Next Article in Journal / Special Issue
Italian University Students’ Resilience during the COVID-19 Lockdown—A Structural Equation Model about the Relationship between Resilience, Emotion Regulation and Well-Being
Previous Article in Journal / Special Issue
Female Dynamics in Authorship of Scientific Publications in the Public Library of Science: A 10-year Bibliometric Analysis of Biomedical Research
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Cultural Differences in How People Deal with Ridicule and Laughter: Differential Item Functioning between the Taiwanese Chinese and Canadian English Versions of the PhoPhiKat-45

1
Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Toronto, ON N6B 1Y6, Canada
2
Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, ON N6A 3K7, Canada
3
Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug, and Child’s Health (NEUROFARBA), Section of Psychology, University of Florence, 50135 Florence, Italy
4
Department of Educational Psychology and Counseling, National Taiwan Normal University, Taipei 106308, Taiwan
5
Department of Educational Psychology and Counseling, National Tsing Hua University, Hsinchu 300044, Taiwan
6
Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, 8006 Zurich, Switzerland
7
Institut für Psychologie, Martin-Luther-Universität Halle-Wittenberg, 06108 Halle, Germany
8
Regional Neurogenetic Centre (CRN), Department of Primary Care, ASP Catanzaro, Viale A. Perugini, 88046 Lamezia Terme, Italy
9
Association for Neurogenetic Research (ARN), 88046 Lamezia Terme, Italy
10
Academy of Cognitive Behavioral Sciences of Calabria (ASCoC), 88046 Lamezia Terme, Italy
11
Department of Social Psychology and Quantitative Psychology, University of Barcelona, 08035 Barcelona, Spain
12
Department of Research Methods in Behavioral Sciences, University of Granada, 18071 Granada, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2023, 13(2), 238-258; https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe13020019
Submission received: 21 November 2022 / Revised: 28 December 2022 / Accepted: 12 January 2023 / Published: 20 January 2023

Abstract

:
The PhoPhiKat-45 measures three dispositions toward ridicule and laughter, including gelotophobia (i.e., the fear of being laughed at), gelotophilia (i.e., the joy of being laughed at), and katagelasticism (i.e., the joy of laughing at others). Despite numerous cultural adaptations, there is a paucity of cross-cultural studies investigating measurement invariance of this measure. Undergraduate students from a Canadian university (N = 1467; 71.4% females) and 14 universities in Taiwan (N = 1274; 64.6% females) completed the English and Chinese PhoPhiKat-45 measures, respectively. Item response theory and differential item functioning analyses demonstrated that most items were well-distributed across the latent continuum. Five of 45 items were flagged for DIF, but all values had negligible effect sizes (McFadden’s pseudo R2 < 0.13). The Canadian sample was further subdivided into subsamples who identified as European White born in Canada (n = 567) and Chinese born in China, Hong Kong, or Taiwan (n = 180). In the subgroup analyses, no evidence of DIF was found. Findings support the utility of this measure across these languages and samples.

1. Introduction

Humour is a multidimensional phenomenon that encompasses a function (e.g., pro-social or mean-spirited) and fulfills complex needs for the individual (e.g., engage with others, mock others) [1]. Ruch [1] proposed that humour measures should have both humour and humourlessness represented, such that some individuals enjoy humorous interactions while others do not and thus avoid it. In response, Ruch and Proyer [2] proposed the measurement of three dispositions toward ridicule and laughter, including gelotophobia, gelotophilia, and katagelasticism. Gelotophobia is defined as the fear of being laughed at, a construct theoretically distinct from social anxiety, in which good-natured laughter may be perceived as a threatening form of ridicule directed towards the target [3,4]. Gelotophobia is highly correlated with fear of social situations, feeling perturbed, and greater stress in the workplace [5]. In contrast to the clinical categorization, a gelotophobic response is distinguished by an increased sensitivity to laughter [5]. Gelotophilia is the joy of being laughed at, in which joint laughter is perceived as a sign of appreciation [2]. Individuals who are high in gelotophilia do not mind being laughed at, as they see the role such as “class clown” desirable. Katagelasticism is the joy of laughing at others, in which the individual high in this trait may feel entitled to laugh at others at their expense if the opportunity presents itself [2].
In terms of measurement, these dispositions are commonly measured using the self-report instrument named PhoPhiKat (for long 45/30 item-version see Ruch and Proyer [2]; for ultrashort 9 item-version see Hofmann et al. [5]). The PhoPhiKat-45 is a self-report measure with supported structural, convergent, and discriminant validity [6,7]. Since its initial empirical validation in German-speaking samples [2], this measure has been translated and adapted in various languages, including Chinese, English, Turkish, Russian, and Spanish [7,8,9,10,11,12].
The PhoPhiKat across cultures. Though prior adaptations of the PhoPhiKat questionnaire are solid contributions to the scientific literature, none of these studies have investigated the cross-cultural validity of this instrument. One might wonder, for instance, whether the information derived from the PhoPhiKat scores might be equally generalized to Eastern and Western countries. The psychometric properties of a revised PhoPhiKat questionnaire for Chinese individuals have been supported across several studies [8,13], where it demonstrated strong construct and structural validity, internal consistency, and test-retest reliability. Similarly, Chen et al. [8] showed promising results for the reliability and validity of the traditional Chinese version, titled PhoPhiKat-TC. Translated versions of the PhoPhiKat were translated from English, to a different language, and then back to English for further analysis [8]. These measures were developed to enable: (1) global psychological investigation and experimental design with psychometrically sound tools; and (2) cross-cultural comparisons between individualistic and collectivistic cultures in order to further study cultural differences in humour and humour-related traits [8].
In the United States, the English version has been utilized in the cross-cultural study of gelotophobia in particular [14,15,16]. With focus on European and Asian Americans, Asian Americans scored higher compared to other cultures on the gelotophobia factor, which was interpreted as a result of the societal norm of collectivism among Asian cultures [8,15,17]. It is critical that measurement equivalence is established across cultures prior to concluding that the overall scale and its individual items have the same meaning across cross-cultural samples, however [18,19,20]. Measurement invariance analyses provide evidence that differences in test scores reflect true latent variable differences than group differences based on measurement bias [21,22]. The difference among cultural norms within a similar geographical location provide rationale for further cross-cultural research on the potential psychometric and observed means-differences in the PhoPhiKat-45.
How culture shapes humour and laughter. The presence of humour is commonplace, however, it may be perceived in different ways depending on one’s cultural norms [23]. As outlined in Jiang, Li, and Hou [23], being humorous is viewed as a positive character trait and often used in everyday life in Western cultures. In Eastern cultures, it is largely not viewed as positively, despite occasional utilization to promote belongingness in attempt to raise social status [23,24]. Numerous studies have demonstrated how humour characteristics (i.e., appreciation, production, correlates) can vary among cultures such as Hong Kong and Taiwan [25,26], pushing forward its importance for further study.
Established in Taiwanese and Chinese samples, previous studies found that those among a collectivistic culture experience humour differently, with Taiwanese and Chinese individuals reporting to be more afraid of being laughed at due to the threat of group harmony [27,28]. Comparatively, as demonstrated in European and North American samples, those in individualistic societies may express less fear of being laughed at as it would not pose a threat to a group environment [28]. As Chinese culture stresses conformity and withholding of particular emotional reactions, aligned with the collectivist view, this information is not unprecedented, similarly to the results for the individualistic [28,29].
Amidst individuals with gelotophobia, there is greater avoidance of social situations with laughter-related emotions, comparable to previous findings revealing evidence associating gelotophobia with low extraversion [28,30,31]. Gelotophobia was linked to a lower life of engagement in a cross-cultural comparison, in addition to a lower life of meaning and pleasure among those in China, which lastly aids the explanation of why some aspects of humour-related emotions and traits are often avoided [11]. Concentrating on gelotophilia, those who are high in this dimension may ‘expose’ themselves to get laughed at, as demonstrated by English North Americans within the same study [28,31]. The comparison between Chinese individuals and English North Americans is important to understand how cultures can vary within as well as across geographical regions [32].
In summary, there are differences within and across cultures in how individuals perceive aspects of humour and humour-related traits. This distinction appears greatest between individualistic and collectivistic cultures, and may determine how a particular cultural group may define humour and its associations with key outcomes [32]. Further, there is stronger background on cross-cultural research relating to gelotophobia, with less information on gelotophilia and katagelasticism. The current investigation undertakes to further extend this literature, and to respond to this important evidence gap, in an evaluation of differences among these factors and their associations with perceptions and cultural beliefs across cultural groups.
The present research. Presently, there are a lack of cross-cultural studies on the PhoPhiKat-45 measure. The current study will respond by using item response theory (IRT) and differential item functioning (DIF) to be the first to investigate the psychometric properties of the measure and its relation to different samples. The present study aims to investigate: (1) the item response parameterization of the English and Chinese version of the PhoPhiKat-45; (2) evaluate uniform and non-uniform DIF for individual items with these two groups; and (3) further subdivide Canadian individuals to those who identified as European White and Chinese to evaluate whether DIF represents differing cultural perceptions within the same geographical region.
The first objective employs IRT, a technique to determine how much information a particular item on a measure or questionnaire may provide to the researcher [29,33,34,35,36,37]. In using an item characteristic curve to determine the relationship between a participant and a given item, IRT can define the precision of items in both the English and Chinese measures. DIF is often utilized to interpret differences between groups within a measure in a meaningful way, such as determining group differences [34,38,39]. When considering previous works, it is hypothesized that the Chinese sample may have a higher likelihood of selecting an answer high in gelotophobia in comparison to the English sample based on measurement biases rather than cultural differences [15,17]. Thus, questions can be asked about biases due to cultural differences, similar to Lampert, Isaacson, and Lyttle’s [15] study, or if another inference can be made. DIF can threaten test validity and highlight the need to thoroughly investigate each measure and account for any dangers to its strength [37,38,40,41,42,43,44]. By employing IRT and DIF in the present study, the aim is to provide evidence for the psychometric properties and cross-cultural measurement invariance of the PhoPhiKat-45 in English and Chinese.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

In the Canadian sample, undergraduate students (N = 1467; 71.4% females) participated in the study online through the undergraduate participant research recruitment pool. Qualtrics, a web-based survey tool, was used for anonymized data collection. The sample identified as European White (n = 610; 41.6%), Asian/Pacific Islander (n = 592; 40.4%), Black/African American (n = 38; 2.6%), Hispanic/Latino (n = 10; 0.7%), Native American (n = 2, 0.1%), and other identity or prefer not to say (n = 215; 14.7%). Students’ ages ranged from 16 to 54 years (M = 18.36, SD = 1.68). Study participation was voluntary, and participants received a credit towards a psychology course. Participants were provided with informed consent and debriefed.
In the Taiwanese Chinese sample, data were collected using paper and pencil from undergraduate students from 14 universities located in Northern, Central, Southern, and Eastern areas in Taiwan (N = 1274; 64.6% females) between the ages of 18 to 29 years (Mage = 19.74; SD = 1.48). Students participated voluntarily and were debriefed after participation.

2.2. Materials and Procedure

PhoPhiKat-45. The PhoPhiKat-45 is a reliable and valid measure with 45 items that assesses gelotophobia (i.e., the fear of being laughed at), gelotophilia (i.e., the joy of being laughed at), and katagelasticism (i.e., the joy of laughing at others) [4,45,46]. Each respondent rates the items on a four-point Likert type scale ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. The measure has demonstrated evidence of strong internal consistency, test-retest reliability, and structural, convergent, and discriminant validity [4,5,8,45,46]. For the English version, Bayesian single-test reliability values with 95% credible intervals (CIs) of posterior means determined by MacDonald’s Omega values for gelotophobia, gelotophilia, and katagelasticism were 0.84 (0.82,0.85), 0.86 (0.85,0.87), and 0.84 (0.83,0.85), respectively. For the Taiwanese Chinese version, Bayesian MacDonald’s Omega values for posterior means were 0.85 (0.84,0.86), 0.84 (0.83,0.86), and 0.84 (0.83,0.85), respectively. Test information functions (TIF) are presented in Figure 1 and Figure 2 (see results) for the Canadian English and Taiwanese Chinese versions, respectively. Note that “I” represents information in the figures.

2.3. Analytic Strategy

The marginal maximum likelihood estimation method was used to evaluate gelotophobia, gelotophilia, and katagelasticism through Samejima’s graded response model (GRM) [47]. Orlando and Thissen’s [48] S-X2 statistics were reported for item fit [49]. Local dependence (LD) is evaluated using Yen’s Q3 statistics for excess item covariation when the latent trait is controlled for [50]. Marais and Andrich [51] recommended critical residual correlation values between 0.10 to 0.30. For this study, a value of 0.30 was used to test the violation of the local independence assumption. Yen’s Q3 values showed no evidence for the violation of the assumption of local item independence for all items in the Canadian English Sample. In the Taiwanese Chinese sample, most items showed no evidence for the violation of the assumption of local item independence; however, items 1 and 7 showed a residual correlation of 0.46. Specifically, these two items shared overlap in content regarding being paranoid about being the person laughed at when overhearing strangers laughing. As such, method variance may lead to residual correlations that the latent trait cannot explain [52].
In Samejima’s graded response model, a single item discrimination value (a) and three category threshold (bi) function values were produced for every item. Discrimination values were categorized as follows: ≤0.24 as very low, 0.25 to 0.64 as low, 0.65 to 1.34 as moderate, 1.35 to 1.69 as high, and ≥1.7 as very high [53]. The threshold parameters (bi) were presented as z-scores (M = 0, SD = 1) and indicate the amount of latent trait required for a 50% probability of endorsing the following response category.
DIF was used to determine whether biases across the Canadian English and Taiwanese Chinese versions existed. Crane et al. [54] recommended an ordinal regression approach to DIF that follows Samejima’s [47] graded response model (GRM) [55,56]. Based on the latent trait, each item incorporates three separate ordinal regression models. Model 1 (scores) includes the model at baseline with response probabilities regressed on the latent trait. Model 2 (scores and group) is a uniform DIF model with the main effect specified by group. Model 3 is a non-uniform DIF model that includes an interaction term between the participants’ latent trait and group-specific covariate [57]. Uniform DIF is identified with the comparisons of models 1 and 2 and non-uniform DIF is established with comparisons between models 2 and 3 [57]. Total DIF effects are established with comparisons between models 1 and 3 [54,58,59].
To establish statistically significant DIF, distinctive criteria with differing sensitivity have been proposed [57]. According to Jeong and Lee [60], McFadden’s pseudo R2 statistic fits Kvalseth’s eight criteria for a reliable R2 [60,61,62]. With previous findings supporting the oversensitivity of χ2 approaches in DIF detection, the threshold of McFadden’s pseudo R2 > 0.035 was used to examine DIF [59]. Jeong and Lee’s [60] recommendations for DIF effect sizes for R2 are as follows: <0.13 as negligible, 0.13 to 0.26 as moderate, and >0.26 as large [59,60]. A 10% change in proportional β represents a meaningful size of uniform DIF [54,63]. Bayesian reliabilities were conducted on JASP version 0.16.3 [64,65]. Samejima’s GRM models and DIF were conducted on R version 4.2.1 packages mirt [66] and lordif [57].

3. Results

3.1. Item Response Theory

Unidimensionality for each of the gelotophobia, gelotophilia, and katagelasticism dimensions was established, such that the first factor should account for >20% of the variance met by both samples [39]. However, the Canadian English sample shows a negative slope for item 13 (i.e., I believe that I make involuntarily a funny impression on others), suggesting misinterpretation for the translation of this item from the original German measure. Thus, it was removed from subsequent analyses [45]. For the Canadian sample, discrimination parameters for the three subscales are as follows: gelotophobia Mdn(a) = 1.18 (Min = 0.77, Max = 1.73), gelotophilia Mdn(a) = 1.29 (Min = 0.80, Max = 2.28), katagelasticism Mdn(a) = 1.29 (Min = 0.63, Max = 2.14). Similarly, discrimination parameters for the three subscales in the Taiwanese Chinese sample are as follows: gelotophobia Mdn(a) = 1.34 (Min = 0.81, Max = 1.91), gelotophilia Mdn(a) = 1.41 (Min = 0.62, Max = 2.55), katagelasticism Mdn(a) = 1.31 (Min = 0.59, Max = 2.12). Details regarding the discrimination and threshold parameters for each item are provided in Appendix A.

3.2. Test Information Function (TIF)

The evaluation of TIFs for the Canadian English (Figure 1) and Taiwanese Chinese (Figure 2) samples showed similar measurement precision across the latent trait (i.e., θ). In the Canadian sample, the information and associated standard errors of measurement (SEs) showed precise measurement for respondents falling approximately within −1.5 to +3.0, −2.0 to +1.5, and −1.0 to +3.0 of the gelotophobia, gelotophilia, and katagelasticism dimensions, respectively, in the latent trait continuum. This is indicated by the maximum I and minimum SE (Figure 1 and Figure 2 for the graphical representation of the TIF). In the Taiwanese Chinese sample, the TIF for gelotophobia, gelotophilia, and katagelasticism showed precise measurement of θ from roughly −1.5 to +3.0, −3.0 to +1.5, and −1.5 to +3.0, respectively, as evidenced by the maximum I and minimum SE.

3.3. DIF Subgroup Analyses

To ascertain whether DIF biases existed when Chinese participants completed the English version of the PhoPhiKat-45, subgroup analyses were conducted within the English Canadian sample. This group was further divided into a group who identified as European White born in Canada (N = 567; 77.4% females; Mage = 18.40; SD = 1.98) and a group who identified as Chinese born in China, Taiwan, or Hong Kong (N = 180; 72.8% females; Mage = 18.73, SD = 1.17). The same criterion threshold of McFadden’s R2 > 0.035 and 10% change in proportional β was used to examine DIF. No significant DIF was identified across the three subscales of gelotophobia, gelotophilia, and katagelasticism.

3.4. DIF and Effect Sizes

The results from the ordinal logistic regression for DIF detections when comparing the Canadian English and Taiwanese Chinese versions are displayed in Table 1. Among the 14 gelotophobia items, 2 (i.e., items 37 and 43) were flagged as DIF items with the McFadden’s pseudo R2 criterion. Among the 15 gelotophilia items, no items were flagged for DIF with the McFadden’s pseudo R2 criterion. Among the 15 katagelasticism items, three items were flagged for DIF with the McFadden’s pseudo R2 criterion. However, all values are categorized with negligible effect sizes. McFadden’s pseudo R2 statistics were negligible for all items (<0.13) with the largest only at 0.13 in pseudo R2 between Models 1 and 3 for item 21. For a proportional 10% β change, item 21 was the only item with a change greater than 10%. In the item response function of item 21, for every response category, the solid line (Canadian English) was located to the left of the corresponding dashed line (Taiwanese Chinese), suggesting that over θ, the Canadian English sample have a greater probability of selecting a higher response option than the Taiwanese Chinese sample for this particular item (Figure 3).

4. Discussion

The purpose of this study was to provide evidence for the psychometric properties of the PhoPhiKat-45 in English and Chinese in two independent samples collected from Canada and Taiwan. Using IRT parameters for the Canadian English and Taiwanese Chinese versions, items were well-distributed across the latent continuum and showed high discrimination parameters allowing differentiation across different levels of gelotophobia, gelotophilia, and katagelasticism. Item characteristic curves for each individual item demonstrated high discrimination parameters that were well spread across the latent continuum for gelotophobia, gelotophilia, and katagelasticism, respectively measured. Notably, one item (i.e., item 13 “I believe that I make involuntarily a funny impression on others”) was excluded from the gelotophobia latent trait analysis due to a negative slope identified that precluded further analyses. The English PhoPhiKat-45 measure was translated from the German version and this item may be read by English speakers as making a funny impression or appearing hilarious or amusing in front of others naturally. Although the Taiwanese Chinese version was translated from the English version, the item performed well in the Taiwanese sample. Future studies should investigate whether this item may be removed or modified for the English measure of the PhoPhiKat-45.
The results showed that single test reliability values were acceptable for all three dimensions of gelotophobia, gelotophilia, and katagelasticism. The measurement precision of the test was evaluated using the TIF [67,68,69]. Interestingly, both the English and Chinese versions of the measure show that the scale does not capture extremely low ends of gelotophobia and katagelasticism and the extremely high end of gelotophilia. It is worth noting that both gelotophobia and katagelasticism are quasi-traits, such that both traits are unipolar constructs intended to measure only the presence or absence of the trait [2,70]. As such, the low end of the gelotophobia spectrum suggests the absence of fear of being laughed at rather than the presence of gelotophilia. These findings corroborate Ruch and Proyer’s [2] initial conceptualization of gelotophobia, that it is not a bipolar trait which would entail both extreme ends of the spectrum as separate entities (i.e., gelotophobia and gelotophilia). These results suggest that the absence of gelotophobia (i.e., no fear of being laughed at) does not indicate presence of gelotophilia (i.e., the joy of being laughed at). As such, both measures are needed for accurate measurement of these separate constructs.
Measurement invariance using DIF was also conducted to investigate whether meaningful interpretation of mean group comparisons may be made without significant biases. At the single-item level, five items were flagged for DIF in the Taiwanese Chinese version against the original English version. Only one item (i.e., item 21 “some people set themselves up for one to make fun at them”) analyzed has 10% change in β and over 0.13 in McFadden’s pseudo R2. Following an approach by Lau, Chiesi, and Saklofske [32] and Li et al. [71], the Canadian sample who completed the measure was subdivided into those who identified as European White born in Canada and those who identified as Chinese born in China, Hong Kong, or Taiwan. In the subgroup analyses, no evidence for DIF was found, suggesting that the DIF identified in the Taiwanese Chinese and English versions are more likely linked to subtle shifts in meaning with the translation process rather than the cultural concepts covered by the item content. Cultural expressions of affect, emotions, and behaviours vary cross cultural groups, which may lead to cross-cultural comparison biases [72]. Overall, there is support for using this scale to assess gelotophobia, gelotophilia, and katagelasticism with Chinese university students in either Chinese or English languages. DIF findings were mostly negligible and do not appear to reflect attainable differences in research or clinical practice.
Several limitations from the present investigation should be addressed in future studies. First, both the Canadian and Taiwanese samples of undergraduate university students represent young and well-educated individuals. Future research should replicate these findings to investigate whether findings are generalizable across age groups (e.g., youth, older adults), clinical status (e.g., those diagnosed with physical illnesses or mental health disorders), and socioeconomic background. Second, although the present study included an extensive assessment of individual item performance of the PhoPhiKat-45, future studies will benefit from investigating whether these variables have the same predictive validity across cultures. Specifically, Chinese individuals conceptualize the value of humour in everyday life differently than those from Western cultures given the early historical and cultural influences of Confucianism, Taoism, and Buddhism (for a review see Yue [17]). With Chinese individuals respecting authority and seniority more than Canadians, Chinese individuals tend to be more careful and conservative when using humour [17]. Whereas a serious disposition may be associated with lower well-being in Western cultures, seriousness is positively associated with well-being in China [73,74,75,76]. For example, Singaporean students in Nevo, Nevo, and Yin’s [77] study represented more conventional traits in relation to jokes and humour, such as aggressiveness, contrast to students in the United States, who joked more about sexual items. In addition, they did not use humour as a way to cope unlike other countries studied, such as Israel, demonstrating that a culture’s values can impact an individual’s humour [77]. With this in mind, future studies should examine whether some levels of gelotophobia may be protective for the individual in a culture where humour may be avoided or seen as distasteful [17,78]. Lastly, the present study reported the proportion of DIF items that may affect group means, but there are other methods such as the reporting of linking errors (i.e., robust linking methods [79]) and DIF variance. Future studies should explore these avenues to enable the assessment of systematic biases between countries.

5. Conclusions

Overall, IRT and DIF analyses conducted on the English and Taiwanese Chinese versions of the PhoPhiKat-45 demonstrated strong psychometric properties for assessing gelotophobia, gelotophilia, and katagelasticism. With the exception of one item, cross-cultural comparisons of the measure did not present biases with significant effect sizes for the individual item characteristics. Through a subgroup analysis, it is suggested that translations may alter item meanings rather than cultural concepts. However, variance across cross cultural groups may result in comparison biases, such as differences in the conceptualization of humour, including cultural influences, trait representation, and personal coping methods. Findings from this investigation support the utility of the PhoPhiKat-45 English and Chinese versions, and support the generalizability of gelotophobia, gelotophilia, and katagelasticism measures across different languages.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: C.L., F.C., H.-C.C., Y.-C.C., W.R., R.P., F.B., D.H.S. and J.T.-M.; methodology: C.L., H.-C.C., Y.-C.C., W.R. and R.P.; software: C.L.; data collection: C.L., H.-C.C., Y.-C.C., W.R., R.P. and D.H.S.; formal analysis: C.L. and F.C.; writing—original draft preparation: C.L., T.S., F.C., L.C.Q., D.H.S., and J.T.-M.; writing—review and editing: C.L., T.S., F.C., L.C.Q., H.-C.C., Y.-C.C., W.R., R.P., F.B., D.H.S. and J.T.-M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Ethical review and approval were sought from the institutions where the samples were collected from. All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards, and approved by the Non-Medical Research Ethics Board of the University of Western Ontario (protocol code 112943, 19 November 2018).

Informed Consent Statement

This study was conducted according to the American Psychological Association (APA) and National Association of Psychology ethical standards for the treatment of human subjects. Data collection was anonymous and involved no identifying information and no medical treatment. Participants were informed that their participation was voluntary, that they could leave the study at any time, and that their data would be treated anonymously. In addition, they were informed that by starting the survey they would be considered to have read and accepted the informed consent. Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available.

Acknowledgments

The first author would like to thank Canadian Institutes of Health Research, Mitacs, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health Discovery and Womenmind program, and Mental Health Research Canada for supporting the first author’s postdoctoral research program. The scholarship awards did not participate in study conceptualization, data collection, data analysis, or manuscript preparation. The authors would like to thank Meaghan Elizabeth Mosier-Farquharson for her help in organizing the appendices.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Gelotophobia item response theory parameters for the Canadian English version.
Table A1. Gelotophobia item response theory parameters for the Canadian English version.
ItemF1S_X2 (df)ab1b2b3
1. When they laugh in my presence I get suspicious.0.5380.77 (73)1.06−1.520.63.1
4. I avoid displaying myself in public because I fear
that people could become aware of my insecurity
and could make fun of me.
0.6862.2 (64)1.58−1.14−0.211.81
7. When strangers laugh in my presence I often relate
it to me personally.
0.5495.7 (71)1.10−1.480.462.75
10. When others make joking remarks about me I feel
being paralyzed.
0.7170.13 (61)1.73−0.820.832.5
16. I control myself strongly in order not to attract
negative attention so I do not make a ridiculous impression.
0.41101.46 (76)0.77−3.86−1.052.18
19. When I have made an embarrassing impression
somewhere, I avoid the place thereafter.
0.59110.88 (67)1.26−2.29−0.431.82
22. Some people set themselves up for one to make fun at them.0.5385.31 (74)1.06−1.231.133.29
25. It takes me very long to recover from having been laughed at.0.7147.68 (62)1.72−0.740.792.22
28. Especially when I feel relatively unconcerned, the
risk is high for me to attract negative attention and
appear peculiar to others.
0.4481.09 (74)0.84−1.841.134.10
31. It is difficult for me to hold eye contact because I
fear being assessed in a disparaging way.
0.6365.0 (67)1.37−0.890.501.98
34. Although I frequently feel lonely, I have the tendency
not to share social activities in order to protect
myself from derision.
0.6368.71 (66)1.39−1.040.632.32
37. When I have made a fool of myself in front of
others I grow completely stiff and lose my ability to
behave adequately.
0.6763.07 (64)1.55−0.940.742.63
40. While dancing I feel uneasy because I am convinced
that those watching me assess me as being ridiculous.
0.4763.45 (77)0.91−1.86−0.251.57
43. If I did not fear making a fool of myself I would
speak much more in public.
0.5276.98 (72)1.02−2.31−0.541.36
Table A2. Gelotophilia item response theory parameters for the Canadian English version.
Table A2. Gelotophilia item response theory parameters for the Canadian English version.
ItemF1S_X2 (df)ab1b2b3
2. When I am with other people, I enjoy making jokes
at my own expense to make the others laugh.
0.6280.39 (69)1.35−2.63−0.971.05
5. I do not hesitate telling friends or acquaintances
something embarrassing or a misfortune that happened
to me, even at the risk of being laughed at.
0.6965.24 (64)1.62−2.40−1.050.62
8. There is no difference for me whether people laugh
at me or laugh with me.
0.45102.66 (78)0.85−1.21.143.45
11. I enjoy it if other people laugh at me.0.6089.81 (68)1.29−1.00.972.98
14. I am the joker in my circle of friends, who entertains
the others (often with jokes at my own expense).
0.5860.56 (72)1.22−1.92−1.101.97
17. I enjoy it if other people poke fun at me since this
might also be a sign of recognition.
0.5868.86 (71)1.22−1.74−0.012.55
20. If someone caught me on a camera while something
embarrassing or a misfortune happen to me, I
would not mind, if s/he send the tape to a television
show that broadcast such videos.
0.5077.56 (76)0.99−0.361.043.25
23. I have talent for being a comedian, cabaret artist or clown.0.4391.03 (80)0.80−1.520.733.34
26. For raising laughs I pleasurably make the most out
of embarrassments or misfortunes that happen to
me which other people would be ashamed of.
0.7453.61 (59)1.87−1.55−0.191.56
29. I enjoy contributing to the open laughter of others
by telling them embarrassing things or misfortunes
that happened to me.
0.8070.03 (52)2.28−2.0−0.820.96
32. When I am with other people and something embarrassing
happens to me (e.g., a slip of the tongue
or a misfortune) I am more pleased than angry and
laugh along with it.
0.5878.63 (70)1.22−2.82−1.01.41
35. If I drop a clanger, I enjoy it a little because I can
hardly wait to tell my friends about this misfortune.
0.6272.32 (68)1.33−2.22−0.312.0
38. I do not mind telling something embarrassing in a
group that happened to me if I know that the others
will find it funny.
0.7672.86 (55)1.96−2.60−1.300.57
41. Nothing much could happen to me that I would be
so ashamed that I would not tell it others.
0.4885.74 (78)0.94−1.70.362.84
44. My friends know me for not being ashamed of telling
them of embarrassing situations that happened to me.
0.6996.57 (62)1.64−1.96−0.491.24
Table A3. Katagelasticism item response theory parameters for the Canadian English version.
Table A3. Katagelasticism item response theory parameters for the Canadian English version.
ItemF1S_X2 (df)ab1b2b3
3. I enjoy exposing others and I am happy when they
get laughed at.
0.7076.12 (65)1.68−0.420.892.39
6. Often, disputes emerged because of funny remarks
or jokes that I make about other people.
0.6968.69 (61)1.61−0.171.292.73
9. When related to making jokes or funny remarks
about other people I rather follow the motto ‘‘An
eye for an eye, a tooth for a tooth’’ than ‘‘If someone
strikes you on the left cheek, offer him the other also.’’
0.43110.14 (78)0.81−2.350.143.10
12. It has happened that humorless persons have broken
o¤ their friendship with me or at least threatened
me to do so, because I overdid ridiculing them
over of something embarrassing or a misfortune
that happened to them.
0.6185.38 (65)1.320.461.763.22
15. If other people poke fun at me than I pay them
back in the same way—but more so.
0.6079.07 (69)1.29−1.530.352.55
18. If it is for entertaining other people it is justified to
make jokes or funny remarks that might be painful
or mean about other people.
0.7875.68 (55)2.14−0.120.962.35
21. Some people set themselves up for one to make fun at them.0.48100.0 (76)0.92−2.70−0.862.20
24. Since it is only fun, I do not see any problems in
compromising others in a funny way.
0.7674.52 (59)2.02−0.790.642.33
27. Laughing at others is part of life. People who do not
like to be laughed at just should fight back.
0.6580.93 (68)1.45−1.330.502.34
30. If I am with a group of people and I am the only
one that notices that someone has done something
embarrassing or that something embarrassing happened
to him/her, than I do not hesitate to tell the
others about it.
0.6374.32 (69)1.37−0.980.762.74
33. I do not have a bad conscience when I laugh at the
misfortunes (e.g., slips of the tongue) of others.
0.35109.39 (80)0.63−3.76−0.652.86
36. Nothing is better than stealing a pretenders thunder
with a funny remark.
0.47102.34 (74)0.92−2.150.482.99
39. It is easier for me to laugh at others than to make
fun of myself.
0.35117.49 (82)0.64−3.240.223.53
42. In my circle of friends I am known for my ‘‘sharp
tongue’’ (e.g., making cynical remarks and jokes about others).
0.5672.01 (75)1.15−1.170.662.34
45. I, myself notice that I sometimes cross the line and
jokes that others experience as painful started harmless
(at least from the viewpoint of demure people).
0.5965.64 (72)1.25−1.090.322.26
Table A4. Gelotophobia item response theory parameters for the Taiwanese Chinese version.
Table A4. Gelotophobia item response theory parameters for the Taiwanese Chinese version.
ItemF1S_X2 (df)ab1b2b3
1. When they laugh in my presence I get suspicious.0.5297.671.05−2.880.163.58
4. I avoid displaying myself in public because I fear
that people could become aware of my insecurity
and could make fun of me.
0.6949.681.63−2.178−0.071.99
7. When strangers laugh in my presence I often relate
it to me personally.
0.5659.511.15−1.621.003.83
10. When others make joking remarks about me I feel
being paralyzed.
0.6170.941.3−2.070.873.27
16. I control myself strongly in order not to attract
negative attention so I do not make a ridiculous impression.
0.6266.541.34−2.3−0.022.46
19. When I have made an embarrassing impression
somewhere, I avoid the place thereafter.
0.6359.201.05−2.880.012.84
22. Some people set themselves up for one to make fun at them.0.5346.911.08−1.572.255.34
25. It takes me very long to recover from having been laughed at.0.5458.271.35−1.251.383.33
28. Especially when I feel relatively unconcerned, the
risk is high for me to attract negative attention and
appear peculiar to others.
0.6287.020.81−2.281.675.25
31. It is difficult for me to hold eye contact because I
fear being assessed in a disparaging way.
0.4358.351.61−0.941.222.82
34. Although I frequently feel lonely, I have the tendency
not to share social activities in order to protect
myself from derision.
0.6949.491.82−0.781.282.94
37. When I have made a fool of myself in front of
others I grow completely stiff and lose my ability to
behave adequately.
0.7365.971.74−1.990.192.48
40. While dancing I feel uneasy because I am convinced
that those watching me assess me as being ridiculous.
0.7270.821.05−2.3−0.082.43
43. If I did not fear making a fool of myself I would
speak much more in public.
0.5261.571.91−1.410.542.36
Table A5. Gelotophilia item response theory parameters for the Taiwanese Chinese version.
Table A5. Gelotophilia item response theory parameters for the Taiwanese Chinese version.
ItemF1S_X2 (df)ab1b2b3
2. When I am with other people, I enjoy making jokes
at my own expense to make the others laugh.
0.6650.781.52−2.93−0.372.27
5. I do not hesitate telling friends or acquaintances
something embarrassing or a misfortune that happened
to me, even at the risk of being laughed at.
0.6644.631.49−3.14−1.371.08
8. There is no difference for me whether people laugh
at me or laugh with me.
0.3479.930.62−3.410.866.00
11. I enjoy it if other people laugh at me.0.5362.901.05−2.610.574.14
14. I am the joker in my circle of friends, who entertains
the others (often with jokes at my own expense).
0.6454.021.41−2.86−3.342.06
17. I enjoy it if other people poke fun at me since this
might also be a sign of recognition.
0.5662.191.16−2.86−0.073.54
20. If someone caught me on a camera while something
embarrassing or a misfortune happen to me, I
would not mind, if s/he send the tape to a television
show that broadcast such videos.
0.3564.310.64−1.422.416.91
23. I have talent for being a comedian, cabaret artist or clown.0.5361.891.07−2.350.283.03
26. For raising laughs I pleasurably make the most out
of embarrassments or misfortunes that happen to
me which other people would be ashamed of.
0.6547.461.45−2.74−0.272.45
29. I enjoy contributing to the open laughter of others
by telling them embarrassing things or misfortunes
that happened to me.
0.8351.98−2.552.08−0.511.72
32. When I am with other people and something embarrassing
happens to me (e.g., a slip of the tongue
or a misfortune) I am more pleased than angry and
laugh along with it.
0.6144.501.30−3.87−1.651.47
35. If I drop a clanger, I enjoy it a little because I can
hardly wait to tell my friends about this misfortune.
0.7745.792.08−2.10−0.401.53
38. I do not mind telling something embarrassing in a
group that happened to me if I know that the others
will find it funny.
0.8058.362.24−2.48−1.041.37
41. Nothing much could happen to me that I would be
so ashamed that I would not tell it others.
0.6849.801.57−2.64−0.062.05
44. My friends know me for not being ashamed of telling
them of embarrassing situations that happened to me.
0.43153.240.82−3.53−0.333.24
Table A6. Katagelasticism item response theory parameters for the Taiwanese Chinese version.
Table A6. Katagelasticism item response theory parameters for the Taiwanese Chinese version.
ItemF1S_X2 (df)ab1b2b3
3. I enjoy exposing others and I am happy when they
get laughed at.
0.7639.522.0−0.581.242.96
6. Often, disputes emerged because of funny remarks
or jokes that I make about other people.
0.7045.051.69−0.711.513.43
9. When related to making jokes or funny remarks
about other people I rather follow the motto ‘‘An
eye for an eye, a tooth for a tooth’’ than ‘‘If someone
strikes you on the right cheek, offer him the other also.’’
0.51102.701.01−2.010.382.93
12. It has happened that humorless persons have broken
o¤ their friendship with me or at least threatened
me to do so, because I overdid ridiculing them
over of something embarrassing or a misfortune
that happened to them.
0.4454.690.830.203.206.19
15. If other people poke fun at me than I pay them
back in the same way—but more so.
0.5569.241.12−1.311.333.49
18. If it is for entertaining other people it is justified to
make jokes or funny remarks that might be painful
or mean about other people.
0.6362.771.38−0.451.773.94
21. Some people set themselves up for one to make fun at them.0.7856.532.12−0.791.262.84
24. Since it is only fun, I do not see any problems in
compromising others in a funny way.
0.6292.681.35−1.670.543.25
27. Laughing at others is part of life. People who do not
like to be laughed at just should fight back.
0.5560.811.13−1.780.933.85
30. If I am with a group of people and I am the only
one that notices that someone has done something
embarrassing or that something embarrassing happened
to him/her, than I do not hesitate to tell the others about it.
0.5358.261.06−1.691.364.05
33. I do not have a bad conscience when I laugh at the
misfortunes (e.g., slips of the tongue) of others.
0.5847.341.23−0.572.163.88
36. Nothing is better than stealing a pretenders thunder
with a funny remark.
0.7570.881.92−0.901.202.80
39. It is easier for me to laugh at others than to make
fun of myself.
0.6160.041.31−1.590.783.19
42. In my circle of friends I am known for my ‘‘sharp
tongue’’ (e.g., making cynical remarks and jokes about others).
0.6484.171.41−0.521.211.98
45. I, myself notice that I sometimes cross the line and
jokes that others experience as painful started harmless
(at least from the viewpoint of demure people).
0.32115.560.58−4.34−0.983.68

References

  1. Ruch, W. Psychology of humor. In The Primer of Humor Research; Ruch, W., Raskin, V., Eds.; Mouton de Gruyter: Berlin, Germany; New York, NY, USA, 2008; Volume 8, pp. 17–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Ruch, W.; Proyer, R.T. Extending the study of gelotophobia: On gelotophiles and katagelasticists. Humor Int. J. Humor Res. 2009, 22, 183–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Platt, T. Emotional responses to ridicule and teasing: Should gelotophobes react differently? Humor 2008, 21, 105–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Torres-Marín, J.; Navarro-Carrillo, G.; Eid, M.; Carretero-Dios, H. Humor styles, perceived threat, funniness of covid-19 memes, and affective mood in the early stages of COVID-19 lockdown. J. Happiness Stud. 2022, 23, 2541–2561. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Hofmann, J.; Ruch, W.; Proyer, R.T.; Platt, T.; Gander, F. Assessing dispositions toward ridicule and laughter in the workplace: Adapting and validating the PhoPhiKat-9 questionnaire. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Ruch, W.; Proyer, R.T. PhoPhiKat-45. Open Test Archive. 2014. Available online: https://www.testarchiv.eu/en/test/9006740 (accessed on 17 May 2022).
  7. Torres-Marín, J.; Proyer, R.T.; López-Benítez, R.; Carretero-Dios, H. Assessing individual differences in the way people deal with ridicule and being laughed at: The Spanish form of the PhoPhiKat-45. Curr. Psychol 2019, 40, 2665–2679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Chen, H.; Chan, Y.; Ruch, W.; Proyer, R. Evaluating the reliability and validity of a traditional Chinese version of the PhoPhiKat-45. Psychol. Test. 2011, 58, 119–145. [Google Scholar]
  9. Dursun, P.; Dalğar, I.; Brauer, K.; Ercüment, Y.; Proyer, R.T. Assessing dispositions towards ridicule and being laughed at: Development and initial validation of the Turkish PhoPhiKat-45. Curr. Psychol. Res. Rev. 2020, 39, 101–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Platt, T.; Ruch, W. The emotions of gelotophobes: Shameful, fearful, and joyless? Humor 2009, 22, 91–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Proyer, R.T.; Ruch, W.; Chen, G. Gelotophobia: Life satisfaction and happiness across cultures. Humor Int. J. Humor Res. 2012, 25, 23–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Stefanenko, E.A.; Ivanova, E.M.; Enikolopov, S.N. Russian adaptation of the PhoPhiKat questionnaire of gelotophobia, gelotophilia, and katagelasticism in adolescents. Psychol. Sci. Educ. 2016, 21, 61–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Yu, F.; Guo, Y. Revision of PhoPhiKat questionnaire. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol. 2009, 17, 557–558, 565. Available online: https://www.proquest.com/psycinfo/docview/742968230/36A7B76F162E46A5PQ/1?acountid=1511 (accessed on 18 May 2022).
  14. Proyer, R.T.; Ruch, W.; Ali, N.S.; Al-Olimat, H.S.; Amemiya, T.; Adal, T.A.; Ansari, S.A.; Arhar, Š.; Asem, G.; Baudin, N.; et al. Breaking ground in cross-cultural research on the fear of being laughed at (gelotophobia): A multi-national study involving 73 countries. Humor Int. J. Humor Res. 2009, 22, 253–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Lampert, M.D.; Isaacson, K.L.; Lyttle, J. Cross-cultural variation in gelotophobia within the United States. Psychol. Test Assess. Model. 2010, 52, 202–216. [Google Scholar]
  16. Proyer, R.T.; Ruch, W. Editorial: Dispositions towards ridicule and being laughed at: Current research on gelotophobia, gelotophilia, and katagelastism. Psychol. Test Assess. Model. 2010, 52, 49–59. [Google Scholar]
  17. Yue, X. Humor and Chinese Culture: A Psychological Perspective; Routledge: Oxfordshire, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  18. Reise, S.P.; Widaman, K.F.; Pugh, R.H. Confirmatory factor analysis and item response theory: Two approaches for exploring measurement invariance. Psychol. Bull. 1993, 114, 552–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Vandenberg, R.J.; Lance, C.E. A review and synthesis of the measurement invariance literature: Suggestions, practices, and recommendations for organizational research. Organ. Res. Methods 2000, 3, 4–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Vijver, F.V.d.; Poortinga, Y.H. Conceptual and methodological issues in adapting tests. In Adapting Educational and Psychological Tests for Cross-Cultural Assessment; Hambleton, R.K., Merenda, P.F., Spielberger, C.D., Eds.; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2004; pp. 39–63. [Google Scholar]
  21. Mellenbergh, G.J. Item bias and item response theory. Int. J. Educ. Res. 1989, 13, 127–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Meredith, W. Measurement invariance, factor analysis and factorial invariance. Psychometrika 1993, 58, 525–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Jiang, T.; Li, H.; Hou, Y. Cultural differences in humour perception, usage, and implications. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Samson, A.C.; Proyer, R.; Ceschi, G.; Pedrini, P.P.; Ruch, W. The fear of being laughed at in Switzerland regional differences and the role of positive psychology. Swiss. J. Psychol. 2011, 70, 53–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Hiranandani, N.A.; Yue, X.D. Humor styles, gelotophobia and self-esteem among Chinese and Indian university students. Asian J. Soc. Psychol. 2014, 17, 319–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Liu, C.; Chiu, F.; Chen, H.; Lin, C. Helpful but insufficient: Incremental theory on challenge-confronting tendencies for students who fear being laughed at. Motiv. Emot. 2013, 38, 367–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Davies, C. Humour theory and the fear of being laughed at. Humor Int. J. Humor Res. 2009, 22, 49–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Chen, H.; Chan, Y.; Ruch, W.; Proyer, R.T. Laughing at others and being laughed at in Taiwan and Switzerland: A cross-cultural perspective. In Humour in Chinese Life and Culture: Resistance and Control in Modern Times; Davis, J.M., Chey, J., Eds.; Scholarship Online: Hong Kong, China, 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Lau, C.; Chiesi, F.; Saklofske, D.H.; Yan, G.; Li, C. How essential is the essential resilience scale? Differential item functioning of Chinese and English versions and criterion validity. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2020, 155, 109666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Chen, G.; Liao, C.; Proyer, R.T.; Ruch, W. A summary on research in gelotophobia. Chin. J. Health Psychol. 2009, 17, 893–895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Torres-Marín, J.; Proyer, R.T.; López-Benítez, R.; Brauer, K.; Carretero-Dios, H. Beyond the big five as predictors of dispositions toward ridicule and being laughed at: The HEXACO model and the dark triad. Scand. J. Psychol. 2019, 60, 473–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Lau, C.; Chiesi, F.; Saklofske, D.H. The state-trait model of cheerfulness: Tests of measurement invariance and latent mean differences in European and Chinese Canadian students. Eur. J. Psychol. 2022, 18, 142–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Embretson, S.E.; Hershberger, S. (Eds.) The New Rules of Measurement: What Every Psychologist and Educator Should Know; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Hanson, B.A. Uniform DIF and DIF defined by differences in item response functions. J. Educ. Behav. Stat. 1998, 23, 244–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Reeve, B. Item Response Theory [IRT]. EQOLWR 2014, 3415–3423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Van der Linden, W.J.; Hambleton, R.K. (Eds.) Handbook of Modern Item Response Theory; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  37. Zanon, C.; Hutz, C.S.; Yoo, H.; Hambleton, R.K. An application of item response theory to psychological test development. Psicol. Reflexão Crítica 2016, 29, 18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Hambleton, R.K.; Swaninathan, H. Item Response Theory: Principles and Applications; Springer Science + Business Media, LLC: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  39. Reckase, M.D. Unifactor latent trait models applied to multifactor tests: Results and implications. J. Educ. Stat. 1979, 4, 207–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Berk, R.; Berk, R.A. (Eds.) Handbook of Methods for Detecting Test Bias; Johns Hopkins University Press: Baltimore, MD, USA, 1982. [Google Scholar]
  41. Chen, W.H.; Revicki, D. Differential Item Functioning (DIF). In Encyclopedia of Quality of Life and Well-Being Research; Michalos, A.C., Ed.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Harrison, D.A. Robustness of IRT parameter estimation to violations of the unidimensionality assumption. J. Educ. Stat. 1986, 11, 91–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Holland, P.W.; Thayer, D.T. Differential item performance and the Mantel-Haenszel procedure. In Test Validity; Wainer, H., Braun, H.I., Eds.; LEA: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1988; pp. 129–145. [Google Scholar]
  44. Thissen, D.; Steinberg, L.; Wainer, H. Use of item response theory in the study of group differences in the trace lines. In Test Validity; Routledge: Oxfordshire, UK, 1988; pp. 147–169. [Google Scholar]
  45. Brauer, K.; Proyer, R.T. Analyzing a German-language Expanded form of the PhoPhiKat-45: Psychometric properties, factorial structure, measurement invariance with the Likert-version, and self-peer convergence. J. Pers. Assess. 2021, 103, 267–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Ruch, W.; Proyer, R.T. The fear of being laughed at: Individual and group differences in gelotophobia. Humor Int. J. Humor Res. 2008, 21, 47–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Samejima, F. Estimation of latent ability using a response pattern of graded scores. Psychom. Monogr. Suppl. 1969, 34 Pt 2, 100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Orlando, M.; Thissen, D. Further Investigation of the Performance of S-X2: An Item Fit Index for Use with Dichotomous Item Response Theory Models. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 2003, 27, 289–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Bock, R.D.; Aitkin, M. Marginal maximum likelihood estimation of item parameters: Application of an EM algorithm. Psychometrika 1981, 46, 443–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Christensen, K.B.; Makransky, G.; Horton, M. Critical Values for Yen’s Q3: Identification of Local Dependence in the Rasch Model Using Residual Correlations. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 2017, 41, 178–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Marais, I.; Andrich, D. Formalizing dimension and response violations of local independence in the unidimensional Rasch model. J. Appl. Meas. 2008, 9, 200–215. [Google Scholar]
  52. Ziegler, M.; Hagemann, D. Testing the unidimensionality of items: Pitfalls and loopholes. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2015, 31, 231–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Baker, F.B.; Kim, S.H. Item Response Theory: Parameter Estimation Techniques, 2nd ed.; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Crane, P.K.; Gibbons, L.E.; Ocepek-Welikson, K.; Cook, K.; Cella, D.; Narasimhalu, K.; Hays, R.D.; Teresi, J.A. A comparison of three sets of criteria for determining the presence of differential item functioning using ordinal logistic regression. Qual. Life Res. 2007, 16, 69–84. Available online: http://www.jstor.org/stable/40212574 (accessed on 18 May 2022). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Clauser, B.E.; Nungester, R.J.; Mazor, K.; Ripkey, D. A comparison of alternative matching strategies for DIF detection in tests that are multidimensional. J. Educ. Meas. 1996, 33, 202–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Swaminathan, H.; Rogers, H.J. Detecting differential item functioning using logistic regression procedures. J. Educ. Meas. 1990, 27, 361–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Choi, S.W.; Gibbons, L.E.; Crane, P.K. lordif: An R Package for Detecting Differential Item Functioning Using Iterative Hybrid Ordinal Logistic Regression/Item Response Theory and Monte Carlo Simulations. J. Stat. Softw. 2011, 39, 1–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  58. Crane, P.K.; van Belle, G.; Larson, E.B. Test bias in a cognitive test: Differential item functioning in the CASI. Stat. Med. 2004, 23, 241–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Jodoin, M.G.; Gierl, M.J. Evaluating Type I Error and Power Rates Using an Effect Size Measure with the Logistic Regression Procedure for DIF Detection. Appl. Meas. Educ. 2001, 14, 329–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Jeong, H.J.; Lee, W.C. Does differential item functioning occur across respondents’ characteristics in safety attitudes questionnaire? Biometics Biostat. Int. J. 2016, 4, 103–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Allison, P.D. Event History and Survival Analysis, 2nd ed.; SAGE Publications, Inc.: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Kvalseth, T.O. Cautionary Note about R2. Am. Stat. 1985, 39, 279–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Crane, P.K.; Gibbons, L.E.; Jolley, L.; van Belle, G. Differential Item Functioning Analysis with Ordinal Logistic Regression Techniques: DIFdetect and difwithpar. Med. Care 2006, 44, S115–S123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Pfadt, J.M.; van den Bergh, D.; Sijtsma, K.; Moshagen, M.; Wagenmakers, E.-J.; Bayesian estimation of single-test reliability coefficients. Multivar. Behav. Res. 2021. in press. Available online: https://psyarxiv.com/exg2y (accessed on 1 December 2022).
  65. JASP Team. JASP (Version 0.16.3). 2020. Available online: https://jasp-stats.org (accessed on 1 December 2022).
  66. Chalmers, R.P. mirt: A multidimensional item response theory package for the R environment. J. Stat. Softw. 2012, 48, 1–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Embretson, S.E. Item Response Theory Models and Spurious Interaction Effects in Factorial ANOVA Designs. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 1996, 20, 201–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Embretson, S.E.; Reise, S.P. Item Response Theory for Psychologists; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers: New York, NY, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  69. Hambleton, R.K.; Swaminathan, H.; Rogers, H.J. Fundamentals of Item Response Theory; Sage Publications, Inc.: London, UK, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  70. Reise, S.P.; Waller, N.G. Item response theory and clinical measurement. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2009, 5, 27–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  71. Li, T.; Saklofske, D.H.; Bowden, S.C.; Yan, G.; Fung, T.S. The Measurement Invariance of the Wong and Law Emotional Intelligence Scale (WLEIS) Across Three Chinese University Student Groups from Canada and China. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 2012, 30, 439–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Peña, E.D. Lost in translation: Methodological considerations in cross-cultural research. Child Dev. 2007, 78, 1255–1264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Carretero-Dios, H.; Benítez, I.; Delgado-Rico, E.; Ruch, W.; López-Benítez, R. Temperamental basis of sense of humour: The Spanish long form of the trait version of the State-Trait-Cheerfulness Inventory. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2014, 68, 77–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Lau, C.; Chiesi, F.; Saklofske, D.H.; Yan, G. What is the temperamental basis of humour like in China? A cross-national examination and validation of the standard version of the State–Trait Cheerfulness Inventory. Int. J. Psychol. 2020, 55, 264–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Ruch, W.; Köhler, G.; van Thriel, C. Assessing the “humorous temperament”: Construction of the facet and standard trait forms of the State-Trait-Cheerfulness Inventory—STCI. Humor Int. J. Humor Res. 1996, 9, 303–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  76. Ruch, W.; Köhler, G.; van Thriel, C. To be in good or bad humor: Construction of the state form of the State-Trait-Cheerfulness-Inventory—STCI. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1997, 22, 477–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  77. Nevo, O.; Nevo, B.; Yin, J.L.S. Singaporean humor: A cross-cultural, cross-gender comparison. J. Gen. Psychol. 2001, 128, 143–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Cheung, C.K.; Yue, X.D. Sojourn students’ humor styles as buffers to achieve resilience. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 2012, 36, 353–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Robitzsch, A. Robust and nonrobust linking of two groups for the Rasch Model with balanced and unbalanced random DIF: A comparative simulation study and the simultaneous assessment of standard errors and linking errors with resampling techniques. Symmetry 2021, 13, 2198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Phophikat-45 test information function for the Canadian Sample. Graphs from left to right depict (a) gelotophobia, (b) gelotophilia, and (c) katagelasticism, respectively, in the Canadian Sample. I represent information and θ represents latent trait. SE represents standard error.
Figure 1. Phophikat-45 test information function for the Canadian Sample. Graphs from left to right depict (a) gelotophobia, (b) gelotophilia, and (c) katagelasticism, respectively, in the Canadian Sample. I represent information and θ represents latent trait. SE represents standard error.
Ejihpe 13 00019 g001
Figure 2. Phophikat-45 test information function for the Taiwanese Chinese Sample. Graphs from left to right depict (a) gelotophobia, (b) gelotophilia, and (c) katagelasticism, respectively, in the Taiwanese Sample. I represent information and θ represents latent trait. SE represents standard error.
Figure 2. Phophikat-45 test information function for the Taiwanese Chinese Sample. Graphs from left to right depict (a) gelotophobia, (b) gelotophilia, and (c) katagelasticism, respectively, in the Taiwanese Sample. I represent information and θ represents latent trait. SE represents standard error.
Ejihpe 13 00019 g002
Figure 3. Differential item functioning in item 21 of the Katagelasticism scale. Black line represents Canadian English sample and dashed red line represents Taiwanese Chinese sample.
Figure 3. Differential item functioning in item 21 of the Katagelasticism scale. Black line represents Canadian English sample and dashed red line represents Taiwanese Chinese sample.
Ejihpe 13 00019 g003
Table 1. Likelihood ratio χ2 statistics, McFadden’s pseudo R2, and change in β between Canadian and Taiwanese samples.
Table 1. Likelihood ratio χ2 statistics, McFadden’s pseudo R2, and change in β between Canadian and Taiwanese samples.
ItemContentχ2 Statistic Δ β1 (%)McFadden’s Pseudo R2 DIF Identified?
Gelotophobia
Scale
χ 12χ 13χ 23Beta12McFadden12McFadden13McFadden23
1When they laugh in my presence I get suspicious.<0.001<0.0010.15483.590.01540.01570.0003No
4I avoid displaying myself in public because I fear that people could become aware of my insecurity and could make fun of me.<0.001<0.0010.06624.30 0.01650.01700.0005No
7When strangers laugh in my presence I often relate it to me personally.<0.0010.00300.57580.27 0.00180.0018<0.0001No
10When others make joking remarks about me I feel being paralyzed.<0.001<0.001<0.0014.630.01870.02210.0034No
16I control myself strongly in order not to attract negative attention so I do not make a ridiculous impression.<0.001<0.001<0.0010.41 0.01600.02140.0054No
19When I have made an embarrassing impression somewhere, I avoid the place thereafter.<0.001<0.0010.32720.10 0.00360.00370.0001No
22If someone has teased me in the past I cannot deal freely with him forever.<0.001<0.0010.00260.340.0020 0.00360.0016No
25It takes me very long to recover from having been laughed at.0.1872<0.001<0.0010.28 0.0003 0.00250.0022No
28Especially when I feel relatively unconcerned, the risk is high for me to attract negative attention and appear peculiar to others.0.64030.78580.60750.10 <0.0001 0.0001<0.0001 No
31It is difficult for me to hold eye contact because I fear being assessed in a disparaging way.<0.001<0.0010.51110.190.0040 0.00410.0001No
34Although I frequently feel lonely, I have the tendency not to share social activities in order to protect myself from derision.<0.001<0.0010.04390.010.00860.00920.0006No
37When I have made a fool of myself in front of others I grow completely stiff and lose my ability to behave adequately.<0.001<0.0010.02789.890.0445 0.04520.0008Yes
40While dancing I feel uneasy because I am convinced that those watching me assess me as being ridiculous.0.18490.38460.69540.180.00020.0003<0.0001 No
43If I did not fear making a fool of myself I would speak much more in public.<0.001<0.001<0.0014.940.0398 0.04590.0062Yes
Gelotophilia
2When I am with other people, I enjoy making jokes at my own expense to make the others laugh.<0.001<0.0010.53863.10 0.02540.02540.0001No
5I do not hesitate telling friends or acquaintances something embarrassing or a misfortune that happened to me, even at the risk of being laughed at.0.29410.36870.34420.07 0.0002 0.00030.0001No
8There is no difference for me whether people laugh
at me or laugh with me.
<0.001<0.0010.00182.19 0.0117 0.01320.0015No
11I enjoy it if other people laugh at me.<0.001<0.0010.00284.30 0.0237 0.02510.0014No
14I am the joker in my circle of friends, who entertains the others (often with jokes at my own expense).<0.001<0.0010.50871.03 0.0044 0.00450.0001No
17I enjoy it if other people poke fun at me since this might also be a sign of recognition.0.00370.00310.07670.34 0.0014 0.00190.0005No
20If someone caught me on a camera while something embarrassing or a misfortune happen to me, I would not mind, if s/he send the tape to a television show that broadcast such videos.0.1197<0.001<0.0010.21 0.0004 0.00390.0035No
23I have talent for being a comedian, cabaret artist or clown.<0.001<0.0010.08431.850.00790.00830.0004No
26For raising laughs I pleasurably make the most out of embarrassments or misfortunes that happen to me which other people would be ashamed of.0.0486<0.001<0.0010.160.00060.00340.0028No
29I enjoy contributing to the open laughter of others by telling them embarrassing things or misfortunes that happened to me.<0.001<0.0010.59343.830.02070.0208<0.0001No
32When I am with other people and something embarrassing happens to me (e.g., a slip of the tongue or a misfortune) I am more pleased than angry and laugh along with it.<0.001<0.0010.87391.240.00650.0065<0.0001No
35If I drop a clanger, I enjoy it a little because I can hardly wait to tell my friends about this misfortune.<0.001<0.001<0.0010.510.00300.01390.0109No
38I do not mind telling something embarrassing in a group that happened to me if I know that the others will find it funny.<0.001<0.0010.45534.910.02720.02730.0001No
41Nothing much could happen to me that I would be so ashamed that I would not tell it others.<0.001<0.0010.00072.670.01420.01590.0017No
44My friends know me for not being ashamed of telling them of embarrassing situations that happened to me.<0.001<0.001<0.0010.260.00490.01090.0059No
Katagelasticism
3I enjoy exposing others and I am happy when they get laughed at.0.07470.13970.38290.420.00050.00060.0001No
6Often, disputes emerged because of funny remarks or jokes that I make about other people.<0.001<0.0010.03903.550.01170.01250.0007No
9When related to making jokes or funny remarks about other people I rather follow the motto ‘‘An eye for an eye, a tooth for a tooth’’ than ‘‘If someone strikes you on the right cheek, offer him the other also.’’0.57180.00540.00150.15<0.00010.00160.0015No
12It has happened that humorless persons have broken o¤ their friendship with me or at least threatened me to do so, because I overdid ridiculing them over of something embarrassing or a misfortune that happened to them.<0.001<0.001<0.0011.960.00340.00680.0034No
15If other people poke fun at me than I pay them back in the same way—but more so.<0.001<0.0010.46360.350.01080.01080.0001No
18If it is for entertaining other people it is justified to make jokes or funny remarks that might be painful or mean about other people.0.0017<0.001<0.0010.930.00160.00830.0066No
21Some people set themselves up for one to make fun at them.<0.001<0.001<0.00113.390.11860.13080.0122Yes
24Since it is only fun, I do not see any problems in compromising others in a funny way.<0.001<0.001<0.0014.930.01780.02070.0029No
27Laughing at others is part of life. People who do not like to be laughed at just should fight back.0.75730.00450.00110.05<0.0010.00170.0017No
30If I am with a group of people and I am the only one that notices that someone has done something embarrassing or that something embarrassing happened to him/her, than I do not hesitate to tell the others about it.0.0560<0.001<0.0010.52<0.0010.00410.0035No
33I do not have a bad conscience when I laugh at the misfortunes (e.g., slips of the tongue) of others.<0.001<0.001<0.0017.950.11940.12640.0070Yes
36Nothing is better than stealing a pretenders thunder with a funny remark.<0.001<0.001<0.0010.910.02390.03100.0071No
39It is easier for me to laugh at others than to make fun of myself.<0.001<0.001<0.0010.450.00890.01390.0050No
42In my circle of friends I am known for my ‘‘sharp tongue’’ (e.g., making cynical remarks and jokes about others).<0.001<0.001<0.0010.540.00920.01110.0019No
45I, myself notice that I sometimes cross the line and jokes that others experience as painful started harmless (at least from the viewpoint of demure people).<0.001<0.001<0.0018.780.03200.04160.0096Yes
Note. χ 12, χ 23, and χ 13 represents the χ 2 likelihood ratio statistic between Models 1 and 2, Models 2 and 3, and Models 1 and 3, respectively. R12, R23, and R13 represent McFadden’s pseudo R2 from a comparison Models 1 and 2, Models 2 and 3, and Models 1 and 3, respectively; Δβ represents the change in β as a percentage
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Lau, C.; Swindall, T.; Chiesi, F.; Quilty, L.C.; Chen, H.-C.; Chan, Y.-C.; Ruch, W.; Proyer, R.; Bruno, F.; Saklofske, D.H.; et al. Cultural Differences in How People Deal with Ridicule and Laughter: Differential Item Functioning between the Taiwanese Chinese and Canadian English Versions of the PhoPhiKat-45. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2023, 13, 238-258. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe13020019

AMA Style

Lau C, Swindall T, Chiesi F, Quilty LC, Chen H-C, Chan Y-C, Ruch W, Proyer R, Bruno F, Saklofske DH, et al. Cultural Differences in How People Deal with Ridicule and Laughter: Differential Item Functioning between the Taiwanese Chinese and Canadian English Versions of the PhoPhiKat-45. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education. 2023; 13(2):238-258. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe13020019

Chicago/Turabian Style

Lau, Chloe, Taylor Swindall, Francesca Chiesi, Lena C. Quilty, Hsueh-Chih Chen, Yu-Chen Chan, Willibald Ruch, René Proyer, Francesco Bruno, Donald H. Saklofske, and et al. 2023. "Cultural Differences in How People Deal with Ridicule and Laughter: Differential Item Functioning between the Taiwanese Chinese and Canadian English Versions of the PhoPhiKat-45" European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education 13, no. 2: 238-258. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe13020019

APA Style

Lau, C., Swindall, T., Chiesi, F., Quilty, L. C., Chen, H. -C., Chan, Y. -C., Ruch, W., Proyer, R., Bruno, F., Saklofske, D. H., & Torres-Marín, J. (2023). Cultural Differences in How People Deal with Ridicule and Laughter: Differential Item Functioning between the Taiwanese Chinese and Canadian English Versions of the PhoPhiKat-45. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education, 13(2), 238-258. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe13020019

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop