Next Article in Journal
Longitudinal Changes in Serum Creatinine Levels and Urinary Biomarkers in Late Preterm Infants during the First Postnatal Week: Association with Acute Kidney Injury and Treatment with Aminoglycoside
Previous Article in Journal
Body Shape Indices in Adolescents Based on the 2009–2012 Korea National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

A Systematic Review on Attachment and Sleep at Preschool Age

William James Center for Research, ISPA—Instituto Universitário, 1100-304 Lisbon, Portugal
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Children 2021, 8(10), 895; https://doi.org/10.3390/children8100895
Submission received: 24 August 2021 / Revised: 1 October 2021 / Accepted: 4 October 2021 / Published: 7 October 2021
(This article belongs to the Section Pediatric Mental Health)

Abstract

:
Sleep is a biological process that impacts nearly every domain of a child’s life. Sleep-wake regulation influences and it is highly influenced by developmental variables related to parent-child relationships, such as attachment. The main goal of the present systematic review is to analyze and integrate the findings of empirical studies investigating the relations between attachment and sleep in preschool age, a period marked by important developmental changes that challenge both attachment system and sleep-wake regulation. A database search was performed using a combination of relevant keywords, leading to the identification of 524 articles, with 19 manuscripts assessed for eligibility; finally, seven studies (2344 children) were included. Overall, the findings were not consistent, with some studies reporting significant associations between attachment security and sleep quality, as well as between attachment insecurity and sleep problems, whereas others did not find significant associations. The results are discussed in light of the available theoretical models and integrated in the context of measurement approaches to attachment and sleep heterogeneity, aiming to guide future research on the topic.

1. Introduction

Sleep is a relationally guided biological process, with implications in nearly every domain of child development, holding a core place in every child’s life. During early infancy and preschool age, children spend approximately half of their time asleep [1,2,3], suggesting that sleep may be particularly important during periods of greater plasticity, playing a role in neuronal maturation and development [4,5,6]. However, the influence of sleep in child development goes far beyond biological processes, impacting the child’s health and well-being [7], cognitive and behavioral functioning [8], affect regulation [9], and social competence [10,11,12].
Despite the importance of sleep in multiple domains of child development, sleep disorders are highly common, occurring in 20 to 50% of children at some point of their development [13,14] and persisting through subsequent years in 8.5 to 12% of the cases [15,16]. Sleep problems may uncover a health issue, considering their associations with negative physical [16] and mental health outcomes [17], pointing to bidirectional links between sleep and health variables. Sleep problems also relate to greater internalizing and externalizing behavioral problems, both concurrently and longitudinally [18,19,20], social-emotional problems [21,22], and poorer executive functioning [23]. Given the high prevalence of sleep problems during early childhood and its detrimental impact in child development, efforts have been made in order to uncover the factors that influence child’s sleep. As such, Sadeh and Anders [24] theorized the transactional model of sleep-wake regulation [24,25,26,27], conceptualizing how sleep both influences and is influenced by the characteristics of child’s developmental contexts, such as culture, environment, and family relationships. According to this model, infant sleep regulation has a dynamic bidirectional relation with: (a) distal determinants that shape the parent context, affecting infant’s sleep secondarily (e.g., cultural and socioeconomic influences); (b) intermediate factors (e.g., parental health, personality, cognitions, and expectations about childhood and sleep); and (c) proximal influences (e.g., infant biomedical and constitutional factors, such as temperament and physiological reactivity, as well as specific parenting interactions) [24]. The authors also emphasize the role of child-parent dyadic relationships, particularly attachment, mediating the relations between sleep, and the referred contexts during the first years of life. However, given the lack of associations between attachment and sleep before 2 years of age [28], possibly due to a high intra-individual variability in sleep patterns, we decided to focus this review on preschool children (2 to 5 years old).
As going to bed constitutes a separation between child and parents, which is frequently experienced with anxiety, and one of the functions of the attachment system is to provide security to the dyad in times of distress, sleep is proposed to act as a trigger to the attachment system. Given the importance of attachment relationships on sleep regulation, the current review will focus on the interpersonal system of the transactional model of sleep regulation. Some theoretical explanations about the connection between attachment and sleep, derived from this model, will be detailed next.

Attachment and Sleep

Attachment relationship is conceptualized as the enduring and stable emotional bond between the child and the caregiver, ensuring the caregiver’s proximity in threatening or stressful situations to provide physical and psychological security to the child [29,30,31]. As the child experiences his/her caregivers as warm, sensitive, and consistent in meeting his needs, or rather as cold, unsupportive, and unpredictable, a secure or insecure attachment is formed correspondingly. Secure attachment bonds relate to better developmental outcomes, such as higher rates of executive functioning [32,33,34], language development [35], emotional understanding [36], social competence [37,38], and resilience [39]. In turn, insecure attachment relationships are related with less optimal developmental outcomes, such as increased risk of developing disease [40], delayed development of discourse [41], and socio-emotional difficulties, measured both cross-sectionally and longitudinally [42,43,44].
The attachment system and sleep-wake regulation have been subject to a considerable amount of theoretical and empirical work that has been trying to uncover how these systems may relate [9,24,25,45,46,47]. Some conceptual and theoretical explanations for the relations between sleep and attachment have been advanced and grouped into four distinct models [47]. The first one suggests that behaviors related to sleep problems are attachment behaviors in reaction to night-time separations [24,25,48]. In light of this model, bedtime resistance could be an expression of separation anxiety and night-wakings would work as a mechanism of proximity seeking towards the attachment figure. The second model theorizes sleep problems as a consequence of attachment insecurity. Fears and worries related to bedtime separation can compromise a child’s capacity to relax and downregulate, physiologically and psychologically, in order to fall asleep [9,46]. In opposition, the third model conceptualizes sleep problems as a source of insecure attachment. Because sleep problems impact children’s emotional regulation [49], it may affect the formation of social bonds [45], posing the dyad at a greater risk of developing an insecure attachment. Finally, the last model hypothesizes that inconsistent parenting may be the cause of the relations between sleep and attachment [46].
Empirical evidence, however, has not always been consistent in supporting the outlined models. Studies examining sleep problems and attachment are still sparse and insufficient, generating results that are often difficult to compare, namely by assessing distinct age ranges, or using different instruments to measure different domains of attachment or sleep. For example, studies evaluating the behavioral quality of attachment (e.g., Strange Situation Procedure [50]; Adaptation for preschool-age [51]; Attachment Q-Sort, [52]), or its representation (e.g., Attachment Story Completion Task, ASCT, [53]), may lead to distinct results [54]. This is particularly significant considering the moderate correlations found between different attachment measures [55,56], suggesting some overlap between measures, but also revealing that different measures may assess distinct aspects of attachment.
Similar challenges are faced in sleep assessment, with different findings regarding objective (e.g., actigraphy) and subjective measures (e.g., parent’s self-report). On the one hand, objective recordings tend to overestimate night-wakings and to miss important behavioral aspects of sleep phenomena, such as bedtime resistance and sleep anxiety [56,57,58,59]. On the other hand, parental reports tend to overrate sleep duration and wake-up time, underestimating bedtime/night-wakings [60,61].
Despite the inconsistence of the research findings between attachment and sleep during infancy, the relations between them across the lifespan have been described in a systematic review [62], suggesting the existence of bidirectional relations between sleep and attachment, starting early in infancy (0 to 2 years old) and continuing throughout life, in children (2 to 18 years old), adults (18 to 64 years old), and seniors (>65 years old). One meta-analysis also examined children’s sleep (between 6 months and preschool age; [28]), describing positive associations between some parameters of sleep and attachment security. Specifically, attachment security correlated with higher sleep efficiency and lower rates of sleep problems, while attachment resistance correlated positively with maternal related sleep problems. Despite the importance of these findings, this meta-analysis focused on the comparison between secure and resistant attachment styles, failing to consider conclusions regarding other attachment styles (i.e., avoidant and disorganized). Moreover, it also neglected the studies that adopt continuous measures of attachment security (i.e., Attachment Q-Sort (AQS) and Attachment Story Completion Task (ASCT)), leaving it understudied how attachment may be related with sleep.
Aiming to overcome some of the limitations of previous reviews (e.g., wide age-range, inconsistent findings), in the current review, we focused on a specific developmental period: preschool age, i.e., between 2 and 5 years old. This age range was defined given that although sleep consolidation happens at around 12 months (i.e., the distribution of sleep in one longer period during the night and a short nap during the day), due to some individual variability in this process, sleep consolidation does not tend to be considered until two years old [63,64,65]. Moreover, preschool age is a period when developmental processes (e.g., the transition to day-care and the redefinition of the parent-child relationship into a goal-corrected partnership) may challenge both the attachment system and sleep-wake regulation [50,66] or the constitution of new roles and responsibilities [67], hampering the relations between sleep quality and attachment at early stages of development [12,68].
Bearing that in mind, as well as the specific contributions of previous reviews, the current review aims to provide a systematized and integrated overview of studies on sleep quality and attachment relationships in preschool children (i.e., age range between 2 and 5 years old). Specifically, it will examine how objective and subjective sleep dimensions may be related to different dimensions of attachment, aiming to illuminate directions for future research.

2. Materials and Methods

The present systematic review follows the PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses; [69]) general guidelines for reporting data concerning the relations between attachment and sleep in children aged between 2 and 5 years old. Every step of the process of identification and selection of articles is specified below.

2.1. Information Resources and Search Strategy

A systematic data search was performed using the following string: sleep AND attachment AND (child* OR toddler* OR preschool* OR years OR months), on titles, abstracts, and keywords, in the following databases: EBSCO (PsycInfo, PsycArticles, Academic Search Complete, and Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Collection) and Web of Science. The search covered the period until 21 September 2020 (no starting date limit), resulting in 524 titles. The initial abstracts, identified as potentially relevant articles, were screened by both authors separately, and a total of 505 papers were excluded at this phase. The remaining 19 articles were fully assessed independently, and after disagreements were resolved by consensus, a total of seven articles were included (Table 1).

2.2. Eligibility Criteria, Information Resources and Search Strategy

Articles were included in this review according to the following inclusion criteria: (1) empirical studies with an available abstract, published in peer-reviewed journals; (2) articles written in English, Portuguese, French, or Spanish (languages mastered by the authors); and (3) articles assessing children’s sleep between 2 and 5 years old.
We excluded all the studies in which: (1) children or parents were not living in natural contexts (e.g., institutionalized or hospitalized children, incarcerated parents, etc.); (2) children were premature or diagnosed with some physical and/or mental illness; (3) the mother or father were diagnosed with some physical and/or mental illness; (4) there was not a specific aim to examine the relations between child’s attachment and sleep; (5) intervention programs or clinical trials were conducted; (6) there were no specific measures of child attachment and/or sleep. The flow diagram for the identification, screening, eligibility and inclusion of studies can be found in Figure 1.
For the purpose of this review, we included studies in which child’s sleep was assessed via actigraphy or sleep diaries for sleep parameters related to sleep quantity and quality, and parental-reported questionnaires for sleep problems and sleep-related behaviors. Given the amount of longitudinal designs found, we excluded studies that evaluated sleep exclusively before 24 months of age, given that, during the first two years of life, children display great intraindividual variability due to maturational processes [77].

2.3. Data Extraction and Items

A data extraction matrix was developed to summarize collected data, identifying: (1) general information about the participants, such as age range, number of participants, country of origin, socioeconomic status, and ethnicity; (2) study design; (3) attachment measure; (4) sleep measure; (5) main findings regarding the relations between attachment and sleep. Data from the full papers were extracted by both researchers independently.

3. Results

3.1. General Description of the Studies: Theoretical Perspectives

Most of the included studies are rooted in the perspective that sleep difficulties may partially result from vicissitudes in the attachment relationship ([9]; ID#1, 3, 4, 5). This framework stands that insecurely attached children may experience more intense fears and worries, which might disrupt sleep. In one case, both frameworks of attachment as a predictor of sleep [9] and sleep as predictor of attachment [44] were considered (ID#2). One study (ID#6) drew upon a conceptual framework considering sleep and attachment as dual products of parenting [46]. Finally, one of the studies did not specify the authors’ standpoint regarding associations between sleep and attachment (ID#7).

3.2. General Sociodemographic Characteristics of the Participants

All the included studies were conducted in North American countries (four in Canada: ID#1, 2, 3, 4, and three in the USA: ID#5, 6, 7), and the majority of the children were Caucasian (80% across all studies; one study did not provide information regarding ethnicity). A large proportion of the children were integrated into intact families, meaning that 89% lived with both parents in the same household (two studies did not specify parental marital status; ID#3, 6). Overall, children belonged to well-educated families, with 81% of the mothers having completed high school and at least 76% holding college degrees (two studies did not provide information on maternal education; #3, 6).

3.3. Study Designs

Most of the studies relied on longitudinal designs, either with attachment and sleep measured at different ages, or with sleep assessments across distinct time points (ID#1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 7), whereas only one study was conceived with a cross-sectional design (ID#6). Although some studies reported sleep assessments before the children were 2 years old (ID#1, 2, 3, 8), these data were not considered in this systematic review. As such, we analyzed longitudinal data from four studies (ID#1, 3, 4, 7), cross-sectional data from two studies (ID#2, 6), and both longitudinal and cross-sectional data from one study (ID#5).

3.4. Empirical Processes on Sleep and Attachment Measures

Most of the articles examined sleep as an outcome of the attachment relationship, whereas only one investigated attachment as an outcome of sleep (ID#2). Moreover, mechanisms influencing the relations between sleep and attachment were examined only by one study (ID#5), uncovering the role of a temperamental variable, i.e., negative emotionality. Finally, one study (ID#7) identified how individual (e.g., child’s sex, temperament) and mother-related characteristics (e.g., sensitivity, depression) accounted for developmental changes in the relations between attachment and sleep. Details on how attachment and sleep have been conceptualized and measured by the included studies will be specified next, followed by a description of the main findings of the associations between sleep and attachment.

3.5. Sleep and Attachment Measures across Studies

Sleep has been described as a complex, multifaceted, and multidimensional phenomenon, which can be studied objectively or via parental report in young children. Sleep assessment may involve different parameters, such as sleep quantity, sleep quality, and sleep-related behaviors. Table 2 illustrates the type of sleep instruments (i.e., actigraphy, sleep diary, questionnaire) used to assess sleep quantity, quality, or sleep problems.
Parameters of sleep quantity and/or quality were the focus of most of the identified research. Sleep parameters of quantity, i.e., sleep duration across a 24 h period, or estimates of it (e.g., bedtime, wake-up time), were one of the most frequent aspects examined (ID#1, 2, 3, 4, 6). Sleep quality variables, e.g., sleep efficiency, defined by the percent of time in bed spent effectively asleep and number of night-wakings, were also often assessed (ID#1, 3, 4, 6, 7). Aspects related to sleep fragmentation (e.g., frequency and length of night-wakings, physical agitation) were the most common parameters of sleep quality examined.
The assessment of sleep quality and quantity tended to rely on both actigraphy, i.e., an objective measure of sleep, and sleep diaries (ID#1, 2, 4). However, one study used only actigraphy (ID#6), and three studies exclusively relied on maternal reported questionnaires (ID#3, 5, 7).
Concerning attachment evaluation, the Strange Situation Paradigm [50] and its modified version suited for preschool age (Preschool Separation-Reunion Procedure, [51]) were frequent procedures to evaluate the attachment of the child to mother/parents (ID#3, 4, 7). Other studies relied on Attachment Q-Sort ([52]; #1, 2, 5) and one assessed attachment representations with the Attachment Story Completion Task ([53]; #6).

3.6. Associations between Attachment and Parameters of Sleep

Overall, studies examining attachment and quantitative parameters of sleep found inconsistent results. Table 3 summarizes the main findings of the primary studies. On the one hand, more securely attached children (at 15 months) slept more at night (at 24 months) than their insecurely attached counterparts, according to actigraphy (ID#1). Moreover, disorganized attachment was related to maternal perceptions of later bedtimes and shorter total sleep durations for their children (ID#3). On the other hand, some studies did not find associations between attachment and sleep parameters such as sleep duration, longest period of uninterrupted sleep, time in bed, or wake-up time (ID#2, 3, 4).
Regarding the quality of sleep, children classified as disorganized tended to be described by the mothers as having more fragmented sleep, i.e., more frequent and longer night-wakings, in comparison to children with other attachment classifications (i.e., secure and insecure-ambivalent; ID#3). More resistant attachment behaviors were also related to mothers’ reports of longer night-waking durations for children at 24 months (ID#4).
In turn, associations between attachment security and objectively measured sleep quality were inconsistent. On the one hand, secure attachment correlated positively with sleep efficiency (ID#1, 6) and negatively with night-waking duration and frequency, as well as with other variables indicative of less sleep quality, such as sleep activity (ID#6). On the other hand, these associations were not found in another study also relying on objective assessments of sleep (ID#4).

3.7. Associations between Atachment and Sleep Problems

The relations between attachment and sleep problems were only examined by two articles, again with inconsistent findings. Negative correlations between attachment and sleep problems were found by one study (ID#5), both cross-sectionally (at 24 months) and longitudinally (at 36 months). Furthermore, one study included a temperamental variable as a moderator of the relation between attachment and sleep, and found that the child’s negative emotionality moderated this association. Attachment security predicted fewer sleep problems for children with high negative emotionality, but not for children with low negative emotionality at 54 months (ID#5). However, another study did not find associations between attachment and sleep problems (ID#7), suggesting that the role of child variables, such as breastfeeding duration and temperament, and of maternal variables, such as maternal sensitivity and depression, impact developmental changes in sleep.

4. Discussion

The main aim of the present systematic review was to summarize research findings concerning the relations between attachment and sleep in children from 2 to 5 years old. A handful of studies suggest that children with secure attachment relationships have better overall sleep than insecurely attached children. For example, it has been reported that children with insecure (ID#1, 2) and with disorganized (ID#3) attachment relationships sleep for fewer hours (ID#1, 3), and, moreover, wakeup more frequently during sleep and stay awake for longer periods (ID#2, 3) than securely attached children, who in turn sleep more efficiently (ID#1, 6).
One possible explanation for these results involves attachment and self-regulation theories [78]. Accordingly, secure attachment relationships are built upon a history of repeated experiences of successful dyadic regulation [28,29,79], resulting in the development of a stronger capacity for physiological [80] and emotional self-regulation [81]. This capacity can be manifested at bedtime, with securely attached children being more able to peacefully self-regulate in order to fall asleep and to settle after a night-waking [9], increasing total sleep time and sleep efficiency. On the contrary, children with insecure/insecure-ambivalent/disorganized attachment classifications would experience a more difficult time when they face the transition to sleep alone in the dark [47,82]. Darkness and loneliness are natural clues to danger, defined as stimuli that, although not inherently dangerous, increase the likelihood of danger, leading to the activation of the attachment system [29]. Insecurely attached children are more prone to intensely experience, and express, separation anxiety than secure children [83]. Accordingly, their emotional reactions when facing night-time separations can increase bedtime resistance, delaying bedtime and shortening total sleep time and sleep efficiency [84]. Concomitantly, a large proportion of insecure children would be more likely to signal their night-wakings (ID#4) as part of a mechanism to seek proximity from caregivers, extending their durations and making their parents more likely to notice and report sleep problems in retrospective questionnaires [85]. This explanation is reinforced by the findings showing that mothers of insecure-ambivalent (ID#4) and disorganized (ID#3) children report more frequent and longer night-wakings than mothers of securely attached children. These findings may explain the abovementioned associations between insecure attachment and sleep.
Another possible explanation is that parents of securely attached children may implement more stable bedtime routines, which, in turn, lead to better sleep [64,86,87]. Although, to our knowledge, no study has investigated the relations between attachment security and bedtime routines, those have been associated with variables concerning parent-child relationships [88,89,90,91] and parenting styles [92], suggesting that the perception of stability originating or reinforced by routines may improve family relationships. However, adopting the framework of attachment theory, it has been reported that parents of securely attached children structure their environment, making it predictable and stable. This capacity had been described as a part of sensitive caregiving that characterizes parents of securely attached children.
However, other studies did not find significant associations between sleep and attachment (ID#2, 4, 7), especially when sleep parameters, such as number and length of night-wakings, were measured objectively (ID#2, 4). This lack of associations suggests that attachment may be linked to sleep dimensions that are easily assessed by maternal reports, such as night-time and sleep-related behaviors, but not through objective measures, such as actigraphy [73]. Hence, different instruments capture distinct dimensions of sleep phenomena. While objective methods (e.g., actigraphy) assess objective sleep duration, timing, and quality (i.e., sleep efficiency) and tend to overestimate night-wakings [57,93,94], subjective methods (e.g., sleep diaries and questionnaires) reflect maternal perceptions of the child’s sleep that are more limited as the child gets older [95]. In that sense, some authors have suggested that relevant sleep parameters must be selected based on developmental considerations [96]. As such, variables like the proportion of night-time sleep and longest period of uninterrupted sleep, which tend to stabilize over time [63], should be considered. These findings point to the relevance of measuring sleep both via maternal reports and objective methods [85], assessing developmental relevant variables, in order to account for behavioral and objective dimensions of sleep that may be related to attachment.
The reported inconsistency among the findings may mirror not only the variability of sleep parameters assessed, but also the diversity of attachment dimensions measured across the studies. Attachment was measured in different contexts (i.e., laboratory ID#3, 4, 6, 7, and home ID#1, 2, 5) and via distinct coding systems (i.e., typological ID#3, 4, 7, and continuous ID#1, 2, 5, 6). There is evidence that typological approaches used to score attachment behaviors in the Strange Situation Paradigm may not be the best assessing for the variability in the quality of mother-child attachment relationships. Otherwise, instruments yielding continuous scores of security seem to capture more dynamic aspects of the attachment relationship [97] and show excellent variation in low-risk samples [73]. Moreover, it is possible that different components of the attachment relationship contribute differently to the child dealing with bedtime separations, impacting distinct sleep parameters. Some studies found that secure attachment, assessed by the AQS [52,98], was associated with higher sleep duration and efficiency (ID#1) and fewer sleep problems (ID#5). It is plausible that the aspects of a child’s secure base behavior, measured in a low-stress, known home environment [52,98], are those that help him/her to bear bedtime separations from the caregiver and to fall asleep. Conversely, studies that assessed attachment separation-reunion behaviors through the Strange Situation Paradigm [50] reported interesting findings regarding disorganized (ID#3) and ambivalent (ID#4) classifications. Both tended to wake up more frequently and for more minutes during the night, and mothers of disorganized children reported shorter sleep durations. Given that bedtime separations and morning or middle-of-the-night reunions are an ecological repetition of the Strange Situation Paradigm, it is credible to think that children who report high levels of separation stress, and cannot calm down easily after reunion, live analogous experiences every day at bedtime.
Although according to the transactional model of sleep-wake regulation [25,26,27,82], parent-child relationships are a major factor impacting child’s sleep, linking directly with attachment theory, and research is still sparse and insufficient. As reported in the present review, no study has investigated attachment and sleep at preschool age since 2015 and all of the studies were conducted in North American countries. This, along with the fact that nearly all of the children who participated in the primary studies belonged to well-educated, medium-high socio-economic status samples, may compromise generalization. It is plausible that attachment quality, as much as sleep parameters, are less likely to differ significantly in such samples, therefore making effects harder to detect. Nevertheless, socio-economic status has been reported to affect both attachment security [99,100,101] and child’s sleep [102,103,104].
Another aspect worth mentioning concerns two assumptions regarding the attachment relationship that should be analyzed carefully: stability across time and stability across distinct relationships. First, the fact that all but one study has a longitudinal design is noteworthy, expressing an effort to uncover how attachment is related to the development of sleep. However, although there is a tendency for attachment stability across the lifespan [105,106], it is also known that attachment classifications can change in the early years, particularly when assessed via behavioral and representational measures [107]. The absence of control for attachment stability possibly weakens the studied effects. Second, all of the studies that assessed attachment behaviors (ID#1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 7) evaluated only mother-child dyads, despite the evidence pointing to modest to moderate correlations between mother-child and father-child attachment [107,108]. Knowing that the presence of at least one secure attachment relationship to one of the parents buffers the negative impacts of insecure attachment to the other parent [109,110], evaluating attachment to one single figure limits the understanding of attachment-sleep relations. Apart from the concordance between child-mother and child-father attachment relationships, it has been established that father-child reciprocal interactions have a unique contribution to child development [111,112,113], for example, in behavioral regulation [114]. The importance of father-related variables [115], such as father’s mental health [116,117,118], paternal emotional support [119], and particularly paternal involvement in child sleep, have been demonstrated in infants [120], preschool-aged [121], and school-aged children [122]. However, to our knowledge, none of the studies that investigated the relations between attachment and sleep at preschool age included measures of father-child attachment, undermining the understanding of father-child attachment influence on sleep.
Despite the relevance of the findings, some limitations of the current review should be addressed. First, the number of primary studies was relatively small, with most of identified studies collecting data from small samples (ID#1, 2, 4, 6). Additionally, most of the children were from middle class western societies and belonged to well-educated families, compromising the generalization of results due to the lack of sample variability and representatively. Secondly, by only including empirical scientific articles, potentially relevant sources of information about the topic published elsewhere (e.g., grey literature, book chapters) were not identified. Third, the use broad keywords during the article search, such as “sleep,” “attachment,” or “child,” may have left out articles related to the research topic indexed with other terms. Lastly, included studies that used objective sleep measures tended to focus on short spans of time (i.e., 72 h, #1, 2, 4), although the recommended duration for the use of actigraphy is at least seven consecutive days [123]. Thus, these findings should be examined carefully.
Paths for future investigation that have been uncovered in the current section are systematized below. First, selected samples should become more heterogeneous regarding sociodemographic and family factors, allowing researchers to study if the association between attachment and sleep is moderated by factors that were not included in the primary studies. Despite the evidence suggestive of the role played by factors such as maternal depression [124,125], maternal anxiety [126], or family socioeconomic status both in attachment security [99] and child’s sleep [103], these were overlooked by the included studies, ignoring the influence of these factors in the relations between attachment and sleep. Second, attachment assessments should privilege instruments that yield continuous scores of attachment, both to the mother and father or other primary caregiver, and consider the possibility of a second assessment if it was first measured before 18 months old. Finally, future studies should focus on capturing sleep phenomena in all their amplitude, using both objective and subjective sleep measures over extended periods, and favoring the assessment of developmental relevant variables.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed equally to this work. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the PORTUGUESE FOUNDATION FOR SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY, FCT-PTDC/MHC-PED/0838/2014, UIDB/04810/2020.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank all of the members of the research team for their assistance and support.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Galland, B.C.; Short, M.A.; Terril, P.; Rigney, G.; Haszard, J.J.; Foster-Owens, M.; Biggs, S. Establishing normal values for pediatric nighttime sleep measured by actigraphy: A systematic review and meta-analyses. Sleep 2018, 41, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Galland, B.C.; Taylor, B.J.; Elder, D.E.; Herbison, P. Normal sleep patterns in infants and children: A systematic review of observational studies. Sleep Med. Rev. 2012, 16, 213–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Iglowstein, I.; Jenni, O.G.; Molinari, L.; Largo, R. Sleep duration from infancy to adolescence: Reference values and generational trends. Pediatrics 2003, 111, 302–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Dahl, R.E. Adolescent brain development: A period of vulnerabilities and opportunities. In Adolescent Brain Development: Vulnerabilities and Opportunities; Dahl, R.E., Spear, L.P., Eds.; New York Academy of Sciences: New York, NY, USA, 2004; pp. 1–22. [Google Scholar]
  5. Gómez, R.L.; Edgin, J.O. Sleep as a window into early neural development: Shifts in sleep-dependent learning effects across early childhood. Child Dev. Perspect. 2015, 9, 183–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Page, J.; Lustenberger, C.; Fröhlich, F. Social, motor, and cognitive development through the lens of sleep network dynamics in infants and toddlers between 12 and 30 months of age. Sleep 2018, 41, zsy024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Matricciani, L.; Paquet, C.; Galland, B.; Short, M.; Olds, T. Children’s sleep and health: A meta-review. Sleep Med. Rev. 2019, 46, 136–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Reynaud, E.; Vecchierini, M.-F.; Heude, B.; Charles, M.-A.; Plancoulaine, S. Sleep and its relation to cognition and behaviour in preschool-aged children of the general population: A systematic review. J. Sleep Res. 2018, 27, 126–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Dahl, R.E. The regulation of sleep and arousal: Development and psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 1996, 8, 3–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Foley, J.; Weinraub, M. Sleep, affect, and social competence from preschool to preadolescence: Distinct pathways to emotional and social adjustment for boys and girls. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Tomisaki, E.; Tanaka, E.; Watanabe, T.; Shinohara, R.; Hirano, M.; Onda, Y.; Mochizuki, Y.; Yato, Y.; Yamakawa, N.; Anme, T. The relationship between the development of social competence and sleep in infants: A longitudinal study. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Ment. Health 2018, 12, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Vaughn, B.E.; Elmore-Staton, L.E.; Shin, N.; El-Sheikh, M. Sleep as a support for social competence, peer relations, and cognitive functioning in preschool children. Behav. Sleep Med. 2015, 13, 92–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Du Mond, C.; Mindell, J.A. Sleep and sleep problems: From birth to 3. Zero Three 2011, 32, 30–35. [Google Scholar]
  14. Owens, J. Classification and epidemiology of childhood sleep disorders. Sleep Med. Clin. 2008, 2, 353–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Gregory, A.M.; Caspi, A.; Eley, T.C.; Moffitt, T.E.; O’Connor, T.G.; Poulton, R. Prospective longitudinal associations between persistent sleep problems in childhood and anxiety and depression disorders in adulthood. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2005, 33, 157–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Simola, P.; Liukkonen, K.; Pitkäranta, A.; Pirinen, T.; Aronen, E.T. Psychosocial and somatic outcomes of sleep problems in children: A 4-year follow-up study. Child Care Health Dev. 2012, 40, 60–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Armstrong, J.M.; Ruttle, P.L.; Klein, M.H.; Essex, M.J.; Benca, R.M. Associations of child insomnia, sleep movement, and their persistence with mental health symptoms in childhood and adolescence. Sleep 2014, 37, 901–909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Conway, A.; Miller, A.L.; Modrek, A. Testing reciprocal links between trouble getting to sleep and internalizing behavior problems, and bedtime resistance and externalizing behavior problem in toddlers. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2017, 48, 678–689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Lam, C.B.; Chung, K. Associations of sleep problems with externalizing behaviors and preacademic performance: The moderating role of family socioeconomic status. Infant Child Dev. 2017, 26, e2026. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Sivertsen, B.; Harvey, A.G.; Reichborn-Kjennerud, T.; Torgersen, L.; Ystrom, E.; Hysing, M. Later emotional and behavioral problems associated with sleep problems in toddlers: A longitudinal study. JAMA Pediatr. 2015, 169, 575–582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Hysing, M.; Sivertsen, B.; Garthus-Niegel, S.; Eberhard-Gran, M. Pediatric sleep problems and social-emotional problems: A population-based study. Infant Behav. Dev. 2016, 42, 111–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Jung, E.; Jin, B. Associations between sleep problems, cognitive and socioemotional functioning from preschool to adolescence. Child Youth Care For. 2019, 48, 829–848. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Nelson, T.D.; Nelson, J.M.; Kidwell, K.M.; James, T.D.; Espy, K.A. Preschool sleep problems and differential associations with specific aspects of executive control in early elementary school. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2015, 40, 167–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Sadeh, A.; Anders, T.F. Infant sleep problems: Origins, assessment, interventions. Infant Ment. Health J. 1993, 14, 17–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Anders, T.F. Infant sleep, nighttime relationships, and attachment. Psychiatry 1994, 57, 11–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Sameroff, A. Principles of development and psychopathology. In Relationship Disturbances in Early Childhood: A Developmental Approach; Sameroff, A., Emde, R., Eds.; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1989; pp. 17–33. [Google Scholar]
  27. Sameroff, A.; Fiese, B. Transactional regulation and early intervention. In Handbook of Early Childhood Intervention, 2nd ed.; Meisels, S., Shonkoff, J., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1990; p. 119. [Google Scholar]
  28. Simard, V.; Chevalier, V.; Bédard, M.-M. Sleep and attachment in early childhood: A series of meta-analyses. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2017, 19, 298–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Bowlby, J. Attachment and Loss: Vol. 1. Attachment; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1969. [Google Scholar]
  30. Bowlby, J. Attachment and Loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and Anger; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1973. [Google Scholar]
  31. Bowlby, J. Attachment and Loss: Vol. 3. Sadness and Depression; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1980. [Google Scholar]
  32. Bernier, A.; Beauchamp, M.H.; Carlson, S.M.; Lalonde, G. A secure base from which to regulate: Attachment security in toddlerhood as a predictor of executive functioning at school entry. Dev. Psychol. 2015, 51, 1177–1189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Heikamp, T.; Trommsdorff, G.; Druey, M.D.; Hübner, R.; Von Suchodoletz, A. Kindergarten children’s attachment security, inhibitory control, and the internalization of rules of conduct. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Matte-Gagné, C.; Bernier, A.; Sirois, M.-S.; Lalonde, G.; Hertz, S. Attachment security and developmental patterns of growth in executive functioning during early elementary school. Child Dev. 2018, 89, 167–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Murray, A.D.; Yingling, J.L. Competence in language at 24 months: Relations with attachment security and home stimulation. J. Genet. Psychology 2000, 161, 133–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Zeegers, M.A.; Meins, E.; Stams, G.J.J.M.; Bögels, S.M.; Colonnesi, C. Does attachment security predict children’s thinking-about-thinking and thinking-about-feeling? A meta-analytic review. Dev. Rev. 2019, 54, 100885. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Murphy, T.P.; Labile, D.J. The influence of attachment security on preschool children’s empathic concern. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2013, 37, 436–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Veríssimo, M.; Santos, A.J.; Fernandes, C.; Shin, N.; Vaughn, B.E. Associations between attachment security and social competence in preschool children. Merrill Palmer Q. 2014, 60, 80–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Rasmussen, P.D.; Storebø, O.J.; Løkkeholt, T.; Voss, L.G.; Shmueli-Goetz, Y.; Bojesen, A.B.; Simonsen, E.; Bilenberg, N. Attachment as a core feature of resilience: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Rep. 2019, 122, 1259–1296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Maunder, R.G.; Hunter, J.J. Attachment relationships as determinants of physical health. J. Am. Acad. Psychoanal. Dyn. Psychiatry 2008, 36, 11–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Kelly, K.R. Insecure attachment representations and child personal narrative structure: Implications for delayed discourse in preschool-age children. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2015, 17, 448–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Priddis, L.; Howieson, N.D. Insecure attachment patterns at five years. What do they tell us? Early Child Dev. Care 2012, 182, 45–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Cooke, J.E.; Stuart-Parrigon, K.L.; Movahed-Abtahi, M.; Koehn, A.J.; Kerns, K.A. Children’s emotion understanding and mother-child attachment: A meta-analysis. Emotion 2016, 16, 1102–1106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Spruit, A.; Goos, L.; Weenink, N.; Rodenburg, R.; Niemeyer, H.; Stams, G.J.; Colonnesi, C. The relation between attachment and depression in children and adolescents: A multilevel meta-analysis. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 2020, 23, 54–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. McNamara, P.; Belsky, J.; Fearon, P. Infant sleep disorders and attachment: Sleep problems in infants with insecure-resistant versus insecure-avoidant attachment to mother. Sleep Hypn. 2003, 5, 7–16. [Google Scholar]
  46. Morrell, J.; Steele, H. The role of attachment security, temperament, maternal perception, and care-giving behavior in persistent infant sleeping problems. Infant Ment. Health J. 2003, 24, 447–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Keller, P. Sleep and attachment. In Sleep and Development: Familial and Socio-cultural Considerations; El-Sheikh, M., Ed.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 49–77. [Google Scholar]
  48. Scher, A. Mother-child interaction and sleep regulation in one-year-olds. Infant Ment. Health J. 2001, 22, 515–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Williams, K.E.; Berthelsen, D.; Walker, S.; Nicholson, J.M. A developmental cascade model of behavioral sleep problems and emotional and attentional self-regulation across early childhood. Behav. Sleep Med. 2017, 15, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. Ainsworth, M.S.; Blehar, M.C.; Waters, E.; Wall, S. Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation; Lawrence Erlbaum: Oxford, UK, 1978. [Google Scholar]
  51. Cassidy, J.; Marvin, R.S.; MacArthur Working Group. Attachment Organization in Preschool Children: Procedures and Coding Manual, 4th ed.; University of Virginia: Charlottesville, VA, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  52. Waters, E.; Deane, K.E. Defining and assessing individual differences in attachment relationships: Q-methodology and the organization of behavior in infancy and early childhood. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 1985, 50, 41–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Bretherton, I.; Ridgeway, D.; Cassidy, J. Assessing internal working models of the attachment relationship: An attachment story completion task for 3-year olds. In Attachment in the Preschool Years: Research and Intervention; Greenberg, M., Cicchetti, D., Cummings, E.M., Eds.; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1990; pp. 273–308. [Google Scholar]
  54. Madigan, S.; Brumariu, L.E.; Villani, V.; Atkinson, L.; Lyons-Ruth, K. Representational and questionnaire measures of attachment: A meta-analysis of relations to child internalizing and externalizing problems. Psychol. Bull. 2016, 147, 367–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Van IJzendoorn, M.H.; Vereijken, C.M.J.L.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J.; Riksen-Walraven, J.M. Assessing attachment security with the Attachment Q-Sort: Meta-analytic evidence for the validity of the observer AQS. Child Dev. 2004, 75, 1188–1213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Smeekens, S.; Riksen-Walraven, J.M.; van Bakel, H. The predictive value of different attachment measures for socioemotional development at age 5 years. Infant Ment. Health J. 2009, 30, 366–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Meltzer, L.J.; Montgomery-Downs, H.W.; Insana, S.P.; Colleen, M.W. Use of actigraphy for assessment in pediatric sleep research. Sleep Med. Rev. 2012, 16, 463–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Owens, J.A.; Spirito, A.; McGuinn, M.; Nobile, C. Sleep habits and sleep disturbance in elementary school-aged children. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2000, 21, 27–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Owens, J.A.; Spirito, A.; McGuinn, M. The Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ): Psychometric properties of a survey instrument for school-aged children. Sleep 2000, 23, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Kushnir, J.; Sadeh, A. Correspondence between reported and actigraphic sleep measures in preschool children: The role of a clinical context. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2013, 9, 1147–1151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Werner, H.; Molinari, L.; Guyer, C.; Jenni, O. Agreement rates between actigraphy, diary, and questionnaire for children’s sleep patterns. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2008, 162, 350–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  62. Adams, G.C.; Stoops, M.A.; Skomro, R.P. Sleep tight: Exploring the relationship between sleep and attachment style across the life span. Sleep Med. Rev. 2014, 18, 495–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Acebo, C.; Sadeh, A.; Seifer, R.; Tzischinsky, O.; Hafer, A.; Carskadon, M.A. Sleep/wake patterns derived from activity monitoring and maternal report for healthy 1- to 5-year-old children. Sleep 2005, 28, 1568–1577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Mindell, J.A.; Li, A.M.; Sadeh, A.; Kwon, R.; Goh, D. Bedtime routines for young children: A dose-dependent association with sleep outcomes. Sleep 2015, 38, 717–722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Paavonen, E.J.; Saarenpää-Heikkilä, O.; Morales-Munoz, I.; Virta, M.; Häkälä, N.; Pölkki, P.; Kylliäinen, A.; Karlsson, H.; Paunio, T.; Karlsson, L. Normal sleep development in infants: Findings from two large birth cohorts. Sleep Med. 2020, 69, 145–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Thompson, R.A.; Labile, D.J.; Ontai, L.L. Early understandings of emotion, morality, and self: Developing a working mode. Adv. Child Dev. Behav. 2004, 31, 137–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Li-Grining, C.P.; Votruba-Drzal, E.; Maldonado-Carreno, C.; Haas, K. Children’s early approaches to learning and academic trajectories through fifth grade. Dev. Psychol. 2010, 45, 1062–1077. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Trejos, N.; Time May be up for Naps in pre-K Class. The Washington Post. Available online: http://www.washingtonpost.com/wp-dyn/articles/A58706-2004Mar14_2-htlm (accessed on 24 August 2004).
  69. Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzlaff, J.; Altman, D.G. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. Ann. Intern. Med. 2009, 151, 264–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Bélanger, M.-È.; Bernier, A.; Simard, V.; Bordeleau, S.; Carrier, J. Attachment and sleep among toddlers: Disentangling attachment security and dependency. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 2015, 80, 125–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Bernier, A.; Bélanger, M.-È.; Tarabulsy, G.; Simard, M.; Carrier, J. My mother is sensitive, but I am too tired to know: Infant sleep as a moderator of prospective relations between maternal sensitivity and infant outcomes. Infant Behav. Dev. 2014, 37, 682–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Pennestri, M.-H.; Moss, E.; O’Donnell, K.; Lecompte, V.; Bouvette-Turcot, A.A.; Atkinson, L.R.; Minde, K.; Gruber, R.; Fleming, A.; Meaney, M.J.; et al. Establishment and consolidation of the sleep-wake cycle as a function of attachment pattern. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2015, 17, 23–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Simard, V.; Bernier, A.; Bélanger, M.-E.; Carrier, J. Infant attachment and toddlers’ sleep assessed by maternal reports and actigraphy: Different measurement methods yield different relations. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 2013, 38, 473–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  74. Troxel, W.M.; Trentacosta, C.J.; Forbes, E.E.; Campbell, S.B. Negative emotionality moderates associations among attachment, toddler sleep, and later problem behaviors. J. Fam. Psychol. 2013, 27, 127–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  75. Vaughn, B.E.; El-Sheikh, M.; Shin, N.; Elmore-Staton, L.; Krzysik, L.; Monteiro, L. Attachment representations, sleep quality, and adaptive functioning in preschool age children. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2011, 13, 525–540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Weinraub, M.; Bender, R.H.; Friedman, S.L.; Susman, E.J.; Knoke, B.; Bradley, R.; Houts, R.; Williams, J. Patterns of developmental change in infants’ nighttime sleep awakenings from 6 through 36 months of age. Dev. Psychol. 2012, 48, 1511–1528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Sadeh, A. Commentary: Comparing actigraphy and parental report as measures of children’s sleep. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 2008, 33, 406–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Calkins, S.D.; Leerkes, E.M. Early attachment processes and the development of emotional self-regulation. In Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications, 3rd ed.; Vohs, K., Baumeister, R.F., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 355–373. [Google Scholar]
  79. Cassidy, J. Emotion regulation: Influences of attachment relationships. Mon. Soc. Res. Child. Dev. 1994, 59, 228–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Gunnar, M.R. Social buffering of stress in development. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2017, 12, 355–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Stefan, C.A.; Avram, J.; Miclea, M. Children’s awareness concerning emotion regulation strategies: Effects of attachment status. Soc. Dev. 2017, 26, 694–708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Sadeh, A.; Tikotzky, L.; Scher, A. Parenting and infant sleep. Sleep Med. Rev. 2010, 14, 89–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Cassidy, J.; Main, M. The relationship between infant-parent attachment and the ability to tolerate brief separation at six years. In Frontiers of Infant Psychiatry; Tyson, R., Galenson, E., Eds.; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1984; Volume 2, pp. 132–136. [Google Scholar]
  84. Schlarb, A.A.; Jaeger, S.; Schneider, S.; In-Albon, T.; Hautzinger, M. Sleep problems and separation anxiety in preschool-aged children: A path analysis. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2016, 25, 902–910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Perpétuo, C.; Fernandes, M.; Veríssimo, M. Comparison between actigraphy records and parental reports of child’s sleep. Front. Pediatr. 2020, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Lee, S.; Hale, L.; Berger, L.M.; Buxton, O.M. Maternal perceived work schedule flexibility predicts child sleep mediated by bedtime routines. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2019, 28, 245–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Sadeh, A.; Mindell, J.A.; Luedtke, K.; Wiegand, B. Sleep and sleep ecology in the first 3 years: A web-based study. J. Sleep Res. 2009, 18, 60–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Ferreti, L.K.; Bub, K.L. Family routines and school readiness during the transition to kindergarten. Early Educ. Dev. 2017, 28, 59–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Fiese, B.H.; Everhart, R.S. Routines and child development. In Encyclopedia of Infant and Early Childhood Development; Haith, M., Benson, J., Eds.; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 2008; pp. 34–41. [Google Scholar]
  90. Hale, L.; Berger, L.M.; LeBourgeois, M.K.; Brooks-Gunn, J. Social and demographic predictors of preschoolers’ bedtime routines. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2009, 30, 394–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  91. Ren, L.; Fan, J. Chinese preescholers’ daily routine and its associations with parent-child relationships and child self-regulation. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2018, 43, 179–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Staples, A.D.; Bates, J.E.; Petersen, I.T., IX. Bedtime routines in early childhood: Prevalence, consistency, and associations with nighttime sleep. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 2015, 80, 141–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Sadeh, A.; Acebo, C. The role of actigraphy in sleep medicine. Sleep Med. Rev. 2002, 6, 113–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  94. Sitnick, S.L.; Goodlin-Jones, B.L.; Anders, T.F. The use of actigraphy to study sleep disorders in preschools: Some concerns about detection of night-time awakenings. Sleep 2008, 31, 395–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Tikotzky, L.; Volkovich, E. Infant nocturnal wakefulness: A longitudinal study comparing three sleep assessment methods. Sleep 2019, 42, zsy191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Tétreault, É.; Bouvette-Turcot, A.-A.; Bernier, A.; Bailey, H. Associations between early maternal sensitivity and children’s sleep throughout early childhood. Infant Child Dev. 2016, 26, e2004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Fraley, R.C.; Spieker, S.J. Are infant attachment patterns continuously or categorically distributed? A taxometric analysis of Strange Situation behavior. Dev. Psychol. 2003, 39, 387–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Waters, E. The attachment Q-set. In Caregiving, Cultural, and Cognitive Perspectives on Secure Base Behavior and Working Models: New Growing Points of Attachment Theory and Research; Waters, E., Vaughn, B., Posada, G., Kondo-Ikemura, K., Eds.; Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1995; pp. 234–246. [Google Scholar]
  99. Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J.; van IJzendoorn, M.H.; Kroonenberg, P.M. Differences in attachment security between African-American and white children: Ethnicity or socio-economic status? Infant Behav. Dev. 2004, 27, 417–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Gedaly, L.R.; Leekers, E.M. The role of sociodemographic risk and maternal behavior in the prediction of infant attachment disorganization. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2016, 18, 554–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  101. van IJzendoorn, M.H.; Goldberg, S.; Kroonenberg, P.M.; Frenkel, O.J. The relative effects of maternal and child problems on the quality of attachment: A meta-analysis of attachment in clinical samples. Child Dev. 1992, 63, 840–858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  102. Buzek, T.; Poulain, T.; Vogel, M.; Engel, C.; Bussler, S.; Körner, A.; Hiemisch, A.; Kiess, W. Relations between sleep duration with overweight and academic stress—Just a matter of the socioeconomic status? Sleep Health 2019, 5, 208–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. El-Sheikh, M.; Bagley, E.J.; Keiley, M.; Elmore-Staton, L.; Chen, E.; Buckhalt, J.A. Economic adversity and children’s sleep problems: Multiple indicators and moderation of effects. Health Psychol. 2013, 32, 849–859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  104. El-Sheikh, M.; Kelly, R.J.; Buckhalt, J.A.; Hinnant, J.B. Children’s sleep and adjustment over time: The role of socioeconomic context. Child Dev. 2010, 81, 870–883. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Waters, E. The reliability and stability of individual differences in infant-mother attachment. Child Dev. 1978, 49, 483–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Waters, E.; Merrick, S.; Treboux, D.; Crowell, J.; Albersheim, L. Attachment security in infancy and early adulthood: A twenty-year longitudinal study. Child Dev. 2000, 71, 684–689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  107. Pinquart, M.; Feußner, C.; Ahnert, L. Meta-analytic evidence for stability in attachments from infancy to early adulthood. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2013, 15, 189–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Fernandes, C.; Veríssimo, M.; Monteiro, L.; Antunes, M.; Vaughn, B.; Santos, A.J. Mothers, fathers, sons, and daughters: Are there sex differences in the organization of secure base behavior during early childhood. Infant Behav. Dev. 2018, 50, 213–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  109. Caldera, Y.M.; Lindsey, E.W. Coparenting, mother-infant interaction, and infant-parent attachment relationship in two-parent families. J. Fam. Psychol. 2006, 20, 275–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Dagan, O.; Sagi-Schwartz, A. Early attachment network with mother and father: An unsettled issue. Child Dev. Perspect. 2018, 12, 115–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Barker, B.; Iles, J.E.; Ramchandani, P.G. Fathers, fathering and child psychopathology. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2017, 15, 87–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Pleck, J.H. Why could father involvement benefit children? Theoretical perspectives. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2007, 11, 196–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Van Bakel, H.J.A.; Hall, R.A.S. The father-infant relationship beyond caregiving sensitivity. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2020, 22, 27–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  114. Lindsey, E.W.; Cremeens, P.R.; Caldera, Y.M. Mother-child and father-child mutuality in two contexts: Consequences for young children’s peer relationships. Infant Child Dev. 2010, 19, 142–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Benedetta, R.; De Stasio, S.; Barni, D. Fathers and sleep: A systematic literature review of bidirectional links between paternal factors and children’s sleep in the first three years of life. Clin. Neuropsychiatry. 2020, 17, 349–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. De Stasio, S.; Ragni, B.; Boldrini, F.; Bevilacqua, F.; Gentile, S. Parental stress and bedtime routines in toddlerhood. ARS 2018, 8, 75–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Millikovsky-Ayalon, M.; Atzaba-Poria, N.; Meiri, G. The role of the father in child sleep disturbance: Child, parent and parent-child relationship. Infant Ment. Health J. 2015, 36, 114–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  118. Ragni, B.; De Stasio, S.; Barni, D.; Gentile, S.; Giampaolo, R. Parental mental health, fathers’ involvement and bedtime resistance in infants. Ital. J. Pediatr. 2019, 45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Bernier, A.; Tétreault, É.; Bélanger, M.-È.; Carrier, J. Paternal involvement and child sleep: A look beyond infancy. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2016, 41, 714–722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Tikotzky, L.; Sadeh, A.; Glickman-Gavrieli, T. Infant sleep and paternal involvement in infant caregiving during the first 6 months of life. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 2010, 36, 36–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  121. Bernier, A.; Bélanger, M.-È.; Bordeleau, S.; Carrier, J. Mothers, fathers, and toddlers: Parental psychosocial functioning as a context for young children’s sleep. Dev. Psychol. 2013, 49, 1375–1384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Keller, P.; El-Sheikh, M. Children’s emotional security and sleep: Longitudinal relations and directions of effects. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2011, 52, 64–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Acebo, C.; Sadeh, A.; Seifer, R.; Tzischinsky, O.; Wolfson, A.R.; Hafer, A.; Carskadon, M.A. Estimating sleep patterns with activity monitoring in children and adolescents: How many nights are necessary for reliable measures? Sleep 1999, 22, 95–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Kim, Y.; Bird, A.; Peterson, E.; Underwood, L.; Morton, S.M.B.; Grant, C.C. Maternal antenatal depression and early childhood sleep: Potential pathways through infant temperament. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 2020, 45, 203–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Liu, J.; Ji, X.; Wang, G.; Li, Y.; Leung, P.W.; Pinto-Martin, J. Maternal emotions during the pre/postnatal periods and children’s sleep behaviors: The mediating role of children’s behavior. J. Affect. Disord. 2020, 273, 138–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  126. Dubois-Comtois, L.; Pennestri, M.-H.; Bernier, A.; Cyr, C.; Godbout, R. Family environment and preschoolers’ sleep: The complementary role of both parents. Sleep Med. 2019, 58, 114–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram for the identification, screening, eligibility, and inclusion of studies.
Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram for the identification, screening, eligibility, and inclusion of studies.
Children 08 00895 g001
Table 1. References of the primary studies.
Table 1. References of the primary studies.
IDReferenceParticipantsDesign
#1Bélanger, Bernier, Simard, Bordeleau, & Carrier (2015) [70]N = 62 (30 girls); mostly Caucasian; Medium-High SESLongitudinal
#2Bernier, Bélanger, Tarabulsy, Simard, & Carrier (2014) [71]N = 63 (27 girls); mostly Caucasian; Medium-High SES; intact families.Longitudinal
#3Pennestri et al. (2015) [72]N = 134 (71 girls); mostly Caucasian; Medium-High SESLongitudinal
#4Simard, Bernier, Bélanger, & Carrier (2013) [73]N = 55 (25 girls); mostly Caucasian; Medium-High SES; intact familiesLongitudinal
#5Troxel, Trentacosta, Forbes, & Campbell (2013) [74]N = 776 (393 girls); mostly Caucasian; Parental education 14 years averageLongitudinal
#6Vaughn et al. (2011) [75]N = 39 (12 girls); 36% from ethnic minorities, mostly AACross-sectional
#7Weinraub et al. (2012) [76]N = 1215. Maternal education 14.3 years average; 13.3% single mothers; 31% low income familiesLongitudinal
SES stands for Socioeconomic Status, and AA stands for African American.
Table 2. Type of sleep instruments and variables measured across the studies.
Table 2. Type of sleep instruments and variables measured across the studies.
IDNAttachment Variables and InstrumentsAge at Sleep AssessmentMeasures
Sleep QuantitySleep QualitySleep Problems
#162Attachment Q-Sort (AQS): security and independence(15 m), 24 mActigraphy, sleep diary: sleep minutes at night and over the 24-h periodActigraphy: sleep efficiency-
#263AQS: security(12 m), 24 mActigraphy, sleep diary: night-time sleep duration and proportion of night time sleep to total sleep--
#3134Preschool Separation-Reunion Procedure: attachment classification (secure, ambivalent, avoidant, disorganized)(6 m, 12 m), 24 m, 36 mAdaptation of Self-Administered Questionnaire for the Mother: bedtime, wake time, sleep latency, nocturnal sleep duration, longest period of uninterrupted sleepAdaptation of SAQM: nr of night wakingsAdaptation of SAQM
#455Strange Situation Procedure (SSP): proximity seeking, contact maintenance, avoidance, resistance24 mActigraphy, sleep diary: sleep duration at night, wake duration at nightActigraphy: nr of nocturnal awakenings-
#5776AQS: security24 m, 36 m--Sleep problems subscale of the CBCL
#639Attachment Story Completion Task (SSP): coherence and security4-5 yActigraphy: sleep duration, total sleep minutes, longest wake episode, sleep latencyActigraphy: wakings after sleep onset, sleep activity mean, overall activity index, sleep efficiency
#71215SSP: security; attachment classification; Separation distress(6 m, 15 m), 24 m, 36 m Maternal interview: night wakings in the previous weekSleep problems subscale from CBCL
The ages placed between brackets refer to ages assessed by primary studies that were excluded from the current review. SAQM stands for Self-Administered Questionnaire for the Mother and CBCL stands for Child Behavior Checklist.
Table 3. Main findings of the primary studies.
Table 3. Main findings of the primary studies.
IDReferenceMain Findings
#1Bélanger, Bernier, Simard, Bordeleau, & Carrier (2015) [70]Positive associations between security and: sleep minutes at night and sleep efficiency. Negative associations between dependency and sleep minutes at night.
#2Bernier, Bélanger, Tarabulsy, Simard, & Carrier (2014) [71]Attachment security was unrelated to night-time sleep duration and to proportion of night-time sleep to total sleep.Insecure attachment was related to night-wakings.
#3Pennestri et al. (2015) [72]Disorganized attachment associated with a shorter duration of nocturnal sleep than secure or ambivalent attachment; more night awakenings; shorter periods of uninterrupted sleep.
#4Simard, Bernier, Bélanger, & Carrier (2013) [73]Ambivalent attachment associated with more minutes awake at night as perceived by their mothers, but not according to actigraphy records.
#5Troxel, Trentacosta, Forbes, & Campbell (2013) [74]Secure attachment correlated with fewer sleep problems. No evidence was found for a statistically significant direct path between attachment security and toddler sleep problems at 36 months. Significant direct relations were found between attachment security and sleep problems, but only among children characterized with high negative emotionality at 6 months.
#6Vaughn et al. (2011) [75]Attachment was positively associated with sleep duration, Total sleep minutes and sleep efficiency; negatively associated with variables reflecting poor sleep quality (number of sleep episodes, sleep activity index, wake minutes after sleep onset).
#7Weinraub et al. (2012) [76]Infant-mother attachment was not related to sleep problems.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Perpétuo, C.; Diniz, E.; Veríssimo, M. A Systematic Review on Attachment and Sleep at Preschool Age. Children 2021, 8, 895. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8100895

AMA Style

Perpétuo C, Diniz E, Veríssimo M. A Systematic Review on Attachment and Sleep at Preschool Age. Children. 2021; 8(10):895. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8100895

Chicago/Turabian Style

Perpétuo, Catarina, Eva Diniz, and Manuela Veríssimo. 2021. "A Systematic Review on Attachment and Sleep at Preschool Age" Children 8, no. 10: 895. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8100895

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop