Screen Time of Preschool-Aged Children and Their Mothers, and Children’s Language Development
Highlights
- Increased screen time for preschool-aged children and their mothers is associated with poorer language development in children.
- Children's screen time alone as well as mothers' screen time are negatively correlated with children's lexical and general language abilities.
- The negative impact on children's language development is cumulative when both children's screen time alone and mothers' screen time are considered together.
- The results suggest the importance of restricting both children's screen time spent alone and mothers' screen time to support children's language development.
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Measures
2.3. Statistical Analyses
3. Results
3.1. Data Description
3.2. Associations between Amount of Screen Time and Children’s Language Skills
3.3. Explanatory Value of Screen Time for Children’s Lexical and General Language Skills
4. Discussion
4.1. Screen Time of Children and Mothers
4.2. Strengths and Limitations
4.3. Clinical Implications
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Screen Time | Language Skills | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Background Variables | Child Alone | Child Co-View | Mother | FinCDI III Words | FPT | FMT | RDSL III Receptive | FinCDI III Total |
Maternal education level | −0.15 | −0.16 * | 0.11 | −0.17 * | 0.03 | 0.01 | 0.09 | −0.13 |
Children’s age | 0.08 | 0.11 | −0.17 * | 0.49 ** | 0.18 * | 0.54 ** | 0.00 | 0.52 ** |
Birth order | 0.13 | −0.26 ** | 0.31 ** | −0.15 | −0.19 * | 0.05 | −0.11 | −0.18 * |
Gender | −0.03 | 0.02 | 0.10 | −0.20 * | 0.01 | −0.15 | −0.16 * | −0.21 * |
Preterm birth | 0.05 | −0.06 | −0.02 | −0.23 ** | −0.17 * | 0.11 | −0.23 ** | −0.23 ** |
References
- Rideout, V.; Robb, M.B. The Common Sense Census: Media Use by Kids Age Zero to Eight; Common Sense Media: San Francisco, CA, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Hill, D.; Ameenuddin, N.; Chassiakos, Y.R.; Cross, C.; Hutchinson, J.; Levine, A.; Boyd, R.; Mendelson, R.; Moreno, M.; Swanson, W.S. Media and Young Minds. Pediatrics 2016, 138, e20162591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- World Health Organization. Guidelines on Physical Activity, Sedentary Behaviour, and Sleep for Children under 5 Years of Age; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2019; ISBN 978-92-4-155053-6. [Google Scholar]
- Määttä, S.; Kaukonen, R.; Vepsäläinen, H.; Lehto, E.; Ylönen, A.; Ray, C.; Erkkola, M.; Roos, E. The Mediating Role of the Home Environment in Relation to Parental Educational Level and Preschool Children’s Screen Time: A Cross-Sectional Study. BMC Public Health 2017, 17, 688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Christakis, D.A.; Gilkerson, J.; Richards, J.A.; Zimmerman, F.J.; Garrison, M.M.; Xu, D.; Gray, S.; Yapanel, U. Audible Television and Decreased Adult Words, Infant Vocalizations, and Conversational Turns: A Population-Based Study. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2009, 163, 554–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Khan, K.S.; Purtell, K.M.; Logan, J.; Ansari, A.; Justice, L.M. Association Between Television Viewing and Parent-Child Reading in the Early Home Environment. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2017, 38, 521–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Madigan, S.; McArthur, B.A.; Anhorn, C.; Eirich, R.; Christakis, D.A. Associations Between Screen Use and Child Language Skills: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2020, 174, 665–675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zimmerman, F.J.; Christakis, D.A.; Meltzoff, A.N. Associations between Media Viewing and Language Development in Children Under Age 2 Years. J. Pediatr. 2007, 151, 364–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Collet, M.; Gagnière, B.; Rousseau, C.; Chapron, A.; Fiquet, L.; Certain, C. Case–Control Study Found That Primary Language Disorders Were Associated with Screen Exposure. Acta Paediatr. 2019, 108, 1103–1109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duch, H.; Fisher, E.M.; Ensari, I.; Font, M.; Harrington, A.; Taromino, C.; Yip, J.; Rodriguez, C. Association of Screen Time Use and Language Development in Hispanic Toddlers: A Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Study. Clin. Pediatr. 2013, 52, 857–865. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rithipukdee, N.; Kusol, K. Factors Associated with the Suspected Delay in the Language Development of Early Childhood in Southern Thailand. Children 2022, 9, 662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dore, R.A.; Logan, J.; Lin, T.-J.; Purtell, K.M.; Justice, L.M. Associations Between Children’s Media Use and Language and Literacy Skills. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1734. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taylor, G.; Monaghan, P.; Westermann, G. Investigating the Association between Children’s Screen Media Exposure and Vocabulary Size in the UK. J. Child. Media 2018, 12, 51–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Chonchaiya, W.; Pruksananonda, C. Television Viewing Associates with Delayed Language Development. Acta Paediatr. 2008, 97, 977–982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bittman, M.; Rutherford, L.; Brown, J.; Unsworth, L. Digital Natives? New and Old Media and Children’s Outcomes. Aust. J. Educ. 2011, 55, 161–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hudon, T.M.; Fennell, C.T.; Hoftyzer, M. Quality Not Quantity of Television Viewing Is Associated with Bilingual Toddlers’ Vocabulary Scores. Infant Behav. Dev. 2013, 36, 245–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moon, J.; Cho, S.Y.; Lim, S.M.; Roh, J.H.; Koh, M.S.; Kim, Y.J.; Nam, E. Smart Device Usage in Early Childhood Is Differentially Associated with Fine Motor and Language Development. Acta Paediatr. 2019, 108, 903–910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gialamas, A.; Haag, D.G.; Mittinty, M.N.; Lynch, J. Which Time Investments in the First 5 Years of Life Matter Most for Children’s Language and Behavioural Outcomes at School Entry? Int. J. Epidemiol. 2020, 49, 548–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dynia, J.M.; Dore, R.A.; Bates, R.A.; Justice, L.M. Media Exposure and Language for Toddlers from Low-Income Homes. Infant Behav. Dev. 2021, 63, 101542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baumwell, L.; Tamis-LeMonda, C.S.; Bornstein, M.H. Maternal Verbal Sensitivity and Child Language Comprehension. Infant Behav. Dev. 1997, 20, 247–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Snijders, V.E.; Bogicevic, L.; Verhoeven, M.; van Baar, A.L. Toddlers’ Language Development: The Gradual Effect of Gestational Age, Attention Capacities, and Maternal Sensitivity. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health 2020, 17, 7926. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff, E. How Social Contexts Support and Shape Language Development. Dev. Rev. 2006, 26, 55–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reed, J.; Hirsh-Pasek, K.; Golinkoff, R.M. Learning on Hold: Cell Phones Sidetrack Parent-Child Interactions. Dev. Psychol. 2017, 53, 1428–1436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wong, R.S.; Tung, K.T.S.; Rao, N.; Leung, C.; Hui, A.N.N.; Tso, W.W.Y.; Fu, K.-W.; Jiang, F.; Zhao, J.; Ip, P. Parent Technology Use, Parent–Child Interaction, Child Screen Time, and Child Psychosocial Problems among Disadvantaged Families. J. Pediatr. 2020, 226, 258–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mendelsohn, A.L.; Berkule, S.B.; Tomopoulos, S.; Tamis-LeMonda, C.S.; Huberman, H.S.; Alvir, J.; Dreyer, B.P. Infant Television and Video Exposure Associated with Limited Parent-Child Verbal Interactions in Low Socioeconomic Status Households. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2008, 162, 411–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Radesky, J.; Miller, A.L.; Rosenblum, K.L.; Appugliese, D.; Kaciroti, N.; Lumeng, J.C. Maternal Mobile Device Use During a Structured Parent–Child Interaction Task. Acad. Pediatr. 2015, 15, 238–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Official Statistics of Finland Educational Structure of Population [e-Publication]. ISSN=2242-2919. Available online: http://www.stat.fi.libproxy.helsinki.fi/til/vkour/2019/vkour_2019_2020-11-05_tie_001_en.html (accessed on 10 September 2022).
- Schults, A.; Tulviste, T.; Urm, A. Toddlers’ Screen Use and Expressive Vocabulary Development: A Longitudinal Study. School of Natural Sciences and Health, Tallinn University: Tallin, Estonia, 2022; under review. [Google Scholar]
- Lauricella, A.R.; Wartella, E.; Rideout, V.J. Young Children’s Screen Time: The Complex Role of Parent and Child Factors. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2015, 36, 11–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stolt, S. Internal Consistency and Concurrent Validity of the Parental Report Instrument for Children Aged between 2,6 and 4,0—The Finnish Version of the Communicative Development Inventories III. Department of Psychology and Speech-Language Pathology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki: Helsinki, Finland, 2022; under review. [Google Scholar]
- Fenson, L.; Marchman, V.A.; Thal, D.J.; Dale, P.S.; Reznick, J.S.; Bates, E. MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories: User’s Guide and Technical Manual, 2nd ed.; Brookes: Baltimore, MD, USA, 2007; ISBN 978-1-55766-884-4. [Google Scholar]
- Eriksson, M. The Swedish Communicative Development Inventory III: Parent Reports on Language in Preschool Children. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2017, 41, 647–654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kunnari, S.; Saaristo-Helin, K.; Savinainen-Makkonen, T. Fonologiatesti: Lasten Äänteellisen Kehityksen Arviointimenetelmä; Niilo Mäki Instituutti: Jyväskylä, Finland, 2012; ISBN 978-951-39-4713-2. [Google Scholar]
- Lyytinen, P. Morfologiatesti: Taivutusmuotojen Hallinnan Mittausmenetelmä Lapsille; Jyväskylän Yliopisto: Jyväskylä, Finland, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Kortesmaa, M.; Heimonen, K.; Merikoski, H.; Warma, M.-L.; Varpela, V. Reynellin Kielellisen Kehityksen Testi—Käsikirja; Psykologien Kustannus Oy: Helsinki, Finland, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Duch, H.; Fisher, E.M.; Ensari, I.; Harrington, A. Screen Time Use in Children under 3 Years Old: A Systematic Review of Correlates. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2013, 10, 102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hoff, E.; Burridge, A.; Ribot, K.M.; Giguere, D. Language Specificity in the Relation of Maternal Education to Bilingual Children’s Vocabulary Growth. Dev. Psychol. 2018, 54, 1011–1019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwab, J.F.; Lew-Williams, C. Language Learning, Socioeconomic Status, and Child-Directed Speech. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2016, 7, 264–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rowe, M.L. Child-Directed Speech: Relation to Socioeconomic Status, Knowledge of Child Development and Child Vocabulary Skill. J. Child Lang. 2008, 35, 185–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zambrana, I.M.; Ystrom, E.; Pons, F. Impact of Gender, Maternal Education, and Birth Order on the Development of Language Comprehension: A Longitudinal Study from 18 to 36 Months of Age. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2012, 33, 146–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Berglund, E.; Eriksson, M.; Westerlund, M. Communicative Skills in Relation to Gender, Birth Order, Childcare and Socioeconomic Status in 18-Month-Old Children. Scand. J. Psychol. 2005, 46, 485–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Roseberry, S.; Hirsh-Pasek, K.; Golinkoff, R.M. Skype Me! Socially Contingent Interactions Help Toddlers Learn Language. Child Dev. 2014, 85, 956–970. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tsuji, S.; Fiévét, A.-C.; Cristia, A. Toddler Word Learning from Contingent Screens with and without Human Presence. Infant Behav. Dev. 2021, 63, 101553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Strouse, G.A.; Samson, J.E. Learning From Video: A Meta-Analysis of the Video Deficit in Children Ages 0 to 6 Years. Child Dev. 2021, 92, e20–e38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carr, A.; Dempster, T. Parent-Child Interactions during Joint Engagement with Touchscreen Technology: A Comparison of Younger versus Older Toddlers. Infant Behav. Dev. 2021, 64, 101587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Strouse, G.A.; O’Doherty, K.; Troseth, G.L. Effective Coviewing: Preschoolers’ Learning from Video after a Dialogic Questioning Intervention. Dev. Psychol. 2013, 49, 2368–2382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Asikainen, M.; Kylliäinen, A.; Mäkelä, T.E.; Saarenpää-Heikkilä, O.; Paavonen, E.J. Exposure to Electronic Media Was Negatively Associated with Speech and Language Development at 18 and 24 Months. Acta Paediatr. 2021, 110, 3046–3053. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cespedes, E.M.; Gillman, M.W.; Kleinman, K.; Rifas-Shiman, S.L.; Redline, S.; Taveras, E.M. Television Viewing, Bedroom Television, and Sleep Duration From Infancy to Mid-Childhood. Pediatrics 2014, 133, e1163–e1171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nathanson, A.I.; Beyens, I. The Relation Between Use of Mobile Electronic Devices and Bedtime Resistance, Sleep Duration, and Daytime Sleepiness Among Preschoolers. Behav. Sleep. Med. 2018, 16, 202–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rescorla, L. Age 17 Language and Reading Outcomes in Late-Talking Toddlers: Support for a Dimensional Perspective on Language Delay. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2009, 52, 16–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Torppa, M.; Lyytinen, P.; Erskine, J.; Eklund, K.; Lyytinen, H. Language Development, Literacy Skills, and Predictive Connections to Reading in Finnish Children with and without Familial Risk for Dyslexia. J. Learn. Disabil. 2010, 43, 308–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Characteristics | n (%) |
---|---|
Child | |
Boy | 81 (49) |
First born | 94 (57) |
Day care | |
Full-time Part-time | 130 (79) 29 (18) |
Parent | |
Education of mothers | |
Comprehensive school High school Bachelor’s degree Master’s degree or higher | 3 (2) 17 (10) 27 (17) 112 (68) |
Education of fathers | |
Comprehensive school High school Bachelor’s degree Master’s degree or higher | 4 (2) 36 (22) 33 (20) 82 (50) |
Variable | M | SD | Min-Max |
---|---|---|---|
Screen time of children | |||
Weekday alone | 40.34 | 30.16 | 0–150 |
Day off alone | 54.72 | 40.34 | 0–210 |
Daily average alone | 44.45 | 31.22 | 0–167 |
Weekday co-view | 30.53 | 28.19 | 0–120 |
Day off co-view | 43.92 | 37.58 | 0–210 |
Daily average co-view | 34.35 | 29.39 | 0–135 |
Screen time of mothers | |||
Weekday | 397.17 | 186.20 | 30–720 |
Day off | 178.81 | 80.39 | 15–360 |
Daily average | 333.54 | 138.35 | 26–579 |
Language skills of children | |||
FinCDI III Words | 67.8 | 16.77 | 7–100 |
FPT (percent.) | 60.3 | 25.31 | 4–98 |
FMT | 23.18 | 14.60 | 0–62 |
RDLS III Receptive (sp) | 103.97 | 12.51 | 50–127 |
FinCDI III Total | 105.29 | 25.66 | 14–151 |
Lexicon | Structure | General Language Ability | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Screen Time (Daily Average) | FinCDI III Words | FPT | FMT | RDSL III Receptive | FinCDI III Total |
Children alone r | −0.10 | −0.12 | −0.03 | −0.10 | −0.12 |
Children alone rp | −0.16 | −0.13 | −0.08 | −0.10 | −0.18 * |
Children co-view r | 0.22 ** | 0.19 * | 0.003 | 0.07 | 0.22 ** |
Children co-view rp | 0.19 * | 0.17 * | −0.07 | 0.07 | 0.19 * |
Mothers r | −0.29 ** | −0.09 | −0.10 | −0.05 | −0.30 ** |
Mothers rp | −0.24 ** | −0.06 | −0.01 | −0.05 | −0.25 ** |
FinCDI III Words | FinCDI III Total | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Explanatory Variables | B | p B | R2adj | R2ch | p Model | B | p B | R2adj | R2ch | p Model |
Screen time of children alone | −0.08 | 0.03 * | 0.28 | 0.02 * | <0.001 ** | −0.13 | 0.02 * | 0.30 | 0.03 * | <0.001 ** |
Screen time co-view | 0.08 | 0.07 | 0.27 | 0.016 | <0.001 ** | 0.11 | 0.08 | 0.29 | 0.014 | <0.001 ** |
Screen time of mothers | −0.02 | 0.01 * | 0.29 | 0.03 * | <0.001 ** | −0.04 | 0.007 ** | 0.32 | 0.04 * | <0.001 ** |
Screen time of children alone | −0.09 | 0.02 * | 0.31 | 0.06 ** | <0.001 ** | −0.14 | 0.02 * | 0.34 | 0.06 ** | <0.001 ** |
and screen time of mothers | −0.02 | 0.02 * | −0.03 | 0.01 * |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Mustonen, R.; Torppa, R.; Stolt, S. Screen Time of Preschool-Aged Children and Their Mothers, and Children’s Language Development. Children 2022, 9, 1577. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9101577
Mustonen R, Torppa R, Stolt S. Screen Time of Preschool-Aged Children and Their Mothers, and Children’s Language Development. Children. 2022; 9(10):1577. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9101577
Chicago/Turabian StyleMustonen, Riikka, Ritva Torppa, and Suvi Stolt. 2022. "Screen Time of Preschool-Aged Children and Their Mothers, and Children’s Language Development" Children 9, no. 10: 1577. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9101577