 indicating a small effect, η2par ≥ 0.06 indicating a moderate effect, and η2par ≥ 0.14 indicating a large effect. In cases of statistically significant main effects, post hoc analyses were additionally performed by comparing means of specific factor levels to identify group differences that may explain the corresponding main effect. The Bonferroni method was used to adjust for multiple post hoc comparisons to control for the overall probability of a Type I error for multiple hypothesis tests.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics

3.1.1. Sample

Sociodemographic characteristics for the groups of monolinguals, simultaneous bilinguals, and sequential bilinguals are depicted in Table 2.
The quality of data matching was checked using chi-square tests of homogeneity and the Kruskal–Wallis test statistics for nonparametric data. Both test statistics indicated that there were no differences between the groups of monolinguals, simultaneous bilinguals, and sequential bilinguals regarding their distributions of parental education (χ2 (6, N = 290) = 0.586, p = 0.997, age (H (2, N = 290) = 0.377, p = 0.828) and sex (χ2 (2, N = 290) = 0.030, p = 0.985).

3.1.2. Test Performances

As indicated by Box’ M test statistics and further investigations of box plots and skewness and kurtosis values, no extreme outliers and no substantial deviations from the multivariate normality or homogeneity of the covariance matrices could be found for the data of monolinguals, simultaneous bilinguals, and sequential bilinguals.
Table 3 presents descriptive statistics, suggested means, and standard deviations for the scaled scores on all 15 WPPSI-IV, the five primary index scores, two ancillary index scores, and FSIQ for the three groups of language background, as well as the total sample.

3.2. Multivariate Analysis of Variance on the WPPSI-IV Primary Index Scores and the FSIQ

The results of the multivariate analysis of variance 1 (MANOVA 1), including the main and interaction effects of language background and parental education on the five WPPSI-IV primary index scores and the FSIQ are presented in Table 4. MANOVA 1 yielded significant multivariate effects of language background (Wilks’s λ = 0.700, F(12, 548) = 2.592, p = 0.013, η2par = 0.045) and parental education (Wilks’s λ = 0.912, F(8, 842) = 1.952, p = 0.010, η2par = 0.041).
Univariate analyses indicated small but significant effects of language background on VCI (F(2, 278) = 8.535, p < 0.001, η2par = 0.058) and the FSIQ (F(2, 278) = 4.499, p = 0.012, η2par = 0.031). Furthermore, significant univariate effects of parental education were found on VCI (F(3, 278) = 5.817, p < 0.001, η2par = 0.059) as well as on the FSIQ (F(3, 278) = 4.260, p = 0.006, η2par = 0.044). Even though no multivariate interaction effect was found, a univariate effect of the interaction between language background and parental education turned out to be significant for VCI (F(6, 278) = 2.290, p = 0.036, η2par = 0.047), WMI (F(2, 278) = 2.507, p = 0.022, η2par = 0.051), and for the FSIQ (F(2, 278) = 2.851, p = 0.010, η2par = 0.058). Figure 1a) illustrates the interaction between language background and parental education on VCI and the interaction effect for the FSIQ is illustrated in Figure 1b).
The results of the post hoc analyses for the main effects in MANOVA 1 are presented in Table 5. On average, sequential bilinguals showed significantly lower VCI scores than simultaneous bilinguals (ΔM = 6.06; SE = 2.355; p = 0.032) and monolinguals (ΔM = −9.15; SE = 1.996; p < 0.001). Moreover, children with a sequential bilingual language background achieved significantly lower FSIQ scores on average than monolingual children (ΔM = −6.38; SE = 1.975; p = 0.004).
Furthermore, post hoc analyses also indicated that, on average, children with parents having no or a low educational level achieved significantly lower VCI scores than children with parents having the highest educational level (ΔM = −7.49; SE = 2.274; p = 0.007). Significantly lower VCI scores were also found for children with parents having a medium educational level compared to children with parents having a high educational level (ΔM = −7.61; SE = 2.540; p = 0.018) and the highest educational level (ΔM = −10.63; SE = 2.161; p < 0.001). In addition, children with parents having the highest educational level were found to achieve higher FSIQ scores on average than children with parents having no or a low educational level (ΔM = −6.92; SE = 2.250; p = 0.014) and a medium educational level (ΔM = −6.66; SE = 2.138; p < 0.012).

3.3. Multivariate Analysis of Variance on the WPPSI-IV Ancillary Index Scores

Table 6 presents a summary of the multivariate analysis of variance 2 (MANOVA 2) with the main and interaction effects of language background and parental education on two WPPSI-IV ancillary index scores. The results indicated significant multivariate effects on two WPPSI-IV ancillary index scores of language background (Wilks’s λ = 0.804, F(4, 554) = 15.959, p < 0.001, η2par = 0.103), parental education (Wilks’s λ = 0.936, F(6, 554) = 3.112, p = 0.005, η2par = 0.033), and the language background × parental education interaction (Wilks’s λ = 0.923, F(12, 554) = 1.883, p = 0.034, η2par = 0.039).
Furthermore, the analysis yielded significant univariate effects of language background on VAI (F(2, 278) = 29.473, p < 0.001, η2par = 0.175) and univariate effects of parental education on VAI (F(3, 278) = 4.432, p = 0.005, η2par = 0.046). The language background × parental education interaction seemed to be significant for NVI (F(6, 278) = 2.702, p = 0.014, η2par = 0.055).
A summary of the results of the post hoc analyses for the main effects of language background and parental education on two WPPSI-IV ancillary indices (MANOVA 2) are presented in Table 7. The results showed that on average, sequential bilinguals demonstrated significantly lower performances than simultaneous bilinguals (ΔM = 8.04; SE = 2.167; p < 0.001) and monolinguals (ΔM = −15.15; SE = 1.836; p < 0.001) on VAI. But also, children growing up simultaneously bilingual indicated significantly lower scores on VAI than monolingual children (ΔM = −7.11; SE = 1.914; p < 0.001).
Table 7 also presented the results about children with parents having the highest educational level, the post hoc analysis indicated higher performances on average for these children on VAI than children with parents having the lowest educational level (ΔM = −6.50; SE = 2.093; p = 0.013), as well as children with parents having a medium educational level (ΔM = −7.71; SE = 1.988; p < 0.001).

3.4. Multivariate Analysis of Variance on the WPPSI-IV Subtest Scaled Scores

A summary of the multivariate analysis of variance 3 (MANOVA 3) with the main and interaction effects of language background and parental education on the 15 WPPSI-IV subtest scaled scores is presented in Table 8. The analysis demonstrated significant multivariate effects on the 15 WPPSI-IV subtests scaled scores of language background (Wilks’s λ = 0.724, F(30, 528) = 3.083, p < 0.001, η2par = 0.149) and parental education (Wilks’s λ = 0.737, F(45, 785) = 1.885, p < 0.001, η2par = 0.097).
All results of the univariate analyses including the main and interaction effects of language background and parental education on the 15 WPPSI-IV subtest scaled scores (MANOVA 3) are presented in Table 8.
An interaction effect of language background × parental education appeared to be significant for the following subtests: SI (F(6, 278) = 2.154, p = 0.048, η2par = 0.044) and VC (F(6, 278) = 2.234, p = 0.040, η2par = 0.046).
The significant post hoc analyses are too numerous to include in this section. A summary of the results of post hoc analyses for the main effects of language background and parental education on the WPPSI-IV subtests scaled scores (MANOVA 3) are presented in Table 9.

4. Discussion

The major aim of the present study was to investigate whether performances on the five WPPSI-IV primary indices and the FSIQ, the two ancillary indices (Vocabulary Acquisition Index, Nonverbal Index), and the 15 WPPSI-IV subtests differ between children aged 4 to 7 years depending on their language background (monolingual, simultaneous bilingual, and sequential bilingual) and parental education (no/low educational level, medium educational level, high educational level, and the highest educational level). Furthermore, a second aim of the present study was to examine if there is an interaction effect between language background and parental education on the performances on the same WPPSI-IV measures in children aged 4 to 7 years.
As indicated by the results of the multivariate analyses of variance, the group of sequential bilinguals showed lower scores on primary and ancillary indices, as well as on subtests measuring verbal abilities compared to monolinguals and simultaneous bilinguals. On average, monolinguals appeared to outperform sequential bilinguals in the WPPSI-IV indices Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI), Vocabulary Acquisition Index (VAI), the Full-Scale IQ (FSIQ), and the WPPSI-IV subtests Information (IN), Similarities (SI), Vocabulary (VO), Comprehension (CO), Receptive Vocabulary (RV), Picture Naming (PN), and Object Assembly (OA). Simultaneous bilinguals were found to outperform sequential bilinguals in the WPPSI-IV indices VCI and VAI as well as in the WPPSI-IV subtests IN, VO, CO, RV, and PN. However, simultaneous bilinguals achieved significantly lower scores than monolinguals only on VAI, RV, and PN. It should be noted that the subtests RV and PN are both used to compute VAI.
These findings are for the most part in line with the current research [14,15,16,17,18,37], postulating that the average levels of performance of simultaneous bilinguals on verbal tasks are more similar to monolinguals than to sequential bilinguals. For this reason, it may be suggested that language acquisition in children who grow up speaking two languages from birth may be very similar to that of monolinguals, as the two separate linguistic systems of simultaneous bilinguals are closely linked.
In addition to verbal abilities, crystallized intelligence, long-term memory, the ability to retain and retrieve knowledge from the environment, verbal perception, comprehension, and expression are required for solving tasks that are related to VCI and the associated subtests IN, SI, VC, and CO. However, if applying a specific vocabulary fails, those tasks can also be completed by paraphrasing the vocabulary. While simultaneous bilinguals may solve such tasks just as well as monolinguals, sequential bilinguals might lack the vocabulary to solve the tasks for VCI and the associated subtests as well as monolingual children. Furthermore, it may be suggested that simultaneous bilinguals tend to outperform sequential bilinguals on VCI and the associated subtests because they learned German in early childhood and, therefore, have probably a more comprehensive vocabulary.
However, some studies have already indicated that bilinguals, including simultaneous bilinguals, tend to score lower on tests measuring receptive vocabulary than monolinguals [19,20,21]. By contrast, VAI and the associated subtests RV and PN measure developmental vocabulary acquisition and are primarily based on expressive and receptive vocabulary. For solving tasks that are related to VAI, RV, and PN, applying the exact vocabulary is required. Given that the vocabulary of simultaneous bilinguals is divided into two closely linked but still separate linguistic systems, simultaneous bilinguals may show lower performances on VAI and the associated subtests than monolinguals.
Furthermore, the potential effects of parental education on the children’s performances regarding the WPPSI-IV primary indices, the FSIQ, two ancillary indices, and the 15 WPPSI-IV subtests were analyzed. The present findings indicated the effects of parental education on similar index and subtest scores that were also found to be affected by language background. In particular, children with parents having a low to medium educational level were found to achieve lower scores on VCI, VAI, and the associated subtests IN, SI, VC, CO, RV, and PN than children with parents having a high or the highest educational level.
These findings are in line with the findings of Eilertsen and colleagues [44], who investigated the relationship between socioeconomic status (SES) and cognitive functioning measured with the WISC-III. As part of the SES, maternal education was found to be the only significant predictor explaining the strong association between SES and VCI. Similarly to the present findings, Eilertsen and colleagues [44] also suggested a small but significant association between the SES and the FSIQ.
Several studies have already emphasized a positive relationship between SES and productive vocabulary, which might likely be influenced by the mean length of utterances in parental speech, especially maternal speech [49]. In addition, Raviv, Kessenich, and Morrison [50] investigated possible mediators for the relationship between SES and children’s language skills and concluded that maternal sensitivity and cognitive stimulation might partially mediate the effects of SES on verbal comprehension, expressive language, and receptive verbal conceptual abilities.
In addition to the main effects of language background and parental education, the present findings also suggested a significant interaction effect of language background and parental education on VCI and the FSIQ. As depicted in Figure 1a), monolinguals and simultaneous bilinguals were found to achieve higher scores on VCI than sequential bilinguals with increasing parental educational levels. While performances on the corresponding tasks were not significantly different across all three groups of language background at the low parental educational level, monolinguals and simultaneous bilinguals both outperformed sequential bilinguals the most at the high or highest parental educational level. A similar interaction effect of language background and parental education was also found on the FSIQ (see Figure 1b). Monolinguals and simultaneous bilinguals were found to achieve higher scores on the FSIQ than sequential bilinguals with increasing parental educational levels. At the low parental educational level, monolinguals, simultaneous bilinguals, and sequential bilinguals achieved on average comparable scores on the FSIQ, whereas monolinguals and simultaneous bilinguals both outperformed sequential bilinguals the most at the high or highest parental educational level.
As Figure 1 illustrates, the interaction effects of language background and parental education on both VCI and the FSIQ were found to be due to the increasing associated scores for monolinguals and simultaneous bilinguals with increasing parental educational levels. In contrast, the level of performance in sequential bilinguals appeared to be quite similar across all parental educational levels, thus being independent of parental education.
Even though these findings indicated no substantial effect of parental education, the test performances of sequential bilinguals regarding VCI and the FSIQ seem to be in line with previous studies suggesting a strong association between maternal education and children’s productive vocabulary as well as language skills [49,50]. It should be noted that the group of sequential bilinguals includes children who learned to speak German at the earliest age of two years and at the latest when they attended kindergarten. However, this could mean that parents have not spoken German with their children by this time and have, therefore, not been able to promote or teach German to their children.
In summary, the findings of the present study clearly suggest that performances on verbal tasks as reflected by VCI, VAI, and the associated subtests may be affected by parental education and language background. Especially in the cases of children with parents having a low to medium educational level, it is, therefore, advisable to use nonverbal tasks and measures such as the Nonverbal Index (NVI) to provide a measure of cognitive ability less dependent on language. Given that the impact of parental education and language background could be at least partly due to limitations in the structural validity of the WPSSI-IV regarding these specific groups, future studies should investigate measurement invariance on the WPPSI-IV across different parental educational levels and different groups of language background. Moreover, future research should clarify whether those effects of language background and parental education in early childhood are also evident in development in later childhood, adolescence, and adulthood as well.

Limitations

There are some limitations in the present study that need to be addressed and should also be considered when replicating or interpreting the present results.
As the present study is cross-sectional in nature, inferences about causality are limited by definition. While there is a theoretical and empirical basis for conducting multivariate variance analyses on cross-sectional data in the research literature, however, further analyses on longitudinal data are required to investigate more causal effects of language background and parental education. This way, longitudinal studies might then provide a more adequate insight into possible causal effects of language background and parental education on children’s cognitive development over time.
It should also be noted that the WPPSI-IV was the only test battery that was used to measure and operationalize cognitive abilities in the present study. Since using alternative and more diverse approaches could improve the reliability of measurements by reducing potential methodological biases and test-specific limitations, future research should focus on multi-methodological measurements of cognitive abilities when investigating possible effects on test performances that may be influenced by parental education and language in early childhood.
Another limitation of the present study is the heterogeneity of the sample. Although it was determined that all children participating in the study spoke German as their first or second language, other spoken languages could be assigned to a variety of different language groups. Since those language groups were not further investigated, the representativeness and comparability of the simultaneous and sequential bilinguals cannot be fully guaranteed. Accordingly, the level of development of the language that was not German was not measured or determined. Future research should, thus, clarify to what extent the present findings can be transferred to different language groups.

5. Conclusions

Despite the aforementioned limitations, the present study represents a further contribution to the research on the effects of language background and parental education on the WPPSI-IV test performances of monolingual, simultaneous bilingual, and sequential bilingual German children aged 4 to 7 years. First of all, simultaneous bilinguals tend to perform more similarly to monolinguals than to sequential bilinguals. Sequential bilinguals were found to be outperformed by monolinguals and simultaneous bilinguals on most verbal tasks, achieving lower scores on VCI and VAI, and on the verbal subtests IN, VC, CO, RV, and PN. Compared to monolinguals, however, simultaneous bilinguals also achieved lower scores on VAI and the associated subtests PN and RV. Moreover, the present findings also indicated that parental education may likely affect the performances on the WPPSI-IV; this is because children with parents having a lower educational level tend to achieve lower scores on VCI, VAI, the FSIQ, and the subtests IN, SI, VC, CO, RV, and PN than children with parents having a higher educational level. In general, future research should clarify whether those effects of language background and parental education might at least partially explain variations in cognitive abilities in later childhood, adolescence, and adulthood as well.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, F.W., M.D. and F.P.; methodology, F.W. and F.P.; validation, F.W., M.D. and F.P.; formal analysis, F.P.; investigation, F.W.; resources, F.W. and M.D.; data curation, F.W.; writing—original draft preparation, F.W.; writing—review and editing, F.P. and M.D.; visualization, F.P. and F.W.; supervision, F.P. and M.D.; project administration, M.D.; funding acquisition, M.D. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical review and approval were waived for this study because we fully informed the parents and children before participation (no deception), it was a counseling (not a clinical) setting, the methods were not invasive, and there were no psychopharmacological interventions. Therefore, in line with the standards of the German Research Society (Deutsche Forschungsgesellschaft, DFG; https://www.dfg.de/de/foerderung/antrag-foerderprozess/faq/geistes-sozialwissenschaften#253024, accessed on 1 April 2024), we did not apply to an ethics committee. Ethical review and approval were, thus, granted by the responsible school/kindergarten authorities and province school boards in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Since the research is based on health service research (Versorgungsforschung) the data are not publicly available.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Czapka, S.; Wotschack, C.; Klassert, A.; Festman, J. A path to the bilingual advantage: Pairwise matching of individuals. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2020, 23, 344–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. DeHouwer, A. Bilingual Language Acquisition. In The Handbook of Child Language; Fletcher, P., MacWhinney, B., Eds.; Wiley Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  3. Tabors, P. One Child, Two Languages; Brookes: Baltimore, MD, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  4. McLaughlin, B. Second-Language Acquisition in Childhood; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  5. DeHouwer, A. Bilingual Language Acquisition. In Handbook of Child Language; Fletcher, P., Whinney, B.M., Eds.; Basil Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 1995; pp. 219–250. [Google Scholar]
  6. Yip, V. Simultaneous Language Acquisition. In The Psycholinguistics of Bilingualism; Grosjean, F., Ping, L., Eds.; Wiley-Blackwell: Malden, MA, USA, 2013; pp. 119–136. [Google Scholar]
  7. DeHouwer, A. Bilingual First Language Acquisition; Multilingual Matters: Bristol, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  8. Paradis, J.; Genesee, F.; Crago, M.B. Dual Language Development & Disorders: A Handbook on Bilingualism & Second Language Learning; Paul H. Brookes: Baltimore, MD, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  9. Rivero, M.; Vilaseca, R.; Cantero, M.-J.; Valls-Vidal, C.; Leiva, D. Relations between Positive Parenting Behavior during Play and Child Language Development at Early Ages. Children 2023, 10, 505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Becker, B.; Klein, O.; Biedinger, N. The development of cognitive, language, and cultural skills from age 3 to 6: A comparison between children of Turkish origin and children of native-born German parents and the role of immigrant parents’ acculturation to the receiving society. Am. Educ. Res. J. 2013, 50, 616–649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Leseman, P.P.M.; Scheel, A.F.; Mayo, A.Y.; Messer, M.H. Bilingual development in early childhood and the languages used at home: Competition for scarce resources? In Streitfall Zweisprachigkeit—The Bilingualism Controversy; Gogolin, I., Neumann, U., Eds.; VS: Wiesbaden, Germany, 2009; pp. 289–316. [Google Scholar]
  12. Gross, M.; Buac, M.; Kaushanskaya, M. Conceptual scoring of receptive and expressive vocabulary measures in simultaneous and sequential bilingual children. Am. J. Speech-Lang. Pathol. 2014, 23, 574–586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Berens, M.S.; Kovelman, I.; Petitto, L.A. Should bilingual children learn reading in two languages at the same time or in sequence? Biling. Res. J. 2013, 36, 35–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Mayo, L.H.; Florentine, M.; Buus, S. Age of second-language acquisition and perception of speech in noise. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 1997, 40, 686–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Chilla, S.; Rothweiler, M.; Babur, E. Kindliche Mehrsprachigkeit. Grundlagen; Störungen; Diagnostik; Reinhardt: München, Germany, 2013; Volume 2. [Google Scholar]
  16. Meisel, J.M. Mehrsprachigkeit in der frühen Kindheit: Zur Rolle des Alters bei Erwerbsbeginn. In Mehrsprachigkeit bei Kindern und Erwachsenen. Erwerb Form Förderung; Anstatt, T., Ed.; Narr Francke Attempto Verlag: Tübingen, Germany, 2007; pp. 93–113. [Google Scholar]
  17. Paradis, J.; Genesee, F.; Crago, M.B. Dual Language Development and Disorders: A Handbook on Bilingualism and Second Language Learning; Paul H. Brookes: Baltimore, MD, USA, 2011; Volume 2. [Google Scholar]
  18. Rothweiler, M.; Ruberg, T. Der Erwerb des Deutschen bei Kindern mit Nichtdeutscher Erstsprache. Sprachliche und Außersprachliche Einflussfaktoren; Deutsches Jugendinstitut: München, Germany, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  19. Ben-Zeev, S. The Influence of Bilingualism on Cognitive Strategy and Cognitive Development. Child Dev. 1977, 48, 1009–1018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Bialystok, E.; Luk, G.; Peets, K.F.; Yang, S. Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2010, 13, 525–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Engel de Abreu, P.M. Working memory in multilingual children: Is there a bilingual effect? Memory 2011, 19, 529–537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Kay-Raining Bird, E.; Cleave, P.; Trudeau, N.; Thordardottir, E.; Sutton, A.; Thorpe, A. The language abilities of bilingual children with Down’s syndrome. Am. J. Speech–Lang. Pathol. 2005, 14, 187–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Paradis, J.; Crago, M.; Genesee, F.; Rice, M. French–English bilingual children with SLI: How do they compare with their monolingual peers? J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2003, 46, 113–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Kaushanskaya, M.; Gross, M.; Buac, M. Effects of classroom bilingualism on task-shifting, verbal memory, and word learning in children. Dev. Sci. 2014, 17, 564–583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Park, J.; Weismer, S.E.; Kaushanskaya, M. Changes in executive function over time in bilingual and monolingual school-aged children. Dev. Psychol. 2018, 54, 1842–1853. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Bailey, C.; Venta, A.; Langley, H. The bilingual [dis]advantage. Lang. Cogn. 2020, 12, 225–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Garcia, A.M.; Ros, R.; Hart, K.C.; Graziano, P.A. Comparing working memory in bilingual and monolingual Hispanic/Latino preschoolers with disruptive behavior disorders. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2018, 166, 535–548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Buac, M.; Gross, M.; Kaushanskaya, M. Predictors of processing-based task performance in bilingual and monolingual children. J. Commun. Disord. 2016, 62, 12–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Danahy, K.; Windsor, J.; Kohnert, K. Counting span and the identification of primary language impairment. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2007, 42, 349–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Railford, M.R.; Coalson, D.L. Essentials of WPPSI-IV Assessment; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  31. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence, 4th ed.; Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  32. Wechsler, D. (Ed.) Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 4th ed.; Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  33. Bialystok, E.; Majumder, S. The relationship between bilingualism and the development of cognitive processes in problem solving. Appl. Psycholinguist. 1998, 19, 69–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Lauchlan, F.; Parisi, M.; Fadda, R. Bilingualism in Sardinia and Scotland: Exploring the cognitive benefits of speaking a ‘minority’ language. Int. J. Biling. 2012, 17, 43–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Karlsson, L.C.; Soveri, A.; Räsänen, P.; Kärnä, A.; Delatte, S.; Lagerström, E.; Mård, L.; Steffansson, M.; Lehtonen, M.; Laine, M. Bilingualism and performance on two widely used developmental neuropsychological test batteries. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0125867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Calvo, A.; Bialystok, E. Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition 2014, 130, 278–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Schweizer, F.; Grieder, S.; Bünger, A.; Grob, A. Vergleich von Intelligenztestleistungen bei monolingualen und bilingualen Kindern und Jugendlichen in den Intelligence and Development Scales–2 (IDS-2). Diagnostica 2021, 67, 36–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Grob, A.; Hagmann-von Arx, P. Intelligence and Development Scale-2 (IDS-2): Intelligenz-und Entwicklungsskalen für Kinder und Jugendliche; Hogrefe: Bern, Switzerland, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  39. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence, 4th ed.; Petermann, F., Daseking, M., Eds.; Pearson: Frankfurt am Main, Germany, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  40. Akhtar, N.; Menjivar, J.A. Chapter 2—Cognitive and Linguistic Correlates of Early Exposure to More than One Language. In Advances in Child Development and Behavior; Benson, J.B., Ed.; Academic Press: Burlington, NJ, USA, 2012; pp. 41–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Morris, T.; Dorling, D.; Smith, G.D. How well can we predict educational outcomes? Examining the roles of cognitive ability and social position in educational attainment. Contemp. Soc. Sci. 2016, 11, 154–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Tamayo Martinez, N.; Xerxa, Y.; Law, J.; Serdarevic, F.; Jansen, P.W.; Tiemeier, H. Double advantage of parental education for child educational achievement: The role of parenting and child intelligence. Eur. J. Public Health 2022, 32, 690–695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Plomin, R.; Deary, I.J. Genetics and intelligence differences: Five special findings. Mol. Psychiatry 2015, 20, 98–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Eilertsen, T.; Thorsen, A.L.; Holm, S.E.H.; Bøe, T.; Sørensen, L.; Lundervold, A.J. Parental socioeconomic status and child intellectual functioning in a Norwegian sample. Scand. J. Psychol. 2016, 57, 399–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Third Edition (WISC-III); The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  46. Daseking, M.; Pauls, F.; Petermann, F.; Becker, A. Welchen Einfluss hat ein Migrationshintergrund auf die kognitiven Leistungen in der WISC-V? Kindh. Entwickl. 2018, 27, 175–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 5th ed.; Petermann, F., Ed.; Pearson: Frankfurt am Main, Germany, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  48. Shanon, B. Faulty language selection in polyglots. Lang. Cogn. Process. 1991, 6, 339–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Hoff, E. The Specificity of Environmental Influence: Socioeconomic Status Affects Early Vocabulary Development via Maternal Speech. Child Dev. 2003, 74, 1368–1378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Raviv, T.; Kessenich, M.; Morrison, F.J. A mediational model of the association between socioeconomic status and three-year-old language abilities: The role of parenting factors. Early Child. Res. Q. 2004, 19, 528–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Means for (a) the VCI scores and (b) the FSIQ scores across the groups of parental education and language background. Note. Language background is defined as the number of acquired languages and the way they were acquired during language development (1 = simultaneous bilingual, 2 = sequential bilingual, 3 = monolingual). Parental education is defined as the highest level of education achieved by either one parent or both (1 = no to low educational level, 2 = medium educational level, 3 = high educational level, 4 = highest educational level). VCI = Verbal Comprehension Index, FSIQ = Full Scale IQ.
Figure 1. Means for (a) the VCI scores and (b) the FSIQ scores across the groups of parental education and language background. Note. Language background is defined as the number of acquired languages and the way they were acquired during language development (1 = simultaneous bilingual, 2 = sequential bilingual, 3 = monolingual). Parental education is defined as the highest level of education achieved by either one parent or both (1 = no to low educational level, 2 = medium educational level, 3 = high educational level, 4 = highest educational level). VCI = Verbal Comprehension Index, FSIQ = Full Scale IQ.
Children 11 00631 g001
Table 1. Questions for measuring language background by classifying children as monolingual, simultaneous bilingual, and sequential bilingual.
Table 1. Questions for measuring language background by classifying children as monolingual, simultaneous bilingual, and sequential bilingual.
QuestionMonolingualSimultaneous BilingualSequential Bilingual
Does the child only understand/speak German? (If so, the parents no longer needed to answer the subsequent questions about language background)YesNoNo
Does the child only understand/speak a language other than German?-NoNo
Does the child understand/speak German and another language?-YesYes
What other language does the child speak/understand besides German?-Just one other language besides GermanJust one other language besides German
At what age did the child speak/understand German?-Birth to 1>1
At what age did the child go to the kindergarten?-Within one year at the latestStarting at age 2 at the earliest
Note. Language background is defined as the number of acquired languages and the way they were acquired during language development. Parental education is defined as the highest level of education achieved by either one parent or both.
Table 2. Sociodemographic characteristics for the groups of monolinguals, simultaneous bilinguals, and sequential bilinguals (N = 290).
Table 2. Sociodemographic characteristics for the groups of monolinguals, simultaneous bilinguals, and sequential bilinguals (N = 290).
Monolingual
(n = 145)
Simultaneous Bilingual
(n = 68)
Sequential Bilingual
(n = 77)
Age in months: M (SD)68.05 (11.84)67.57 (11.97)68.32 (11.92)
n (%)n (%)n (%)
Sex (female)65 (44.8)31 (45.6)34 (44.2)
Parental education:
1 = no to low educational level34 (23.4)15 (22.1)19 (24.7)
2 = medium educational level41 (28.3)18 (26.5)23 (29.9)
3 = high educational level25 (17.2)12 (17.6)13 (16.9)
4 = highest educational level45 (31.0)23 (33.8)22 (28.6)
Note. Language background is defined as the number of acquired languages and the way they were acquired during language development. Parental education is defined as the highest level of education achieved by either one parent or both.
Table 3. Descriptive statistics (means and standard deviations) on the five WPPSI-IV primary index scores, the FSIQ, two ancillary index scores, and 15 subtests for the total sample and language background groups.
Table 3. Descriptive statistics (means and standard deviations) on the five WPPSI-IV primary index scores, the FSIQ, two ancillary index scores, and 15 subtests for the total sample and language background groups.
VariablesDescriptive Statistics
NMSD
Verbal Comprehension Indexsimultaneous bilingual68101.6213.32
sequential bilingual7795.5614.11
monolingual145104.7015.92
Total sample290101.5515.31
Visual Spatial Indexsimultaneous bilingual68104.1616.62
sequential bilingual77100.7014.43
monolingual145105.1415.10
Total sample290103.7315.60
Fluid Reasoning Indexsimultaneous bilingual68104.4115.37
sequential bilingual7798.9813.01
monolingual145102.9215.86
Total sample290102.2315.13
Working Memory Indexsimultaneous bilingual68102.5414.36
sequential bilingual7799.7414.38
monolingual145103.3414.88
Total sample290102.2014.65
Processing Speedsimultaneous bilingual68100.6912.50
sequential bilingual77101.6914.78
monolingual145102.9515.57
Total sample290102.0914.67
Full-Scale IQsimultaneous bilingual68103.5713.48
sequential bilingual7798.1614.08
monolingual145104.5315.25
Total sample290102.6114.75
Vocabulary Acquisition Indexsimultaneous bilingual6897.3212.61
sequential bilingual7789.2913.06
monolingual145104.4313.77
Total sample29098.7414.72
Nonverbal Indexsimultaneous bilingual68104.6614.99
sequential bilingual7799.8213.88
monolingual145104.0015.19
Total sample290103.0414.89
Block Designsimultaneous bilingual6810.913.37
sequential bilingual7710.192.86
monolingual14510.603.22
Total sample29010.573.17
Informationsimultaneous bilingual6810.212.59
sequential bilingual778.872.97
monolingual14510.853.01
Total sample29010.173.01
Matrix Reasoningsimultaneous bilingual6810.693.02
sequential bilingual779.842.86
monolingual14510.383.18
Total sample29010.313.07
Bug Searchsimultaneous bilingual6810.402.79
sequential bilingual7710.063.30
monolingual14510.313.20
Total sample29010.273.13
Picture Memorysimultaneous bilingual6810.383.13
sequential bilingual779.842.84
monolingual14510.862.97
Total sample29010.483.13
Similaritiessimultaneous bilingual6810.473.10
sequential bilingual779.682.81
monolingual14510.913.20
Total sample29010.483.11
Picture Conceptssimultaneous bilingual6810.823.23
sequential bilingual779.822.67
monolingual14510.623.06
Total sample29010.463.02
Cancellationsimultaneous bilingual689.762.59
sequential bilingual7710.422.63
monolingual14510.593.09
Total sample29010.352.87
Zoo Locationssimultaneous bilingual6810.472.48
sequential bilingual7710.042.80
monolingual14510.252.98
Total sample29010.242.82
Object Assemblysimultaneous bilingual6810.402.93
sequential bilingual779.952.87
monolingual14511.072.90
Total sample29010.612.93
Vocabularysimultaneous bilingual6810.542.79
sequential bilingual778.863.01
monolingual14510.772.62
Total sample29010.212.88
Animal Codingsimultaneous bilingual6810.262.82
sequential bilingual7710.513.16
monolingual14510.663.06
Total sample29010.523.03
Comprehensionsimultaneous bilingual6810.382.78
sequential bilingual779.012.89
monolingual14510.262.96
Total sample2909.962.94
Receptive Vocabularysimultaneous bilingual689.602.57
sequential bilingual778.212.53
monolingual14510.832.92
Total sample2909.842.95
Picture Namingsimultaneous bilingual689.492.76
sequential bilingual778.032.79
monolingual14510.812.73
Total sample2909.762.98
Note. Language background is defined as the number of acquired languages and the way they were acquired during language development.
Table 4. Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA 1)—main and interaction effects of language background and parental education on the five WPPSI-IV primary index scores and the FSIQ.
Table 4. Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA 1)—main and interaction effects of language background and parental education on the five WPPSI-IV primary index scores and the FSIQ.
VariablesMultivariate EffectsUnivariate Effects
Fixed FactorDVWilk’s λFdf1/df2pη2parFdf1/df2pη2par
1Language
background
VCI0.9122.15412/5480.0130.0458.5352/278<0.0010.058
VSI 2.5342/2780.0810.018
FRI 2.9082/2780.0560.020
WMI 1.0992/2780.3350.008
PSI 0.4782/2780.6210.003
FSIQ 4.4992/2780.0120.031
2Parental
education
VCI0.8821.95218/7730.0100.0415.8173/278<0.0010.059
VSI 2.5603/2780.0550.027
FRI 1.1263/2780.3390.012
WMI 0.7973/2780.4960.009
PSI 2.6133/2780.0520.027
FSIQ 4.2603/2780.0060.044
Interaction 1 × 2VCI0.8401.35336/12020.0810.0292.2906/2780.0360.047
VSI 1.5286/2780.1690.032
FRI 2.0826/2780.0550.043
WMI 2.5076/2780.0220.051
PSI 1.4116/2780.2100.030
FSIQ 2.8516/2780.0100.058
Note. Language background is defined as the number of acquired languages and the way they were acquired during language development (1 = simultaneous bilingual, 2 = sequential bilingual, 3 = monolingual). Parental education is defined as the highest level of education achieved by either one parent or both (1 = no to low educational level, 2 = medium educational level, 3 = high educational level, 4 = highest educational level). DV = dependent variable. VCI = Verbal Comprehension Index, VSI = Visual Spatial Index, FRI = Fluid Reasoning Index, WMI = Working Memory Index, PSI = Processing Speed Index, FSIQ = Full Scale IQ. η2par = partial eta squared. Significant multivariate and univariate effects (p < 0.050) are marked in bold.
Table 5. Post hoc analyses for the main effects of language background and parental education on the WPPSI-IV primary index scores and the FSIQ (MANOVA 1).
Table 5. Post hoc analyses for the main effects of language background and parental education on the WPPSI-IV primary index scores and the FSIQ (MANOVA 1).
VariablesDescriptive StatisticsTest Statistics (Post Hoc)
DVFixed FactorFactor LevelNMSDCom-parisonΔMSEpCI (95%)
VCILanguage background1bi_sim68101.6213.321 vs. 26.062.3550.0320.39; 11.73
2bi_se7795.5614.111 vs. 3−3.092.0800.417−8.10; 1.92
3mono145104.7015.922 vs. 3−9.151.996<0.001−13.95; −4.34
Parental education1no/low6899.2217.041 vs. 22.872.321>0.999−3.30; 9.04
2medium8296.3513.771 vs. 3−4.742.6370.440−11.75; 2.27
3high50103.9612.641 vs. 4−7.492.2740.007−13.53; −1.45
4highest90106.7114.922 vs. 3−7.612.5400.018−14.35; −0.86
2 vs. 4−10.362.161<0.001−16.10; −4.62
3 vs. 4−2.752.496>0.999−9.38; 3.88
FSIQLanguage background1bi_sim68103.5713.481 vs. 25.522.3300.062−0.19; 11.03
2bi_se7798.1614.081 vs. 3−0.962.058>0.999−5.91; 4.00
3mono145104.5315.252 vs. 3−6.381.9740.004−11.13; −1.62
Parental education1no/low6899.3516.991 vs. 2−0.272.296>0.999−5.83; 6.37
2medium8299.6213.141 vs. 3−6.012.6080.132−12.94; 0.92
3high50105.3614.951 vs. 4−6.922.2500.014−12.90; −0.95
4highest90106.2813.222 vs. 3−5.742.5120.139−12.41; 0.94
2 vs. 4−6.662.1380.012−12.34; −0.98
3 vs. 4−0.922.470>0.999−7.48; 5.64
Note. DV = dependent variable. VCI = Verbal Comprehension Index, FSIQ = Full Scale IQ. Language background is defined as the number of acquired languages and the way they were acquired during language development (1 bi_sim = simultaneous bilingual, 2 bi_se = sequential bilingual, 3 mono = monolingual). Parental education is defined as the highest level of education achieved by either one parent or both (1 no/low = no to low educational level, 2 medium = medium educational level, 3 high = high educational level, 4 highest = highest educational level). Significant multivariate and univariate effects (p < 0.050) are marked in bold.
Table 6. Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA 2)—main and interaction effects of language background and parental education on two WPPSI-IV ancillary index scores.
Table 6. Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA 2)—main and interaction effects of language background and parental education on two WPPSI-IV ancillary index scores.
VariablesMultivariate EffectsUnivariate Effects
Fixed factorDVWilk’s λFdf1/df2pη2parFdf1/df2pη2par
1Language backgroundVAI0.80415.9594/554<0.0010.10329.4732/278<0.0010.175
NVI 2.3732/2780.0950.017
2Parental
education
VAI0.9363.1126/5540.0050.0334.4323/2780.0050.046
NVI 2.5303/2780.0580.029
Interaction 1 × 2VAI0.9231.88312/5540.0340.0390.9736/2780.4440.021
NVI 2.7026/2780.0140.055
Note. Language background is defined as the number of acquired languages and the way they were acquired during language development (1 = simultaneous bilingual, 2 = sequential bilingual, 3 = monolingual). Parental education is defined as the highest level of education achieved by either one parent or both (1 = no to low educational level, 2 = medium educational level, 3 = high educational level, 4 = highest educational level). DV = dependent variable. VAI = Vocabulary Acquisition Index, NVI = Nonverbal Index. η2par = partial eta squared. Significant multivariate and univariate effects (p < 0.050) are marked in bold.
Table 7. Post hoc analyses for the main effects of language background and parental education on the WPPSI-IV ancillary index scores (MANOVA 2).
Table 7. Post hoc analyses for the main effects of language background and parental education on the WPPSI-IV ancillary index scores (MANOVA 2).
VariablesDescriptive StatisticsTest Statistics (Post Hoc)
DVFixed FactorFactor LevelNMSDComparisonΔMSEpCI (95%)
VAILanguage background1bi_sim6897.3212.611 vs. 28.042.167<0.0012.82; 13.26
2bi_se7789.2913.061 vs. 3−7.111.914<0.001−11.72; −2.50
3mono145104.4313.772 vs. 3−15.151.836<0.001−19.57; −10.73
Parental education1no/low6896.5017.091 vs. 21.212.136>0.999−4.47; 6.88
2medium8295.2913.771 vs. 3−3.302.426>0.999−9.75; 3.15
3high5099.8014.431 vs. 4−6.502.0930.013−12.06; −0.94
4highest90103.0013.362 vs. 3−4.512.3370.329−10.72; 1.70
2 vs. 4−7.711.988<0.001−12.99; −2.42
3 vs. 4−3.202.2970.988−9.30; 2.90
Note. DV = dependent variable. VAI = Vocabulary Acquisition Index. Language background is defined as the number of acquired languages and the way they were acquired during language development (1 bi_sim = simultaneous bilingual, 2 bi_se = sequential bilingual, 3 mono = monolingual). Parental education is defined as the highest level of education achieved by either one parent or both (1 no/low = no to low educational level, 2 medium = medium educational level, 3 high = high educational level, 4 highest = highest educational level). Significant differences between factor levels (p < 0.050) are marked in bold.
Table 8. Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA 3)—main and interaction effects of language background and parental education on the 15 WPPSI-IV subtest scaled scores.
Table 8. Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA 3)—main and interaction effects of language background and parental education on the 15 WPPSI-IV subtest scaled scores.
VariablesMultivariate EffectsUnivariate Effects
Fixed FactorDVWilk’s λFdf1/df2pη2parFdf1/df2pη2par
1Language backgroundBD0.7243.08330/528<0.0010.1491.1042/2780.3330.008
IN 11.1212/278<0.0010.074
MR 2.1212/2780.1220.015
BS 0.0722/2780.9310.001
PM 2.1502/2780.1180.015
SI 3.1622/2780.0440.022
PC 2.0082/2780.1360.014
CA 1.7112/2780.1830.012
ZL 0.3452/2780.7090.002
OA 5.0552/2780.0070.035
VC 10.3792/278<0.0010.069
AC 0.2442/2780.7830.002
CO 5.4352/2780.0050.038
RV 20.1562/278<0.0010.127
PN 23.2662/278<0.0010.143
2Parental educationBD0.7371.88545/785<0.0010.0972.2133/2780.0870.023
IN 7.1503/278<0.0010.072
MR 0.4833/2780.6940.005
BS 6.2063/278<0.0010.063
PM 1.4293/2780.2340.015
SI 5.1123/2780.0020.052
PC 1.0833/2780.3560.012
CA 0.9663/2780.4090.010
ZL 0.5543/2780.6460.006
OA 2.1923/2780.0890.023
VC 3.3173/2780.0200.035
AC 3.1743/2780.0250.033
CO 3.2003/2780.0240.033
RV 3.8043/2780.0110.039
PN 3.1983/2780.0240.033
Interaction 1 × 2BD0.7011.08190/14910.2890.0571.4476/2780.1970.030
IN 1.5006/2780.1780.031
MR 1.8976/2780.0810.039
BS 1.7476/2780.1100.039
PM 1.9176/2780.0780.040
SI 2.1546/2780.0480.044
PC 1.5956/2780.1490.033
CA 0.8126/2780.5610.017
ZL 1.7996/2780.0990.037
OA 1.3186/2780.2490.028
VC 2.2346/2780.0400.046
AC 0.9036/2780.4930.019
CO 0.9496/2780.4600.020
RV 1.2636/2780.2750.027
PN 0.4906/2780.8160.010
Note. Language background is defined as the number of acquired languages and the way they were acquired during language development (1 = simultaneous bilingual, 2 = sequential bilingual, 3 = monolingual). Parental education is defined as the highest level of education achieved by either one parent or both (1 = no to low educational level, 2 = medium educational level, 3 = high educational level, 4 = highest educational level). BD = Block Design, IN = Information, MR = Matrix Reasoning, BS = Bug Search, PM = Picture Memory, SI = Similarities, PC = Picture Concepts, CA = Cancellation, ZL = Zoo Locations, OA = Object Assembly, VC = Vocabulary, AC = Animal Coding, CO = Comprehension, RV = Receptive Vocabulary, PN = Picture Naming. η2par = partial eta squared. Significant multivariate and univariate effects (p < 0.050) are marked in bold.
Table 9. Post hoc analyses for the main effects of language background and parental education on the WPPSI-IV subtest scaled scores (MANOVA 3).
Table 9. Post hoc analyses for the main effects of language background and parental education on the WPPSI-IV subtest scaled scores (MANOVA 3).
VariablesDescriptive StatisticsTest Statistics (Post Hoc)
DVFixed FactorFactor LevelNMSDComparisonΔMSEpCI (95%)
INLanguage background1bi_sim6810.212.601 vs. 21.340.4600.0120.23; 2.44
2bi_se778.872.971 vs. 3−0.640.4060.345−1.62; 0.34
3mono14510.853.012 vs. 3−1.980.390<0.001−2.92; −1.04
Parental education1no/low6810.283.081 vs. 21.460.4530.0080.26; 2.67
2medium828.822.761 vs. 3−0.260.515>0.999−1.63; 1.11
3high5010.542.841 vs. 4−0.840.4440.352−2.02; 0.34
4highest9011.122.882 vs. 3−1.720.4960.004−3.04; −0.41
2 vs. 4−2.310.422<0.001−3.43; −1.18
3 vs. 4−0.580.487>0.999−1.88; 0.71
BSParental education1no/low689.093.201 vs. 2−1.390.5000.036−2.72; −0.06
2medium8210.483.181 vs. 3−2.070.5680.002−3.58; −0.56
3high5011.163.141 vs. 4−1.380.4900.032−2.68; −0.08
4highest9010.472.812 vs. 3−0.680.547>0.999−2.14; 0.77
2 vs. 40.010.466>0.999−1.23; 1.25
3 vs. 40.690.538>0.999−0.74; 2.12
SILanguage background1bi_sim6810.473.101 vs. 20.790.4940.325−0.39; 1.98
2bi_se779.682.811 vs. 3−0.440.4360.942−1.49; 0.61
3mono14510.913.202 vs. 3−1.240.4180.010−2.24; −0.23
Parental education1no/low689.533.411 vs. 2−0.460.486>0.999−1.75; 0.83
2medium829.992.921 vs. 3−1.470.5520.049−2.94; 0.01
3high5011.002.571 vs. 4−1.830.477<0.001−3.09; −0.56
4highest9011.363.072 vs. 3−1.010.5320.349−2.43; 0.40
2 vs. 4−1.370.4530.017−2.57; −0.16
3 vs. 4−0.360.523>0.999−1.75; 1.03
OALanguage background1bi_sim6810.402.931 vs. 20.450.478>0.999−0.70; 1.60
2bi_se779.952.871 vs. 3−0.670.4220.339−1.69; 0.35
3mono14511.072.902 vs. 3−1.120.4050.018−2.10; −0.14
VCLanguage background1bi_sim6810.542.791 vs. 21.690.449<0.0010.61; 2.77
2bi_se778.863.011 vs. 3−0.220.397>0.999−1.18; 0.73
3mono14510.772.622 vs. 3−1.910.381<0.001−2.82; −0.99
Parental education1no/low689.602.871 vs. 2−0,190.443>0.999−1.37; 0.99
2medium829.792.921 vs. 3−1.360.5030.044−2.69; −0.02
3high5010.962.731 vs. 4−1.020.4340.117−2.17; 0.13
4highest9010.622.812 vs. 3−1.170.4840.099−2.45; 0.12
2 vs. 4−0.830.4120.270−1.92; 0.27
3 vs. 40.340.476>0.999−0.93; 1.60
ACParental education1no/low689.763.171 vs. 2−0.860.4940.504−2.17; 0.46
2medium8210.623.031 vs. 3−1.480.5610.049−2.97; 0.02
3high5011.242.801 vs. 4−0.850.4840.490−2.13; 0.44
4highest9010.612.962 vs. 3−0.620.541> 0.999−2.06; 0.82
2 vs. 40.010.460> 0.999−1.21; 1.23
3 vs. 40.630.532> 0.999−0.78; 2.04
COLanguage background1bi_sim6810.382.781 vs. 21.370.4760.0130.22; 2.52
2bi_se779.012.891 vs. 30.120.421> 0.999−0.89; 1.13
3mono14510.262.962 vs. 3−1.250.4040.007−2.22; −0.28
Parental education1no/low689.222.661 vs. 2−0.390.469> 0.999−1.64; 0.86
2medium829.613.001 vs. 3−1.420.5330.049−2.84; 0.01
3high5010.643.101 vs. 4−1.230.4600.046−2.46; −0.01
4highest9010.462.882 vs. 3−1.030.5140.275−2.40; 0.33
2 vs. 4−0.850.4370.324−2.01; 0.32
3 vs. 40.180.505> 0.999−1.16; 1.53
RVLanguage background1bi_sim689.602.571 vs. 21.400.4450.0060.32; 2.47
2bi_se778.212.531 vs. 3−1.220.3930.006−2.17; −0.28
3mono14510.832.922 vs. 3−2.620.377< 0.001−3.53; −1.71
Parental education1no/low689.353.311 vs. 20.120.439> 0.999−1.05; 1.29
2medium829.232.691 vs. 3−0.590.499> 0.999−1.91; 0.74
3high509.942.971 vs. 4−1.370.4300.010−2.51; −0.23
4highest9010.722.672 vs. 3−0.710.4800.849−1.98; 0.57
2 vs. 4−1.490.4090.002−2.58; −0.40
3 vs. 4−0.780.4720.592−2.04; 0.47
PNLanguage background1bi_sim689.492.761 vs. 21.460.4550.0040.36; 2.55
2bi_se778.032.791 vs. 3−1.320.4010.003−2.29; −0.35
3mono14510.812.732 vs. 3−2.780.385< 0.001−3.71; −1.85
Parental education1no/low689.413.321 vs. 20.280.448> 0.999−0.91; 1.47
2medium829.132.711 vs. 3−0.650.509> 0.999−2.00; 0.70
3high5010.062.801 vs. 4−1.010.4390.132−2.18; 0.16
4highest9010.422.932 vs. 3−0.930.4900.359−2.23; 0.38
2 vs. 4−1.290.4170.013−2.40; −0.18
3 vs. 4−0.360.482> 0.999−1.64; 0.92
Note. DV = dependent variable. IN = Information, BS = Bug Search, SI = Similarities, OA = Object Assembly, VC = Vocabulary, AC = Animal Coding, CO = Comprehension, RV = Receptive Vocabulary, PN = Picture Naming. Language background is defined as the number of acquired languages and the way they were acquired during language development (1 bi_sim = simultaneous bilingual, 2 bi_se = sequential bilingual, 3 mono = monolingual). Parental education is defined as the highest level of education achieved by either one parent or both (1 no/low = no to low educational level, 2 medium = medium educational level, 3 high = high educational level, 4 highest = highest educational level). Significant differences between factor levels (p < 0.050) are marked in bold.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Walter, F.; Daseking, M.; Pauls, F. The Effects of Language Background and Parental Education on Measures of Cognitive Ability: An Analysis of the WPPSI-IV Cognitive Profiles of Monolingual, Simultaneous Bilingual, and Sequential Bilingual German Children Aged 4 to 7 Years. Children 2024, 11, 631. https://doi.org/10.3390/children11060631

AMA Style

Walter F, Daseking M, Pauls F. The Effects of Language Background and Parental Education on Measures of Cognitive Ability: An Analysis of the WPPSI-IV Cognitive Profiles of Monolingual, Simultaneous Bilingual, and Sequential Bilingual German Children Aged 4 to 7 Years. Children. 2024; 11(6):631. https://doi.org/10.3390/children11060631

Chicago/Turabian Style

Walter, Franziska, Monika Daseking, and Franz Pauls. 2024. "The Effects of Language Background and Parental Education on Measures of Cognitive Ability: An Analysis of the WPPSI-IV Cognitive Profiles of Monolingual, Simultaneous Bilingual, and Sequential Bilingual German Children Aged 4 to 7 Years" Children 11, no. 6: 631. https://doi.org/10.3390/children11060631

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop