Sympathy-Empathy and the Radicalization of Young People
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Relationship between the Sympathy/Empathy System and Radicalization in Young People
2.1. The Sympathy/Empathy System
2.2. Sympathy/Empathy and Sensitivity to Propaganda
2.3. Empathy/Sympathy and Radical Group Identification
- (1)
- Developmental immaturity of the SE system with regard to the development of top-down regulation abilities. Several studies suggest that adolescents’ neural response patterns may differ from adults’ patterns in situations that evoke cognitive or emotional empathy [72]. The development of neural circuits underlying empathy from childhood (7 years) to adulthood (40 years) through fMRI starts with emotional empathy that appears earlier than cognitive empathy, but this development also shows a gradual shift from a visceral response to pain as a potential threat to a more detached and regulated appraisal of the stimulus [73]. Neuroimagery studies have shown the following: stronger automatic responses in adolescents who witness another in a painful situation [74]; a negative relationship between empathetic accuracy and brain activation (this relationship is compatible with adolescents becoming immersed in their own emotions while sharing the emotional experience of the target) [75]; and compensatory hyperactivation of emotionally related brain areas to compensate for adolescents’ lower emotional empathy ability [76].
- (2)
- SOD is weaker in adolescents due to fragility in self-consciousness and awareness [25,77]. In addition, individual vulnerabilities may also increase this trend. As the development of self-consciousness is closely linked with self-emotional development, SOD has been shown to be disturbed in some psychopathologies such as borderline personality disorder [78].
- (3)
- Collusion between adolescence and radicalization. Emotions and issues elicited by radicalization echo normal adolescent issues (such as guilt, shame, sexuality, and the need for rupture and change), just as the offering of radicalization resonates with adolescents’ usual coping mechanisms (such as projective identification, polarized attribution of values, intellectualization, and ascetism) [58,79], thus increasing self-other merging.
- (4)
- Adaptation of propaganda to target. We must acknowledge that recruiters have shown acute abilities in the cognitive aspects of empathy; these abilities are supported by recruiters’ ability to propose a wide range of propaganda messages [31] and then to subsequently adapt the message to each specific target [37]. This is not neutral for these young people; the perception of being the target of a perspective-taker have been shown to lead to an important self-other merging [80] as well as a soothing feeling of being understood [81]. This relief of no longer being alone to face uncertainty seems to play an important role in the early stages of the radicalization of young people [79].
2.4. Motivational Aspects of SE and Radical Commitment
2.4.1. Altruism
2.4.2. Group Belongingness
2.4.3. Self-Regulatory Emotional Control
2.4.4. Perceived Injustice or Unfairness
3. Consistency between Empathy and Radical Beliefs and Behaviors and the Type of Empathy Possibly Involved in Radicalization
3.1. Is Empathy Consistent with Radical Views, Beliefs and Behaviors?
3.2. Disentangling Affective and Cognitive Empathy
4. Intermediary Summary
5. Radicalization and Empathy in an Intergroup Context
5.1. In-Group vs. Out-Group
5.2. Empathy and Dehumanization
6. Discussion
6.1. The Contradiction of Empathy
6.2. The Contradiction of Radicalization among Adolescents
7. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Campelo, N.; Oppetit, A.; Neau, F.; Cohen, D.; Bronsard, G. Who are the European youths willing to engage in radicalisation? A multidisciplinary review of their psychological and social profiles. Eur. Psychiatry J. Assoc. Eur. Psychiatr. 2018, 52, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Allemand, M.; Steiger, A.E.; Fend, H.A. Empathy development in adolescence predicts social competencies in adulthood. J. Pers. 2015, 83, 229–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van der Graaff, J.; Carlo, G.; Crocetti, E.; Koot, H.M.; Branje, S. Prosocial Behavior in Adolescence: Gender Differences in Development and Links with Empathy. J. Youth. Adolesc. 2018, 47, 1086–1099. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Van Dongen, J.D.M. The Empathic Brain of Psychopaths: From Social Science to Neuroscience in Empathy. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Milone, A.; Cerniglia, L.; Cristofani, C.; Inguaggiato, E.; Levantini, V.; Masi, G.; Paciello, M.; Simone, F.; Muratori, P. Empathy in Youths with Conduct Disorder and Callous-Unemotional Traits. Neural. Plast. 2019, 2019, 9638973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bruneau, E. The Marionettes’s Threads: How the Tugs of Empathy and Dehumanization Can Lead to Intergroup Conflict. In The Brains That Pull the Triggers; Fried, I., Berthoz, A., Mirdal, M., Eds.; Odile Jacob: Paris, France; New York, NY, USA, 2021; pp. 336–351. [Google Scholar]
- Merari, A.; Diamant, I.; Bibi, A.; Broshi, Y.; Zakin, G. Personality Characteristics of “Self Martyrs”/“Suicide Bombers” and Organizers of Suicide Attacks. Terror. Polit. Violence 2009, 22, 87–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pedahzur, A.; Perliger, A.; Weinberg, L. Altruism and Fatalism: The Characteristics of Palestinian Suicide Terrorists. Deviant. Behav. 2003, 24, 405–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fried, I. Syndrome E. Lancet 1997, 350, 1845–1847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Decety, J. Why Empathy Is Not a Reliable Source of Information in Moral Decision Making. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2021, 30, 425–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Botbol, M.; Garret-Gloanec, N.; Besse, A. L’empathie: Au Carrefour des Sciences et de la Clinique. Doin: Montrogue, France, 2014; p. 332. [Google Scholar]
- Decety, J.; Jackson, P.L. The functional architecture of human empathy. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 2004, 3, 71–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Decety, J. The neural pathways, development and functions of empathy. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2015, 3, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Waal, F.B.M. The “Russian doll” model of empathy and imitation. In On Being Moved: From Mirror Neurons to Empathy; John Benjamins Publishing: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2007; pp. 49–69. [Google Scholar]
- Tooley, U.A.; Bassett, D.S.; Mackey, A.P. Environmental influences on the pace of brain development. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2021, 22, 372–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Preston, S. A perception-action model for empathy. Empathy Ment. Illn. 2007, 1, 428–447. [Google Scholar]
- Gallese, V. The roots of empathy: The shared manifold hypothesis and the neural basis of intersubjectivity. Psychopathology 2003, 36, 171–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gazzola, V.; Aziz-Zadeh, L.; Keysers, C. Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans. Curr. Biol. 2006, 16, 1824–1829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lamm, C.; Bukowski, H.; Silani, G. From shared to distinct self-other representations in empathy: Evidence from neurotypical function and socio-cognitive disorders. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2016, 371, 20150083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jackson, P.; Brunet, E.; Meltzoff, A.; Decety, J. Empathy examined through the neural mechanisms involved in imagining how I feel versus how you feel pain. Neuropsychologia 2006, 44, 752–761. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Berthoz, A.; Thirioux, B. A Spatial and Perspective Change Theory of the Difference Between Sympathy and Empathy. Paragrana 2010, 19, 32–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lambrey, S.; Doeller, C.; Berthoz, A.; Burgess, N. Imagining being somewhere else: Neural basis of changing perspective in space. Cereb. Cortex 2012, 22, 166–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Thirioux, B.; Mercier, M.R.; Blanke, O.; Berthoz, A. The cognitive and neural time course of empathy and sympathy: An electrical neuroimaging study on self-other interaction. Neuroscience 2014, 267, 286–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berthoz, A. Physiologie du changement de point de vue. L’Empathie. Odile Jacob 2004, 251–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steinbeis, N. The role of self-other distinction in understanding others’ mental and emotional states: Neurocognitive mechanisms in children and adults. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2016, 371, 20150074. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bischof-Köhler, D. Self object and interpersonal emotions. Identification of own mirror image, empathy and prosocial behavior in the 2nd year of life. Z Psychol. Z Angew Psychol. 1994, 202, 349–377. [Google Scholar]
- Håkansson Eklund, J.; Summer Meranius, M. Toward a consensus on the nature of empathy: A review of reviews. Patient. Educ. Couns. 2021, 104, 300–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Oppetit, A.; Campelo, N.; Bouzar, L.; Pellerin, H.; Hefez, S.; Bronsard, G.; Bouzar, D.; Cohen, D. Do Radicalized Minors Have Different Social and Psychological Profiles From Radicalized Adults? Front. Psychiatry 2019, 10, 644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hassan, G.; Brouillette-Alarie, S.; Séraphin, A.; Frau-Meigs, D.; Lavoie, L.; Fetiu, A.; Varela, W.; Borokhovski, E.; Venkatesh, V.; Rousseau, S.; et al. Exposure to Extremist Online Content Could Lead to Violent Radicalization: A Systematic Review of Empirical Evidence. Int. J. Dev. Sci. 2018, 12, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sikkens, E.; van San, M.; Sieckelinck, S.; Boeije, H.; de Winter, M. Participant Recruitment through Social Media: Lessons Learned from a Qualitative Radicalization Study Using Facebook. Field. Methods 2017, 29, 130–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yoder, K.J.; Ruby, K.; Pape, R.; Decety, J. EEG distinguishes heroic narratives in ISIS online video propaganda. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 19593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tamborini, R.; Stiff, J.; Heidel, C. Reacting to Graphic Horror: A Model of Empathy and Emotional Behavior. Commun. Res. 1990, 17, 616–640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoffner, C.A.; Levine, K.J. Enjoyment of Mediated Fright and Violence: A Meta-Analysis. In Mass Media Effects Research: Advances Through Meta-Analysis; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2007; pp. 215–244. [Google Scholar]
- Bagozzi, R.P.; Moore, D.J. Public Service Advertisements: Emotions and Empathy Guide Prosocial Behavior. J. Mark. 1994, 58, 56–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zillmann, D. Mechanisms of emotional involvement with drama. Poetics 1995, 23, 33–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holbrook, D.; Taylor, M. Terrorism as Process Narratives: A Study of Pre-Arrest Media Usage and the Emergence of Pathways to Engagement. Terror. Polit. Violence 2019, 31, 1307–1326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bouzar, D.; Martin, M. Pour quels motifs les jeunes s’engagent-ils dans le djihad? Neuropsychiatr. Enfance. Adolesc. 2016, 64, 353–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pemberton, A.; Aarten, P.G.M. Narrative in the Study of Victimological Processes in Terrorism and Political Violence: An Initial Exploration. Stud. Confl. Terror. 2018, 41, 541–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Botbol, M.; Campelo, N.; Lacour-Gonay, C.; Roche-Rabreau, D.; Teboul, R.; Chambry, J.; Michel, D. Psychiatrie et Radicalisation—Rapport du groupe de travail de la Fédération Française de Psychiatrie. 2020. Available online: https://fedepsychiatrie.fr/wp-content/uploads/2020/08/FFP-Rapport-Psychiatrie-et-Radicalisation-Janvier-2020-V3.pdf (accessed on 15 September 2022).
- Khosrokhavar, F. Radicalisation. Éditions de la Maison des Sciences de L’homme: Paris, France, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Mar, R.A.; Oatley, K.; Djikic, M.; Mullin, J. Emotion and narrative fiction: Interactive influences before, during, and after reading. Cogn. Emot. 2011, 25, 818–833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Braddock, K. The utility of narratives for promoting radicalization: The case of the Animal Liberation Front. Dyn. Asymmetric. Confl. 2014, 8, 38–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mahood, S.; Rane, H. Islamist narratives in ISIS recruitment propaganda. J. Int. Commun. 2017, 23, 15–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lazarus, R. Emotion and Adaptation; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Holland, A.C.; O’Connell, G.; Dziobek, I. Facial mimicry, empathy, and emotion recognition: A meta-analysis of correlations. Cogn. Emot. 2021, 35, 150–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wicker, B.; Keysers, C.; Plailly, J.; Royet, J.P.; Gallese, V.; Rizzolatti, G. Both of us disgusted in My insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron 2003, 40, 655–664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Oaten, M.; Stevenson, R.J.; Williams, M.A.; Rich, A.N.; Butko, M.; Case, T.I. Moral Violations and the Experience of Disgust and Anger. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bhikram, T.; Abi-Jaoude, E.; Sandor, P. OCD: Obsessive-compulsive disgust? The role of disgust in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. JPN 2017, 42, 300–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Harris, L.T.; Fiske, S.T. Social groups that elicit disgust are differentially processed in mPFC. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2007, 2, 45–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hare, T.A.; Tottenham, N.; Galvan, A.; Voss, H.U.; Glover, G.H.; Casey, B.J. Biological substrates of emotional reactivity and regulation in adolescence during an emotional go-nogo task. Biol. Psychiatry 2008, 63, 927–934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Van Stekelenburg, J. Radicalization and Violent Emotions. PS Polit. Sci. Polit. 2017, 50, 936–939. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barsade, S. The Ripple Effect: Emotional Contagion and Its Influence on Group Behavior. Adm. Sci. Q. 2002, 47, 644–675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lazerus, T.; Ingbretsen, Z.A.; Stolier, R.M.; Freeman, J.B.; Cikara, M. Positivity bias in judging ingroup members’ emotional expressions. Emotion 2016, 16, 1117–1125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Weisbuch, M.; Ambady, N. Affective divergence: Automatic responses to others’ emotions depend on group membership. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 95, 1063–1079. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Lebreton, M.; Kawa, S.; Forgeot d’Arc, B.; Daunizeau, J.; Pessiglione, M. Your goal is mine: Unraveling mimetic desires in the human brain. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 2012, 32, 7146–7157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pessiglione, M. The brain Valuation System That Pulls the trigger. In The Brains That Pull the Triggers; Fried, I., Berthoz, A., Mirdal, M., Eds.; Odile Jacob: Paris, France; New York, NY, USA, 2020; pp. 313–330. [Google Scholar]
- Cheng, Y.; Lin, C.P.; Liu, H.L.; Hsu, Y.Y.; Lim, K.E.; Hung, D.; Decety, J. Expertise Modulates the Perception of Pain in Others. Curr. Biol. 2007, 17, 1708–1713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Benslama, F. The subjective impact of the jihadist offer. Interdiscip. J. Relig. Transform. Contemp. Soc. 2016, 2, 75–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Busselle, R.; Bilandzic, H. Fictionality and Perceived Realism in Experiencing Stories: A Model of Narrative Comprehension and Engagement. Commun. Theory 2008, 18, 255–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Graaf, A.; Hoeken, H.; Sanders, J.; Beentjes, J. Identification as a Mechanism of Narrative Persuasion. Commun Res. 2011, 29, 39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Argo, J.J.; Zhu, R.; Dahl, D.W. Article JD served as editor and BS served as associate editor for this. Fact or Fiction: An Investigation of Empathy Differences in Response to Emotional Melodramatic Entertainment. J. Consum. Res. 2008, 34, 614–623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taylor, L. Investigating Fans of Fictional Texts: Fan Identity Salience, Empathy, and Transportation. Psychol. Pop. Media Cult. 2014, 1, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schuurman, B.; Horgan, J. Rationales for terrorist violence in homegrown jihadist groups: A case study from The Netherlands. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2016, 27, 55–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bazex, H.; Mensat, J.Y. Qui sont les djihadistes français? Analyse de 12 cas pour contribuer à l’élaboration de profils et à l’évaluation du risque de passage à l’acte. Ann. Méd.-Psychol. Rev. Psychiatr. 2016, 174, 257–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Prebble, S.C.; Addis, D.R.; Tippett, L.J. Autobiographical memory and sense of self. Psychol. Bull. 2013, 139, 815–840. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hogg, M.A.; Kruglanski, A.; van den Bos, K. Uncertainty and the Roots of Extremism. J. Soc. Issues 2013, 69, 407–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ludot, M.; Radjack, R.; Moro, M.R. Radicalisation djihadiste et psychiatrie de l’adolescent. Neuropsychiatry Enfance. Adolesc. 2016, 64, 522–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davis, M.H.; Conklin, L.; Smith, A.; Luce, C. Effect of perspective taking on the cognitive representation of persons: A merging of self and other. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1996, 70, 713–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Galinsky, A.D.; Moskowitz, G.B. Perspective-taking: Decreasing stereotype expression, stereotype accessibility, and in-group favoritism. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2000, 78, 708–724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sheikh, H.; Gómez, Á.; Atran, S. Empirical evidence for the devoted actor model. Curr. Anthropol. 2016, 57, S13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Swann, W.B.; Jetten, J.; Gómez, Á.; Whitehouse, H.; Bastian, B. When group membership gets personal: A theory of identity fusion. Psychol. Rev. 2012, 119, 441–456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Khanjani, Z.; Mosanezhad jeddi, E.; Hekmati, I.; Khalilzade, S.; Etemadinia, M.; Andalib, M. Comparison of Cognitive Empathy, Emotional Empathy, and Social Functioning in Different Age Groups. Aust. Psychol. 2015, 50, 80–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Decety, J.; Michalska, K. Neurodevelopmental changes in the circuits underlying empathy and sympathy from childhood to adulthood. Dev. Sci. 2010, 13, 886–899. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mella, N.; Studer, J.; Gilet, A.L.; Labouvie-Vief, G. Empathy for Pain from Adolescence through Adulthood: An Event-Related Brain Potential Study. Front. Psychol. 2012, 3, 501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kral, T.R.A.; Solis, E.; Mumford, J.A.; Schuyler, B.S.; Flook, L.; Rifken, K.; Patsenko, E.G.; Davidson, R.J. Neural correlates of empathic accuracy in adolescence. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2017, 12, 1701–1710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kim, E.J.; Son, J.W.; Park, S.K.; Chung, S.; Ghim, H.R.; Lee, S.; Shin, C.; Kim, S.; Ju, G. Cognitive and Emotional Empathy in Young Adolescents: An fMRI Study. Soa--Chongsonyon Chongsin Uihak. J. Child. Adolesc. Psychiatry 2020, 31, 121–130. [Google Scholar]
- Sebastian, C.; Burnett, S.; Blakemore, S.J. Development of the self-concept during adolescence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2008, 12, 441–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baptista, A.; Cohen, D.; Jacquet, P.O.; Chambon, V. The Cognitive, Ecological, and Developmental Origins of Self-Disturbance in Borderline Personality Disorder. Front. Psychiatry 2021, 12, 707091. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rolling, J.; Corduan, G. La radicalisation, un nouveau symptôme adolescent? Neuropsychiatry Enfance. Adolesc. 2017, 66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldstein, N.J.; Vezich, I.S.; Shapiro, J.R. Perceived perspective taking: When others walk in our shoes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2014, 106, 941–960. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Rogers, C.R. Empathic: An unappreciated way of being. Couns. Psychol. 1975, 5, 2–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Karagiannis, E. European Converts to Islam: Mechanisms of Radicalization. Polit. Relig. Ideol. 2012, 13, 99–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Galinsky, A.; Ku, G.; Wang, C.S. Perspective-Taking and Self-Other Overlap: Fostering Social Bonds and Facilitating Social Coordination. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 2005, 8, 109–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Vescio, T.K.; Sechrist, G.B.; Paolucci, M.P. Perspective taking and prejudice reduction: The mediational role of empathy arousal and situational attributions. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 33, 455–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Finlay, K.; Stephan, W. Improving Intergroup Relations: The Effects of Empathy on Racial Attitudes. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2006, 30, 1720–1737. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Batson, C.; Polycarpou, M.; Harmon-Jones, E.; Imhoff, H.; Mitchener, E.; Bednar, L.; Klein, T.; Highberger, L. Empathy and Attitudes: Can Feeling for a Member of a Stigmatized Group Improve Feelings Toward the Group? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1997, 72, 105–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Galinsky, A.D.; Wang, C.S.; Ku, G. Perspective-takers behave more stereotypically. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 95, 404–419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, L.G.E.; Blackwood, L.; Thomas, E.F. The Need to Refocus on the Group as the Site of Radicalization. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 15, 327–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferguson, N.; McAuley, J.W. Dedicated to the cause: Identity development and violent extremism. Eur. Psychol. 2021, 26, 6–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davidov, M.; Zahn-Waxler, C.; Roth-Hanania, R.; Knafo, A. Concern for Others in the First Year of Life: Theory, Evidence, and Avenues for Research. Child Dev. Perspect. 2013, 7, 126–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Khosrokhavar, F. Les trajectoires des jeunes jihadistes français. Etudes 2015, 6, 33–44. [Google Scholar]
- Campelo, N.; Bouzar, L.; Oppetit, A.; Pellerin, H.; Hefez, S.; Bronsard, G.; Cohen, D.; Bouzar, D. Joining the Islamic State from France between 2014 and 2016: An observational follow-up study. Palgrave Commun. 2018, 4, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paraschakis, A.; Michopoulos, I.; Douzenis, A. Profile of Islamic suicide bombers: A literature review. Eur. Psychiatry 2016, 33, 754. [Google Scholar]
- Batson, C.D.; Batson, J.G.; Slingsby, J.K.; Harrell, K.L.; Peekna, H.M.; Todd, R.M. Empathic joy and the empathy-altruism hypothesis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1991, 61, 413–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Batson, C.D.; Sager, K.; Garst, E.; Kang, M.; Rubchinsky, K.; Dawson, K. Is empathy-induced helping due to self–other merging? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1997, 73, 495–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Singer, T.; Seymour, B.; O’Doherty, J.P.; Stephan, K.E.; Dolan, R.J.; Frith, C.D. Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature 2006, 439, 466–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Decety, J.; Echols, S.; Correll, J. The blame game: The effect of responsibility and social stigma on empathy for pain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2010, 22, 985–997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Doosje, B.; Loseman, A.; van den Bos, K. Determinants of Radicalization of Islamic Youth in the Netherlands: Personal Uncertainty, Perceived Injustice, and Perceived Group Threat. J. Soc. Issues 2013, 69, 586–604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kruglanski, A.W.; Gelfand, M.J.; Bélanger, J.J.; Sheveland, A.; Hetiarachchi, M.; Gunaratna, R. The psychology of radicalization and deradicalization: How significance quest impacts violent extremism. Polit. Psychol. 2014, 35, 69–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- FeldmanHall, O.; Dalgleish, T.; Evans, D.; Mobbs, D. Empathic concern drives costly altruism. NeuroImage 2015, 105, 347–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Barchi-Ferreira, A.; Osório, F. Associations between oxytocin and empathy in humans: A systematic literature review. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2021, 129, 105268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Decety, J.; Svetlova, M. Putting together phylogenetic and ontogenetic perspectives on empathy. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2012, 2, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Decety, J.; Pape, R.; Workman, C.I. A multilevel social neuroscience perspective on radicalization and terrorism. Soc. Neurosci. 2018, 13, 511–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Decety, J.; Lamm, C. Empathy versus personal distress: Recent evidence from social neuroscience. In The Social Neuroscience of Empathy; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Singer, T.; Lamm, C. The social neuroscience of empathy. Ann. Acad. Sci. 2009, 1156, 81–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Batson, C.D.; Fultz, J.; Schoenrade, P.A. Distress and empathy: Two qualitatively distinct vicarious emotions with different motivational consequences. J. Pers. 1987, 55, 19–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eisenberg, N.; Fabes, R.A.; Miller, P.A.; Fultz, J.; Shell, R.; Mathy, R.M.; Reno, R.R. Relation of sympathy and personal distress to prosocial behavior: A multimethod study. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1989, 57, 55–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Eisenberg, N.; Fabes, R.A.; Schaller, M.; Miller, P.A. Sympathy and personal distress: Development, gender differences, and interrelations of indexes. New Dir. Child Dev. 1989, 44, 107–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fabi, S.; Weber, L.; Leuthold, H. Empathic concern and personal distress depend on situational but not dispositional factors. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0225102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Valdivia-Salas, S.; Martín-Albo, J.; Cruz, A.; Villanueva-Blasco, V.J.; Jiménez, T.I. Psychological Flexibility with Prejudices Increases Empathy and Decreases Distress Among Adolescents: A Spanish Validation of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire–Stigma. Front. Psychol. 2021, 11, 3911. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cristofori, I.; Zhong, W.; Cohen-Zimerman, S.; Bulbulia, J.; Gordon, B.; Krueger, F.; Grafman, J. Brain networks involved in the influence of religion on empathy in male Vietnam War veterans. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 11047. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Milgram, S. Behavioral Study of obedience. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 1963, 67, 371–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cheetham, M.; Pedroni, A.F.; Antley, A.; Slater, M.; Jäncke, L. Virtual milgram: Empathic concern or personal distress? Evidence from functional MRI and dispositional measures. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2009, 3, 29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Van den Bos, K. Unfairness and Radicalization. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2020, 71, 563–588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Charkawi, W.; Dunn, K.; Bliuc, A.M. The influences of social identity and perceptions of injustice on support to violent extremism. Behav. Sci. Terror. Polit. Aggress. 2021, 13, 177–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leuzinger-Bohleber, M. From Free Speech to IS—Pathological Regression of Some Traumatized Adolescents from a Migrant Background in Germany. Int. J. Appl. Psychoanal. Stud. 2016, 13, 213–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmitt, M.; Baumert, A.; Gollwitzer, M.; Maes, J. The Justice Sensitivity Inventory: Factorial Validity, Location in the Personality Facet Space, Demographic Pattern, and Normative Data. Soc. Justice Res. 2010, 23, 211–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baumert, A.; Schmitt, M. Justice Sensitivity. In Handbook of Social Justice and Research; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Decety, J.; Yoder, K.J. Empathy and motivation for justice: Cognitive empathy and concern, but not emotional empathy, predict sensitivity to injustice for others. Soc. Neurosci. 2016, 11, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Wijn, R.; Bos, K. Toward a better understanding of the justice judgment process: The influence of fair and unfair events on state justice sensitivity. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2010, 40, 1294–1301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Simi, P.; Sporer, K.; Bubolz, B.F. Narratives of Childhood Adversity and Adolescent Misconduct as Precursors to Violent Extremism: A Life-Course Criminological Approach. J. Res. Crime Delinq. 2016, 53, 536–563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Koehler, D. Violent extremism, mental health and substance abuse among adolescents: Towards a trauma psychological perspective on violent radicalization and deradicalization. J. Forensic Psychiatry Psychol. 2020, 31, 455–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greenberg, D.M.; Baron-Cohen, S.; Rosenberg, N.; Fonagy, P.; Rentfrow, P.J. Elevated empathy in adults following childhood trauma. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0203886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Lim, D.; DeSteno, D. Suffering and compassion: The links among adverse life experiences, empathy, compassion, and prosocial behavior. Emotion 2016, 16, 175–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yoder, K.J.; Decety, J. The Good, the Bad, and the Just: Justice Sensitivity Predicts Neural Response during Moral Evaluation of Actions Performed by Others. J. Neurosci. 2014, 34, 4161–4166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Altemeyer, B.; Hunsberger, B.E. Authoritarianism, religious fundamentalism, quest, and prejudice. Int. J. Psychol. Relig. 1992, 2, 113–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roy, O. Le Djihad et la mort; Seuil: Paris, France, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Verkuyten, M. Religious Fundamentalism and Radicalization Among Muslim Minority Youth in Europe. Eur. Psychol. 2018, 23, 21–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCauley, C.; Moskalenko, S. Mechanisms of Political Radicalization: Pathways Toward Terrorism. Terror. Polit. Violence 2008, 20, 415–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zmigrod, L.; Rentfrow, P.; Robbins, T. Cognitive Inflexibility Predicts Extremist Attitudes. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 989. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Piaget, J. The moral judgment of the child. Harcourt, Brace: Oxford, UK, 1932. [Google Scholar]
- Kohlberg, L. Moral stages and moralization. A cognitive developmental approach. J. Study Educ. Dev. 1982, 5, 33–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kloo, D.; Perner, J.; Aichhorn, M.; Schmidhuber, N. Perspective taking and cognitive flexibility in the Dimensional Change Card Sorting (DCCS) task. Cogn. Dev. 2010, 25, 208–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hirai, M.; Muramatsu, Y.; Nakamura, M. Role of the Embodied Cognition Process in Perspective-Taking Ability during Childhood. Child Dev. 2020, 91, 214–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gauthier, S.; Anzalone, S.M.; Cohen, D.; Zaoui, M.; Chetouani, M.; Villa, F.; Berthoz, A.; Xavier, J. Behavioral Own-Body-Transformations in Children and Adolescents With Typical Development, Autism Spectrum Disorder, and Developmental Coordination Disorder. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meins, E.; Fernyhough, C.; Arnott, B.; Leekam, S.R.; de Rosnay, M. Mind-mindedness and theory of mind: Mediating roles of language and perspectival symbolic play. Child Dev. 2013, 84, 1777–1790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Sikkens, E.; van San, M.; Sieckelinck, S.; de Winter, M. Parents’ Perspectives on Radicalization: A Qualitative Study. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2018, 27, 2276–2284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- San, M.; Sieckelinck, S.; Winter, M. Ideals adrift: An educational approach to radicalization. Ethics Educ. 2013, 8, 276–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bradley, C. The Interconnections Between Religious Fundamentalism, Spirituality and the Four Dimensions of Empathy. Sociol. Fac. Publ. 2009, 51. [Google Scholar]
- Friedman, J.P.; Jack, A.I. What Makes You So Sure? Dogmatism, Fundamentalism, Analytic Thinking, Perspective Taking and Moral Concern in the Religious and Nonreligious. J. Relig. Health. 2018, 57, 157–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jack, A.I.; Friedman, J.P.; Boyatzis, R.E.; Taylor, S.N. Why Do You Believe in God? Relationships between Religious Belief, Analytic Thinking, Mentalizing and Moral Concern. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0149989. [Google Scholar]
- Pretus, C.; Hamid, N.; Sheikh, H.; Ginges, J.; Tobeña, A.; Davis, R.; Villarroya, O.; Atran, S. Neural and Behavioral Correlates of Sacred Values and Vulnerability to Violent Extremism. Front. Psychol. 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, Y.; Li, W.; Zhang, T.; Zhang, J.; Jin, Z.; Li, L. Probing the role of the right inferior frontal gyrus during Pain-Related empathy processing: Evidence from fMRI and TMS. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2020, 42, 1518–1531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gervais, W.M. Perceiving Minds and Gods: How Mind Perception Enables, Constrains, and Is Triggered by Belief in Gods. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. J. Assoc. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 8, 380–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Schjoedt, U.; Stødkilde-Jørgensen, H.; Geertz, A.W.; Roepstorff, A. Highly religious participants recruit areas of social cognition in personal prayer. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2009, 4, 199–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vonk, J.; Pitzen, J. Believing in other minds: Accurate mentalizing does not predict religiosity. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2017, 115, 70–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lillard, A.S.; Kavanaugh, R.D. The contribution of symbolic skills to the development of an explicit theory of mind. Child Dev. 2014, 85, 1535–1551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Happé, F.G.E. An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1994, 24, 129–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Speranza, M.; Corcos, M.; Guilbaud, O.; Loas, G.; Jeammet, P. Alexithymia, Personality, and Psychopathology. Am. J. Psychiatry 2005, 162, 1029–1030. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watson, P.; Hood, R.; Morris, R. Dimensions of Religiosity and Empathy. J. Psychol. Christ. 1985, 4, 73–85. [Google Scholar]
- Saroglou, V. Religion and the five factors of personality: A meta-analytic review. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2002, 32, 15–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Batson, C.; Gray, R.A. Religious orientation and helping behavior: Responding to one’s own or the victim’s needs? J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1981, 40, 511–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Łowicki, P.; Zajenkowski, M.; Cappellen, P.V. It’s the heart that matters: The relationships among cognitive mentalizing ability, emotional empathy, and religiosity. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2020, 161, 109976. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Francis, L.J.; Croft, J.S.; Pyke, A. Religious diversity, empathy, and God images: Perspectives from the psychology of religion shaping a study among adolescents in the UK. In Religion, Education and Society; Routledge: London, UK, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Shamay-Tsoory, S.; Aharon-Peretz, J.; Perry, D. Two Systems for Empathy: A Double Dissociation Between Emotional and Cognitive Empathy in Inferior Frontal Gyrus Versus Ventromedial Prefrontal Lesions. Brain J. Neurol. 2008, 132, 617–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pino, M.C.; De Berardis, D.; Mariano, M.; Vellante, F.; Serroni, N.; Valchera, A.; Valenti, M.; Mazza, M. Two systems for empathy in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Mentalizing and experience sharing. Rev. Bras. Psiquiatr. 2016, 38, 307–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Kerr-Gaffney, J.; Harrison, A.; Tchanturia, K. Cognitive and Affective Empathy in Eating Disorders: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Front. Psychiatry 2019, 10, 102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hein, G.; Singer, T. I feel how you feel but not always: The empathic brain and its modulation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2008, 18, 153–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tajfel, H.; Turner, J.C. An integrative theory of inter-group conflict. In The Social Psychology of Inter-Group Relations; Brooks/Cole: Monterey, CA, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Fourie, M.; Subramoney, S.; Gobodo-Madikizela, P. A Less Attractive Feature of Empathy: Intergroup Empathy Bias. In Empathy: An Evidencebased Interdisciplinary Perspective; INTECH: London, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Avenanti, A.; Sirigu, A.; Aglioti, S.M. Racial bias reduces empathic sensorimotor resonance with other-race pain. Curr. Biol. 2010, 20, 1018–1022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Gutsell, J.; Inzlicht, M. Intergroup differences in the sharing of emotive states: Neural evidence of an empathy gap. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2011, 7, 596–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Olenski, A.R.; Zimerman, A.; Coussens, S.; Jena, A.B. Behavioral Heuristics in Coronary-Artery Bypass Graft Surgery. N. Engl. J. Med. 2020, 382, 778–779. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hein, G.; Silani, G.; Preuschoff, K.; Batson, C.D.; Singer, T. Neural responses to ingroup and outgroup members’ suffering predict individual differences in costly helping. Neuron 2010, 68, 149–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tajfel, H.; Billig, M.G.; Bundy, R.P.; Flament, C. Social categorization and intergroup behaviour. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 1971, 1, 149–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Bavel, J.J.; Packer, D.J.; Cunningham, W.A. The Neural Substrates of In-Group Bias: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Investigation. Psychol. Sci. 2008, 19, 1131–1139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vaughn, D.; Savjani, R.; Cohen, M.; Eagleman, D. Empathic Neural Responses Predict Group Allegiance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Amodio, D.M. The neuroscience of prejudice and stereotyping. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2014, 15, 670–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cikara, M.; Van Bavel, J. The Neuroscience of Intergroup Relations: An Integrative Review. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 9, 245–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Doosje, B.; Moghaddam, F.M.; Kruglanski, A.W.; de Wolf, A.; Mann, L.; Feddes, A.R. Terrorism, radicalization and de-radicalization. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2016, 11, 79–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moghaddam, F.M. The staircase to terrorism: A psychological exploration. Am. Psychol. 2005, 60, 161–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montalan, B.; Lelard, T.; Godefroy, O.; Mouras, H. Behavioral Investigation of the Influence of Social Categorization on Empathy for Pain: A Minimal Group Paradigm Study. Front. Psychol. 2012, 3, 389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cikara, M.; Fiske, S.T. Their pain, our pleasure: Stereotype content and schadenfreude. Ann. Acad. Sci. 2013, 1299, 52–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cikara, M.; Botvinick, M.M.; Fiske, S.T. Us Versus Them: Social Identity Shapes Neural Responses to Intergroup Competition and Harm. Psychol. Sci. 2011, 22, 306–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cikara, M.; Jenkins, A.C.; Dufour, N.; Saxe, R. Reduced self-referential neural response during intergroup competition predicts competitor harm. NeuroImage 2014, 96, 36–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Trawalter, S.; Hoffman, K.M.; Waytz, A. Racial bias in perceptions of others’ pain. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e48546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fiske, S.; Cuddy, A.J.C.; Glick, P. Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2007, 11, 77–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vorauer, J.D.; Sucharyna, T.A. Potential negative effects of perspective-taking efforts in the context of close relationships: Increased bias and reduced satisfaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 104, 70–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vorauer, J.D.; Martens, V.; Sasaki, S.J. When trying to understand detracts from trying to behave: Effects of perspective taking in intergroup interaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 96, 811–827. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Skorinko, J.L.; Sinclair, S.A. Perspective taking can increase stereotyping: The role of apparent stereotype confirmation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 49, 10–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sun, S.; Zuo, B.; Wu, Y.; Wen, F. Does perspective taking increase or decrease stereotyping? The role of need for cognitive closure. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2016, 94, 21–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harris, L.T.; Fiske, S.T. Dehumanizing the lowest of the low: Neuroimaging responses to extreme out-groups. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 847–853. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leyens, J.P.; Rodríguez, A.; Rodriguez-Torres, R.; Gaunt, R.; Paladino, M.; Vaes, J.; Demoulin, S. Psychological Essentialism and the Differential Attribution of Uniquely Human Emotions to Ingroups and Outgroups. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2001, 31, 395–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fiske, S. How Ordinary People Become Violent: Dehumanizing Out-Groups. In The Brains That Pull the Triggers; Fried, I., Berthoz, A., Mirdal, M., Eds.; Odile Jacob: Paris, France; New York, NY, USA, 2021; pp. 146–159. [Google Scholar]
- Cialdini, R.B.; Schaller, M.; Houlihan, D.; Arps, K.; Fultz, J.; Beaman, A.L. Empathy-based helping: Is it selflessly or selfishly motivated? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1987, 52, 749–758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perry, S.; Hasisi, B. Rational Choice Rewards and the Jihadist Suicide Bomber. Terror Polit Violence. 2015, 27, 53–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zagury Zagury, D. Du deuil de soi à l’idéal en apothéose. In Idéal et Cruauté; Editions lignes: Paris, France, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Todorov, A.; Harris, L.T.; Fiske, S. Toward socially inspired social neuroscience. Brain Res. 2006, 1079, 76–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bruneau, E.; Cikara, M.; Saxe, R. Parochial Empathy Predicts Reduced Altruism and the Endorsement of Passive Harm. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2017, 8, 934–942. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bruneau, E.; Saxe, R. The Power of Being Heard: The Benefits of “Perspective-Giving” in the Context of Intergroup Conflict. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 48, 855–866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Feddes, A.; Mann, L.; Doosje, B. Increasing self-esteem and empathy to prevent violent radicalization: A longitudinal quantitative evaluation of a resilience training focused on adolescents with a dual identity. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2015, 45, 400–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Misiak, B.; Samochowiec, J.; Bhui, K.; Schouler-Ocak, M.; Demunter, H.; Kuey, L.; Raballo, A.; Gorwood, P.; Frydecka, D.; Dom, G. A systematic review on the relationship between mental health, radicalization and mass violence. Eur. Psychiatry 2019, 56, 51–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Coid, J.; Bhui, K.; Macmanus, D.; Kallis, C.; Bebbington, P.; Ullrich, S. Extremism, religion and psychiatric morbidity in a population-based sample of young men. Br. J. Psychiatry 2016, 209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Botbol, M. Clinical Approach to Difficult Adolescents: Implication for Syndrome E. In The Brains That Pull the Triggers; Fried, I., Berthoz, A., Mirdal, M., Eds.; Odile Jacob: Paris, France; New York, NY, USA, 2021; pp. 277–292. [Google Scholar]
- Bronsard, G.; Cohen, D.; Diallo, I.; Pellerin, H.; Varnoux, A.; Podlipski, M.A.; Gerardin, P.; Boyer, L.; Campelo, N. Adolescents Engaged in Radicalisation and Terrorism: A Dimensional and Categorical Assessment. Front. Psychiatry 2022, 12, 774063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berthoz, A.; Thirioux, B. Empathy, Sympathy, Empathy Breakdown and Dehumanization: New Phenomenological and Neurobiological H. In The Brains That Pull The Triggers; Fried, I., Berthoz, A., Mirdal, M., Eds.; Odile Jacob: Paris, France; New York, NY, USA, 2021; pp. 168–180. [Google Scholar]
- Koechlin, E. Neural Cognitive Foundations of Rule Compliance in Humans. In The Brains That Pull The Triggers; Fried, I., Berthoz, A., Mirdal, M., Eds.; Odile Jacob: Paris, France; New York, NY, USA, 2021; pp. 257–276. [Google Scholar]
- Ernst, M.; Pine, D.S.; Hardin, M. Triadic model of the neurobiology of motivated behavior in adolescence. Psychol. Med. 2006, 36, 299–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wai, M.; Tiliopoulos, N. The affective and cognitive empathic nature of the dark triad of personality. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2012, 52, 794–799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paulhus, D.L.; Williams, K.M. The Dark Triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. J. Res. Pers. 2002, 36, 556–563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Czarna, A.Z.; Wróbel, M.; Dufner, M.; Zeigler-Hill, V. Narcissism and Emotional Contagion: Do Narcissists “Catch” the Emotions of Others? Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2015, 6, 318–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dinsdale, N.; Crespi, B.J. The borderline empathy paradox: Evidence and conceptual models for empathic enhancements in borderline personality disorder. J. Pers. Disord. 2013, 27, 172–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Lavenne-Collot, N.; Dissaux, N.; Campelo, N.; Villalon, C.; Bronsard, G.; Botbol, M.; Cohen, D. Sympathy-Empathy and the Radicalization of Young People. Children 2022, 9, 1889. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9121889
Lavenne-Collot N, Dissaux N, Campelo N, Villalon C, Bronsard G, Botbol M, Cohen D. Sympathy-Empathy and the Radicalization of Young People. Children. 2022; 9(12):1889. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9121889
Chicago/Turabian StyleLavenne-Collot, Nathalie, Nolwenn Dissaux, Nicolas Campelo, Charlotte Villalon, Guillaume Bronsard, Michel Botbol, and David Cohen. 2022. "Sympathy-Empathy and the Radicalization of Young People" Children 9, no. 12: 1889. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9121889
APA StyleLavenne-Collot, N., Dissaux, N., Campelo, N., Villalon, C., Bronsard, G., Botbol, M., & Cohen, D. (2022). Sympathy-Empathy and the Radicalization of Young People. Children, 9(12), 1889. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9121889