Next Article in Journal
Emotional Reactivity and Behavioral Problems in Preschoolers: The Interplay of Parental Stress, Media-Related Coping, and Child Screen Time
Previous Article in Journal
Outcomes of Pediatric Patients with Excessive Postural Tachycardia After Attending an Intensive Interdisciplinary Pain Program: A Pilot Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
A Systematic Review of Treatment for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: The Sensory Processing and Sensory Integration Approach
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Relations Between Sensory Modulation, Hyper Arousability and Psychopathology in Adolescents with Anxiety Disorders

1
Department of Occupational Therapy, Faculty of Social Welfare and Health Sciences, University of Haifa, Haifa 3498838, Israel
2
Child and Adolescent Mental Health Clinic, Haemeq Medical Center, Afula 1834111, Israel
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Children 2025, 12(2), 187; https://doi.org/10.3390/children12020187
Submission received: 3 January 2025 / Revised: 2 February 2025 / Accepted: 3 February 2025 / Published: 5 February 2025

Abstract

:
Background: Sensory modulation may play a significant role in psychiatric conditions, including anxiety, and explain arousability levels, behavioral disorders, and functional deficits. Yet, studies about sensory modulation in adolescents with anxiety disorders are scarce. Purpose: To profile the prevalence of sensory modulation difficulties (SMDs) in adolescents with anxiety and examine their relations to arousability and psychopathology. The study compared adolescents with anxiety disorders to healthy controls using physiological measures and self-reports that reflect daily life scenarios. Then, the study examined the relationship between SMDs, arousability, and psychopathological severity in the study group. Method: Participants were 106 adolescents, aged 10.5–18 years and their parents. The study group included 44 participants diagnosed with anxiety disorder by psychiatrists. The control group included 62 healthy participants matched by age and gender to the study group. Parents completed the demographic questionnaire and the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL). The adolescents completed The Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale (RCMAS) and the Adolescent/Adult Sensory Profile (AASP) and underwent the electrodermal activity (EDA) and pulse rate tests while listening to extreme sensory stimuli of auditory startles. Results: Based on AASP, the study group had a higher prevalence of SMDs expressed in lower sensory seeking, difficulties in registering sensory stimuli, and higher sensory sensitivity and avoidance. The study group presented higher arousability while listening to the startles as manifested in higher heart rate and EDA responses. The physiological results correlated with SMD levels measured by the AASP self-reports. SMDs correlated with psychopathological severity. Conclusions: SMDs may characterize adolescents with anxiety disorders and impact their arousability, symptoms severity, and daily functioning. Therefore, sensory modulation should be evaluated using both self-reports (to reflect implications in real life from patients’ own voices) along with objective measures to explain daily behaviors by underlying physiological mechanisms. This may focus intervention towards better health, function, and development.

1. Introduction

Anxiety disorders are prevalent among 20.5% of children and adolescents around the world [1]. As outlined in The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-V), anxiety disorders include social anxiety disorder (SAD), specific phobias, generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), panic disorder (PD), separation anxiety, selective mutism, and agoraphobia. Among children and adolescents under the age of 18, the definition of anxiety disorder is relevant if symptoms last for at least six months [2]. Anxiety disorders often coexist with other psychiatric conditions, like attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), major depression, somatic symptom disorders, personality disorders, and substance abuse disorders [3,4]. The symptoms of anxiety disorders include worry about the future, obsessive and repetitive thoughts, and excessive fear [2,5] that physiologically are manifested in hyper-arousability and elevated sympathetic activity (increased heart rate, shortness of breath, sweating, tremor, chest pain, nausea, etc.). Hyper-arousability may be triggered by a disproportionate response to stimuli from the environment that are perceived as threatening [6]. This may explain the high prevalence of sensory modulation difficulties (SMDs), mainly sensory hypersensitivity in patients with anxiety disorders [7,8,9,10].
Sensory modulation refers to the ability of the central nervous system to modulate and organize sensory input in a graded manner according to environmental demands [11]. Difficulties in sensory modulation result in hypersensitivity or hyposensitivity and lead to unmodulated behavior and malfunction. One main model that refers to the implications of sensory modulation on behavior is Dunn’s model. According to this model, the interaction between the neural threshold for sensory input and the strategies used for coping with the neural threshold (active/passive) defines behavioral and functional outcomes [11]. This interaction results in four profiles: sensory sensitivity (low neural threshold and passive strategy)—people who do not actively eliminate unpleasant stimuli; sensation avoidance (low neural threshold and an active strategy)—people who actively avoid unpleasant stimuli, such as avoiding walking on sand; low registration (high neural threshold and passive strategy)—people who need extensive intensity of sensory stimuli to recognize it, but do not actively seek rich sensory input (as sports activity), therefore, may miss information, and be less attentive; sensory seeking (high neural threshold and an active strategy)—people who actively seek for intense stimuli in their environment, as going to parties; coloring their rooms; do extreme sport [12]. One of the gold standard measures for sensory modulation is the Sensory Profile [13]. Based on the sensory profile, SMDs are prevalent among 57% of children with psychiatric disorders [14]. The significant benefit of the sensory profile is that it reflects SMDs as expressed in behavioral responses to sensory input in daily life scenarios.
Indeed, when the ability to modulate sensory information is impaired, it affects the individual’s ability to properly perceive the environment and function in it. Therefore, anxiety and hyper-arousability may be elevated, as manifested in objective physiological measures such as electrodermal activity (EDA) [15], higher cortisol levels [16], and elevated heart rate [17,18,19,20]. In children, anxiety symptoms were manifested in physiological measures such as over-reactive skin conductance (EDA) [21]. Hence, the objective and subjective measures may explain unmodulated behaviors such as “fight or flight,” impulsivity, distraction, nonadaptive behavior and daily dysfunction [22].
Hence, the complexity of the symptoms of anxiety disorders, the prevalent comorbidities of SMDs as well as their severe consequences on a child’s/adolescent’s daily life, serve the rationale for studying the interrelation between these factors in depth in order to improve intervention. This knowledge is especially critical in adolescence where development is essential for determining later human life and wellbeing. Therefore, this study aimed to (1) profile sensory modulation of adolescents with anxiety disorders, as compared to healthy controls, using objective and subjective measures and (2) among the study group, examine the relationship between sensory modulation and anxiety psychopathology.
Study hypotheses: (1) adolescents with anxiety disorders would have a significantly higher prevalence of SMDs as expressed in self-reports and in objective measures of sympathetic hyper-arousability than healthy controls; (2) among the study group—(a) SMDs according to self-reports would correlate with the objective manifestations and (b) SMDs would significantly correlate with severe psychopathology.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

This study is a cross-sectional and correlative study. Based on the power calculation of MANOVA global effects, with f2 = 0.07 at a significance level of p = 0.05 and power = 0.80, this study was supposed to include 142 adolescents. However, due to various constraints, this study included 106 adolescents. The participants’ age range was 10.5–18 years. The study group included 44 adolescents, 21 boys and 23 girls, with a mean age of 13.69 (SD = 1.99), diagnosed with anxiety disorder by psychiatrists, according to the DSM-5 and a clinical interview. All participants of the study group were patients of the Child and Adolescent Mental Health Clinic (blinded). The control group included 62 healthy adolescents, 33 boys and 29 girls, mean age = 13.39 (SD = 1.99), with normal development and emotional health, based on their parents’ report and their Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) normal scores (Achenbach and Rescorla, 2001) [23]. The study groups were matched by age (t = −0.76, p = 0.80) and gender (χ2 (1) = 0.31, p = 0.57) but significantly differed in mother’s education (χ2 (1) = 15.66, p < 0.001) and in socioeconomic status (χ2 (2) = 21.14, p < 0.001) (see Table 1 and Table 2). Mothers’ education and socioeconomic status parameters were significantly positively correlated. Therefore, statistical adjustments were performed and the mother’s education was held as a covariant. Exclusion criteria: adolescents with unstable severe health/chronic illnesses who are taking pharmacological treatment that has a detrimental effect on the nervous system or addictive substances (beyond the psychiatric medications in the study group) and those with uncorrected sensory impairment (e.g., by glasses).

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Subjective Measures—Self-Reports

Completed by Parents

A sociodemographic questionnaire.
The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) [23] was used to profile psychopathological symptoms. This standard measure screens behavioral and emotional problems in children and adolescents ages 4–18. This measure includes two sections: 20 items that measure social functioning and 113 items that measure emotional–behavioral problems (behavioral profile). For this study, the behavioral part was used. Answers are marked on a 0–2 scale (0 = not true for my child, 1 = somewhat/sometimes true, and 2 = very true or often true). Parents rate the items of problems based on the past six months. Raw scores are converted into T scores that indicate whether the participant exhibits abnormal behaviors with respect to norms by age and gender. A clinical cross-sectional score above 63, based on the total T-score, indicates clinical functioning, while scores between 60–63 suggest borderline functioning and scores below 60 are considered within the normal range. This measure demonstrates high test-retest reliability of 0.9, strong internal consistency of 0.72–0.97, and good criterion validity [23].

Completed by the Adolescents

The Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale (RCMAS) [24] aims to profile anxiety levels in children and adolescents aged 6–19 years. The RCMAS includes 37 items, divided into three scales: The Physiological Scale consists of 10 items related to physical symptoms of anxiety, such as sleep problems, stomach issues and breathing difficulties. The Worry and Oversensitivity Scale consists of 11 items related to excessive, unfocused worrying and concern about external stressors. The Social Concern and Concentration Scale includes 7 items related to fears of social isolation and attention difficulties. The remaining 9 items comprise The Lie Scale, which measures social desirability and “fake good”. Each item describes an experience, respondents note “Yes” or “No”. If the response is “Yes” it indicates the presence of an anxiety symptom and it receives a score of 1. If the response is “No” it indicates the absence of an anxiety symptom and it receives a score of 0. The total score is calculated based on the three scales of the RCMAS which consist of 28 of the 37 items (the 9 items of the lie scale are excluded). Higher scores indicate higher levels of anxiety.
The RCMAS has good psychometric properties [24,25]. According to the standardization sample data [24], a T-score of 60 is equivalent to the cut-off score of 19 (M = 11.70, SD = 6.21).
Adolescent/Adult Sensory Profile (AASP) Questionnaire [26]: This 60-item self-report measures sensory modulation among individuals aged 11 and above. Respondents rate the frequency of their behavioral reactions to sensory experiences in everyday life (for example: “I’m uncomfortable wearing certain fabrics”; “I avoid elevators and/or escalators because I dislike the movement”). Answers are scored on a 1–5 Likert scale (1 = almost never, 5 = almost always). The higher the score, the more extreme the sensory modulation pattern. The AASP has four outcome measures in accordance with the four quadrants of Dunn’s model (see above): low registration, sensory seeking, sensory sensitivity, and sensation avoidance (15 items in each category). Five categories are identified in each sensory modulation pattern: “much less than most people,” “less than most people,” “similar to most people,” “more than most people,” and “much more than most people” [26]. In the present study, the five categories were combined into three: (1) “less than most people”—that is, one to two standard deviations below the norm, (2) “similar to most people”—within the norm, and (3) “more than most people”—one to two standard deviations above the norm. Sensory modulation norms exist for different age groups (11–17, 18–64, 65 and over). SMDs are defined for participants found in the “less” or “more than most people” ranges. Internal reliability was found to range from 0.64–0.78 [26]. Discriminant validity was found between groups in the psychiatric population [27].

2.2.2. Objective Measures

In order to reflect the physiological manifestations of SMDs, and based on the literature on SMDs, the following measures examine arousability/sympathetic reactivity to sensory stimuli of auditory startles:
An electrodermal activity test (EDA) [28] measures the skin conductivity (in units of micro-siemens) due to the activity of the sweat glands in the skin in response to a stressogenic stimulus. Two 5 mm electrodes (Mindlife, Jerusalem, Israel) are connected to the second and fourth fingertips of the non-dominant hand using a Velcro strap. The electrodes are connected to a sensor, receiver, and amplifier, providing 10 samples per second [28]. The experiment is divided into three phases, while the subject is sitting in front of a computer screen watching kaleidoscope shapes in order to neutralize stimuli distractions from the environment and to increase compliance which is often used in psychophysiology research [29]. The first phase lasts a minute (for acclimatization). In this phase, the subjects hear white noise through the headphones. The second phase—the startles session—includes the auditory startles which appear randomly, 40 times, for 3 min, at a volume of 90 dB with each stimulus lasting 30 ms. In the third and final phase, the relaxation session, white noise is heard again for one minute (allowing adjustment). The experiment lasts 5 min in total.
The outcome measures of the EDA test are (1) reaction time (in seconds) and amplitude height in response to the first startle; (2) time (in seconds) and height of the maximum amplitude during the 3 min of auditory startles—to measure maximal arousability; (3) the following measures reflected adaptation: time and height of minimal amplitude during the 3 min of auditory startles session; and (4) time and height of maximum and minimum amplitude at the time of relaxation session (third session).
A pulse oximeter (Contec Systems CMS50N; Qinhuangdao, China) is an additional measure of sympathetic activity reflected in heart rate in response to auditory startles. The pulse oximeter was attached to the other finger of the hand that is not connected to the EDA test during all EDA sessions.

2.3. Procedure

After obtaining ethics approval from the [blinded] and from the Helsinki Committee [blinded], adolescents and their parents from the study group were recruited via advertisements published at the Child and Adolescent Mental Health Clinic [blinded]. Participants in the control group were recruited through similar advertisements published in neighborhoods in the same geographic area.
Parents interested in participating with their children in this study were contacted by the study conductor and a meeting was scheduled with them at the clinic (study group) or at their homes (control group). In the meeting, the participants and their parents received a detailed explanation about the study and observing ethical rules declaration. The parents signed the informed consent form and completed the demographic questionnaire and the CBCL. Adolescents underwent the EDA test and the heart rate measurement. Then, they completed the RCMAS and the AASP with the study conductor (See Figure 1).
All questionnaires were anonymous. Each questionnaire received a serial number without identifying details of the participants and the data was stored in a protected folder on the researcher’s computer and all paper data was kept in a locked closet.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

Data was analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS)-28. After analyzing descriptive statistics and testing normality, the data distribution was found to be normal in all study measures, except for the CBCL scores in the control group. Therefore, chi-square analysis examined the prevalence of SMDs according to the AASP norms, clinical emotional behavior problems according to the CBCL and anxiety problems according to the RCMAS cutoff scores. T-tests examined the differences between the groups in socio-demographic parameters. For examining the first hypothesis, a univariate analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) examined the differences between the study groups in total scores of the RCMAS and heart rate. A multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) examined the differences between groups in the subscales scores of the RCMAS, AASP, and EDA scores. Mother’s education was held as a covariate throughout the analyses. A Mann–Whitney test examined the differences between groups in CBCL scores. For examining the second hypothesis, the Pearson correlation test examined the correlations between the subjective (AASP) and objective (EDA and heartrate) measures and between sensory modulation and psychopathological symptoms severity according to the RCMAS and the CBCL. The significance level was p ≤ 0.05.
In our study, the control group is larger in terms of the number of participants than the study group due to difficulties in recruiting participants for the study group. Furthermore, some participants were missing part of the data (for example, one of the questionnaires), so the degrees of freedom varied accordingly. In the context of the EDA test, for example, one participant in the study group had no responses to the test, and six participants showed no response to the first stimulus.

3. Results

Before examining the study’s hypotheses, both groups were compared in their CBCL and RCMAS scores to verify inclusion criteria and provide the profile of each group.

3.1. The Differences in Emotional and Behavioral Modulation Psychopathology Between Both Groups According to the CBCL

According to the Mann–Whitney test, emotional and behavioral modulation problems were significantly more prevalent among the study group compared to the control group, as reflected in all CBCL scales and in the total score: the study group had greater difficulties in internalizing and externalizing symptoms. The study group had higher difficulties in anxiety symptoms, depression symptoms, and somatic complaints. Moreover, the study group showed significant behavioral problems such as social, thought and attention problems, rule-breaking, and aggressive behavior. (See Table 3, Figure 2)
According to chi-square analysis and the cutoff total CBCL score of 63, the study group had a significantly higher prevalence of clinical emotional and behavioral modulation problems (62.8%, CI95 48–76%) than the controls (0%, CI95 0–6%) (χ2 = 51.059, p < 0.001). The Fischer exact test indicated a significant difference between groups (p < 0.001).

3.2. The Differences in Anxiety Rates Between Both Groups According to the RCMAS

According to the MANCOVA and ANCOVA tests, anxiety symptoms were more prevalent among the study group, as reflected in all RCMAS scales: physiological, worry and over-responsivity, social concern, and concentration symptoms, except for the lie scale, and in total score, accordingly. The effect size for the observed anxiety symptoms variables was the partial eta square ηp2 which ranged from 0.289 to 0.373, indicating large effect sizes (see Table 4, Figure 3).
According to chi-square analysis and the cutoff score of 19, the study group had a significantly higher prevalence of anxiety (54.5%, CI95 40.07–68.3%) than the controls (4.8%, CI95 1.66–13.29%) (χ2 = 33.496, p < 0.001). The Fischer exact test indicated a significant difference between groups (p < 0.001).
Hypothesis 1.
Differences between the study groups in sensory modulation.

3.3. Subjective Measure—The AASP Self-Report

According to the MANCOVA test, a significantly higher prevalence of SMDs was found among the study group as manifested in the four AASP sensory profiles: adolescents with anxiety disorders had significantly greater sensory sensitivity and avoidance, lower ability to register sensory input, and lower tendency to seek sensory input. The effect size of the observed variables of SMDs was the partial eta square ηp2 that ranged from 0.145 to 0.278, indicating large effect sizes (see Table 5, Figure 4). As presented in Table 6, according to chi-square analysis, this was also manifested in higher prevalence of abnormal SMDs (based on AASP normal/abnormal ranges) among the study group, which showed greater sensory sensitivity (χ2 = 26.025, p < 0.001), higher difficulty to register sensory input (χ2 = 21.328, p < 0.001), greater sensation avoidance (χ2 = 29.457, p < 0.001), and lower tendency to seek for sensory input (χ2 = 19.082, p < 0.001). The Fischer exact test indicated a significant difference between groups (p < 0.001).

3.4. Objective Measures–EDA and Heartrate

According to the MANCOVA test, greater reactivity to auditory startles was found in the EDA of the study group as compared to the controls. In response to the first stimulus, the study group had a significantly shorter reaction time and higher amplitude height. When referring to session 2 (the startles session) and 3 (the relaxation session), the study group had a significantly higher height of the maximum amplitude, reflecting higher arousability and difficulties in adapting and relaxing.
According to the ANCOVA test, the study group also showed higher heart rate than the control group at the beginning and the end of the EDA test. The effect size of all the observed variables was the partial eta square ηp2 that ranged from 0.048 to 0.386, indicating small to large effect sizes (see Table 7, Figure 5 and Figure 6). Nevertheless, no significant differences were found between the study groups in the heart rate delta (starting point and ending of the EDA procedure).
Hypothesis 2.
Correlations between variables in the study group.

3.5. The Correlations Between Subjective and Objective Measures

According to the Pearson correlation test, medium significant negative correlations were found between the AASP and the physiological manifestations—the EDA and heart rate. Lower sensory seeking correlated with higher amplitude height in response to the first startle (r = −0.327, p = 0.05), with the maximal (r = −0.415, p = 0.007) and the minimal amplitude (r = −0.458, p = 0.003) during the startles’ session as well as with the maximal (r = −0.332, p = 0.034) and the minimal amplitude (r = −0.355, p = 0.023) after the startles’ session—in the relaxation session. Lower sensory seeking also correlated with the time of the maximal amplitude (r = −0.340, p = 0.03) in relaxation and with elevated heart rate at the beginning of the EDA test (before starting the startles session) (r = −0.355, p = 0.034).

3.6. The Correlations Between the Subjective AASP and the Psychopathological Symptoms in Both Groups

As reported in Table 8, according to the Pearson correlation test, in children with anxiety, medium to large significant positive correlations were found between the AASP patterns’ scores and anxiety psychopathological symptoms according to the RCMAS: greater sensory sensitivity and sensation avoidance significantly positively correlated with higher levels of anxiety, especially, with the physiological, worry, and over-responsivity, social concern problems, and total anxiety with large effect sizes. Lower registration of sensory input significantly positively correlated with the RCMAS physiological scale and total anxiety score with medium effect sizes.
In the control group, medium to large significant positive correlations were found between the AASP patterns’ scores and anxiety psychopathological symptoms according to the RCMAS: greater sensory sensitivity, low registration and sensation avoidance significantly positively correlated with higher levels of anxiety, especially, with the physiological, worry and over-responsivity, social concern problems, and total anxiety.
Medium significant positive correlations were found between AASP scales and CBCL scales. In the study group: sensation avoidance significantly positively correlated with emotional psychopathological symptoms of withdrawn/depressed, somatic complaints, and internalization. AASP sensation avoidance correlated with worse CBCL psychopathological symptoms of withdrawal/depression, somatic complaints, and internalization. In the control group, greater sensory sensitivity significantly positively correlated with higher anxious/depressed symptoms and somatic complaints; greater low registration significantly positively correlated with higher anxious/depressed symptoms; greater sensation avoidance significantly positively correlated with higher anxious/depressed symptoms and internalization. (See Table 8).

4. Discussion

The present study aimed to profile SMDs in adolescents with anxiety disorders and examine their relation to psychopathology. This study used subjective self-reports to reflect SMDs in daily life scenarios from the individual’s own voice, and objective measures to reflect underlying mechanisms that may explain behavioral responses and functional restrictions.
The first hypothesis was confirmed: SMDs were more prevalent among the study group. The AASP subjective self-report found that adolescents with anxiety disorders had higher sensory sensitivity and avoidance, lower ability to register sensory input, and lower tendency to seek sensory input than the controls. A recent meta and retrospective analysis of people with various psychiatric conditions found that based on AASP, SMDs were prevalent and expressed in elevated sensory sensitivity, sensation avoidance, low registration, and a lower tendency to seek sensory input [30,31]. Our results confirm earlier findings according to which SMDs are prevalent among children and adolescents with anxiety disorders [32].
Previous reports which also used both self-reports and objective measures (such as EDA, and functional neuroimaging), were performed on various age groups—from early childhood to adulthood but referred to multiple psychiatric disorders. Tolin et al. (2021) who focused on adults with anxiety disorders used EDA and heart rate recordings in response to auditory startles and found sympathetic hyper-arousability according to both measures. They also found lower autonomic recovery after removing the auditory stressor [18].
Our study focused on anxiety in adolescence and highlighted how important it is to elaborate on the knowledge regarding SMDs, their relation to anxiety severity, and their functional implications in this critical developmental stage. Moreover, our study emphasized the sensitivity of the AASP in demonstrating unmodulated perception of sensory stimuli among adolescents with anxiety disorders. Support for that was found by the objective measures (EDA and heart rate) which depicted higher reactivity and lower adaptation to sensory input. Even though most adolescents in the study group were under medications, they still exhibited an elevated physiological hyper-arousability—in response to the first stressor auditory startle, during the startles’ session (in which adaptation should have been found, in response to the repeated auditory stimuli), and even during the third session of relaxation, emphasizing the lingering effect of sensory stimuli. This information may shed light on daily function obstacles that these adolescents experience. Therefore, hyper-arousability should be evaluated in adolescents with anxiety disorders in order to deeply understand its consequences on patients’ functioning and pathogenesis severity.
With respect to the second hypothesis, SMDs (both sensory sensitivity and sensation avoidance), significantly correlated with anxiety psychopathology severity as manifested in RCMAS results (higher levels of worry and over-responsivity, social concern, and concentration problems) in both study groups. In the study group, the relations with anxiety psychopathology but not with other psychopathological symptoms of the CBCL may be related to the fact that this measure includes behavioral and externalizing symptoms not related to anxiety. Moreover, CBCL was measured according to parents’ reports, while the RCMAS was according to the adolescents’ reports themselves. The relation between SMDs and psychopathology severity was found in studies on other clinical populations. For example, MacLennan, Roach, and Tavassoli (2020) studied 41 autistic children aged between 3 and 14 years using parent and self-reported measures and found that greater sensory hyperreactivity correlated with elevated phobia-related symptoms, total anxiety, and separation anxiety [33]. Interestingly, in the present study, patterns related to sensory hypersensitivity correlated with worse anxiety psychopathology, while sensory seeking correlated with a better clinical profile and more adaptive reactivity and modulation to sensory input. Our results are consistent with prior research on patients with psychiatric disorders indicating that sensory seeking correlated with a better clinical profile and a better quality of life and, therefore, might presumably serve as a protective pattern [34].
The results of the present study support the previous literature highlighting the role of SMDs in anxiety development [35] and psychopathology [6,7]. Studies found that SMDs in adolescents with anxiety disorders are related to nonadaptive behavior [36], worse social functioning [37], and sleep problems [38]. Therefore, SMDs in anxiety disorders should receive clinical and scientific attention [31]. Detailed evaluation of sensory modulation should be performed based on self-reports and objective physiological measures. Non-invasive physiological indicators of autonomic activity [39] such as EDA offer an excellent opportunity to assess and monitor the physiological alterations under different conditions of anxiety [40,41]. Practically, these measures may exhibit clinical utility for aiding clinicians in monitoring the progress of treatment [42,43]. In research, these measures may help in conceptualizing biopsychological processes that can be targeted to optimize care [44]. This detailed information may focus intervention according to the individual unique sensory profiles, unique pathogenesis, and real-life context [34].
In therapy, sensory-based intervention strategies may be relevant for adolescents with anxiety disorders [6,45]. Therapists should elevate the awareness of adolescents, parents, teachers, and health providers to the involvement of SMDs in the pathogenesis of anxiety and on daily function; Applying sensory stimuli that fit the individual’s sensory profile may assist in modulating behavior and function. For example, weighted blankets, Yuckee balls, and wraps may enhance the perception of sensory input to low registrators. For hyper-sensitive individuals, relaxation techniques using sensory-based strategies (such as massage) may be relevant [45]. Sensory modulation intervention may be combined with cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) and provide strategies to cope with challenging sensory situations in daily life [45].

5. Conclusions

SMDs may be prevalent in adolescents with anxiety disorders and explain behavioral hyper-arousablity and functional deficits. SMDs may be related to psychopathological symptoms of anxiety. Since sensory modulation plays a crucial role in the way we perceive the world, connect with others, and function, SMDs should be evaluated in adolescents with anxiety disorders. Evaluations should include self-reports and objective physiological measurements. This may improve intervention outcomes, decrease in the severity of the psychopathological symptoms, and improve function, development, and well-being.

6. Research Limitations

The findings of the present study should be interpreted in light of the following limitations: the study groups varied in certain socio-demographic parameters. The study group included various anxiety disorders with different coexistence psychiatric disorders. Therefore, it is recommended to conduct future studies on larger samples with similar socio-demographic characteristics and specific anxiety disorders to enable the generalizability of the results.
Regarding discrepancies identified between the RCMAS and CBCL questionnaires’ correlational results with the AASP, these should be assessed by principal component analysis (PCA) or other multivariate analysis in future studies, using a larger sample.
Finally, most participants in the study group were under psychiatric medication (such as antidepressants), future studies should consider the possible impacts of medications on the results.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization G.H., A.A.-M., H.J., R.A.-T. and B.E.-Y.; methodology G.H. and B.E.-Y.; formal analysis G.H., H.J., R.A.-T. and B.E.-Y.; data curation, G.H., A.A.-M., H.J. and R.A.-T.; writing—original draft preparation, G.H. and B.E.-Y.; writing—review and editing G.H. and B.E.-Y.; supervision, B.E.-Y. and A.A.-M.; project administration, G.H., A.A.-M., H.J. and R.A.-T. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki [BLINDED] (approval code: 0045-22-EMC, approved on 31 October 2022) and the Institutional Review Board of the [BLINDED] (approval code: 333/18, approved on 17 September 2018; an extension- 333/18-1 was approved on 18 December 2022).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are not publicly available due to privacy and ethical restrictions.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Racine, N.; McArthur, B.A.; Cooke, J.E.; Eirich, R.; Zhu, J.; Madigan, S. Global prevalence of depressive and anxiety symptoms in children and adolescents during COVID-19: A meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2021, 175, 1142–1150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013; Volume 5. [Google Scholar]
  3. D’Agati, E.; Curatolo, P.; Mazzone, L. Comorbidity between ADHD and anxiety disorders across the lifespan. Int. J. Psychiatry 2019, 23, 238–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Kessler, R.C.; Chiu, W.T.; Demler, O.; Walters, E.E. Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2005, 62, 617–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Ramsawh, H.J.; Chavira, D.A.; Stein, M.B. Burden of anxiety disorders in pediatric medical settings: Prevalence, phenomenology, and a research agenda. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2010, 164, 965–972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Wallis, K.; Sutton, D.; Bassett, S. Sensory modulation for people with anxiety in a community mental health setting. Occup. Ther. Ment. Health 2018, 34, 122–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. MacLennan, K.; Rossow, T.; Tavassoli, T. The relationship between sensory reactivity, intolerance of uncertainty and anxiety subtypes in preschool-age autistic children. Autism 2021, 25, 2305–2316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Rodriguez, M.; Kross, E. Sensory emotion regulation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2023, 27, 379–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Engel-Yeger, B.; Dunn, W. The relationship between sensory processing difficulties and anxiety level of healthy adults. Br. J. Occup. Ther. 2011, 74, 210–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Serafini, G.; Engel-Yeger, B.; Vazquez, G.H.; Pompili, M.; Amore, M. Sensory processing disorders, duration of current episode, and severity of side effects in major affective and anxiety disorders. Eur. Psychiatr. 2016, 33, s224–s225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Dunn, W. The impact of sensory processing abilities on the daily lives of young children and their families: A conceptual model. Infants Young Child. 1997, 9, 23–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Dunn, W. Sensory Profile 2; Psych Corporation: Bloomington, MN, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  13. Dunn, W. Sensory Profile: User’s Manual; Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  14. Mubarak, A.; Cyr, C.; St-André, M.; Paquette, D.; Emond-Nakamura, M.; Boisjoly, L.; Palardy, S.; Adin, S.; Stikarovska, I. Child attachment and sensory regulation in psychiatric clinic-referred preschoolers. Clin. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2017, 22, 572–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Lane, S.J.; Reynolds, S.; Thacker, L. Sensory over-responsivity and ADHD: Differentiating using electrodermal responses, cortisol, and anxiety. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2010, 4, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. McLean, M.A.; Niknafs, N.; Scoten, O.C.; Chau, C.M.; MacKay, M.; Weinberg, J.; Synnes, A.; Miller, S.P.; Grunau, R.E. Sensory processing and cortisol at age 4 years: Procedural pain-related stress in children born very preterm. Dev. Psychobiol. 2021, 63, 915–930. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Kushki, A.; Drumm, E.; Pla Mobarak, M.; Tanel, N.; Dupuis, A.; Chau, T.; Anagnostou, E. Investigating the autonomic nervous system response to anxiety in children with autism spectrum disorders. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e59730. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Tolin, D.F.; Lee, E.; Levy, H.C.; Das, A.; Mammo, L.; Katz, B.W.; Diefenbach, G.J. Psychophysiological assessment of stress reactivity and recovery in anxiety disorders. J. Anxiety Disord. 2021, 82, 102426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Pruneti, C.A.; Lento, R.M.; Fante, C.; Carrozzo, E.; Fontana, F. Autonomic arousal and differential diagnosis in clinical psychology and psychopathology. J. Psychopathol. 2010, 16, 43–52. [Google Scholar]
  20. Kotianova, A.; Kotian, M.; Slepecky, M.; Chupacova, M.; Prasko, J.; Tonhajzerova, I. The differences between patients with panic disorder and healthy controls in psychophysiological stress profile. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2018, 14, 435–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Dieleman, G.C.; Huizink, A.C.; Tulen, J.H.; Utens, E.M.; Creemers, H.E.; van der Ende, J.; Verhulst, F.C. Alterations in HPA-axis and autonomic nervous system functioning in childhood anxiety disorders point to a chronic stress hypothesis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2015, 51, 135–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Engel-Yeger, B. The involvement of altered sensory modulation in neurological conditions and its relevance to neuro-rehabilitation: A narrative literature review. Disabil. Rehabil. 2021, 43, 2511–2520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Achenbach, T.M.; Rescorla, L. Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms and Profiles; University of Vermont Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families: Burlington, VT, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  24. Reynolds, C.R.; Richmond, B.O. Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale. RCMAS Manual; Western Psychological Services: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  25. Reynolds, C.R.; Harding, R.E. Outcome in two large sample studies of factorial similarity under six methods of comparison. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1983, 43, 723–728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Brown, C.; Dunn, W. Adolescent-Adult Sensory Profile: User’s Manual; Therapy Skill Builders: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  27. Serafini, G.; Engel-Yeger, B.; Vazquez, G.H.; Pompili, M.; Amore, M. Sensory Processing Disorders are Associated with Duration of Current Episode and Severity of Side Effects. Psychiatry Investig. 2017, 14, 51–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Fowles, D.C. The eccrine system and electrodermal activity. Psychophysiol. Syst. Process. Appl. 1986, 1, 51–96. [Google Scholar]
  29. Parma, V.; Cellini, N.; Guy, L.; McVey, A.J.; Rump, K.; Worley, J.; Maddox, B.; Bush, J.; Bennett, A.; Franklin, M.; et al. Profiles of autonomic activity in autism spectrum disorder with and without anxiety. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2021, 51, 4459–4470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Brown, C.; Karim, R.; Steuter, M. Retrospective analysis of studies examining sensory processing preferences in people with a psychiatric condition. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2020, 74, 7404205130p1–7404205130p11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Van den Boogert, F.; Klein, K.; Spaan, P.; Sizoo, B.; Bouman, Y.H.; Hoogendijk, W.J.; Roza, S.J. Sensory processing difficulties in psychiatric disorders: A meta-analysis. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2022, 151, 173–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Houghton, D.C.; Stein, D.J.; Cortese, B.M. Exteroceptive sensory abnormalities in childhood and adolescent anxiety and obsessive-compulsive disorder: A critical review. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2020, 59, 78–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. MacLennan, K.; Roach, L.; Tavassoli, T. The relationship between sensory reactivity differences and anxiety subtypes in autistic children. Autism Res. 2020, 13, 785–795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Engel-Yeger, B.; Muzio, C.; Rinosi, G.; Solano, P.; Geoffroy, P.A.; Pompili, M.; Amore, M.; Serafini, G. Extreme sensory processing patterns and their relation with clinical conditions among individuals with major affective disorders. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2016, 236, 112–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. McMahon, K.; Anand, D.; Morris-Jones, M.; Rosenthal, M.Z. A path from childhood sensory processing disorder to anxiety disorders: The mediating role of emotion dysregulation and adult sensory processing disorder symptoms. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2019, 13, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Mulligan, S.; Douglas, S.; Armstrong, C. Characteristics of idiopathic sensory processing disorder in young children. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2021, 15, 647928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. John, T.S.; Estes, A.; Begay, K.K.; Munson, J.; Reiter, M.A.; Dager, S.R.; Kleinhans, N. Characterizing social functioning in school-age children with sensory processing abnormalities. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2022, 52, 1361–1373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Mazurek, M.O.; Petroski, G.F. Sleep problems in children with autism spectrum disorder: Examining the contributions of sensory over-responsivity and anxiety. Sleep Med. 2015, 16, 270–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Nikolić, M.; Aktar, E.; Bögels, S.; Colonnesi, C.; De Vente, W. Bumping heart and sweaty palms: Physiological hyperarousal as a risk factor for child social anxiety. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2018, 59, 119–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Sebastião, R. Classification of Anxiety Based on EDA and HR. In International Conference on IoT Technologies for HealthCare; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2020; pp. 112–123. [Google Scholar]
  41. Desai, U.; Shetty, A.D. Electrodermal activity (EDA) for treatment of neurological and psychiatric disorder patients: A review. In Proceedings of the 2021 7th International Conference on Advanced Computing and Communication Systems (ICACCS), IEEE, Coimbatore, India, 19–20 March 2021; Volume 1, pp. 1424–1430. [Google Scholar]
  42. McCormack, C.C.; Mennies, R.J.; Silk, J.S.; Stone, L.B. How anxious is too anxious? State and trait physiological arousal predict anxious youth’s treatment response to brief cognitive behavioral therapy. BMC Psychol. 2020, 8, 48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Pop-Jordanova, N.; Pop-Jordanov, J. Electrodermal activity and stress assessment. Prilozi 2020, 41, 5–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Christian, C.; Cash, E.; Cohen, D.A.; Trombley, C.M.; Levinson, C.A. Electrodermal activity and heart rate variability during exposure fear scripts predict trait-level and momentary social anxiety and eating-disorder symptoms in an analogue sample. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2023, 11, 134–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Mavhunga, T. The Effectiveness, Acceptability and Practicality of a CBT-Sensory Modulation Intervention for Children 4–7 Years Old Presenting with Anxiety. Ph.D. Thesis, Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand, 2024. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Experimental schedule completed by the participants and their parents.
Figure 1. Experimental schedule completed by the participants and their parents.
Children 12 00187 g001
Figure 2. Differences between groups in CBCL scores.
Figure 2. Differences between groups in CBCL scores.
Children 12 00187 g002
Figure 3. Differences between groups in RCMAS scores.
Figure 3. Differences between groups in RCMAS scores.
Children 12 00187 g003
Figure 4. Differences between groups in AASP scores.
Figure 4. Differences between groups in AASP scores.
Children 12 00187 g004
Figure 5. Differences between groups in EDA and pulse rate scores.
Figure 5. Differences between groups in EDA and pulse rate scores.
Children 12 00187 g005
Figure 6. EDA recording of a study group participant. Note: 1 = reaction time and amplitude height in response to the first startle; 2 = time and height of the maximum amplitude during the 3 min of auditory startles; 3 = the time and height of minimal amplitude during the 3 min of auditory startles session; 4 = time and height of maximum amplitude at the time of relaxation session; 5 = time and height of minimum amplitude at the time of relaxation session.
Figure 6. EDA recording of a study group participant. Note: 1 = reaction time and amplitude height in response to the first startle; 2 = time and height of the maximum amplitude during the 3 min of auditory startles; 3 = the time and height of minimal amplitude during the 3 min of auditory startles session; 4 = time and height of maximum amplitude at the time of relaxation session; 5 = time and height of minimum amplitude at the time of relaxation session.
Children 12 00187 g006
Table 1. Prevalence of anxiety disorders among the study group.
Table 1. Prevalence of anxiety disorders among the study group.
Psychiatric DiagnosisNumber of Participants
(n = 44)
Percentile %
Anxiety818.2
Anxiety and ADHD1227.3
Anxiety and ODD24.5
Anxiety and depression511.4
Anxiety and adaptive disorder36.8
Anxiety, ADHD and ODD36.8
Anxiety, ADHD, and adaptive disorder24.5
Anxiety, ADHD, and depression 12.3
Anxiety, ADHD, and OCD36.8
Anxiety, ADHD, and PTSD24.5
Anxiety, adaptive and borderline personality disorders12.3
Anxiety, depression, and personality disorder12.3
Anxiety, ADHD, and tics12.3
Note: ADHD = Attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder; ODD = Oppositional defiant disorder; OCD = Obsessive-compulsive disorder; PTSD = Post-traumatic stress disorder.
Table 2. Sociodemographic characteristics of both groups.
Table 2. Sociodemographic characteristics of both groups.
Control Group (n = 62)95% CIsStudy Group
(n = 44)
95% CIs
nPercentile %Lower BoundUpper BoundnPercentile %Lower BoundUpper Bound
Elementary school0000.0580000.080
High school812.90.0660.2341022.70.1280.37
Mother educationProfessional11.60.0020.0851125.00.1450.394
Academic5385.50.7460.9212250.00.3580.641
Elementary school0000.05812.30.0040.118
High school34.80.0160.13211250.1450.394
Father educationProfessional69.70.0450.1951022.70.1280.37
Academic5385.50.7460.9212045.50.3170.599
Below average23.20.0080.1101329.50.1810.442
Socioeconomic
status
Average69.70.0450.1951022.70.1280.37
Above average5487.10.7650.9332147.70.3370.620
Table 3. Comparing CBCL scores between groups according to the Mann–Whitney test.
Table 3. Comparing CBCL scores between groups according to the Mann–Whitney test.
Study Group
(n = 43)
Mean + SD
Control Group
(n = 60)
Mean + SD
Mann-Whitney
U
p
Anxious/Depressed10.09 ± 4.72.12 ± 2.27129<0.001 ***
Withdrawn/Depressed5.86 ± 3.790.92 ± 1.44264<0.001 ***
Somatic complaints5.21 ± 4.540.90 ± 1.27418<0.001 ***
Social problems6.14 ± 4.240.93 ± 1.56204.5<0.001 ***
Thought problems6.37 ± 4.230.75 ± 1.09217<0.001 ***
Attention problems8.88 ± 4.312.00 ± 2.53224.5<0.001 ***
Rule-breaking 3.65 ± 3.250.68 ± 0.98516.5<0.001 ***
Aggressive behavior11.14 ± 6.772.40 ± 3.147250.5<0.001 ***
Other conditions5.44 ± 3.411.67 ± 1.76417.5<0.001 ***
Internalization68.33 ± 8.3747.38 ± 7.9577.5<0.001 ***
Externalization60.05 ± 8.6643.53 ± 8.05236.5<0.001 ***
Total problems65.79 ± 7.7742.75 ± 10.1886.5<0.001 ***
Note: SD = standard deviation; *** p ≤ 0.001.
Table 4. Comparing RCMAS scores between groups according to MANCOVA and ANCOVA tests.
Table 4. Comparing RCMAS scores between groups according to MANCOVA and ANCOVA tests.
Study Group
(n = 44)
Mean + SE
Control Group (n = 62)
Mean + SE
F (1,106)ηp2 95% CIs
pLower BoundUpper Bound
Physiological6.388 ± 0.3553.289 ± 0.29541.9280.289<0.001 ***2.1494.047
Worry and over-responsivity8.239 ± 0.3884.863 ± 0.32341.4790.287<0.001 ***2.3364.416
Social concern and concentration4.395 ± 0.2691.865 ± 0.22448.4660.320<0.001 ***1.8093.250
Lie2.876 ± 0.3612.781 ± 0.3000.0380.0000.846−0.8711.061
Total anxiety score18.298 ± 0.8129.724 ± 0.67561.2440.373<0.001 ***
Note: SE = standard error; ηp2 (partial η2) = effect size; *** p < 0.001.
Table 5. Comparing AASP scores between groups according to the MANCOVA test.
Table 5. Comparing AASP scores between groups according to the MANCOVA test.
Study Group (n = 43)
Mean + SE
Control Group (n = 62)
Mean + SE
F (1,105)ηp2 95% CIs
pLower BoundUpper Bound
Sensory sensitivity46.311 ± 1.59932.832 ± 1.31239.2660.278<0.001 ***9.21217.745
Low registration39.388 ± 1.47331.150 ± 1.20817.2820.145<0.001 ***4.30712.168
Sensory seeking41.787 ± 1.10149.406 ± 0.90326.4830.206<0.001 ***−10.556−4.682
Sensation avoidance40.680 ± 1.40830.190 ± 1.15430.7010.231<0.001 ***6.73514.245
Note: SE = standard error; ηp2 (partial η2) = effect size; *** p < 0.001.
Table 6. Prevalence of SMDs among the study group as compared to the controls according to the AASP, according to chi-square analysis.
Table 6. Prevalence of SMDs among the study group as compared to the controls according to the AASP, according to chi-square analysis.
GroupStudy Group
n %
95% CIsControl Group
n %
95% CIsχ2 (2)pFischer
Sensory ModelLower BoundUpper BoundLower BoundUpper Bound
Sensory sensitivityBelow norm12.30.0040.1201219.40.1140.30826.025<0.001 ***<0.001 ***
At norm1330.20.1860.4513861.30.4880.724
Above norm2967.40.5250.7951219.40.1140.308
Low registrationBelow norm24.70.0120.1541524.20.1520.36121.328<0.001 ***
At norm2558.10.4330.71644710.5870.807
Above norm1637.20.2430.52134.80.0160.132
Sensory
seeking
Below norm1944.20.3040.58969.70.0450.19519.082<0.001 ***
At norm2455.80.4110.6955080.60.6910.885
Above norm0000.08269.70.0450.195
Sensation avoidanceBelow norm24.70.0120.1542032.30.2190.44629.457<0.001 ***
At norm1637.20.2430.5213556.40.4400.680
Above norm2558.10.4330.716711.30.0550.215
*** p < 0.001.
Table 7. Comparing EDA and heart rate between groups according to MANCOVA and ANCOVA tests.
Table 7. Comparing EDA and heart rate between groups according to MANCOVA and ANCOVA tests.
Study Group
(n = 37)
Mean + SE
Control Group
(n = 60)
Mean + SE
F (1,97)ηp2 95% CIs
pLower BoundUpper Bound
Phase 2–first startleArousabilityResponse time to 1st stimulus 72.043 ± 0.26874.757 ± 0.20659.2060.386<0.001 ***−3.413−2.013
Amplitude height—1st stimulus 4.776 ± 0.4533.372 ± 0.3485.5530.0560.021 *0.2212.588
Study Group
(n = 43)
Mean + SE
Control Group
(n = 62)
Mean + SE
F (1,105)ηp2
Phase 2–startles sessionArousabilityTime to get to maximum amplitude 147.979 ± 8.909161.840 ± 7.3071.3380.0130.250130.304165.647
Height of the maximum amplitude 5.952 ± 0.4904.062 ± 0.4028.2130.0750.005 **4.9806.925
AdaptationTime to get to minimum amplitude 156.328 ± 9.409151.482 ± 7.7170.1470.0010.703137.665174.991
Height of the minimum amplitude 3.098 ± 0.3142.696 ± 0.2580.9030.0090.3442.4753.720
Phase 3–relaxation sessionArousabilityTime to get to maximum amplitude 268.962 ± 3.567276.430 ± 2.9252.4230.0230.123261.887276.037
Height of the maximum amplitude 5.303 ± 0.4913.718 ± 0.4035.7620.0530.018 *4.3306.277
AdaptationTime to get to minimum amplitude 282.181 ± 3.60279.326 ± 2.9530.3480.0030.557275.040289.323
Height of the minimum amplitude 3.406 ± 0.3963.131 ± 0.3250.2600.0030.6112.6164.189
Heartrate
(phases 1 and 3)
ArousabilityPulse rate at the beginning of the EDA test90.810 ± 2.37281.683 ± 1.9728.1260.0730.005 **
AdaptationPulse rate at the end of the EDA test88.053 ± 2.1181.543 ± 1.7575.2050.0480.025 *
Note: SE = standard error; ηp2 (partial η2) = effect size; EDA = Electro-dermal activity; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p ≤ 0.001.
Table 8. The correlations between sensory modulation according to the AASP and psychopathological symptoms according to the RCMAS and the CBCL in the study group according to the Pearson correlation test.
Table 8. The correlations between sensory modulation according to the AASP and psychopathological symptoms according to the RCMAS and the CBCL in the study group according to the Pearson correlation test.
Study Group
(n = 43)
Control Group
(n = 62)
Sensory SensitivityLow RegistrationSensory SeekingSensation AvoidanceSensory SensitivityLow RegistrationSensory SeekingSensation Avoidance
RCMASPhysiological0.531 ***0.471 **0.480.721 ***0.581 ***0.415 ***−0.0850.427 ***
Worry and over-responsivity0.482 ***0.2780.0520.570 ***0.679 ***0.462 ***−0.1490.520 ***
Social concern and concentration0.544 ***0.1940.2580.536 ***0.523 ***0.442 ***−0.1440.350 **
Lie−0.047−0.090−0.044−0.124−0.250−0.1800.192−0.118
Total anxiety score0.621 ***0.392 **0.1230.731 ***0.715 ***0.533 ***−0.1650.513 ***
Study Group
(n = 42)
Control Group
(n= 60)
CBCLAnxious/Depressed0.147−0.0250.1320.2120.351 **0.273 *−0.0660.344 **
Withdrawn/Depressed0.2770.226−0.0440.482 ***0.0730.158−0.0690.159
Somatic complaints0.2360.1550.1960.309 *0.289 *0.2270.0200.223
Social problems0.0740.1030.0700.1670.1680.2250.0660.156
Thought problems0.1500.1180.1690.1670.042−0.006−0.031−0.008
Attention problems−0.1070.2090.1250.0380.0340.101−0.1020.083
Rule-breaking−0.095−0.2720.169−0.053−0.0300.1860.1840.090
Aggressive behavior−0.086−0.0120.0890.112−0.0500.0010.1180.059
Other conditions0.1670.0290.0480.170−0.116−0.0670.213−0.089
Internalization0.1560.0580.0740.325 *0.3070.240−0.0710.290 *
Externalization−0.041−0.1320.0990.007−0.0350.0430.1550.105
Total problems0.0880.0530.1240.2060.0740.0910.0810.148
Note: * p ≤ 0.05; ** p ≤ 0.01; *** p ≤ 0.001.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Hammud, G.; Avital-Magen, A.; Jabareen, H.; Adler-Tsafir, R.; Engel-Yeger, B. The Relations Between Sensory Modulation, Hyper Arousability and Psychopathology in Adolescents with Anxiety Disorders. Children 2025, 12, 187. https://doi.org/10.3390/children12020187

AMA Style

Hammud G, Avital-Magen A, Jabareen H, Adler-Tsafir R, Engel-Yeger B. The Relations Between Sensory Modulation, Hyper Arousability and Psychopathology in Adolescents with Anxiety Disorders. Children. 2025; 12(2):187. https://doi.org/10.3390/children12020187

Chicago/Turabian Style

Hammud, Ginan, Ayelet Avital-Magen, Hiba Jabareen, Reut Adler-Tsafir, and Batya Engel-Yeger. 2025. "The Relations Between Sensory Modulation, Hyper Arousability and Psychopathology in Adolescents with Anxiety Disorders" Children 12, no. 2: 187. https://doi.org/10.3390/children12020187

APA Style

Hammud, G., Avital-Magen, A., Jabareen, H., Adler-Tsafir, R., & Engel-Yeger, B. (2025). The Relations Between Sensory Modulation, Hyper Arousability and Psychopathology in Adolescents with Anxiety Disorders. Children, 12(2), 187. https://doi.org/10.3390/children12020187

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop