Next Article in Journal
Changes in Nutritional State and Cardiovascular Parameters in Alimentary Obese Children after a Month-Long Stay in Children’s Treatment Center
Next Article in Special Issue
An Evaluation of Health-Related Quality of Life in Children with Nasal Septum Deviation
Previous Article in Journal
The Incidence of Bracing Treatment on Static and Dynamic Baropodometric Parameters in Adolescent Idiopathic Scoliosis
Previous Article in Special Issue
Retropharyngeal, Parapharyngeal and Peritonsillar Abscesses
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Commentary

Deaf Children Need Rich Language Input from the Start: Support in Advising Parents

1
Department of Communication, University of California at San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA
2
Department of Linguistics, Gallaudet University, Washington, DC 20002, USA
3
Department of Linguistics, Swarthmore College, Swarthmore, PA 19081, USA
4
Division of Social Sciences, Department of Communication and Dean, University of California at San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA
5
Department of Deaf Studies and Sign Language Interpreting, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, 10019 Berlin, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Children 2022, 9(11), 1609; https://doi.org/10.3390/children9111609
Submission received: 23 September 2022 / Revised: 13 October 2022 / Accepted: 19 October 2022 / Published: 22 October 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Pediatric Otolaryngology-Expert Reviews and Advances)

Abstract

:
Bilingual bimodalism is a great benefit to deaf children at home and in schooling. Deaf signing children perform better overall than non-signing deaf children, regardless of whether they use a cochlear implant. Raising a deaf child in a speech-only environment can carry cognitive and psycho-social risks that may have lifelong adverse effects. For children born deaf, or who become deaf in early childhood, we recommend comprehensible multimodal language exposure and engagement in joint activity with parents and friends to assure age-appropriate first-language acquisition. Accessible visual language input should begin as close to birth as possible. Hearing parents will need timely and extensive support; thus, we propose that, upon the birth of a deaf child and through the preschool years, among other things, the family needs an adult deaf presence in the home for several hours every day to be a linguistic model, to guide the family in taking sign language lessons, to show the family how to make spoken language accessible to their deaf child, and to be an encouraging liaison to deaf communities. While such a support program will be complicated and challenging to implement, it is far less costly than the harm of linguistic deprivation.

1. Introduction

While an emphasis on early language acquisition (from birth and in the first years of life) is important advice to parents, we recognize that providing a deaf child with adequate access to language remains challenging both for parents and the professionals advising them. (We use deaf to include all levels of hearing loss affecting linguistic input.) We argue that children who are born deaf or become deaf in early childhood need multimodal and comprehensible language exposure and engagement in joint activity with parents, caretakers, siblings, and others in their social world. For ease of exposition, we use the cover term parents to include all these relevant parties. In addition, we argue that choosing speech to the exclusion of another modality is a false choice that runs a greater risk of delayed development and /or language deprivation for the deaf child. Sign language and spoken language can be used side by side without compromising each other to greatly reduce the risk of harm to deaf children. There is no simple way to compare research regarding deaf children’s linguistic and cognitive development with regard to sign language versus spoken language since studies vary in how they lump together different language environments and since studies vary in how they assess linguistic and cognitive development. To make our arguments, then, we first outline the risks that face children who are deaf in early childhood. We discuss the complexities of the developmental process when a deaf child is born into a hearing family that does not have a history of sign language use or awareness. Then, we argue that children who are deaf in early childhood need to be immersed in a multimodal and multilingual environment, within which they are exposed to sign language with the frequency and richness that assures age-appropriate first language acquisition. Even when most parents of deaf newborns do not know sign language, achieving sufficient language acquisition begins at home and continues in school.
At the end, we refer the reader to Humphries and colleagues [1,2], where common questions asked by parents of deaf children are answered in a straightforward way with the best available evidence. The questions are listed in Appendix A for the reader’s reference.

2. Risks Facing Deaf Children

Children exposed to accessible language in early childhood will acquire it naturally. Early, multimodal, and accessible language exposure is of crucial importance: infancy is a sensitive period in the acquisition of language [3,4,5]. Delays in exposing children to accessible language carry cognitive and psycho-social risks [6,7,8]. Children with language delay, regardless of the cause of that delay, are too often “on a trajectory of academic failure and social difficulties” [9] (p. 120). This stands to reason: our ability to read and write depends on linguistic competence, and a great part of our ability to interact with other human beings depends on our ability to understand others and to be understood by others. Late learning of a first language affects the architecture of the brain [10,11,12,13,14,15], which may be (part of) why acquisition is so difficult for deaf late learners of sign languages who do not already have firm competence in a spoken language. Word order in one-clause sentences, for example, seems possible to be learned later [16] but word order in complex sentences seems less readily learned [17]. Even those with severe cognitive deficits, such as Williams Syndrome [18] benefit from early language exposure and multimodality.
Deaf adults face an increased risk of chronic health difficulties that correlate with adverse childhood communication experiences [19]. They are more likely to be under-educated [20], be un(der)employed [21,22], be abused emotionally and physically [23,24,25,26,27,28], experience food insecurity [29], experience injustices at the hand of the criminal justice system [30,31], and experience poor health outcomes [32,33,34] partly due to limited access to or inappropriate health services [35,36,37,38].
Scholars and many medical professionals know the risks of late acquisition of a language. Yet many deaf children are still not receiving adequate exposure to accessible language early enough [39]. Rather, families that seek help and sometimes want a “cure” for their child’s deafness, often are given advice that is one-dimensional, i.e., unimodal, when what the child needs is a multimodal multilingual environment.
Sign languages are natural human languages, just as spoken languages are. For many arguments and citations to that effect, we refer to the American Speech–Language–Hearing Association [40,41]. Sign languages support analogical-reasoning abilities [42], executive function [43], non-verbal working memory [44], and the wide range of cognitive abilities that spoken languages support [45]; the human need for language is satisfied equally by sign languages and spoken languages [46,47]. However, there is a crucial difference regarding accessibility: sign languages are immediately accessible to all children, hearing and deaf, including deaf/blind [48,49], while spoken languages might be fully accessible only to some deaf children and only some of the time.
For the past two decades, the Joint Committee on Infant Hearing in the United States has been recommending that deaf children’s language development be assessed routinely [50,51], but the recommendation is not widely followed, most likely because of the difficulties in properly assessing this development, given that deaf children are a heterogenous group with respect to their language access profile, where both quantity and quality of the accessible language matters [52,53]. As a result of this lack of monitoring, delays in making strides in language development might not be detected until the child is seriously behind. It is common for parents to persist with speech until it becomes undeniable that the child is not developing as hoped. At that point, they finally turn to sign language for reliable communication (see Pfister [54] for a description in Mexico City; much of the experience described is common across North America) but by then, unfortunately, significant time has been lost for the child’s cognitive development [55,56]. Children with typical language development make huge developmental strides in language and cognition in short periods of time, such that a few weeks in early childhood is significant in a child’s life. If it is not absolutely clear that a deaf child is making timely developmental strides and that there is strong, direct two-way communication between children and caregivers, no parent/caregiver should wait [19].
Sometimes parents request suboptimal treatment for their children—perhaps from denial, perhaps from previous unhelpful advice, for financial reasons, or for religious reasons—and medical professionals find themselves having to decide whether parents’ requests are in the best interest of the deaf child. In a study of such decisions in the United Kingdom, doctors were less likely to agree to the suboptimal treatment if it increased the risk of death or the risk of long-term disability than if it increased the risk of pain [57]. Similar findings (with even less willingness to allow parents to make decisions that risked harm to their children) appear in a study of American oncologists [58]. Medical professionals must transcend their own biases, and sometimes their past training, to advise parents not to wait when there is little or no evidence of the normative leaps and bounds in the child’s early linguistic and cognitive development.
The health of the overwhelming majority of deaf children is at stake in this discussion [59,60,61]. While deaf babies born to signing deaf parents, generally, are exposed to accessible language from day one; 96% of deaf babies are born to non-signing hearing parents [62], and it is those children who are most at risk of delayed language development. Many medical professionals advise parents to have their child undergo surgery for a cochlear implant (CI) and, for hearing parents, to raise the child with speech and hearing only [63]. This advice has led parents to trust that a CI will, essentially, make their child “hearing”, often giving them an unwarranted sense of security in their decision. However, success with CI is enormously variable: a CI alone often cannot be relied upon to provide the kind of language access necessary for healthy language development [60,64]. Many children with a CI or bilateral CIs do not have sufficient access to spoken language input for timely language and cognitive development, even after years of intensive aural rehabilitation (auditory training and speech exercises) with family and speech therapists [65,66,67,68,69,70,71]. Perhaps most important of all, we lack ways to accurately predict the likelihood of the benefit of a CI for individual deaf children [2,72,73,74].
The ethical action for medical professionals is to avoid assuring parents that a CI is a clear path to hearing and language. Three kinds of evidence to this effect are readily available to scholars and medical professionals. First, deaf children lag behind their hearing peers in language development even with a CI or bilateral CIs [75,76], and even when they have been implanted at a very early age [77]. Delays are evident in their vocabulary, reading, and writing abilities [75,78,79,80,81,82]. Children with CIs exhibit weaknesses in complex, high-order language processing across the board [83]. While some implanted children demonstrate semantic processing of vocabulary after 12 months of CI use, others fail to show improved processing after two years of CI use. Since that time period goes past the sensitive period for language acquisition for the latter group, it is no surprise that that group had poor language outcomes overall [84].
Second, if a medical device is providing what is needed—i.e., if it is effective—then deaf people themselves will be incentivized to use it. However, CI use is not uniform, implicitly calling into question its efficacy. Studies have varied on continued CI use 10 years after implantation: from 63% to 87.8% [85,86,87,88,89]. There is also considerable disagreement among members of deaf communities as to whether being implanted in childhood improves their daily lives or not [90], where many point to limitations in auditory perception as a reason for discontinuing CI use [91]. Variation in efficacy for language acquisition is not the same as efficacy for access to sound, further complicating the benefits of CI use.
Third, unlike normal hearing, CI input is not “on” all the time, and children often use CIs inconsistently. They are not or cannot be used during certain play or athletic activities. On a daily basis, and in entirely ordinary circumstances, background noise in the environment is a factor, and that is when CIs make the job of distinguishing language exceedingly difficult [92]. Two critically important environments are school and the family dinner table. If such environments do not afford intact and visual language input via sign language, the child has reduced opportunities for incidental or contextual learning [45,93].

3. Sign Language Exposure Ensures Accessibility

Here we address questions related to the benefit of sign languages for a deaf child. We distinguish natural sign languages, used by deaf communities, from manually coded versions of spoken languages, such as ”Signed Exact English” (SEE), and other manual codes for spoken language. We know of no evidence that using such codes exclusively offers a viable or sufficient first language foundation (cf. [94]).
Sometimes the rationale for denying a deaf child a sign language is based on the assumption that learning a sign language would compete for the child’s attention and possibly change the brain in such a way as to interfere with learning a spoken language. These assumptions are false: no such interference in brain behavior or cognition occurs [95,96,97]. Indeed, sign languages and spoken languages both are robustly left hemisphere-lateralized with respect to production, regardless of the difference in the modality of language input [98], and sign languages and spoken languages are cortically organized in terms of specialization of the dominant perisylvian system [99]. In particular, regardless of whether a child signs or speaks, their oculomotor behavior during reading is the same [100]. A minimum expectation, then, should be that learning to sign would not interfere with spoken language abilities at all, no matter how early bimodal-bilingualism is introduced [101,102,103,104].
On the contrary, signing benefits the cognitive abilities of the deaf child that is also learning a spoken language, just as bilingualism between spoken languages benefits cognitive abilities [105], particularly bilingualism in the early years of life [106]. Early bimodal-bilingualism affects neural activation in a way that may well enhance linguistic and cognitive processing [107]. In fact, regardless of the use of a CI, sign language proficiency positively correlates to high literacy attainment, including writing [3,108,109,110,111,112,113,114,115,116,117,118,119,120,121,122,123,124,125,126,127,128,129,130,131,132]. It also correlates positively to a facility with spoken language [133,134]. Sign language knowledge simply supports spoken language knowledge across the board [135,136,137], and this advantage goes both ways: sign language and spoken language promote the development of the other [67,127,133,138,139,140,141]. One way that signing can help a deaf child develop a spoken language is that signing directs visual attention to the participants in a conversation, an important step in communication in general and in recognizing lip actions [142] (see p. 1117) and in turn-taking in conversation [143]. Learning such visual attention can help with speechreading, in particular, which is what deaf children with CI rely on to varying degrees [144]. Indeed, sign language acquisition improves the linguistic and cognitive abilities of hearing children with developmental language disorders [145].

4. Understanding Bimodal-Bilingualism

Older views on the use of sign language and spoken language in the same environment misconstrue the nature of either modality and how each language is used by deaf people and deaf families as well as hearing families who successfully use sign language. The simple truth is that learning a language, spoken or sign, does not harm a child [146].
For example, Fitzpatrick and colleagues [104] offer what they call “a systematic review of the effectiveness of early sign and oral language intervention” and conclude that there is insufficient evidence that the addition of sign language fosters the acquisition of spoken language. That study is seriously flawed, however, as Hall [64] shows, since it does not distinguish between natural sign languages and artificial communication systems that use the hands—what are known as manually coded languages (MCLs, such as SEE mentioned earlier). This flaw is found in other works, as well, such as Geers and colleagues [147]. In fact, differences in findings between deaf children exposed to sign languages from deaf children exposed to other sign systems/MCLs are significant, where sign languages aid the deaf child in overall language development but MCLs do not [148]. MCLs lack characteristics of natural languages across the components of grammar. They are articulatorily unnatural in that they often “violate the perceptual fit between central and peripheral visual processes” [45] (p. 85). This makes MCLs difficult to use as a means of communication and inappropriate as a first language. While we do not know of studies addressing whether MCLs increase vocabulary, whatever positive effect they might have (or not have) in this regard is not reflected in the studies we have read. MCLs do not enable bilingualism or language transfer [119,149]. Using MCLs to test and measure the effectiveness of bimodal-bilingualism is misleading, because the bilingual component is missing, as MCLs are not naturally evolved languages. MCLs are an impoverished surrogate for either spoken language or sign language.
What do true bilingual-bimodal language interactions look like, then? In such environments sign language is used by as many people as possible in the child’s presence, whether the child is looking or not. In a natural environment, deaf children will respond to constant exposure to sign language. A spoken language is also present in the environment via speech, hearing, and print. For example, in the case of French and French Sign Language (LSF), French speech and hearing are also present in the environment as well as reading and writing. Both LSF and French are used in accordance with the deaf child’s ability to perceive and respond to them. This does not mean speaking and signing at the same time, but that combination does no harm as long as LSF is ever present and its structural integrity is not compromised by attempts to make LSF map directly onto French speech. It may be better to use LSF in some situations and French in different situations. Children growing up bilingual are not confused by using and switching between languages, even within the same sentence [150] (but see remarks in [151]), and as their overall linguistic competence grows, they successfully use mixed-language sentences just as adults do [152].
Bimodal-bilingualism for the family means using a natural sign language with the deaf infant or toddler throughout the day, and using a spoken language, as well, particularly reading and writing—its visual forms. If the child actively responds to speech, one would use a spoken language, as well as sign—not at the same time necessarily, but perhaps in tandem or alternatingly. Additionally, whether or not the child responds to speech, one would introduce the child to printed texts of the ambient spoken language—in shared reading activities at home, particularly with the help of recently developed bimodal-bilingual books [153,154,155,156]. These activities are most helpful when interactive strategies are employed, such as mimicry, asking open-ended questions, praising freely, and taking care to articulate signs in the child’s visual field [157]. In addition, doing structured activities targeting phonological awareness in a sign language as brief as 12 min daily for up to two months can produce positive effects on deaf children’s phonological awareness ([158,159]. An added benefit of the bimodal-bilingual approach is that the deaf child feels accepted and valued as a deaf person, which is a significant part of making a strong family bond [160,161].

5. Language Acquisition Begins at Home

Planning sign language acquisition in the home with appropriate family measures is happening in many countries, but this planning is largely undocumented and unresearched [162]. One difficulty is that institutional policies shift with each new player on the field exerting influence on the kinds and levels of support for families [163]. In the next section, we turn to the matter of language planning and policy in educational institutions. Here, instead, we focus on families who must cope with the confusing swirl of different recommendations.
Acquisition of sign language for the child begins with parents learning it as well, sometimes along with the child. As the parents’ fluency develops, so will the child’s access to language. Families and, particularly, caregivers need immediate and supportive information about sign language learning and connections to peer groups, especially since the early advice they receive substantially influences their attitudes toward deafness, thus, shaping the language planning they adopt [164]. Parents must have opportunities to learn sign language in a way that will not intimidate them nor make them feel incompetent, both for family bonding and because the degree to which a parent feels empowered and confident in their language choice with their deaf child correlates to better language and cognitive development of the child [165,166,167,168,169]. In fact, in a study of Spanish-speaking hearing mothers of deaf children in North America, a mother became so comfortable with signing that she used it naturally even with hearing members of the family without realizing it [170]. Most parents are highly motivated and learn to sign to varying degrees [171], where even moderate fluency offers great benefits to the child [161]. In 2014, 23% of families in the USA reported signing with their deaf children regularly [172].
Deaf children’s sign language acquisition will likely outpace their parents’ due to a child’s early childhood enhanced capacity to acquire language. Adults often do not become highly fluent, but they can still be effective communicators in sign language, which is what their child needs. Their child will acquire sign language naturally, transcending their parents’ abilities and being entirely fluent [173].
Different supports for helping families provide rich sign language input have been suggested. For example, Humphries and colleagues [174] call for government resources to fund sign language instruction for families of deaf children until the child is at least age 12. They also call for research on adult second-language learning in a second modality so that countries can improve the effectiveness of sign language instruction for hearing adults. Likewise, Koulidobrova, Kuntze, and Dostal [175] call for more resources to be allocated to parents who want to learn ASL and more research on how families can be helped in raising their child with sign as well as speech. Alfano [170] suggests parent-oriented workshops to teach parents how to communicate with their children, family get-togethers so that parents with deaf children can share strategies and resources on language activities, game-playing activities to promote ASL learning, and classes in ASL with transportation provided. Humphries and colleagues [2] urge parents to use all available resources, including doctors, local and national deaf community centers, schools for the deaf, deaf education services, articles, books, and the internet to find information and support. Weaver and Starner [176] offer a mobile app (SMARTSign) for learning ASL on one’s phone. Lillo-Martin, Gale, and Chen Pichler [177] outline, in detail, many actions that can help, from informing parents properly about bilingualism to informing medical professionals about language development in deaf children.
We know little about how well these suggestions work or what will optimize parental learning of sign [178], but we do know the importance of deaf people—individuals and communities—in the process of supporting deaf children to learn to sign [179,180,181,182,183,184,185,186] and in supporting families to learn how to help deaf children succeed [187,188,189,190,191] and to learn about deaf culture and deaf ways of managing in a hearing world [192]. Wille, Van Lierde, and Van Herreweghe [193] report that in Flanders, Belgium, where Flemish Sign Language was recognized as an official language in 2006, a parental support intervention practice was established for hearing parents of deaf children in 2014. The communicative strategies used by hearing parents and other family members with their deaf children varied from those used by deaf parents with deaf children (who all developed fluency in sign language). For example, hearing parents, in this study, relied more strongly on oral/aural strategies and explicit means of gaining visual attention (such as repositioning the child and waving), while deaf parents use more tactile and implicit means of gaining visual attention (such as waiting for the child to look up before signing). This case study demonstrates that hearing parents would benefit from learning the strategies used by deaf adults and deaf parents.
We agree with calls for research to find effective instruction for parents and for resources to pay for that instruction. In addition, we suggest an approach (as one best-practice example) that builds on the Utah State University Deaf Mentor Experimental Project described by Watkins, Pittman, and Walden [140]: deaf mentors made regular home visits to families with deaf children, ages 0–5 years, sharing ASL with them. In their three-year study comparing deaf children in the Utah Deaf Mentor program with deaf children without deaf mentors in Tennessee, where half the Tennessee children used only spoken language and the other half used total communication and MCLs, the children with deaf mentors exhibited greater linguistic, cognitive, and social progress than the children without deaf mentors. A study of a similar program involving deaf mentors in the UK found that children and families with mentors were more confident in the possibility of a good future for their children and this allowed families to set high expectations for the children and for children to have the confidence to strive to meet those expectations [194].
The approach we suggest here is also influenced heavily by evidence from a model used at the Seattle Children’s Hospital. There, Crezee, and Roat [195] found that bilingual patient navigators are more effective than professional interpreters in the success of the hospital’s treatment of deaf children as measured by “no-show rates, number of unplanned hospitalizations, average length of stay, and staff/family confidence in the family ability to care for the patient at home.” This success happened through a trust relationship, where a navigator helps everyone understand missed inferences, restates everything in plain language, alerts everyone to barriers to “implementation of treatment plans”, teaches basic skills in how to talk with teachers, and so on. Deaf mentors have, in fact, been a resource for decades [196], with efforts such as these implemented in varying places with varying consistency, and many models are possible [197,198].
Consider the following scenario. What if, upon the birth of a deaf child, a sign language “navigator” was made available to the family immediately? This person would be a bimodal-bilingual deaf adult trained to be a navigator. These individuals would:
be a bilingual model for the child as well as all family members;
help the family choose and sign up for sign language classes or tutoring;
be with the family for a significant number of hours each day using sign language with the child and all family members, helping them to communicate in various ways, including through signing, writing, and even gesturing;
help the family to connect to the local deaf community for socialization activities;
help the family to connect to other families with hearing parents and deaf children, as well as deaf families with deaf children;
help parents become advocates for their children [199].
The navigator can quell the family’s initial anxiety about their ability to raise a deaf child [200] and be there as a support at every step. As a result, bilingual and bimodal language interaction will be part of daily routines and play, which means everyone can exercise their right to enjoy the presence of a new baby in the house, promoting the well-being of the whole family [201] and everyone will have a consistent sign language model [202]. Further, parents will not be shy to communicate with their children in several modes, including pointing and other gestures. Mixed communication modes are natural in raising a hearing child [203,204,205,206]; there is no need to fear the same in raising a deaf child. Human communication is essentially multimodal, whether the primary language is speech or sign [207]. Parents can embellish their signing and speech with gestures they find natural as their own vocabulary in the sign language grows, knowing that the child always has a good signing model in the navigator.
This presence of a deaf adult can ease the family’s initial fears for their child’s future; the family will know a professional deaf adult well and be reassured that their own child can grow up to be a productive member of society. A deaf model, being bimodal-bilingual, can demonstrate to the family how people who are multilingual choose the language of a particular interaction [208,209]. Further, bilingualism can mean biculturalism, which has implications for behavior and identity [61].
Ideally, navigators would be available for a family from the birth of the child up to the point when the child enters primary school. In this way, the family will benefit from the presence of someone who knows the situation and all the relevant parties, and can help the family and medical and educational professionals in making recommendations that the family is able and likely to follow [198,210]. This approach requires that navigator training be available and that the government pay for deaf adults to work with families of deaf children. Then, a deaf adult can provide training in language and cognitive development milestones and can alert the relevant parties when needed.
The benefits to individuals and to society of such an approach to supporting parents will outweigh the costs by a significant amount. The human and economic benefits of investing in support for parents (e.g., in the form of navigator training) are well worth the cost, as deaf children will be far more likely to become happy, healthy, and productive members of society, contributing to the overall economic base of society. Hamilton and Clark [211] report that, as of 2020, a curriculum built around a deaf mentor and developed by Sensory Kids Impaired Home Intervention (SKI-HI) [212] is used in seven to ten states. Their study of families that made use of this particular deaf mentor program found that the children benefited by having a language-rich environment and by acquiring deaf practices of learning strategies and resources; they conclude that deaf mentors help secure the keys to success for deaf children.

6. Continuing Bimodal-Bilingualism into Schooling

Deaf children with hearing parents who are raised in a bimodal-bilingual environment can succeed in both languages [56], where success correlates positively with the length of time the child is enrolled in a bimodal-bilingual school environment [213], and where success in accessing a spoken language is strictly via its written form [117,214,215]. Neither the medical profession nor families are trained educators and, thus, they are not fully aware of what happens once the child goes to school. However, both the medical profession and parents can lobby for what should happen. It is important, then, to know what the bilingual-bimodal school environment looks like when practiced appropriately.
Howerton-Fox and Falk [216] argue that deaf children should be considered “English learners” and receive the kind of support given in the educational system to children for whom English is not their first language. Some countries have compared a bilingual approach to an oral-only approach for deaf children—with mixed results—but it is not always clear what their label of “bilingual” means. Razalli and colleagues [217], however, did a study with precise information. They compared the literacy achievements of deaf children in Malay using two educational programs. In one, spoken/written Malay was paired with Manually Coded Malay; in the other, spoken/written Malay was paired with Malay Sign Language. Deaf children achieved significantly higher skills in the program that used Malay Sign Language—that is, in a truly bilingual-bimodal program that compared two natural languages. To evaluate one type of educational approach vis a vis another, care must be taken in the research design to ensure that the sign language being evaluated is, indeed, a sign language and not a code for the spoken language.
Humphries and colleagues [2] argue that schooling for deaf children should have a focus on translanguaging, which involves everyone in the classroom using one’s full linguistic repertoire during activities and conversations. While the main components of translanguaging are the languages in the students’ and teachers’ repertoires—the sign languages and the spoken languages—many other forms of media are involved, such as print, video, recordings, and visuals. Deaf children coming to school from a home that is bilingual-bimodal will already have a head start in the translanguaging process. In the most supportive type of schooling, deaf children will be brought together in the classroom [218], so they can support each other socially and emotionally, as well as academically.
Another focus of schooling should be multi-literacy [219,220,221], including the preliteracy skills of understanding what a story is, what characters are, and why characters behave as they do in the stories. Vocabulary should be enhanced by building on sign language skills. There should be explicit teaching of phonemic awareness and morphological awareness in both spoken language and sign language texts [158,159]. Deaf children often benefit from lessons with a deaf teacher on the structure of their sign language, with explicit comparisons between the child’s sign language and the ambient spoken language [222] and on deaf cultural history and processes [223].
Humphries and colleagues [2] also make several schooling recommendations regarding inclusivity. Schooling is most effective when deaf children are grouped together in their grade level and assigned to teachers who have strong skills in signing. Being with other deaf students is critical to developing social skills and belonging in young children and youth. Qualified educational sign language interpreters should be in classrooms when there are staff and students who are emerging sign language learners [224]. Inclusivity is different from inclusion. Inclusion is often interpreted in ways that isolate deaf students from each other and from the language support they need. Bilingual bimodal schooling for deaf children makes sure deaf children are schooled in a rich academic, social, and cultural learning community.

Author Contributions

T.H., G.M., D.J.N., C.P. and C.R. contributed to planning, discussion, and writing. T.H., G.M., D.J.N., C.P. and C.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study did not require ethical approval.

Informed Consent Statement

This study did not include human subjects.

Data Availability Statement

This study is not based on a corpus of data. Rather, the arguments come from the cited works.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A. Questions and Answers for Parents

Humphries and colleagues [1] (hereafter H2016) argue that medical professionals should advise parents of deaf children to start themselves and their children on learning a sign language as soon as their child’s auditory status is detected. H2016 address six common questions parents have and offer straightforward, evidence-based answers:
(H2016 1). What will give my child the best chances of learning to talk?
(H2016 2). How can my child learn sign language if I do not sign myself?
(H2016 3). Will there be less family disruption and less work if I raise my child strictly orally (without signing)?
(H2016 4). Will signing adversely affect my child’s academic achievements? After all, bilingualism is confusing for a child.
(H2016 5). Can we wait to see if our child succeeds with a CI before working to learn to sign?
(H2016 6). Will I lose my child to deaf culture?
Humphries and colleagues [2] (hereafter H2019) urge medical professionals to counsel parents with the knowledge that deafness is not an illness (no one is sick), deafness is not implicated in delayed development (rather, linguistic deprivation is), and learning to sign is both possible and feasible. They then address additional questions, repeating one (numbered H2016 2 above) and offering answers based on work that has come to the fore since H2016.
(H2019 1). How can I teach my child signing if I don’t sign myself?
(H2019 2). If my child learns to sign from the deaf community, do I still have to learn a⋯⋯sign language?
(H2019 3). How do I, as a parent, go about learning a sign language? How do I meet the local deaf community and introduce my deaf child into it?
(H2019 4). Is it a problem if I communicate with my child in a mixture of signing, gesturing, and speaking?
(H2019 5). What is a good model for the kind of language and communication environment that my deaf child and I should strive to be in?
(H2019 6). Apart from learning a sign language and using it with my deaf child, what else should I be doing as a parent?
(H2019 7). I am overwhelmed with conflicting advice. What messages about my deaf child and me should I be paying attention to?

References

  1. Humphries, T.; Kushalnagar, P.; Mathur, G.; Napoli, D.J.; Padden, C.; Rathmann, C.; Smith, S. Language choices for deaf infants: Advice for parents regarding sign languages. Clin. Pediatr. 2016, 55, 513–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Humphries, T.; Kushalnagar, P.; Mathur, G.; Napoli, D.J.; Rathmann, C.; Smith, C. Support for parents of deaf children: Common questions and informed, evidence-based answers. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 2019, 118, 134–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Mayberry, R.I.; Del Giudice, A.A.; Lieberman, A.M. Reading achievement in relation to phonological coding and awareness in deaf readers: A meta-analysis. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2011, 16, 164–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Pakulak, E.; Neville, H. Biological bases of language development. In Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development; [online]; Tremblay, R.E., Boivin, M., Peters, R.D.e.V., Eds.; Center of Excellence for Early Child Development, Canadian Language and Literacy Research Network: Montreal, QC, Canada, 2010; pp. 1–7. [Google Scholar]
  5. Lillo-Martin, D.C.; Henner, J. Acquisition of sign languages. Ann. Rev. Linguist. 2021, 7, 395–419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Glickman, N.S.; Hall, W.C. (Eds.) Language Deprivation and Deaf Mental Health; Routledge: Abingdon-on-Thames, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  7. Kushalnagar, P.; Bruce, S.; Sutton, T.; Leigh, I.W. Retrospective parent-child communication difficulties and risk for depression in deaf adults. J. Dev. Phys. Disabil. 2017, 29, 25–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  8. Dammeyer, J. Psychosocial development in a Danish population of children with cochlear implants and deaf and hard-of-hearing children. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2009, 15, 50–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Kaiser, A.P.; Chow, J.C.; Cunningham, J. A case for early language and behavior screening: Implications for policy and child development. Policy Insights Behav. Brain Sci. 2022, 9, 120–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Mayberry, R.I.; Davenport, T.; Roth, A.; Halgren, E. Neurolinguistic processing when the brain matures without language. Cortex 2018, 99, 390–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Mayberry, R.I.; Kluender, R. Rethinking the critical period for language: New insights into an old question from American Sign Language. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2018, 21, 886–905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Woll, B. The consequences of very late exposure to BSL as an L1. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2018, 21, 936–937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Mayberry, R.I.; Chen, J.-K.; Witcher, P.; Klein, D. Age of acquisition effects on the functional organization of language in the adult brain. Brain Lang. 2011, 119, 16–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Lieberman, A.M.; Borovsky, A.; Hatrak, M.; Mayberry, R.I. Real-time processing of ASL signs: Delayed first language acquisition affects organization of the mental lexicon. J. Exp. Psychol Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2015, 41, 1130–1139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Ferjan Ramirez, N.; Leonard, M.K.; Torres, C.; Hatrak, M.; Halgren, E.; Mayberry, R.I. Neural language processing in adolescent first-language learners. Cereb. Cortex 2013, 24, 2772–2783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Cheng, Q.; Mayberry, R.I. Acquiring a first language in adolescence: The case of basic word order in American Sign Language. J. Child Lang. 2019, 46, 214–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Boudreault, P.; Mayberry, R.I. Grammatical processing in American Sign Language: Age of first-language acquisition effects in relation to syntactic structure. Lang. Cogn. Process. 2006, 21, 608–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Bellugi, U.; Lai, Z.; Wang, P. Language, communication, and neural systems in Williams syndrome. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 1997, 3, 334–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Kushalnagar, P.; Ryan, C.; Paludneviciene, R.; Spellun, A.; Gulati, S. Adverse childhood communication experiences associated with an increased risk of chronic diseases in adults who are deaf. Am. J. Prevent. Med. 2020, 59, 548–554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Garberoglio, C.L.; Palmer, J.L.; Cawthon, S.W.; Sales, A. Deaf People and Educational Attainment in the United States: 2019; Texas Scholar Works, University of Texas Libraries: Austin, TX, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  21. Garberoglio, C.L.; Palmer, J.L.; Cawthon, S.W.; Sales, A. Deaf People and Employment in the United States: 2019; Department of Educational Psychology, Texas Scholar Works, University of Texas Libraries: Austin, TX, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Winn, S.L. Employment outcomes for the congenitally deaf in Australia: Has anything changed? Am. Ann. Deaf 2007, 152, 382–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Schenkel, L.S.; Rothman-Marshall, G.; Schlehofer, D.A.; Towne, T.L.; Burnash, D.L.; Priddy, B.M. Child maltreatment and trauma exposure among deaf and hard of hearing young adults. Child Abus. Negl. 2014, 38, 1581–1589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Jones, C.; Stalker, K.; Franklin, A.; Fry, D.A.; Cameron, A.; Taylor, J. Enablers of help-seeking for deaf and disabled children following abuse and barriers to protection: A qualitative study. Child Fam. Soc. Work 2017, 22, 762–771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Morton, D.; Marcus, A. Mixed Gendered Group Psychotherapy of Sexually Abused Deaf College Students; Western Connecticut State University: Danbury, CT, USA, 2021; Available online: https://westcollections.wcsu.edu/handle/20.500.12945/1174/restricted-resource?bitstreamId=d496ec2e-3939-41ff-9fea-43839930033b (accessed on 1 August 2022).
  26. Knutson, J.F.; Johnson, C.R.; Sullivan, P.M. Disciplinary choice of mothers of deaf children and mothers of normally hearing children. Child Abus. Negl. 2004, 28, 925–937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Sullivan, P.M.; Knutson, J.F. Maltreatment and disabilities: A population based epidemiological study. Child Abus. Negl. 2000, 24, 1257–1273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Kvam, M. Sexual abuse of deaf children. A retrospective analysis of the prevalence and characteristics of childhood sexual abuse among Deaf adults in Norway. Child Abus. Negl. 2004, 28, 241–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Kushalnagar, P.; Moreland, C.J.; Simons, A.; Holcomb, T. Communication barrier in family linked to increased risks for food insecurity among deaf people who use American Sign Language. Public Health Nutr. 2018, 21, 912–916. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  30. Vernon, M.C.; Miller, K. Obstacles faced by deaf people in the criminal justice system. Am. Ann. Deaf 2005, 150, 283–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Kelly, L.M. Sounding out d/Deafness: The experiences of d/Deaf prisoners. J. Crim. Psychol. 2018, 8, 20–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Fellinger, J.; Holzinger, D.; Pollard, R. Mental health of deaf people. Lancet 2012, 379, 1037–1044. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Smith, S.R.; Kushalnagar, P.; Hauser, P.C. Deaf adolescents’ learning of cardiovascular health information: Sources and access challenges. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2015, 20, 408–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Pick, L. Health care disparities in the deaf community: Individuals with hearing loss experience health care inequities. American Psychological Association Spotlight on Disability Newsletter. 2013. Available online: https://www.apa.org/pi/disability/resources/publications/newsletter/2013/11/deaf-community (accessed on 1 August 2022).
  35. Steinberg, A.G.; Barnett, S.; Meador, H.E.; Wiggins, E.A.; Zazove, P. Health care system accessibility. J. Gen. Int. Med. 2006, 21, 260–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Hemingway, B. Is a lack of research masking barriers to equitable health for deaf British Columbians? In/Versions J. Vanc. Isl. Public Interest Res. Group 2018, 16–23. Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Bryan-Hemingway/publication/331473860_Is_a_Lack_of_Research_Masking_Barriers_to_Equitable_Health_for_Deaf_British_Columbians/links/5ca54a8e92851c8e64b0f754/Is-a-Lack-of-Research-Masking-Barriers-to-Equitable-Health-for-Deaf-British-Columbians.pdf (accessed on 1 August 2022).
  37. weBarnett, S.; Franks, P. Health care utilization and adults who are deaf: Relationship with age at onset of deafness. Health Serv. Res. 2002, 37, 105–120. [Google Scholar]
  38. Kushalnagar, P.; Paludneviciene, R.; Kushalnagar, R. Video remote interpreting technology in health care: Cross-sectional study of deaf patients’ experiences. JMIR Rehabil. Assist. Technol. 2019, 6, e13233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Karchmer, M.; Mitchell, R. Demographic and achievement characteristics of deaf and hard-of-hearing students. In The Oxford Handbook of Deaf Studies, Language, and Education; Marschark, M., Spencer, P., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2003; pp. 21–37. [Google Scholar]
  40. ASHA. Final Report: Ad Hoc Committee to Develop a Position Statement on American Sign Language. 2019. Available online: https://www.asha.org/siteassets/reports/ahc-asl-position-statement.pdf (accessed on 1 August 2022).
  41. ASHA. Position Statement: American Sign Language (ASL). 2019. Available online: https://www.asha.org/policy/ps2019-00354/ (accessed on 1 August 2022).
  42. Henner, J.; Novogrodsky, R.; Caldwell-Harris, C.; Hoffmeister, R.J. The development of American Sign Language–based analogical reasoning in signing deaf children. J. Speech. Lang Hear. Res. 2019, 62, 93–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Hall, M.L.; Eigsti, I.-M.; Bortfeld, H.; Lillo-Martin, D. Auditory deprivation does not impair executive function, but language deprivation might: Evidence from a parent-report measure in deaf native signing children. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2017, 22, 9–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Marshall, C.; Jones, A.; Denmark, T.; Mason, K.; Atkinson, J.; Botting, N.; Morgan, G. Deaf children’s non-verbal working memory is impacted by their language experience. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Mayberry, R.I. Cognitive development in deaf children: The interface of language and perception in neuropsychology. In Handbook of Neuropsychology, 2nd ed.; Part II; Segalowitz, A.J., Rapin, I., Eds.; Elsevier Science B.V.: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2002; Volume 8, pp. 71–107. [Google Scholar]
  46. Bavelier, D.; Newport, E.L.; Supalla, T. Children Need Natural Languages, Signed or Spoken; The Dana Foundation: New York, NY, USA, 2003; Available online: http://www.dana.org/Cerebrum/Default.aspx?id=39306 (accessed on 12 August 2018).
  47. Krentz, U.C.; Corina, D.P. Preference for language in early infancy: The human language bias is not speech specific. Dev. Sci. 2008, 11, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Mesch, J. Tactile Sign Language: Turn Taking and Question in Signed Conversations of Deaf-Blind People; Signum: Camarillo, CA, USA, 2001; ISBN 3-927731-0-3. [Google Scholar]
  49. Edwards, T. From compensation to integration: Effects of the pro-tactile movement on the sublexical structure of Tactile American Sign Language. J. Pragmat. 2014, 69, 22–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Joint Committee on Infant Hearing. Year 2000 position statement: Principles and guidelines for early hearing detection and intervention programs. Am. J. Audiol. 2000, 9, 9–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Muse, C.; Harrison, J.; Yoshinaga-Itano, C.; Grimes, A.; Brookhouser, P.E.; Epstein, S.; Buchman, C.; Mehl, A.; Vohr, B.; Moeller, M.P.; et al. Supplement to the JCIH 2007 position statement: Principles and guidelines for early intervention after confirmation that a child is deaf or hard of hearing. Pediatrics 2013, 131, e1324–e1349. [Google Scholar]
  52. Hall, M.L. The input matters: Assessing cumulative language access in deaf and hard of hearing individuals and populations. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Bowers, L.M.; Dostal, H.; Wolbers, K.A.; Graham, S.C. The assessment of written phrasal constructs and grammar of Deaf and Hard of Hearing Students with varying expressive language abilities. Educ. Res. Int. 2018, 2018, 2139626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Pfister, A.E. Predicament and pilgrimage: Hearing families of deaf children in Mexico City. Med. Anthropol. 2019, 38, 195–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Hall, M.L.; Hall, W.C.; Caselli, N.K. Deaf children need language, not (just) speech. First Lang. 2019, 39, 367–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Kraus, K.; Gagne, D. Learning ASL as a late second language depends on the strength of the first language foundation. In Proceedings of the 45th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, MA, USA, 5–8 November 2020; Dionne, D., Vidal Covas, L.-A., Eds.; Cascadilla Press: Somerville, MA, USA, 2021; pp. 431–448. [Google Scholar]
  57. Nair, T.; Savulescu, J.; Everett, J.; Tonkens, R.; Wilkinson, D. Settling for second best: When should doctors agree to parental demands for suboptimal medical treatment? J. Med. Ethics 2017, 43, 831–840. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Nassin, M.L.; Mueller, E.L.; Ginder, C.; Kent, P.M. Family refusal of chemotherapy for pediatric cancer patients: A national survey of oncologists. J. Pediatr. Hematol. Oncol. 2015, 37, 351–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Sacks, O. Seeing Voices; Harper Collins: New York, NY, USA, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  60. Humphries, T.; Kushalnagar, P.; Mathur, G.; Napoli, D.J.; Padden, C.; Rathmann, C.; Smith, S.R. Language acquisition for deaf children: Reducing the harms of zero tolerance to the use of alternative approaches. Harm Reduct. J. 2012, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Wilkinson, E.; Morford, J.P. How bilingualism contributes to healthy development in deaf children: A public health perspective. Matern. Child Health J. 2020, 24, 1330–1338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Moores, D. Educating the Deaf: Psychology, Principles, and Practices; Houghton Mifflin: Boston, MA, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  63. Mauldin, L. Made to Hear: Cochlear Implants and Raising Deaf Children; Univeristy of Minnesota Press: Minneapolis, MN, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  64. Hall, W.C. What you don’t know can hurt you: The risk of language deprivation by impairing sign language development in deaf children. Matern. Child Health J. 2017, 21, 961–965. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Mauldin, L. Don’t look at it as a miracle cure: Contested notions of success and failure in family narratives of pediatric cochlear implantation. Soc. Sci. Med. 2019, 228, 117–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Thoutenhoofd, E.D.; Archbold, S.; Gregory, S.; Lutman, M.E.; Nikolopoulos, T.; Sach, T.H. Paediatric Cochlear Implantation: Evaluating Outcomes; Whurr: London, UK, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  67. Geers, A.E. The process and early outcomes of cochlear implantation by three years of age. In Advances in the Spoken Language Development of Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Children; Spencer, P.E., Marschark, M., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2006; pp. 271–297. [Google Scholar]
  68. Yoshinaga-Itano, C. Early identification, communication modality, and the development of speech and spoken language skills: Patterns and considerations. In Advances in the Spoken Language Development of Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Children; Spencer, P.E., Marschark, M., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2006; pp. 298–327. [Google Scholar]
  69. O’Reilly, R.; Mangiardi, A.; Bunnell, T. Cochlear implants. In Access: Multiple Avenues for Deaf People; DeLuca, D., Leigh, I.W., Lindgren, K.A., Napoli, D.J., Eds.; Gallaudet University Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2008; pp. 38–74. [Google Scholar]
  70. Martin, D.; Bat-Chava, Y.; Lalwani, A.; Waltzman, S.B. Peer relationships of deaf children with cochlear implants: Predictors of peer entry and peer interaction success. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2010, 16, 108–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  71. Pisoni, D.B.; Conway, C.M.; Kronenberger, W.G.; Horn, D.L.; Karpicke, J.; Henning, S.C. Efficacy and effectiveness of cochlear implants in deaf children. In Deaf Cognition: Foundations and Outcomes; Marschark, M., Hauser, P.C., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2008; pp. 52–101. [Google Scholar]
  72. Fink, N.E.; Wang, N.-Y.; Visaya, J.; Niparko, J.K.; Quittner, A.L.; Eisenberg, L.S.; CDACI Investigative Team. Childhood development after cochlear implantation (CDaCI) study: Design and baseline characteristics. Cochlear Implants Int. 2007, 8, 92–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Giraud, A.-L.; Lee, H.-J. Predicting cochlear implant outcome from brain organization in the deaf. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 2007, 25, 381–390. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  74. Peterson, N.R.; Pisoni, D.B.; Miyamoto, R.T. Cochlear implants and spoken language processing abilities: Review and assessment of the literature. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 2010, 28, 237–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Easterbrooks, S.R.; Lederberg, A.R.; Antia, S.; Schick, B.; Kushalnagar, P.; Webb, M.-Y.; Branum-Martin, L.; McDonald Connor, C. Reading among diverse DHH learners: What, how, and for whom? Am. Ann. Deaf 2015, 159, 419–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Spellun, A.; Kushalnagar, P. Sign language for deaf infants: A key intervention for a developmental emergency. Clin. Ped. 2018, 57, 1613–1615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Rinaldi, P.; Baruffaldi, F.; Burdo, S.; Caselli, M.C. Linguistic and pragmatic skills in toddlers with cochlear implant. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2013, 48, 715–725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Arfe, B.; Ghiselli, S.; Montino, S. The written language of children with cochlear implant. Hear. Balance Commun. 2016, 14, 103–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Lund, E.; Dinsmoor, J. Taxonomic knowledge of children with and without cochlear implants. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2016, 47, 236–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Spencer, L.J.; Barker, B.A.; Tomblin, J.B. Exploring the language and literacy outcomes of pediatric cochlear impact users. Ear Hear. 2003, 24, 236–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  81. Qi, S.; Mitchell, R.E. Large-scaled academic achievement testing of deaf and hard-of-hearing students: Past, present, and future. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2012, 17, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  82. Talli, I.; Tsalighopoulos, M.; Okalidou, A. The relation between short-term memory and vocabulary skills in Greek children with cochlear implants: The role of hearing experience. First Lang. 2018, 38, 359–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Kronenberger, W.G.; Pisoni, D.B. Assessing higher order language processing in long-term cochlear implant users. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 2019, 28, 1537–1553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Vavatzanidis, N.K.; Mürbe, D.; Friederici, A.D.; Hahne, A. Establishing a mental lexicon with cochlear implants: An ERP study with young children. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  85. Beadle, E.A.; McKinley, D.J.; Nikolopoulos, T.P.; Brough, J.; O’Donoghue, G.M.; Archbold, S.M. Long-term functional outcomes and academic-occupational status in implanted children after 10 to 14 years of cochlear implant use. Otol. Neurotol. 2005, 26, 1152–1160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Uziel, A.S.; Sillon, M.; Vieu, A.; Artieres, F.; Piron, J.-P.; Daures, J.-P.; Mondain, M. Ten-year follow-up of a consecutive series of children with multichannel cochlear implants. Otol. Neurotol. 2007, 28, 615–628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Spencer, L.J.; Gantz, B.J.; Knutson, J.F. Outcomes and achievement of students who grew up with access to cochlear implants. Laryngoscope 2004, 114, 1576–1581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  88. Contrera, K.J.; Choi, J.S.; Blake, C.R.; Betz, J.F.; Niparko, J.K.; Lin, F.R. Rates of long-term cochlear implant use in children. Otol. Neurotol. 2014, 35, 426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  89. Waltzman, S.B.; Cohen, N.L.; Green, J.; Roland, J.T., Jr. Long-term effects of cochlear implants in children. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 2002, 126, 505–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Putnam, B.; Alexander, S.P.; McMenamin, K.; Welch, D. Deaf community views on paediatric cochlear implantation. N. Z. Med. J. 2022, 135, 26. [Google Scholar]
  91. Salehomoum, M. Cochlear implant nonuse: Insight from deaf adults. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2020, 25, 270–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. Evaluating Hearing Loss for Individuals with Cochlear Implants; National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2021; Available online: https://nap.nationalacademies.org/catalog/26057/evaluating-hearing-loss-for-individuals-with-cochlear-implants (accessed on 1 August 2022).
  93. Hall, W.C.; Smith, S.R.; Sutter, E.J.; DeWindt, L.A.; Dye, T.D.V. Considering parental hearing status as a social determinant of deaf population health: Insights from experiences of the “dinner table syndrome”. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0202169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  94. Tamati, T.N.; Pisoni, D.B.; Moberly, A.C. Speech and language outcomes in adults and children with cochlear implants. Ann. Rev. Ling. 2022, 8, 299–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Lyness, C.R.; Woll, B.; Campbell, R.; Cardin, V. How does visual language affect crossmodal plasticity and cochlear implant success? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013, 37, 2621–2630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. MacSweeney, M. Cognition and deafness. In Issues in Deaf Education; Gregory, S., Knight, P., MacCracken, W., Powers, S., Watson, L., Eds.; David Fulton: London, UK, 1998; pp. 20–27. [Google Scholar]
  97. Corina, D.P.; Blau, S.; LaMarr, T.; Lawyer, L.A.; Coffey-Corina, S. Auditory and visual electrophysiology of deaf children with cochlear implants: Implications for cross-modal plasticity. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  98. Payne, H.; Gutierrez-Sigut, E.; Woll, B.; MacSweeney, M. Cerebral lateralisation during signed and spoken language production in children born deaf. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2019, 36, 100619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  99. Campbell, R.; MacSweeney, M.; Woll, B. Cochlear implantation (CI) for prelingual deafness: The relevance of studies of brain organization and the role of first language acquisition in considering outcome success. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 834. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  100. Tomasuolo, E.; Roccaforte, M.; Di Fabio, A. Reading and deafness: Eye tracking in deaf readers with different linguistic background. App. Ling. 2019, 40, 992–1008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Zanjani, M.M.O.; Hasanzadeh, S.; Rahgozar, M.; Shemshadi, H.; Purdy, S.C.; Bakhtiari, B.M.; Vahab, M. Comparing the effect of auditory-only and auditory-visual modes in two groups of Persian children using cochlear implants: A randomized clinical trial. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 2013, 77, 1545–1550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Petitto, L.A.; Katerelos, M.; Levy, B.G.; Gauna, K.; Tétreault, K.; Ferraro, V. Bilingual signed and spoken language acquisition from birth: Implications for the mechanisms underlying early bilingual language acquisition. J. Child Lang. 2001, 28, 453–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  103. Grosjean, F.; Li, P. The Psycholinguistics of Bilingualism; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  104. Fitzpatrick, E.M.; Hamel, C.; Stevens, A.; Pratt, M.; Moher, D.; Doucet, S.P.; Neuss, D.; Bernstein, A.; Na, E. Sign language and spoken language for children with hearing loss: A systematic review. Pediatrics 2016, 137, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  105. Adesope, O.O.; Lavin, T.; Thompson, T.; Ungerleider, C. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Rev. Educ. Res. 2010, 80, 207–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  106. Luk, G.; De Sa, E.; Bialystok, E. Is there a relation between onset age of bilingualism and ennhancement of cognitive control? Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2011, 14, 588–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Jasińska, K.K.; Petitto, L.A. Development of neural systems for reading in the monolingual and bilingual brain: New insights from functional near infrared spectroscopy. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2014, 39, 421–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  108. Clark, M.D.; Hauser, P.C.; Miller, P.; Kargin, T.; Rathmann, C.; Guldenoglu, B.; Kubus, O.; Spurgeon, E.; Israel, E. The Importance of early sign language acquisition for deaf readers. Read. Writ. Q. 2016, 32, 127–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Strong, M.; Prinz, P.M. A study of the relationship between American Sign Language and English literacy. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 1997, 2, 37–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  110. Strong, M.; Prinz, P.M. Is American Sign Language skill related to English literacy? In Language Acquisition by Eye; Chamberlain, C., Morford, J., Mayberry, R., Eds.; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2000; pp. 131–142. [Google Scholar]
  111. Mayer, C.; Akamatsu, C.T. Bilingualism and literacy. In Oxford Handbook of Deaf Studies, Language, and Education; Marschark, M., Spencer, P.E., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2003; pp. 136–150. [Google Scholar]
  112. Padden, C.; Ramsey, C. American Sign Language and reading ability in deaf children. In Language Acquisition by Eye; Chamberlain, C., Morford, J., Mayberry, R., Eds.; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2000; pp. 165–189. [Google Scholar]
  113. Paul, P. Processes and components of reading. In Oxford Handbook of Deaf Studies, Language, and Education; Marschark, M., Spencer, P.E., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2003; pp. 97–109. [Google Scholar]
  114. Wilbur, R. Sign language and successful bilingual development of deaf children. J. Inst. Soc. Res. 2001, 56, 1039–1079. [Google Scholar]
  115. Wilbur, R. How to prevent educational failure. In Signs and Voices: Deaf Culture, Identity, Language, and Arts; Lindgren, K.A., DeLuca, D., Napoli, D.J., Eds.; Gallaudet University Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2008; pp. 117–138. [Google Scholar]
  116. Chamberlain, C.; Mayberry, R.I. Theorizing about the relation between American Sign Language and reading. In Language Acquisition by Eye; Chamberlain, C., Morford, J.P., Mayberry, R.I., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2000; pp. 221–259. ISBN 978-0-8058-2937-2. [Google Scholar]
  117. Chamberlain, C.; Mayberry, R.I. American Sign Language syntactic and narrative comprehension in skilled and less skilled readers: Bilingual and bimodal evidence for the linguistic basis of reading. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2008, 29, 367–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  118. Hermans, D.H.; Ormel, E.; Knoors, H.; Verhoeven, L. The relationship between the reading and signing skills of deaf children in bilingual education programs. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2008, 13, 518–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  119. Goldin-Meadow, S.; Mayberry, R.I. How do profoundly deaf children learn to read? Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 2001, 16, 222–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Moores, D. Print literacy: The acquisition of reading and writing skills. In Deaf Learners: Developments in Curriculum and Instruction; Moores, D., Martin, D., Eds.; Gallaudet University Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2006; pp. 41–55. [Google Scholar]
  121. Holmer, E.; Heimann, M.; Rudner, M. Evidence of an association between sign language phonological awareness and word reading in deaf and hard-of-hearing children. Res. Develop. Disabil. 2016, 48, 145–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  122. Allen, T.E.; Letteri, A.; Choi, S.H.; Dang, D. Early visual language exposure and emergent literacy in preschool deaf children: Findings from a national longitudinal study. Am. Ann. Deaf 2014, 159, 346–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  123. Van Beijsterveldt, M.; Van Hell, J. Evaluative expression in deaf children’s written narratives. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2009, 44, 675–692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  124. Yoshinaga-Itano, C.; Sedey, A.L. Early speech development in children who are deaf or hard of hearing: Interrelationships with language and hearing. Volta Rev. 2000, 100, 181–211. [Google Scholar]
  125. Woolfe, T.; Herman, R.; Roy, P.; Woll, B. Early vocabulary development in deaf native signers: A British Sign Language adaptation of the communicative development inventories. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2010, 51, 322–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  126. Kovelman, I.; Baker, S.A.; Petitto, L.A. Age of first bilingual language exposure as a new window into bilingual reading development. Biling Lang. Cogn. 2008, 11, 203–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  127. Mounty, J.L.; Pucci, C.T.; Harmon, K.C. How deaf American Sign Language/English bilingual children become profcient readers: An emic perspective. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2014, 19, 333–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  128. Freel, B.L.; Brittany, L.; Clark, M.D.; Anderson, M.L.; Gilbert, G.L.; Musyoka, M.M.; Hauser, P.C. Deaf individuals’ bilingual abilities: American Sign Language proficiency, reading skills, and family characteristics. Psychology 2011, 2, 18–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  129. Basha, T. Signed language proficiency and writing skill of deaf children in special and integrated primary schools in Addis Ababa. Turk. Int. J. Spec. Educ. Guid. Couns. (TIJSEG) 2014, 3, 1–48. [Google Scholar]
  130. Scott, J.A.; Hoffmeister, R.J. American Sign Language and academic English: Factors influencing the reading of bilingual secondary school deaf and hard of hearing students. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2017, 22, 59–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  131. Yoshinaga-Itano, C.; Baca, R.L.; Sedey, A.L. Describing the trajectory of language development in the presence of severe to profound hearing loss: A closer look at children with cochlear implants versus hearing aids. Otol. Neurotol. 2010, 31, 1268–1274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  132. Kyle, F.E. Reading development in deaf children: The fundamental role of language skills. In Evidence-Based Practices in Deaf Education; Knoors, H., Marschark, M., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2018; pp. 217–235. [Google Scholar]
  133. Hassanzadeh, S. Outcomes of cochlear implantation in deaf children of deaf parents: Comparative study. J. Laryngol. Otol. 2012, 126, 989–994. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  134. Jiménez, M.S.; Pino, M.J.; Herruzo, J. A comparative study of speech development between deaf children with cochlear implants who have been educated with spoken or spoken+ sign language. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 2009, 73, 109–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  135. Hoffmeister, R.J.; Henner, J.; Caldwell-Harris, C.L.; Novogrodsky, R. Deaf children’s ASL vocabulary and ASL syntax knowledge supports English knowledge. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2022, 27, 37–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  136. Petitto, L.A.; Holowka, S. Evaluating attributions of delay and confusion in young bilinguals: Special insights from infants acquiring a signed and a spoken language. Sign Lang. Stud. 2002, 3, 4–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Petitto, L.A.; Langdon, C.; Stone, A.; Andriola, D.; Kartheiser, G.; Cochran, C. Visual sign phonology: Insights into human reading and language from a natural soundless phonology. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2016, 7, 366–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  138. Davidson, K.; Lillo-Martin, D.; Chen Pichler, D. Spoken English language development among native signing children with cochlear implants. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2014, 19, 238–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  139. Krausneker, V. Language use and awareness of deaf and hearing children in a bilingual setting. In Sign Bilingualism: Language Development, Interaction and Maintenance in Sign Language Contact Situations; Plaza-Pust, C., Morales-Lopez, E., Eds.; John Benjamins Publishing: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2008; pp. 195–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  140. Watkins, S.; Pittman, P.; Walden, B. The deaf mentor experimental project for young children who are deaf and their families. Am. Ann. Deaf 1998, 143, 29–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  141. Rinaldi, P.; Caselli, M.C. Language development in a bimodal bilingual child with cochlear implant: A longitudinal study. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2014, 17, 798–809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  142. Harris, M.; Chasin, J. Visual attention in deaf and hearing infants: The role of auditory cues. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2005, 46, 1116–1123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  143. De Vos, C.; Hilbrink, E.; Alvarez van Tussenbroek, I.; van Zuilen, M.; Levinson, S.C. Modality-Specific Patterns in the Development of Joint Attention in Infants Acquiring Sign Language Natively. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Sign Language Acquisition (ICSLA), Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1–3 July 2015; Available online: https://pure.mpg.de/pubman/faces/ViewItemOverviewPage.jsp?itemId=item_2168494 (accessed on 9 March 2020).
  144. Bergeson, T.R.; Pisoni, D.B.; Davis, R.A.O. Development of audiovisual comprehension skills in prelingually deaf children with cochlear implants. Ear Hear. 2005, 26, 149–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  145. Van Berkel-van Hoof, L.; Hermans, D.; Knoors, H.E.T.; Verhoeven, L. Effects of signs on word learning by children with developmental language disorder. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2019, 62, 1798–1812. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  146. Humphries, T.; Kushalnagar, P.; Mathur, G.; Napoli, D.J.; Padden, C.; Rathmann, C.; Smith, C. Discourses of prejudice in the professions: The case of sign languages. J. Med. Ethics 2017, 43, 648–652. Available online: http://jme.bmj.com/cgi/content/full/medethics-2015-103242 (accessed on 1 August 2022). [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  147. Geers, A.E.; Mitchell, C.M.; Warner-Czyz, A.; Wang, N.Y.; Eisenberg, L.S. CDaCI Investigative Team. Early sign language exposure and cochlear implantation benefits. Pediatrics 2017, 140, e20163489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  148. Rinaldi, P.; Pavani, F.; Caselli, M.C. Developmental, cognitive, and neurocognitive perspectives on language development in children who use cochlear implants. In The Oxford Handbook of Deaf Studies in Learning and Cognition; Marschark, M., Knoors, H., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2020; pp. 33–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  149. Johnson, R.E. Unlocking the Curriculum: Principles for Achieving Access in Deaf Education; Working Paper 89-3; Scientific Communications Program, Gallaudet Research Institute: Washington, DC, USA, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  150. Morini, G.; Newman, R.S. Dónde está la ball? Examining the effect of code switching on bilingual children’s word recognition. J. Child Lang. 2019, 46, 1238–1248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  151. Byers-Heinlein, K.; Jardak, A.; Fourakis, E.; Lew-Williams, C. Effects of language mixing on bilingual children’s word learning. Biling. 2022, 25, 55–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  152. Yow, W.Q.; Tan, J.S.H.; Flynn, S. Code-switching as a marker of linguistic competence in bilingual children. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2018, 21, 1075–1090. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  153. Easterbrooks, S.R.; Trussell, J.W. Encouraging emergent reading in deaf and hard-of-hearing children. In The Oxford Handbook of Deaf Studies in Language; Marscharck, M., Spencer, P.E., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2016; pp. 377–392. [Google Scholar]
  154. Malzkuhn, M.; Herzig, M. Bilingual storybook app designed for deaf children based on research principles. In Proceedings of the 12th International Conference on Interaction Design and Children, New York, NY, USA, 24–27 June 2013; ACM Digital Library at TCD Trinity College Dublin: Dublin, Ireland, 2013; pp. 499–502. [Google Scholar]
  155. Mirus, G.; Napoli, D.J. Developing language and (pre)literacy skills in deaf preschoolers through shared reading activities with bimodal-bilingual eBooks. J. Multiling. Educ. Res. 2018, 8, 10. [Google Scholar]
  156. Caldwell-Harris, C.L.; Hoffmeister, R.J. Learning a second language via print: On the logical necessity of a fluent first language. Front. Commun. 2022, 161, 900399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  157. Dirks, E.; Wauters, L. It takes two to read: Interactive reading with young deaf and hard-of-hearing children. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2018, 23, 261–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  158. Holcomb, L.; Wolbers, K. Effects of ASL rhyme and rhythm on deaf children’s engagement behavior and accuracy in recitation: Evidence from a single case design. Children 2020, 7, 256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  159. Holcomb, L.; Golos, D.; Moses, A.; Broadrick, A. Enriching Deaf Children’s American Sign Language Phonological Awareness: A Quasi-Experimental Study. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2022, 27, 26–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  160. Kushalnagar, P.; Topolski, T.D.; Schick, B.; Edwards, T.C.; Skalicky, A.M.; Patrick, D.L. Mode of communication, perceived level of understanding, and perceived quality of life in youth who are deaf or hard of hearing. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2011, 16, 512–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  161. Van Gent, T.; Goedhart, A.W.; Knoors, H.E.T.; Westenberg, P.M.; Treffers, P.D.A. Self-concept and ego development in deaf adolescents: A comparative study. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2012, 17, 333–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  162. Leeson, L.; Van den Bogaerde, B. (What we don’t know about) sign languages in higher education in Europe: Mapping policy and practice to an analytical framework. Sociolinguistica 2020, 34, 31–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  163. Van Herreweghe, M.; De Meulder, M.; Vermeerbergen, M. From erasure to recognition (and Back Again?): The case of Flemish Sign Language. In The Oxford Handbook of Deaf Studies in Language; Marscharck, M., Spencer, P.E., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2016; pp. 45–61. [Google Scholar]
  164. Matthijs, L.; Hardonk, S.; Sermijn, J.; Van Puyvelde, M.; Leigh, G.; Van Herreweghe, M.; Loots, G. Mothers of deaf children in the 21st Century. Dynamic positioning between the medical and cultural–linguistic discourses. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2017, 22, 365–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  165. Knoors, H.; Marschark, M. Language planning for the 21st century: Revisiting bilingual language policy for deaf children. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2012, 17, 291–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  166. Hintermair, M. Parental resources, parental stress, and socioemotional development of deaf and hard of hearing children. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2006, 11, 493–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  167. Prakash, S.S.; Prakash, S.G.R.; Ravichandran, A.; Susan, K.Y.; Alex, W. Measuring levels of stress and depression in mothers of children using hearing aids and cochlear implants: A comparative study. Int. J. Spec. Educ. 2013, 28, 37–44. [Google Scholar]
  168. Punch, R.; Hyde, M. Rehabilitation efforts and stress in parents of children with cochlear implants. Aust. N. Z. J. Audiol. 2010, 32, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  169. Chaudhury, S. Anxiety and depression in mothers of deaf children: Awareness needed. Med. J. Dr. DY Patil Univ. 2014, 7, 720. [Google Scholar]
  170. Alfano, A.R. Communication between Spanish-speaking mothers and their children with hearing loss who use ASL. Commun. Disord. Q. 2019, 2, 88–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  171. McKee, R.; Vale, M. Parents of Deaf and Hearing Impaired Children: Survey Report. Victoria University of Wellington, Deaf Studies Research Unit. 2014. Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Rachel-Mckee-2/publication/274835873_NZSL_Vitality_Parent_Survey_Report_March_2014/links/552adbd90cf29b22c9c18c31/NZSL-Vitality-Parent-Survey-Report-March-2014.pdf (accessed on 1 August 2022).
  172. Gallaudet Research Institute. Regional and National Summary; Gallaudet Research Institute: Washington, DC, USA, 2014; Available online: https://www.gallaudet.edu/documents/Research-Support-and-International-Affairs/Intl%20Affairs/Demographics/AS14_RegNat.pdf (accessed on 1 August 2022).
  173. Singleton, J.L.; Newport, E.L. When learners surpass their models: The acquisition of American Sign Language from inconsistent input. Cogn. Psychol. 2004, 49, 370–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  174. Humphries, T.; Kushalnagar, P.; Mathur, G.; Napoli, D.J.; Padden, C.; Rathmann, C. Ensuring language acquisition for deaf children: What linguists can do. Language 2014, 90, e31–e52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  175. Koulidobrova, E.; Kuntze, M.; Dostal, H.M. If you use ASL, should you study ESL? Limitations of a modality-b(i)ased policy. Language 2018, 94, 99–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  176. Weaver, K.A.; Starner, T. We need to communicate! helping hearing parents of deaf children learn American Sign Language. In Proceedings of the 13th International ACM SIGACCESS Conference on Computers and Accessibility, Dundee, Scotland, 24–26 October 2011; Alcover, B.M., Jaume-i-Capó, A., Varona, J., Martinez-Bueso, P., Chiong, A.M., Eds.; AMC Digital Library: Dublin, Ireland, 2011; pp. 91–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  177. Lillo-Martin, D.C.; Gale, E.; Chen Pichler, D. Family ASL: An early start to equitable education for deaf children. Top. Early Child. Spec. Educ. 2021, 11, 02711214211031307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  178. Chen Pichler, D. Constructing a profile of successful L2 signer hearing parents of deaf children. Senri Ethnol. Stud. 2021, 107, 115–131. [Google Scholar]
  179. Petersen, A.; Kinoglu, S.; Gozali-Lee, E.; MartinRogers, N. Lifetrack’s Deaf Mentor Family Program: An Evaluation of the Experiences and Outcomes for Participating Families; Wilder Research: Saint Paul, MN, USA, 2016; Available online: https://www.wilder.org/sites/default/files/imports/LifetrackDeafMentorProgram_8-16.pdf (accessed on 1 August 2022).
  180. Gale, E.; Berke, M.; Benedict, B.; Olson, S.; Putz, K.; Yoshinaga-Itano, C. Deaf adults in early intervention programs. Deaf. Educ. Int. 2019, 23, 3–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  181. Gale, E.; Berke, M.; Olson, S.; Benedict, B. Deaf Leadership [Video File]. Presented by the Family-Centered Early Intervention 2018 Deaf Leadership Committee, Produced by Barmherzige Brüder Linz: Linz, Austria, 2018, July 25. Available online: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=fMxL4eYAMxY&feature=youtu.be (accessed on 1 August 2022).
  182. Pittman, P.; Benedict, B.; Olson, S.; Sass-Lehrer, M. Collaboration with deaf and hard-of-hearing communities. In Early Intervention for Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Infants, Toddlers, and Their Families: Interdisciplinary Perspectives; Sass-Lehrer, M., Ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2016; pp. 135–166. [Google Scholar]
  183. Gale, E. Collaborating with deaf adults in early intervention. Young Except. Child. 2021, 24, 225–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  184. Napier, J.; Leigh, G.; Nann, S. Teaching sign language to hearing parents of deaf children: An action research process. Deaf. Educ. Int. 2007, 9, 83–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  185. Hauser, P.C. Deaf scientists need deaf mentors. In Proceedings of the 6th International Deaf Academics and 33 Researchers Conference, Lisbon, Portugal, 18–20 July 2013. [Google Scholar]
  186. Listman, J.; Rogers, K.D.; Hauser, P.C. Community cultural wealth and deaf adolescents’ resilience. In Resilience in Deaf Children: Adaptation through Emerging Adulthood; Zand, D.H., Pierce, K.J., Eds.; Springer Science: Berlin, Germany, 2011; pp. 279–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  187. Clark, M.; Daggett, D. Exploring the presence of a deaf American cultural life script. Deaf. Educ. Int. 2015, 17, 194–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  188. Humphries, T. The modern deaf self: Indigenous practices and educational imperatives. In Literacy and Deaf People: Cultural and Contextual Perspectives; Brueggemann, B.J., Ed.; Gallaudet University Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2004; pp. 29–46. [Google Scholar]
  189. Lieberman, A.M.; Hatrak, M.; Mayberry, R.I. The development of eye gaze control for linguistic input in deaf children. In BUCLD 35: Proceedings of the 35th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development; Cristia, A., Seidl, A., Eds.; Cascadilla: Somerville, MA, USA, 2011; pp. 391–403. [Google Scholar]
  190. Lieberman, A.M.; Hatrak, M.; Mayberry, R.I. Learning to look for language: Development of joint attention in young deaf children. Lang. Learn. Dev. 2014, 10, 19–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  191. Corina, D.; Singleton, J. Developmental social cognitive neuroscience: Insights from deafness. Child Dev. 2009, 80, 952–967. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  192. Leigh, I.W.; Andrews, J.F.; Harris, R. Deaf Culture: Exploring Deaf Communities in the United States; Plural Publishing: San Diego, CA, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  193. Wille, B.; Van Lierde, K.; Van Herreweghe, M. Parental strategies used in communication with their deaf infants. Child Lang. Teach. Ther. 2019, 35, 165–183. [Google Scholar]
  194. Rogers, K.; Young, A. Being a deaf role model: Deaf people’s experiences of working with families and deaf young people. Deaf. Educ. Int. 2011, 13, 2–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  195. Crezee, I.H.M.; Roat, C.E. Bilingual patient navigator or healthcare interpreter: What’s the difference and why does it matter? Cogent. Med. 2019, 6, 181087776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  196. Jacobi, M. Mentoring and undergraduate academic success: A literature review. Rev. Educ. Res. 1991, 61, 505–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  197. Rhodes, J.; DuBois, D. Mentoring relationships and programs for youth. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2008, 17, 254–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  198. Rhodes, J.; Spencer, R.; Keller, T.; Liang, B.; Noam, G. A model for the influence of mentoring relationships on youth development. J. Community Psychol. 2006, 34, 691–707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  199. Goldberg, S.S. Special Education Law: A Guide for Parents, Advocates, and Educators; Springer Science & Business Media: Berlin, Germany, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  200. Green, V.A. Parental reaction to diagnosis of infant hearing loss. Adv. Ment. Health 2020, 18, 62–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  201. Moeller, M.P.; Carr, G.; Seaver, L.; Stredler-Brown, A.; Holzinger, D. Best practices in family-centered early intervention for children who are deaf or hard of hearing: An international consensus statement. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2013, 18, 429–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  202. Hamilton, B. The Deaf Mentor Program: Benefits to Families and Professionals. Ph.D. Thesis, Lamar University, Beaumont, TX, USA, 2017. Unpublished Dissertation. [Google Scholar]
  203. Kita, S. (Ed.) Pointing: Where Language, Culture, and Cognition Meet; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  204. Liszkowski, U. Before L1: A differentiated perspective on infant gestures. Gesture 2008, 8, 180–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  205. Crais, E.; Douglas, D.D.; Campbell, C.C. The intersection of the development of gestures and intentionality. J. Speech. Lang. Hear. Res. 2004, 47, 678–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  206. Capone, N.; McGregor, K.K. Gesture development: A review for clinical and research practices. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2004, 47, 173–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  207. Levinson, S.C.; Holler, J. The origin of human multi-modal communication. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 2014, 369, 20130302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  208. Grosjean, F. Bilingual: Life and Reality; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  209. Grosjean, F. The Mysteries of Bilingualism: Unresolved Issues; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  210. Rhodes, J.; Grossman, J.; Resch, N. Agents of change: Pathways through which mentoring relationships influence adolescents’ academic adjustment. Child Dev. 2000, 71, 1662–1671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  211. Hamilton, B.; Clark, M.D. The deaf mentor program: Benefits to families. Psychology 2020, 11, 713–736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  212. Pittman, P.; SKI-HI Institute (Utah State University); HOPE, Inc. The Deaf Mentor Curriculum: A Resource Manual for Home-Based, Bilingual-Bicultural Programming for Young Children Who are Deaf; SKI-HI Institute, Utah State University: Logan, UT, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  213. Henner, J.; Hoffmeister, R.J.; Fish, S.; Rosenburg, P.; DiDonna, D. Bilingual education works even for deaf children of hearing parents. In Proceedings of the 2015 Conference of the American Educational Research Association, Chicago, IL, USA, 16–20 April 2015. [Google Scholar]
  214. Grosjean, F. The bilingualism and biculturalism of the deaf. In Studying Bilinguals; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2008; pp. 221–237. [Google Scholar]
  215. Hoffmeister, R.J.; Caldwell-Harris, C.L. Acquiring English as a second language via print: The task for deaf children. Cognition 2014, 132, 229–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  216. Howerton-Fox, A.; Falk, J.L. Deaf children as ‘English learners’: The psycholinguistic turn in deaf education. Educ. Sci. 2019, 9, 133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  217. Razalli, A.R.; Anal, A.; Mamat, N.; Hashim, T. Effects of bilingual approach in Malay language teaching for hearing impaired students. Int. J. Acad. Res. Progress. Educ. Dev. 2018, 7, 109–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  218. Oliva, G.A.; Lytel, L. Turning the Tide: Making Life Better for Deaf and Hard of Hearing School Children; Gallaudet University Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  219. Swanwick, R. Dialogic teaching and translanguaging in deaf education. In Evidence-Based Practices in Deaf Education; Knoors, H., Marschark, M., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2018; pp. 81–110. [Google Scholar]
  220. Nussbaum, D.; Waddy-Smith, B.; Doyle, J. Students who are deaf and hard of hearing and use sign language: Considerations and strategies for developing spoken language and literacy skills. In Seminars in Speech and Language; Thieme Medical Publishers: Stuttgart, Germany, 2012; Volume 33, Number 4; pp. 310–321. [Google Scholar]
  221. Easterbrooks, S.R.; Dostal, H.M. (Eds.) The Oxford Handbook of Deaf Studies in Literacy; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  222. Allen, S. English and ASL: Classroom activities to shed some light on the use of two languages. In Signs and Voices: Deaf Culture, Identity, Language, and Arts; Lindgren, K.A., DeLuca, D., Napoli, D.J., Eds.; Gallaudet University Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2008; pp. 139–149. [Google Scholar]
  223. Marschark, M.; Lang, H.G.; Albertini, J.A. Educating Deaf Students: From Research to Practice; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  224. Marschark, M.; Peterson, R.; Winston, E.A. (Eds.) Sign Language: Interpreting and Interpreter Education, Directions for Research and Practice; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2005. [Google Scholar]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Humphries, T.; Mathur, G.; Napoli, D.J.; Padden, C.; Rathmann, C. Deaf Children Need Rich Language Input from the Start: Support in Advising Parents. Children 2022, 9, 1609. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9111609

AMA Style

Humphries T, Mathur G, Napoli DJ, Padden C, Rathmann C. Deaf Children Need Rich Language Input from the Start: Support in Advising Parents. Children. 2022; 9(11):1609. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9111609

Chicago/Turabian Style

Humphries, Tom, Gaurav Mathur, Donna Jo Napoli, Carol Padden, and Christian Rathmann. 2022. "Deaf Children Need Rich Language Input from the Start: Support in Advising Parents" Children 9, no. 11: 1609. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9111609

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop