Biolinguistics: A Scientometric Analysis of Research on (Children’s) Molecular Genetics of Speech and Language (Disorders)
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. The Rise of Biolinguistics
1.2. The Scope of Biolinguistics
- Theoretical linguistics (syntax, morphology, phonology, lexicon, etc.)
- Computational linguistics and parsing
- Mathematical modelling and simulation
- Child language acquisition and multilingualism
- Comparative linguistics (e.g., typology)
- Perceptual studies
- Speech disorders (developmental verbal dyspraxia, language impairment, dyslexia, etc.)
- Cross-species comparative work (nonhuman communication, songbirds, etc.)
- Language change
- Language contact (pidgins, creoles, etc.)
1.3. Scientific Contributions for Biolinguistics
1.4. Molecular Genetics of Speech and Language (Disorders)
1.5. Purpose of the Present Study
2. Methods
2.1. Research Methods
2.2. Measures
2.3. Data-Collection and Sample
2.4. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Result Overview
3.2. Bibliometric Indicators for the Study of Biolinguistics
3.2.1. Overview of Biolinguistics Studies from Scopus, Web of Science, and Lens
3.2.2. Biolinguistics Knowledge Production Size by Year
3.3. Production of Biolinguistics Research by Country and University
3.4. Production of Biolinguistics Research by Journal and Publisher
3.5. Production of Biolinguistics by Research Area, Keywords, and Cooccurrence
3.6. Production of Biolinguistics by Authors
3.7. Scientometric Indicators for the Study of Biolinguistics
3.8. Impact of Research on Biolinguistics by Clusters, Citation Counts, Citation Bursts, Centrality, and Sigma
3.8.1. Clusters
3.8.2. Citation Counts
3.8.3. Bursts
3.8.4. Centrality
3.8.5. Sigma
4. Discussion
5. Practical Implications
6. Theoretical Implications
7. Limitations and Future Directions
8. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Boeckx, C.; Grohmann, K.K. Introducing the Volume. In The Cambridge Handbook of Biolinguistics; Boeckx, C., Grohmann, K.K., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2013; pp. 1–3. [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins, L. Biolinguistics: A Historical Perspective. In The Cambridge Handbook of Biolinguistics; Boeckx, C., Grohmann, K.K., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2013; pp. 4–11. [Google Scholar]
- Koji, F.; Cedric, A.B. Introduction: The Biolinguistic Program: A New Beginning. In Advances in Biolinguistics: The Human Language Faculty and Its Biological Basis; Koji, F., Cedric, A.B., Eds.; Taylor and Francis: London, UK, 2016; pp. 1–6. [Google Scholar]
- McGilvray, J. The Philosophical Foundations of Biolinguistics. In The Cambridge Handbook of Biolinguistics; Boeckx, C., Grohmann, K.K., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2013; pp. 22–46. [Google Scholar]
- Meader, C.L.; Muyskens, J.H. Handbook of Biolinguistics/by Clarence L. Meader and John H. Muyskens; Weller: Toledo, OH, USA, 1950. [Google Scholar]
- Piattelli-Palmarini, M. Biolinguistics, Yesterday, Today, and Tomorrow. In The Cambridge Handbook of Biolinguistics; Boeckx, C., Grohmann, K.K., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2013; pp. 12–21. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, N. The Logical Structure of Linguistic Theory; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA, 1955; Volume MS. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, N. Syntactic Structures; Mouton: The Hague, The Netherlands, 1957. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, N. A Review of B. F. Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. Language 1959, 35, 26–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Skinner, B.F. Verbal Behavior; Century psychology series; Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York, NY, USA, 1957. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, N. Aspects of the Theory of Syntax; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1965. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, N. Biolinguistic Explorations: Design, Development, Evolution. Int. J. Philos. Stud. 2007, 15, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lenneberg, E.H. Biological Foundations of Language [by] Eric H. Lenneberg. With Appendices by Noam Chomsky and Otto Marx; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1967. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, N. Some Concepts and Consequences of the Theory of Government and Binding/Noam Chomsky; Linguistic inquiry monographs; 6; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1982; ISBN 026203090X. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, N. The Minimalist Program; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Al-Mutairi, F.R. The Minimalist Program: The Nature and Plausibility of Chomsky’s Biolinguistics (Cambridge Studies in Linguistics 143); Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, N. Language and Thought; Moyer Bell: London, UK, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins, L. Introduction. In Biolinguistics: Exploring the Biology of Language; Jenkins, L., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins, L. Unification in Biolinguistics. In Variation and Universals in Biolinguistics; Jenkins, L., Ed.; Elsevier: Leiden, The Netherlands; Boston, MA, USA, 2004; pp. 317–339. [Google Scholar]
- Mendívil-Giró, J.-L. The Myth of Language Diversity. In Language, from a Biological Point of View: Current Issues in Biolinguistics; Boeckx, C., del Carmen Horno-Chéliz, M., Mendívil-Giró, J.L., Eds.; Cambridge Scholars Publishing: Newcastle, UK, 2012; pp. 85–133. [Google Scholar]
- Steigerwald, J. Goethe’s Morphology: Urphänomene and Aesthetic Appraisal. J. Hist. Biol. 2002, 35, 291–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hauser, M.D.; Chomsky, N.; Fitch, W.T. The Faculty of Language: What Is It, Who Has It, and How Did It Evolve? Science 2002, 298, 1569–1579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Larson, R.K.; Déprez, V.; Yamakido, H. Introduction. In The Evolution of Human Language: Biolinguistic Perspectives; Larson, R.K., Déprez, V., Yamakido, H., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2010; pp. 1–13. [Google Scholar]
- Boeckx, C.; del Carmen Horno-Chéliz, M.; Mendívil-Giró, J.L. Introduction: Integrating Linguistics and Biology. In Language, from a Biological Point of View: Current Issues in Biolinguistics; Boeckx, C., del Carmen Horno-Chéliz, M., Mendívil-Giró, J.L., Eds.; Cambridge Scholars Publishing: Newcastle, UK, 2012; pp. 1–11. [Google Scholar]
- Di Sciullo, A.M.; Jenkins, L. Biolinguistics and the Human Language Faculty. Language 2016, 92, e205–e236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Di Sciullo, A.-M.; Boeckx, C. Introduction: Contours of the Biolinguistic Research Agenda. In The Biolinguistic Enterprise: New Perspectives on the Evolution and Nature of the Human Language Faculty; Di Sciullo, A.-M., Boeckx, C., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2011; pp. 1–16. [Google Scholar]
- Carroll, S.B. Endless Forms Most Beautiful: The New Science of Evo Devo and the Making of the Animal Kingdom, with Illustrations by Jamie W. Carroll, Josh P. Klaiss, Leanne, M. Olds, 1st ed.; W.W. Norton & Co.: New York, NY, USA, 2005; ISBN 0393060160. [Google Scholar]
- Raimy, E. The Big-Tent View of Biolinguistics. Bioscience 2012, 62, 925–927. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cedric, B.; Kleanthes, K.G. The Biolinguistics Manifesto. Biolinguistics 2007, 1, 1–8. [Google Scholar]
- Fitch, W.T. Prolegomena to a Future Science of Biolinguistics. Biolinguistics 2009, 3, 283–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boeckx, C. Biolinguistics: Facts, Fiction, and Forecast. Biolinguistics 2013, 7, 20160007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martins, P.T.; Boeckx, C. What We Talk about When We Talk about Biolinguistics. Linguist. Vanguard 2016, 2, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boeckx, C.; Grohmann, K.K. Remark: Putting Phases in Perspective. Syntax 2007, 10, 204–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boeckx, C.; Benítez-Burraco, A. Biolinguistics. In The Design, Development and Evolution of Human Language: Biolinguistic Explorations; Fujita, K., Fukui, N., Yusa, N., Ike-Uchi, M., Eds.; Kaitakusha: Tokyo, Japan, 2014; pp. 8–30. [Google Scholar]
- Pleyer, M.; Hartmann, S. Constructing a Consensus on Language Evolution? Convergences and Differences between Biolinguistic and Usage-Based Approaches. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Behme, C. Is the Ontology of Biolinguistics Coherent? Lang. Sci. 2015, 47, 32–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bickerton, D. The Child, the Bioprogram and the Input Data: A Commentary on Cziko. First Lang. 1989, 9, 33–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bowling, D.L. The Continuing Legacy of Nature versus Nurture in Biolinguistics. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2017, 24, 140–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kirby, S. Culture and Biology in the Origins of Linguistic Structure. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2017, 24, 118–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Balari, S.; Benítez-Burraco, A.; Longa, V.M.; Lorenzo, G. The Fossils of Language: What Are They? Who Has Them? How Did They Evolve? In The Cambridge Handbook of Biolinguistics; Boeckx, C., Grohmann, K.K., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 489–523. [Google Scholar]
- de Aquino Silva, N.M.; da Motta Sampaio, T.O. Origin of Language: A Biolinguistic Approach. Rev. Dos Trab. Iniciação Científica Da UNICAMP 2018, 26, 20396. [Google Scholar]
- Mao, T.; Man, Z.; Lin, H.; Yang, C. How Biological Elements Interact with Language: The Biolinguistic Inquiry. Front. Biosci. 2020, 25, 930–947. [Google Scholar]
- Bolender, J. Wittgenstein and Biolinguistics: Building upon the Second Picture Theory. Biolinguistics 2018, 12, 14–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trettenbrein, P.C. The “Grammar” in Universal Grammar: A Biolinguistic Clarification. Quest. Answ. Linguist. 2015, 2, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brody, M. Some Biolinguistic Remarks. Acta Linguist. Acad. 2019, 66, 335–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feeney, A. Saussurian Biolinguistics? Bouchard’s Offline Brain Systems and Sign Theory of Language. Cogn. Semiot. 2018, 11, 37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rahul, D.R.; Ponniah, R.J. Understanding the Robustness of Incidental Vocabulary Acquisition through Reading: Qualitative Insights from Biolinguistics. Rupkatha J. Interdiscip. Stud. Humanit. 2020, 12, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chomsky, N. On the Bioligical Basis of Language Capacities. In The Neuropsychology of Language: Essays in Honor of Eric Lenneberg; Rieber, R.W., Ed.; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA, 1976; pp. 1–40. [Google Scholar]
- Fitch, W. What Would Lenneberg Think? Biolinguistics in the Third Millennium. Biolinguistics 2017, 11, 445–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Valencia, O.V.R. Historia Genética Del Lenguaje. Rev. Soc. Boliv. Pediatría 2014, 53, 75–80. [Google Scholar]
- Kelley, J.L.; Swanson, W.J. Positive Selection in the Human Genome: From Genome Scans to Biological Significance. Annu. Rev. Genom. Hum. Genet. 2008, 9, 143–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vargha-Khadem, F.; Gadian, D.G.; Copp, A.; Mishkin, M. FOXP2 and the Neuroanatomy of Speech and Language. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2005, 6, 131–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fitch, W.T. The Biology and Evolution of Speech: A Comparative Analysis. Annu. Rev. Linguist. 2018, 4, 255–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pulvers, J.N.; Journiac, N.; Arai, Y.; Nardelli, J. MCPH1: A Window into Brain Development and Evolution. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caraffi, S.G.; Pollazzon, M.; Farooq, M.; Fatima, A.; Larsen, L.A.; Zuntini, R.; Napoli, M.; Garavelli, L. MCPH1: A Novel Case Report and a Review of the Literature. Genes 2022, 13, 634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schneider, E.; El Hajj, N.; Richter, S.; Roche-Santiago, J.; Nanda, I.; Schempp, W.; Riederer, P.; Navarro, B.; Bontrop, R.E.; Kondova, I.; et al. Widespread Differences in Cortex DNA Methylation of the “Language Gene” CNTNAP2 between Humans and Chimpanzees. Epigenetics 2014, 9, 533–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kos, M.; van den Brink, D.; Snijders, T.M.; Rijpkema, M.; Franke, B.; Fernandez, G.; Hagoort, P. CNTNAP2 and Language Processing in Healthy Individuals as Measured with ERPs. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e46995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wong, P.C.M.; Kang, X.; Wong, K.H.Y.; So, H.C.; Choy, K.W.; Geng, X. ASPM-Lexical Tone Association in Speakers of a Tone Language: Direct Evidence for the Genetic-Biasing Hypothesis of Language Evolution. Sci. Adv. 2020, 6, eaba5090. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watkins, K.E.; Dronkers, N.F.; Vargha-Khadem, F. Behavioural Analysis of an Inherited Speech and Language Disorder: Comparison with Acquired Aphasia. Brain 2002, 125, 452–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fisher, S.E.; Lai, C.S.L.; Monaco, A.P. Deciphering the Genetic Basis of Speech and Language Disorders. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2003, 26, 57–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vernes, S.C.; Nicod, J.; Elahi, F.M.; Coventry, J.A.; Kenny, N.; Coupe, A.M.; Bird, L.E.; Davies, K.E.; Fisher, S.E. Functional Genetic Analysis of Mutations Implicated in a Human Speech and Language Disorder. Hum. Mol. Genet. 2006, 15, 3154–3167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilcke, A.; Ligges, C.; Burkhardt, J.; Alexander, M.; Wolf, C.; Quente, E.; Ahnert, P.; Hoffmann, P.; Becker, A.; Müller-Myhsok, B.; et al. Imaging Genetics of FOXP2 in Dyslexia. Eur. J. Hum. Genet. 2012, 20, 224–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Takahashi, H.; Takahashi, K.; Liu, F.-C. FOXP Genes, Neural Development, Speech and Language Disorders. In Forkhead Transcription Factors; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2009; pp. 117–129. ISBN 9781441915986. [Google Scholar]
- Newbury, D.F.; Monaco, A.P. Molecular Genetics of Speech and Language Disorders. Curr. Opin. Pediatr. 2002, 14, 696–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lai, C.S.L.; Fisher, S.E.; Hurst, J.A.; Vargha-Khadem, F.; Monaco, A.P. A Forkhead-Domain Gene Is Mutated in a Severe Speech and Language Disorder. Nature 2001, 413, 519–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Locke, J.L.; Bogin, B. Language and Life History: A New Perspective on the Development and Evolution of Human Language. Behav. Brain Sci. 2006, 29, 259–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wassink, T.H.; Piven, J.; Vieland, V.J.; Pietila, J.; Goedken, R.J.; Folstein, S.E.; Sheffield, V.C. Evaluation of FOXP2 as an Autism Susceptibility Gene. Am. J. Med. Genet.-Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 2002, 114, 566–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newbury, D.F.; Bonora, E.; Lamb, J.A.; Fisher, S.E.; Lai, C.S.L.; Baird, G.; Jannoun, L.; Slonims, V.; Stott, C.M.; Merricks, M.J.; et al. FOXP2 Is Not a Major Susceptibility Gene for Autism or Specific Language Impairment. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2002, 70, 1318–1327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gauthier, J.; Joober, R.; Mottron, L.; Laurent, S.; Fuchs, M.; De Kimpe, V.; Rouleau, G.A. Mutation Screening of FOXP2 in Individuals Diagnosed with Autistic Disorder. Am. J. Med. Genet. 2003, 118A, 172–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gong, X.; Jia, M.; Ruan, Y.; Shuang, M.; Liu, J.; Wu, S.; Guo, Y.; Yang, J.; Ling, Y.; Yang, X.; et al. Association between TheFOXP2 Gene and Autistic Disorder in Chinese Population. Am. J. Med. Genet. 2004, 127B, 113–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spence, S.J. The Genetics of Autism. Semin. Pediatr. Neurol. 2004, 11, 196–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pennington, B.F.; Bishop, D.V.M. Relations among Speech, Language, and Reading Disorders. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2009, 60, 283–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kang, C.; Drayna, D. Genetics of Speech and Language Disorders. Annu. Rev. Genom. Hum. Genet. 2011, 12, 145–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grimaldi, M. Toward a Neural Theory of Language: Old Issues and New Perspectives. J. Neurolinguist. 2012, 25, 304–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fisher, S.E.; Vernes, S.C. Genetics and the Language Sciences. Annu. Rev. Linguist. 2015, 1, 289–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Graham, S.A.; Fisher, S.E. Understanding Language from a Genomic Perspective. Annu. Rev. Genet. 2015, 49, 131–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johansson, S. Language Abilities in Neanderthals. Annu. Rev. Linguist. 2015, 1, 311–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lidz, J.; Gagliardi, A. How Nature Meets Nurture: Universal Grammar and Statistical Learning. Annu. Rev. Linguist. 2015, 1, 333–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chenausky, K.V.; Tager-Flusberg, H. The Importance of Deep Speech Phenotyping for Neurodevelopmental and Genetic Disorders: A Conceptual Review. J. Neurodev. Disord. 2022, 14, 36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Irie, K.; Doi, M.; Usui, N.; Shimada, S. Evolution of the Human Brain Can Help Determine Pathophysiology of Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Front. Neurosci. 2022, 16, 871979. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nowak, M.A. Evolutionary Biology of Language. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2000, 355, 1615–1622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barbieri, M. On the Origin of Language. Biosemiotics 2010, 3, 201–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steels, L. Modeling the Cultural Evolution of Language. Phys. Life Rev. 2011, 8, 339–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benítez-Burraco, A. Problematic Aspects of the Genetic Analysis of the Specific Disorders of the Language: FOXP2 as Paradigm. Neurologia 2012, 27, 225–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van der Lely, H.K.J.; Pinker, S. The Biological Basis of Language: Insight from Developmental Grammatical Impairments. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014, 18, 586–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boeckx, C. Biolinguistics: Forays into Human Cognitive Biology. J. Anthropol. Sci. 2013, 91, 63–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wu, J. An Overview of Researches on Biolinguistics. Can. Soc. Sci. 2014, 10, 171–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Glänzel, W.; Schoepflin, U. Little Scientometrics, Big Scientometrics... and Beyond? Scientometrics 1994, 30, 375–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chellappandi, P.; Vijayakumar, C.S. Bibliometrics, Scientometrics, Webometrics/Cybermetrics, Informetrics and Altmetrics-An Emerging Field in Library and Information Science Research. Int. J. Educ. 2018, 7, 5–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Egghe, L. Little Science, Big Science... and Beyond. Scientometrics 1994, 30, 389–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parkinson, M. The Evaluation of Research by Scientometric Indicators. Libr. Manag. 2011, 32, 226–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huang, Y.; Glänzel, W.; Zhang, L. Tracing the Development of Mapping Knowledge Domains. Scientometrics 2021, 126, 6201–6224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sooryamoorthy, R. Scientometrics for the Humanities and Social Sciences; Routledge: Oxfordshire, UK, 2020; ISBN 9780367626860. [Google Scholar]
- Alduais, A.; Alduais, A.; Alfadda, H.; Allegretta, S. Clinical Linguistics: Analysis of Mapping Knowledge Domains in Past, Present and Future. Children 2022, 9, 1202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Birkle, C.; Pendlebury, D.A.; Schnell, J.; Adams, J. Web of Science as a Data Source for Research on Scientific and Scholarly Activity. Quant. Sci. Stud. 2020, 1, 363–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burnham, J.F. Scopus Database: A Review. Biomed. Digit. Libr. 2006, 3, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pranckutė, R. Web of Science (WoS) and Scopus: The Titans of Bibliographic Information in Today’s Academic World. Publications 2021, 9, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Penfold, R. Using the Lens Database for Staff Publications. J. Med. Libr. Assoc. 2020, 108, 520–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, C. The CiteSpace Manual: Version 1.01; College of Computing and Informatics: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2014; pp. 1–84. [Google Scholar]
- van Eck, N.J.; Waltman, L. VOSviewer Manual: Manual for VOSviewer Version 1.6.18; Univeristeit Leiden: Leiden, The Netherlands, 2022. [Google Scholar]
- Freeman, L.C. Centrality in Social Networks. Soc. Netw. 1979, 1, 215–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kleinberg, J. Bursty and Hierarchical Structure in Streams. In Proceedings of the Eighth ACM SIGKDD International Conference on Knowledge Discovery and Data Mining-KDD’02, Edmonton, AB, Canada, 23–26 July 2002; ACM Press: New York, NY, USA, 2002; p. 91. [Google Scholar]
- Chen, C. CiteSpace: A Practical Guide for Mapping Scientific Literature; Nova Publishers: Hauppauge, NY, USA, 2016; ISBN 9781536102956. [Google Scholar]
- Benítez-Burraco, A. A Biolinguistic Approach to Language Disorders: Towards a Paradigm Shift in Clinical Linguistics. In Advances in Biolinguistics; Routledge: Oxfordshire, UK, 2016; pp. 256–271. [Google Scholar]
- Kirby, S. Apes, Language and the Human Mind; Linguistic Society of America: New York, NY, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Christiansen, M.H.; Kirby, S. Language Evolution; OUP Oxford: Oxford, UK, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Pinker, S. The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language; Penguin UK: New York, NY, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Arbib, M.A. From Monkey-like Action Recognition to Human Language: An Evolutionary Framework for Neurolinguistics. Behav. Brain Sci. 2005, 28, 105–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bybee, J. Language, Usage and Cognition; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Pinker, S.; Bloom, P. Natural Language and Natural Selection. Behav. Brain Sci. 1990, 13, 707–727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gazzola, V.; Aziz-Zadeh, L.; Keysers, C. Empathy and the Somatotopic Auditory Mirror System in Humans. Curr. Biol. 2006, 16, 1824–1829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Christiansen, M.H.; Chater, N. Language as Shaped by the Brain. Behav. Brain Sci. 2008, 31, 489–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steels, L. Language as a Complex Adaptive System. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Parallel Problem Solving from Nature, Paris, France, 18–20 September 2000; pp. 17–26. [Google Scholar]
- Kirby, S.; Cornish, H.; Smith, K. Cumulative Cultural Evolution in the Laboratory: An Experimental Approach to the Origins of Structure in Human Language. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2008, 105, 10681–10686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Corballis, M.C. From Mouth to Hand: Gesture, Speech, and the Evolution of Right-Handedness. Behav. Brain Sci. 2003, 26, 199–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferrer i Cancho, R.; Sole, R.V. Least Effort and the Origins of Scaling in Human Language. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2003, 100, 788–791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fitch, W.T. The Evolution of Language; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Fitch, W.T.; Arbib, M.A.; Donald, M. A Molecular Genetic Framework for Testing Hypotheses about Language Evolution. In Proceedings of the Evolution of Language, Itrecht, The Netherlands, 14–17 April 2010; World Scientific: Singapore, 2010; pp. 137–144. [Google Scholar]
- An Introduction to Evolutionary Musicology. In The Origins of Music; Brown, S.; Merker, B.; Wallin, C. (Eds.) The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Fitch, W.T.; Hauser, M.D.; Chomsky, N. The Evolution of the Language Faculty: Clarifications and Implications. Cognition 2005, 97, 179–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Emery, N.J. Cognition, Evolution, and Behavior. Anim. Behav. 2010, 80, 769–770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chomsky, N. Three Factors in Language Design. Linguist. Inq. 2005, 36, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bigelow, J. The Evolution of Communications. Next Gener. Pharm. 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Cancho, R.F.I.I.; Solé, R.V. The Small World of Human Language. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2001, 268, 2261–2265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Daddesio, T.C. The Biology and Evolution of Language. Am. J. Semiot. 1986, 4, 169–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kronenfeld, D.B. The Ecology of Language Evolution.:The Ecology of Language Evolution. Am. Anthropol. 2003, 105, 856–857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Matthews, L.; Gopinath, G.; Gillespie, M.; Caudy, M.; Croft, D.; de Bono, B.; Garapati, P.; Hemish, J.; Hermjakob, H.; Jassal, B.; et al. Reactome Knowledgebase of Human Biological Pathways and Processes. Nucleic Acids Res. 2009, 37, D619–D622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Christiansen, M.H.; Chater, N. Creating Language: Integrating Evolution, Acquisition, and Processing; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2016; ISBN 9780262334778. [Google Scholar]
- Fitch, W. Empirical Approaches to the Study of Language Evolution. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2017, 24, 3–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chomsky, N. Language and Responsibility: Based on Conversations with Mitsou Ronat; Pantheon Books: New York, NY, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello, M. The Item-Based Nature of Children’s Early Syntactic Development. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2000, 4, 156–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hauser, M.D.; Carey, S.; Hauser, L.B. Spontaneous Number Representation in Semi--Free--Ranging Rhesus Monkeys. Proc. R. Soc. London. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 2000, 267, 829–833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chomsky, N. Language and Nature. Mind 1995, 104, 1–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fitch, W.T.; Hauser, M.D. Computational Constraints on Syntactic Processing in a Nonhuman Primate. Science 2004, 303, 377–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pinker, S. Words and Rules: The Ingredients of Language, 1st ed.; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello, M. The New Psychology of Language: Cognitive and Functional Approaches to Language Structure; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NY, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Hauser, M.D. A Nonhuman Primate’s Expectations about Object Motion and Destination: The Importance of Self-propelled Movement and Animacy. Dev. Sci. 1998, 1, 31–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chomsky, N.; Otero, C.P. Language and Politics; Black Rose Books: Montréal, QC, Canada, 1988; ISBN 0921689357. [Google Scholar]
- Bickerton, D. Language & Species; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1990; ISBN 0226046109. [Google Scholar]
- Steels, L.; Kaplan, F. Stochasticity as a Source of Innovation in Language Games. In Artificial life VI: Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Artificial Life; MTI Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Christiansen, M.H.; Ellefson, M.R. Linguistic Adaptation Without Linguistic Constraints: The Role of Sequential Learning in Language Evolution. Transit. Lang. 2002, 335–358. [Google Scholar]
- Monaghan, P.; Chater, N.; Christiansen, M.H. The Differential Role of Phonological and Distributional Cues in Grammatical Categorisation. Cognition 2005, 96, 143–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rizzolatti, G.; Arbib, M.A. Language within Our Grasp. Trends Neurosci. 1998, 21, 188–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pinker, S.; Jackendoff, R. The Faculty of Language: What’s Special about It? Cognition 2005, 95, 201–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Batali, J. Computational Simulations of the Emergence of Grammar. In Approaches to the Evolution of Language: Social and Cognitive Bases; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Popescu-Belis, A.; Batali, J. Incremental Simulations of the Emergence of Grammar: Towards Complex Sentence-Meaning Mappings; University of California at San Diego: San Diego, CA, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Nowak, M.A.; Plotkin, J.B.; Jansen, V.A.A. The Evolution of Syntactic Communication. Nature 2000, 404, 495–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vogt, P.; Coumans, H. Investigating Social Interaction Strategies for Bootstrapping Lexicon Development. J. Artif. Soc. Soc. Simul. 2003, 6, 1–4. [Google Scholar]
- Cangelosi, A.; Parisi, D. The Emergence of a “Language” in an Evolving Population of Neural Networks. Conn. Sci. 1998, 10, 83–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Development Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2020; Available online: https://Www.R-Project.Org/ (accessed on 10 March 2022).
- Tamariz, M. Experimental Studies on the Cultural Evolution of Language. Annu. Rev. Linguist. 2017, 3, 389–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oliphant, M. The Learning Barrier: Moving from Innate to Learned Systems of Communication. Adapt. Behav. 1999, 7, 371–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oliphant, M.W.; Batali, J.; Elman, J. Formal Approaches to Innate and Learned Communication: Laying the Foundation for Language; University of California: San Diego, CA, USA; Ann Arbor, MI, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Briscoe, T. Grammatical Assimilation. In Language Evolution; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2003; pp. 295–316. [Google Scholar]
- Deacon, T.W. Evolutionary Perspectives on Language and Brain Plasticity. J. Commun. Disord. 2000, 33, 273–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Raviv, L.; Meyer, A.; Lev-Ari, S. Larger Communities Create More Systematic Languages. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2019, 286, 20191262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cavalli-Sforza, L.L. Genes, Peoples, and Languages. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1997, 94, 7719–7724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Donald, M. Representation: Ontogenesis and Phylogenesis. Behav. Brain Sci. 1994, 17, 714–715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bates, E. Language about Me and You: Pronominal Reference and the Emerging Concept of Self. In The Self in Transition: Infancy to Childhood; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1990; pp. 165–182. [Google Scholar]
- Lieberman, P.; Hurford, J.R.; Studdert-Kennedy, M.; Knight, C. Approaches to the Evolution of Language. J. R. Anthropol. Inst. 1999, 5, 656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- CHENEY, D.L.; SEYFARTH, R.M.; PALOMBIT, R. The Function and Mechanisms Underlying Baboon ‘Contact’ Barks. Anim. Behav. 1996, 52, 507–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Corballis, M.C. Evolution of Language and Laterality: A Gradual Descent? Cah. Psychol. Cogn. Psychol. Cogn. 1998, 17, 1148–1155. [Google Scholar]
- Lieberman, P.; Blumstein, S.E. Speech Physiology, Speech Perception, and Acoustic Phonetics; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, IL, USA, 1988; ISBN 9780521308663. [Google Scholar]
- Darwin, C. The Origin of Species: Complete and Fully Illustrated; Gramercy Books: Bexley, UK, 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Ferrari, P.F.; Gallese, V.; Rizzolatti, G.; Fogassi, L. Mirror Neurons Responding to the Observation of Ingestive and Communicative Mouth Actions in the Monkey Ventral Premotor Cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2003, 17, 1703–1714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Corballis, M.C. The Gestural Origins of Language: Human Language May Have Evolved from Manual Gestures, Which Survive Today as a “Behavioral Fossil” Coupled to Speech. Am. Sci. 1999, 87, 138–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benítez-Burraco, A.; Boeckx, C. Universal Grammar and Biological Variation: An EvoDevo Agenda for Comparative Biolinguistics. Biol. Theory 2014, 9, 122–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kirby, S. Function, Selection, and Innateness: The Emergence of Language Universals; OUP Oxford: Oxford, UK, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, N.; Noam, C. On Nature and Language; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, N. Language and Mind; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Pinker, S. How the Mind Works; New York Norton: New York, NY, USA, 1997; Volume 524. [Google Scholar]
- Amari, S. The Handbook of Brain Theory and Neural Networks; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Bickerton, D. Some Problems for Biolinguistics. Biolinguistics 2014, 8, 73–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van Eck, N.J.; Waltman, L. Visualizing Bibliometric Networks. In Measuring Scholarly Impact; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2014; pp. 285–320. ISBN 9783319103761. [Google Scholar]
- van Eck, N.J.; Waltman, L.; Dekker, R.; van den Berg, J. A Comparison of Two Techniques for Bibliometric Mapping: Multidimensional Scaling and VOS. J. Am. Soc. Inf. Sci. Technol. 2010, 61, 2405–2416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moral-Muñoz, J.A.; Herrera-Viedma, E.; Santisteban-Espejo, A.; Cobo, M.J. Software Tools for Conducting Bibliometric Analysis in Science: An up-to-Date Review. Prof. Inf. 2020, 29, e290103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Element | Definition/Specification/Retrieved Data | Database/Software | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Indicator | Scopus | WOS | Lens | ||
Bibliometric | |||||
Year | Production size by year | √ | √ | √ | |
Country | Top countries publishing in the field | √ | √ | √ | |
University | Top universities, research centres, etc. | √ | √ | √ | |
Source | Top journals, book series, etc. | √ | √ | √ | |
Publisher | Top publishers | × | √ | √ | |
Subject area | Top fields associated with the field | √ | √ | √ | |
Author | Top authors publishing in the field | √ | √ | √ | |
Citation | Top cited documents | √ | √ | √ | |
Scientometric | CiteSpace | VOSviewer | |||
Betweenness centrality | A path between nodes and is achieved when located between two nodes [101] | √ | × | ||
Burst detection | Determines the frequency of a certain event in certain period (e.g., the frequent citation of a certain reference during a period of time) [102] | √ | × | ||
Co-citation | When two references are cited by a third reference [103]. CiteSpace provides document co-citation network for references, and author co-citation network for authors.In VOSviewer, co-citation defined as “the relatedness of items is determined based on the number of times they are cited together” [100] (p. 5). Units of analysis include cited authors, references, or sources. | √ | √ | ||
Silhouette | Used in cluster analysis to measure consistency of each cluster with its related nodes [99] | √ | × | ||
Sigma | To measure strength of a node in terms of betweenness centrality citation burst [99] | √ | × | ||
Clusters | “We can probably eyeball the visualized network and identify some prominent groupings” [99] (p. 23). | √ | √ | ||
Citation | “The relatedness of items is determined based on the number of times they cite each other” [100] (p. 5). Units of analysis include documents, sources, authors, organizations, or countries. | √ | √ | ||
Keywords | CiteSpace provides co-occurring author keywords and keywords plus.In VOSviewer, co-occurrence analysis is defined as “the relatedness of items is determined based on the number of documents in which they occur together” [100] (p. 5). Units of analysis include author keywords, all keywords, or keywords plus. | √ | √ |
Scopus (TITLE-ABS-KEY ({biolinguistics}) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“biology of language”) OR TITLE-ABS-KEY (“language evolution”)) AND (LIMIT-TO(DOCTYPE, “ar”) OR LIMIT-TO (DOCTYPE, “cp”) OR LIMIT-TO (DOCTYPE, “re”) OR LIMIT-TO (DOCTYPE, “ch”) OR LIMIT-TO (DOCTYPE, “bk”)) Monday, 18 April, 2022, 1570 document results, 1973–2022 |
WOS “biolinguistics” (Topic) or “biology of language” (Topic) or “language evolution” (Topic) and Articles or Proceedings Papers or Editorial Materials or Review Articles or Book Chapters or Book Reviews or Early Access or Books (Document Types) Monday, 18 April, 2022, 1440 results, 1988–2022 |
Lens (Title: (AND (biolinguistics AND)) OR (Abstract: (AND (biolinguistics AND)) OR Full Text: (AND (biolinguistics AND)))) OR ((Title: (AND (“biology of language” AND)) OR (Abstract: (AND (“biology of language” AND)) OR Full Text: (AND (“biology of language” AND)))) OR (Title: (AND (“language evolution” AND)) OR (Abstract: (AND (“language evolution” AND)) OR Full Text: (AND (“language evolution” AND))))) Filters: Stemming = Disabled Publication Type = (journal article, unknown, book chapter, book, conference proceedings article, dissertation, preprint, conference proceedings) Author Display Name = (excl Chris J Myers, excl Michael Hucka) Monday, 18 April, 2022, Scholarly Works (5275), 1935–2022 |
No. | Source Title | Citation | Citations by Database | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Scopus | WOS | Lens | |||
1 | Language, usage and cognition | [109] | 1089 | × | 780 |
2 | Natural language and natural selection | [110] | 949 | × | 1825 |
3 | From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics | [108] | 656 | 574 | × |
4 | Empathy and the Somatotopic Auditory Mirror System in Humans | [111] | 537 | 503 | × |
5 | Language as shaped by the brain | [112] | 483 | 449 | × |
6 | Language as a complex adaptive system: Position paper | [113] | 415 | 411 | × |
7 | Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: An experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language | [114] | 407 | 382 | × |
8 | From mouth to hand: Gesture, speech, and the evolution of right-handedness | [115] | 398 | 373 | × |
9 | Least effort and the origins of scaling in human language | [116] | 338 | 301 | × |
10 | The evolution of language | [117] | 315 | × | 844 |
11 | A molecular genetic framework for testing hypotheses about language evolution | [118] | × | 500 | × |
12 | An introduction to evolutionary musicology | [119] | × | 287 | × |
13 | The evolution of the language faculty: Clarifications and implications | [120] | × | 299 | × |
14 | Cognition, evolution, and behavior | [121] | × | × | 1395 |
15 | Three Factors in Language Design | [122] | × | × | 1314 |
16 | The Evolution of Communication | [123] | × | × | 1211 |
17 | The small world of human language | [124] | × | × | 855 |
18 | The Biology and Evolution of Language | [125] | × | × | 813 |
19 | The Ecology of Language Evolution | [126] | × | × | 797 |
20 | Reactome knowledgebase of human biological pathways and processes. | [127] | × | × | 780 |
Cluster ID | Size | Silhouette | Label (LSI) | Label (LLR) | Label (MI) | Average Year |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Scopus | ||||||
0 | 210 | 0.728 | Language evolution | Gestural communication (556.54, 1.0 × 10−4) | Year (1.53) | 2010 |
1 | 191 | 0.594 | Language evolution | Structural design (208.62, 1.0 × 10−4) | Year (1.86) | 2006 |
2 | 183 | 0.722 | Language evolution | Cultural evolution (755.77, 1.0 × 10−4) | Year (3.39) | 2012 |
3 | 70 | 0.822 | Language evolution | Neural network (282.82, 1.0 × 10−4) | Year (0.56) | 2005 |
4 | 55 | 0.944 | Language evolution | Sprachwerkzeuge al (language tools) (260.18, 1.0 × 10−4) | Linguistic perspective (0.54) | 1984 |
5 | 53 | 0.91 | Language evolution | Human language faculty (295.06, 1.0 × 10−4) | Human language-ready brain (0.17) | 2000 |
6 | 38 | 0.926 | Language evolution | Evolutionary biology (332.38, 1.0 × 10−4) | Road map (0.24) | 2001 |
7 | 36 | 0.984 | Theoretical perspective | Theoretical perspective (67.79, 1.0 × 10−4) | Language evolution (0.12) | 1984 |
WOS | ||||||
0 | 171 | 0.791 | Language evolution | Exorcising Grice’s ghost (305.11, 1.0 × 10−4) | Role (0.92) | 2011 |
1 | 158 | 0.705 | Language evolution | Human language evolution (269.87, 1.0 × 10−4) | Role (1.82) | 2003 |
2 | 141 | 0.821 | Language evolution | Language evolution (620.39, 1.0 × 10−4) | Role (1.94) | 2013 |
3 | 132 | 0.746 | Language evolution | Cultural variation (316.76, 1.0 × 10−4) | Role (2.02) | 2005 |
4 | 120 | 0.745 | Language evolution | Molecular biology (466.71, 1.0 × 10−4) | Role (1.13) | 2013 |
WoS | Scopus | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Citation | Reference | Cluster ID | Citation | Reference | Cluster ID |
316 | Hauser [132] | 1 | 378 | Chomsky [130] | 4 |
308 | Chomsky [133] | 1 | 340 | Tomasello [131] | 0 |
303 | Fitch [134] | 1 | 298 | Pinker [135] | 1 |
294 | Pinker [135] | 1 | 280 | Kirby [105] | 2 |
279 | Tomasello [136] | 1 | 277 | Hauser [137] | 1 |
274 | Kirby [105] | 2 | 276 | Fitch [120] | 1 |
242 | Chomsky [138] | 1 | 231 | Bickerton [139] | 1 |
209 | Bickerton [37] | 1 | 202 | Steels [140] | 3 |
188 | Christiansen [141] | 2 | 194 | Christiansen [142] | 2 |
187 | Arbib [143] | 1 | 179 | Jackendoff [144] | 1 |
WoS | Scopus | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Burst | Reference | Cluster ID | Burst | Reference | Cluster ID |
19.03 | Steels [113] | 3 | 20.38 | Batali [145] | 3 |
17.45 | Batali [146] | 3 | 16.56 | Steels [140] | 3 |
17.12 | Nowak [147] | 3 | 15.64 | Vogt [148] | 3 |
15.86 | Cangelosi [149] | 3 | 15.59 | Cangelosi [149] | 3 |
15.64 | R Core Team [150] | 2 | 14.37 | Tamariz [151] | 2 |
12.32 | Tamariz [151] | 2 | 13.59 | Nowak [147] | 6 |
11.39 | Pinker [135] | 1 | 12.3 | Oliphant [152] | 3 |
11.11 | Oliphant [153] | 3 | 11.25 | Kirby [105] | 2 |
10.62 | Briscoe [154] | 3 | 11.03 | Deacon [155] | 1 |
10.41 | Raviv [156] | 2 | 10.85 | Cavalli-Sforza [157] | 6 |
WoS | Scopus | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Centrality | Reference | Cluster ID | Centrality | Reference | Cluster ID |
103 | Kirby [105] | 2 | 108 | Donald [158] | 0 |
99 | Bickerton [37] | 1 | 103 | Bates [159] | 0 |
96 | Arbib [143] | 1 | 102 | Hurford [160] | 1 |
95 | Cheney [161] | 0 | 101 | Cheney [161] | 0 |
93 | Corballis [162] | 1 | 98 | Kirby [105] | 2 |
91 | Tomasello [136] | 1 | 94 | Lieberman [163] | 1 |
90 | Hurford [160] | 3 | 91 | Chomsky [130] | 4 |
82 | Fitch [134] | 1 | 89 | Darwin [164] | 1 |
81 | Rizzolatti [165] | 0 | 85 | Corballis [166] | 0 |
81 | Chomsky [15] | 1 | 84 | Bickerton [139] | 1 |
WoS | Scopus | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sigma | Reference | Cluster ID | Sigma | Reference | Cluster ID |
0 | Kirby [105] | 2 | 0 | Donald [158] | 0 |
0 | Bickerton [37] | 1 | 0 | Bates [159] | 0 |
0 | Arbib [143] | 1 | 0 | Hurford [160] | 1 |
0 | Cheney [161] | 0 | 0 | Cheney [161] | 0 |
0 | Corballis [162] | 1 | 0 | Kirby [105] | 2 |
0 | Tomasello [136] | 1 | 0 | Lieberman [163] | 1 |
0 | Hurford [160] | 3 | 0 | Chomsky [130] | 4 |
0 | Fitch [134] | 1 | 0 | Darwin [164] | 1 |
0 | Rizzolatti [165] | 0 | 0 | Corballis [166] | 0 |
0 | Chomsky [15] | 1 | 0 | Bickerton [139] | 1 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Alduais, A.; Almaghlouth, S.; Alfadda, H.; Qasem, F. Biolinguistics: A Scientometric Analysis of Research on (Children’s) Molecular Genetics of Speech and Language (Disorders). Children 2022, 9, 1300. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9091300
Alduais A, Almaghlouth S, Alfadda H, Qasem F. Biolinguistics: A Scientometric Analysis of Research on (Children’s) Molecular Genetics of Speech and Language (Disorders). Children. 2022; 9(9):1300. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9091300
Chicago/Turabian StyleAlduais, Ahmed, Shrouq Almaghlouth, Hind Alfadda, and Fawaz Qasem. 2022. "Biolinguistics: A Scientometric Analysis of Research on (Children’s) Molecular Genetics of Speech and Language (Disorders)" Children 9, no. 9: 1300. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9091300
APA StyleAlduais, A., Almaghlouth, S., Alfadda, H., & Qasem, F. (2022). Biolinguistics: A Scientometric Analysis of Research on (Children’s) Molecular Genetics of Speech and Language (Disorders). Children, 9(9), 1300. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9091300