Associations between Fundamental Movement Skills, Physical Fitness, Motor Competency, Physical Activity, and Executive Functions in Pre-School Age Children: A Systematic Review
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
- Search strategy
- Eligibility criteria
- Research type
- Area of interest
- Population
- Language
- Selection of studies and data extraction
- Assessment of methodological quality
- Data Extraction and Synthesis
- Strong evidence: at least three high-quality studies (quality score of 76% and above) with consistent results, where >66% find a significant association in the same direction and no more than 25% find an opposing association.
- Weak evidence: at least 3 average or below average quality studies with consistently positive results for or against the relationship of motor and cognitive skills.
- Insufficient evidence: fewer than 3 studies of whatever quality or below average or inadequate quality studies with inconsistent results for an association between motor and cognitive skills.
- No evidence: when there was only one study showing an association between motor and cognitive skills.
3. Results
Reference (Author, Year, Country) | Study Design | Sample (Size (n), Age, % Girls) | Motor Assessment | Executive Function Assessment | Outcomes |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
[57], Italy | RCT | 110 (47 girls, 63 boys), 5.23 ± 0.45 years 64 (30 girls, 34 boys), 5.18 ± 0.45 years | EYMSC | IPDA | Regression: Motor task a significant predictor of behavior subscale [F(1,165) = 8.61, p < 0.01, η2 p = 0.05], motor activity subscale [F(1,172) = 8.54, p < 0.01, η2 p = 0.05], linguistic comprehension subscale, [F(1,165) = 11.13, p < 0.01, η2 p = 0.06], oral expression subscale, [F(1,165) = 7.98, p = 0.01, η2 p = 0.05], metacognition subscale, [F(1,165) = 14.37, p < 0.01, η2 p = 0.08 |
[63], Estonia | Longitudinal | 147 children (51% boys), 6.6 and 7.6 years | Actigraph PREFIT EUROFIT | Boehm-3 | Regression: LPA (p = 0.017), MPA (p = 0.006), VPA (p = 0.011), MVPA (p = 0.007), and TPA (p = 0.012) at 6.6 years were associated with higher conceptual skills at 7.6 years. No significant associations between PA and verbal or perceptual ability. |
[45], Russia | Cohort | 261 (boys n = 130, girls n = 131), 5.77 ± 0.32 years | Broad jump; sit and reach test; shuttle run 4 × 5 m | DCCS | Regression: Inhibitory control (p = 0.002) and working memory (visual p = 0.02 and verbal p = 0. 03)) positively linked with physical fitness. Cognitive flexibility (p = 0.12) was not associated with PF. |
[19], Switzerland | Longitudinal | 134 children (68 girls and 66 boys), 6.42 ± 0.32 years | MABC-2 KTK | Flanker task; Backward color recall task; Heidelberger Rechentest | Correlation: PF predicts academic achievement (AA) indirectly through EFs. Significant association between PF and EFs r = 0.71 p < 0.05. |
[40], China | RCT | 57 (31 boys and 26 girls), 4.40 ± 0.29 years | Actigraph | FIS WMS GNG SCA | Regression: No significant influence on inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility with PA p > 0.05. |
[47], Switzerland | Cohort | 555 (52.8% boys), 3.9 ± 0.7 years | Actigraph ZNA 3-5 | IDS-P NEPSY | Regression: PA had no effect on EFs. Fine motor skills with EFs (β = 0.17) |
[61], Switzerland | Cohort | 156 (51% girls), 6.5 ± 4 years | MABC-2 KKT | Flanker task Backward color recall task | Correlation: Both gross motor skills (r = 0.75) and fine motor skills (r = 0.67) correlated significantly with executive functions. |
[39], Germany | RCT | 101 (IG—48, 6 ± 0.43 years, 50% boys; CG—53, 6 ± 0.6 years, 45% boys) | MABC-2 | Simon-says task; computer-based hearts-and-flowers task | Correlation: Effect of acute coordinative exercise is temporally limited and emerges only for motor inhibition but not for cognitive inhibition or shifting. |
[59], Canada | Cross-sectional | 95 (69.5% boys), 4.5 ± 0.7 years | Actigraph TGMD-2 | EYT | Regression: Movement behavior compositions were significantly associated with working memory (p = 0.01) and vocabulary (p = 0.00). |
[58], South Africa | Cross-sectional | 129 (64 urban children (47% girls), mean age 4.4 years; 65 rural children (59% girls), mean age 4.9 years | TGMD-2 | EYT | Regression: Inhibition (locomotor, p = 0.047 and object control skills, p = 0.02) and working memory (p = 0.039), but not shifting, were associated with gross motor skills. PA was not associated with inhibition and shifting but was negatively associated with working memory. |
[43], Australia | RCT | n = 111 (64 boys; Mage = 4.94 ± 0.56 years); integrated condition (n = 31), nonintegrated condition (n = 23), gesturing condition (n = 31), conventional condition (n = 26) | Actigraph | Free-Recall and Cued Recall Tests | Regression: Children in the integrated physical exercise condition achieved the highest learning outcomes. |
[60], Italy | Cross-sectional | 65 children (32 boys 4.86 ± 1.04 years, and 33 girls 5.13 ± 0.89 years) | MABC-2 | CMP, PRT; Quaiser, the Corsi block tapping test, the digit span test | Correlation: A high correlation between two working memory tests and both mental rotation and balance was shown. |
[42], Switzerland | Cross-sectional | 124 children (54% girls) aged 5 to 6 years (M = 5.9 ± 0.48) | MABC-2 KTK | OpenSesame Flanker task DCCS | Correlation: The findings demonstrate that the challenges and demands inherent in any motor task influence the magnitude of the motor–EFs link. That is, difficult (i.e., less automated) motor tasks require EFs more substantially than easy (i.e., more automated) motor tasks. |
[62], Mexico | Cohort | 148 (56.76% boys) age 21.5 ± 3.7 | Peabody Motor Scale | McCarthy Scales | Regression: Early motor performance contributes to the establishment of cognitive abilities at 5 years. |
[64], Belgium | Longitudinal | 54 (age kindergarten 5.98 ± 0.26; age first grade 6.95 ± 0.26 | Pedometer | The Dutch version of the Automated Working Memory Assessment; Flanker Task, Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment | Regression: Performance on a measure of the visuospatial sketchpad, the central executive, and fluency was predicted by children’s amount of daily PA after controlling for initial task performance. |
- Methodological Quality of Studies
- Assessment of FMS/MC, PA, and Executive function
- Assessment of constructs from the motor domain
- -
- -
- -
- -
- Executive function assessment
- Relationship between FMS and executive functions
- Relationship between MC and executive functions
- Relationship between PA and executive functions
- Relationship between PF and executive functions
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Dwyer, T.; Sallis, J.F.; Blizzard, L.; Lazarus, R.; Dean, K. Relation of Academic Performance to Physical Activity and Fitness in Children. Pediatric Exerc. Sci. 2001, 13, 225–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ludyga, S.; Koutsandréou, F.; Reuter, E.M.; Voelcker-Rehage, C.; Budde, H. A Randomized Controlled Trial on the Effects of Aerobic and Coordinative Training on Neural Correlates of Inhibitory Control in Children. J. Clin. Med. 2019, 8, 184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schmidt, R.A. Motor Learning & Performance. In From Principles to Practice; Human Kinetics Books: Champaign, IL, USA, 1991; p. 310. [Google Scholar]
- Fleishman, E.A. The Structure and Measurement of Physical Fitness; Englewood Cliffs, N.J., Ed.; Prentice-Hall: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1964. [Google Scholar]
- Newell, K. Constraints on the Development of Coordination. In Motor Development in Children: Aspects of Coordination and Control; Martinus Nijhoff: Leiden, The Netherlands, 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Sugden, D.; Sugden, L. The assessment of movement skill problems in 7- and 9-year-old children. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 1991, 61, 329–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fleishman, E.A. On the relation between abilities, learning, and human performance. Am. Psychol. 1972, 27, 1017–1032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Seashore, R.H. Individual differences in motor skills. J. Gen. Psychol. 1930, 3, 38–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siscovick, D.S.; Laporte, R.E.; Newman, J.H.; Iverson, D.C.; Fielding, J.E. Physical activity, exercise, and physical fitness: Definitions and distinctions for health-related research. Public Health Rep. 1985, 100, 126. [Google Scholar]
- Ortega, F.B.; Ruiz, J.R.; Castillo, M.J.; Sjöström, M. Physical fitness in childhood and adolescence: A powerful marker of health. Int. J. Obes. 2008, 32, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ulrich, D.A.; Sanford, C.B. Test of Gross Motor Development; Examiner’s Manual: Austin, TX, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Luz, C.; Rodrigues, L.P.; Almeida, G.; Cordovil, R. Development and validation of a model of motor competence in children and adolescents. J. Sci. Med. Sport 2016, 19, 568–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stodden, D.F.; Goodway, J.D.; Langendorfer, S.J.; Roberton, M.A.; Rudisill, M.E.; Garcia, C.; Garcia, L.E. A Developmental Perspective on the Role of Motor Skill Competence in Physical Activity: An Emergent Relationship. Quest 2008, 60, 290–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dumith, S.C.; Hallal, P.C.; Reis, R.S.; Kohl, H.W. Worldwide prevalence of physical inactivity and its association with human development index in 76 countries. Prev. Med. 2011, 53, 24–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rarick, G.L.; George, L.; Adams, F.H. Physical Activity; Human Growth and Development; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1973; p. 406. [Google Scholar]
- Robinson, L.E.; Stodden, D.F.; Barnett, L.M.; Lopes, V.P.; Logan, S.W.; Rodrigues, L.P.; D’Hondt, E. Motor Competence and its Effect on Positive Developmental Trajectories of Health. Sports Med. 2015, 45, 1273–1284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Silva, C.; Annamalai, K. Entropy Generation and Human Aging: Lifespan Entropy and Effect of Physical Activity Level. Entropy 2008, 10, 100–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lezak, M.D.; Howieson, D.B.; Loring, D.W.; Fischer, J.S. Neuropsychological Asessment, 4th ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, MI, USA, 2004; p. 1016. [Google Scholar]
- Oberer, N.; Gashaj, V.; Roebers, C.M. Executive functions, visual-motor coordination, physical fitness and academic achievement: Longitudinal relations in typically developing children. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2018, 58, 69–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diamond, A. Executive Functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P.; Emerson, M.J.; Witzki, A.H.; Howerter, A.; Wager, T.D. The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 2000, 41, 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Clarke, A.D.B.; Clarke, A.M.; Reiman, S. Cognitive And Social Changes In The Feebleminded—Three Further Studies. Br. J. Psychol. 1958, 49, 144–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piaget, J. The Child’s Conception of the World; Routledge & K. Paul: London, UK, 1929; p. 379. [Google Scholar]
- Berndt, T.J.; Perry, T.B. Child Development; McGraw-Hill Higher Education Co.: Irvine, CA, USA, 1996; p. 272. [Google Scholar]
- Robertson, S.S.; Johnson, S.L.; Masnick, A.M.; Weiss, S.L. Robust coupling of body movement and gaze in young infants. Dev. Psychobiol. 2007, 49, 208–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Adolph, K.E. Learning to Move. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2008, 17, 213–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Best, J.R. Effects of Physical Activity on Children’s Executive Function: Contributions of Experimental Research on Aerobic Exercise. Dev. Rev. 2010, 30, 331–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lehto, J.E.; Juujärvi, P.; Kooistra, L.; Pulkkinen, L. Dimensions of executive functioning: Evidence from children. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2003, 21, 59–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leisman, G.; Moustafa, A.A.; Shafir, T. Thinking, Walking, Talking: Integratory Motor and Cognitive Brain Function. Front. Public Health 2016, 4, 49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Ludyga, S.; Gerber, M.; Brand, S.; Pühse, U.; Colledge, F. Effects of Aerobic Exercise on Cognitive Performance Among Young Adults in a Higher Education Setting. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 2018, 89, 164–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hillman, C.H.; Erickson, K.I.; Kramer, A.F. Be smart, exercise your heart: Exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008, 9, 58–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Howie, E.K.; Pate, R.R. Physical activity and academic achievement in children: A historical perspective. J. Sport Health Sci. 2012, 1, 160–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Chaddock-Heyman, L.; Hillman, C.H.; Cohen, N.J.; Kramer, A.F., III. The importance of physical activity and aerobic fitness for cognitive control and memory in children. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child. Dev. 2014, 79, 25–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Whooten, R.; Kerem, L.; Stanley, T. Physical Activity in Adolescents and Children and Relationship to Metabolic Health. Curr. Opin. Endocrinol. Diabetes Obes. 2019, 26, 25–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Davis, N.M.; Ford, G.W.; Anderson, P.J.; Doyle, L.W. Developmental coordination disorder at 8 years of age in a regional cohort of extremely-low-birthweight or very preterm infants. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2007, 49, 325–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ellemberg, D.; St-Louis-Deschênes, M. The effect of acute physical exercise on cognitive function during development. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2010, 11, 122–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Esmaeilzadeh, S.; Ebadollahzadeh, K. Physical Fitness, Physical Activity and Sedentary Activities of 7 to 11 Year Old Boys with Different Body Mass Indexes. Asian J. Sports Med. 2012, 3, 105–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tomporowski, P.D.; Davis, C.L.; Miller, P.H.; Naglieri, J.A. Exercise and children’s intelligence, cognition, and academic achievement. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2008, 20, 111–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stein, M.; Auerswald, M.; Ebersbach, M. Relationships between Motor and Executive Functions and the Effect of an Acute Coordinative Intervention on Executive Functions in Kindergartners. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Wen, X.; Zhang, Y.; Gao, Z.; Zhao, W.; Jie, J.; Bao, L. Effect of Mini-Trampoline Physical Activity on Executive Functions in Preschool Children. BioMed Res. Int. 2018, 2018, 2712803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Barnett, L.M.; van Beurden, E.; Morgan, P.J.; Brooks, L.O.; Beard, J.R. Childhood motor skill proficiency as a predictor of adolescent physical activity. J. Adolesc. Health. 2009, 44, 252–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Maurer, M.N.; Roebers, C.M. Towards a better understanding of the association between motor skills and executive functions in 5- to 6-year-olds: The impact of motor task difficulty. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2019, 66, 607–620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mavilidi, M.F.; Okely, A.D.; Chandler, P.; Cliff, D.P.; Paas, F. Effects of Integrated Physical Exercises and Gestures on Preschool Children’s Foreign Language Vocabulary Learning. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2015, 27, 413–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mulvey, K.L.; Taunton, S.; Pennell, A.; Brian, A. Head, Toes, Knees, SKIP! Improving Preschool Children’s Executive Function Through a Motor Competence Intervention. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2018, 40, 233–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veraksa, A.; Tvardovskaya, A.; Gavrilova, M.; Yakupova, V.; Musálek, M. Associations Between Executive Functions and Physical Fitness in Preschool Children. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 3282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Völgyi, E.; Rezaie, R. Physical Activity, Body Composition and Resting Cortical Activity in Preschool Children. International. J. Sch. Cogn. Psychol. 2015, 2, 124–129. Available online: https://www.omicsonline.com/open-access/physical-activity-body-composition-and-resting-cortical-activity-in-preschoolchildren-ijscp-1000124.php?aid=55776 (accessed on 15 December 2021).
- Zysset, A.E.; Kakebeeke, T.H.; Messerli-Bürgy, N.; Meyer, A.H.; Stülb, K.; Leeger-Aschmann, C.S.; Schmutz, E.A.; Arhab, A.; Ferrazzini, V.; Kriemler, S.; et al. The validity of parental reports on motor skills performance level in preschool children: A comparison with a standardized motor test. Eur. J. Pediatr. 2018, 177, 715–722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bidzan-Bluma, I.; Lipowska, M. Physical Activity and Cognitive Functioning of Children: A Systematic Review. Int. J. Env. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 800. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carson, V.; Hunter, S.; Kuzik, N.; Wiebe, S.A.; Spence, J.C.; Friedman, A.; Slater, L.; Hinkley, T. Systematic review of physical activity and cognitive development in early childhood. J. Sci. Med. Sport. 2016, 19, 573–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zeng, N.; Ayyub, M.; Sun, H.; Wen, X.; Xiang, P.; Gao, Z. Effects of Physical Activity on Motor Skills and Cognitive Development in Early Childhood: A Systematic Review. BioMed Res. Int. 2017, 2017, 2760716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Singh, A.S.; Saliasi, E.; van den Berg, V.; Uijtdewilligen, L.; de Groot, R.H.M.; Jolles, J.; Andersen, L.B.; Bailey, R.; Chang, Y.-K.; Diamond, A.; et al. Effects of physical activity interventions on cognitive and academic performance in children and adolescents: A novel combination of a systematic review and recommendations from an expert panel. Br. J. Sports Med. 2019, 53, 640–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Van der Fels, I.M.J.; Te Wierike, S.C.M.; Hartman, E.; Elferink-Gemser, M.T.; Smith, J.; Visscher, C. The relationship between motor skills and cognitive skills in 4-16 year old typically developing children: A systematic review. J. Sci. Med. Sport. 2015, 18, 697–703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Page, M.J.; McKenzie, J.E.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021, 29, 89. [Google Scholar]
- Sirriyeh, R.; Lawton, R.; Gardner, P.; Armitage, G. Reviewing studies with diverse designs: The development and evaluation of a new tool. J. Eval. Clin. Pract. 2012, 18, 746–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Croon, E.; Sluiter, J.; Kuijer, P.P.; Frings-Dresen, M. The effect of office concepts on worker health and performance: A systematic review of the literature. Ergonomics 2005, 48, 119–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cook, D.J.; Mulrow, C.D.; Haynes, R.B. Systematic Reviews: Synthesis of Best Evidence for Clinical Decisions. Ann. Intern. Med. 1997, 126, 376–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alesi, M.; Costa, S.; Bianco, A.; Pepi, A. A teacher-led motor programme to enhance pre-literacy and motor skills in kindergarten children. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2021, 18, 367–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cook, C.J.; Howard, S.J.; Scerif, G.; Twine, R.; Kahn, K.; Norris, S.A.; Draper, C.E. Associations of physical activity and gross motor skills with executive function in preschool children from low-income South African settings. Dev. Sci. 2019, 22, e12820. Available online: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30801916/ (accessed on 3 February 2022). [CrossRef]
- Kuzik, N.; Naylor, P.J.; Spence, J.C.; Carson, V. Movement behaviours and physical, cognitive, and social-emotional development in preschool-aged children: Cross-sectional associations using compositional analyses. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0237945. [Google Scholar]
- Lehmann, J.; Quaiser-Pohl, C.; Jansen, P. Correlation of motor skill, mental rotation, and working memory in 3- to 6-year-old children. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2014, 11, 560–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oberer, N.; Gashaj, V.; Roebers, C.M. Motor skills in kindergarten: Internal structure, cognitive correlates and relationships to background variables. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2017, 52, 170–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Osorio-Valencia, E.; Torres-Sánchez, L.; López-Carrillo, L.; Rothenberg, S.J.; Schnaas, L. Early motor development and cognitive abilities among Mexican preschoolers. Child Neuropsychol. A J. Norm. Abnorm. Dev. Child. Adolesc. 2018, 24, 1015–2105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reisberg, K.; Riso, E.M.; Jürimäe, J. Preschool physical activity and fitness predicts conceptual, verbal and perceptual skills at school. J. Sports Sci. 2021, 39, 1988–1995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vandenbroucke, L.; Seghers, J.; Verschueren, K.; Wijtzes, A.I.; Baeyens, D. Longitudinal Associations Between Objectively Measured Physical Activity and Development of Executive Functioning Across the Transition to First Grade. J. Phys. Act. Health 2016, 13, 895–902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Okely, A.D.; Booth, M.L.; Patterson, J.W. Relationship of physical activity to fundamental movement skills among adolescents. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2001, 33, 1899–1904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zelazo, P.D.; Müller, U. Executive Function in Typical and Atypical Development. In The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development, 2nd ed.; Wiley-Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2010; pp. 574–603. ISBN 978-1-4051-9116-6. [Google Scholar]
- Xin, F.; Chen, S.T.; Clark, C.; Hong, J.T.; Liu, Y.; Cai, Y.J. Relationship between Fundamental Movement Skills and Physical Activity in Preschool-aged Children: A Systematic Review. Int. J. Env. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pan, C.Y.; Chu, C.H.; Tsai, C.L.; Sung, M.C.; Huang, C.Y.; Ma, W.Y. The impacts of physical activity intervention on physical and cognitive outcomes in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism 2017, 21, 190–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kamijo, K.; Pontifex, M.B.; O’Leary, K.C.; Scudder, M.R.; Wu, C.T.; Castelli, D.M.; Hillman, C.H. The effects of an afterschool physical activity program on working memory in preadolescent children. Dev. Sci. 2011, 14, 1046–1058. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zach, S.; Shalom, E. The influence of acute physical activity on working memory. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2016, 122, 365–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McVay, J.C.; Kane, M.J. Why does working memory capacity predict variation in reading comprehension? On the influence of mind wandering and executive attention. J. Exp. Psychol. General 2012, 141, 302–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Niemistö, D.; Finni, T.; Cantell, M.; Korhonen, E.; Sääkslahti, A. Individual, Family, and Environmental Correlates of Motor Competence in Young Children: Regression Model Analysis of Data Obtained from Two Motor Tests. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 2548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Himes, J.H. Long-Term Longitudinal Studies and Implications for the Development of an International Growth Reference for Children and Adolescents. Food Nutr Bull. 2006, 27 (Suppl. 5), S199–S211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Study | Criteria | |||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | 13 | 14 | 15 | 16 | Score | % | |
Alesi 2021 | 2 | 3 | 3 | 1 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 1 | 3 | 2 | n/a | 2 | 2 | n/a | 1 | 1 | 31 | 74% |
Reisberg 2021 | 2 | 1 | 3 | 2 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 2 | 2 | 2 | n/a | 3 | 3 | n/a | 1 | 2 | 32 | 76% |
Veraksa 2021 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 2 | 2 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 3 | 2 | n/a | 3 | 3 | n/a | 1 | 0 | 32 | 76% |
Oberer 2018 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 1 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 2 | 2 | 3 | n/a | 3 | 2 | n/a | 1 | 1 | 31 | 74% |
Wen 2018 | 2 | 2 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 2 | 2 | 2 | 3 | n/a | 3 | 3 | n/a | 2 | 2 | 35 | 83% |
Zysset 2018 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 1 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 2 | 3 | 3 | n/a | 3 | 3 | n/a | 2 | 1 | 34 | 81% |
Oberer 2017 | 3 | 2 | 3 | 1 | 2 | 3 | 3 | 2 | 3 | 2 | n/a | 2 | 2 | n/a | 2 | 3 | 33 | 79% |
Stein 2017 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 2 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 3 | n/a | 3 | 3 | n/a | 2 | 2 | 36 | 86% |
Kuzik 2020 | 1 | 1 | 2 | 2 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | n/a | 3 | 3 | n/a | 2 | 3 | 35 | 83% |
Cook 2019 | 2 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 2 | n/a | 3 | 3 | n/a | 1 | 3 | 38 | 90% |
Mavilidi 2015 | 2 | 3 | 3 | 2 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 3 | 1 | 1 | n/a | 2 | 1 | n/a | 1 | 2 | 28 | 67% |
Lehmann 2014 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 1 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 2 | 3 | 2 | n/a | 3 | 2 | n/a | 1 | 0 | 30 | 71% |
Maurer 2019 | 2 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 2 | 3 | 1 | 3 | 1 | 2 | n/a | 2 | 2 | n/a | 1 | 3 | 29 | 69% |
Osorio Valencia 2018 | 2 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 2 | 3 | 1 | 3 | 1 | 2 | n/a | 2 | 2 | n/a | 2 | 0 | 27 | 64% |
Vandenbroucke 2016 | 2 | 2 | 2 | 2 | 3 | 1 | 2 | 2 | 2 | 2 | n/a | 1 | 2 | n/a | 1 | 2 | 26 | 62% |
Motor Skill | Cognitive Skill | No Association | Weak Association | Moderate Association | Strong Association | Evidence |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Fundamental motor skills | Working-memory | [57,58,59] a | Strong (strong correlation) | |||
Inhibition | [58] | No | ||||
Shifting | [58] | No | ||||
Verbal skills | [59] a | No | ||||
Motor competency | Working-memory | [47] b | [42] | [60,61,62] | Strong (moderately strong) | |
Inhibition | [39,47] b | [42] | [61] | Weak (weak moderate | ||
Shifting | [39,47] b | [42] | [61] | Weak (weak moderate) | ||
Verbal skills | [62] | No | ||||
PA | Working-memory | [40,47] | [59] a | [64] | Weak (weak) | |
Inhibition | [40,47,64] b | Weak (weak) | ||||
Shifting | [40] | [64] | Insufficient | |||
Verbal skills | [63] c | [59] b | [43] | Weak (weak) | ||
Perception | [63] c | No | ||||
Physical fitness | Working-memory | [61] | [45] | Insufficient | ||
Inhibition | [19] | [45] | Insufficient | |||
Shifting | [45] | [19] | Insufficient | |||
Verbal skills | [63] c | No |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Malambo, C.; Nová, A.; Clark, C.; Musálek, M. Associations between Fundamental Movement Skills, Physical Fitness, Motor Competency, Physical Activity, and Executive Functions in Pre-School Age Children: A Systematic Review. Children 2022, 9, 1059. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9071059
Malambo C, Nová A, Clark C, Musálek M. Associations between Fundamental Movement Skills, Physical Fitness, Motor Competency, Physical Activity, and Executive Functions in Pre-School Age Children: A Systematic Review. Children. 2022; 9(7):1059. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9071059
Chicago/Turabian StyleMalambo, Chipo, Aneta Nová, Cain Clark, and Martin Musálek. 2022. "Associations between Fundamental Movement Skills, Physical Fitness, Motor Competency, Physical Activity, and Executive Functions in Pre-School Age Children: A Systematic Review" Children 9, no. 7: 1059. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9071059
APA StyleMalambo, C., Nová, A., Clark, C., & Musálek, M. (2022). Associations between Fundamental Movement Skills, Physical Fitness, Motor Competency, Physical Activity, and Executive Functions in Pre-School Age Children: A Systematic Review. Children, 9(7), 1059. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9071059