The Moving Mandala: Exploring the Pro-Social Effects of Musical and Non-Musical Synchrony in Children in a Virtual World
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Methods
2.1. MR Movement Task
2.2. Prosociality Measures
3. Results
4. Discussion
4.1. Musical Prediction
4.2. Rhythmic Grouping
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Allen, R., & Heaton, P. (2010). Autism, music, and the therapeutic potential of music in alexithymia. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 27(4), 251–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Amos, P. (2013). Rhythm and timing in autism: Learning to dance. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 7, 27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Anshel, A., & Kipper, D. A. (1988). The influence of group singing on trust and cooperation. Journal of Music Therapy, 25(3), 145–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Atwood, S., Schachner, A., & Mehr, S. A. (2022). Expectancy effects threaten the inferential validity of synchrony-prosociality research. Open Mind, 6, 280–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cirelli, L. K. (2018). How interpersonal synchrony facilitates early pro-social behavior. Current Opinion in Psychology, 20, 35–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cirelli, L. K., Wan, S. J., & Trainor, L. J. (2014). Fourteen-month-old infants use interpersonal synchrony as a cue to direct helpfulness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 369, 20130400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cross, L., Atherton, G., Wilson, A. D., & Golonka, S. (2017). steps: Mental simulation of coordinated rhythmic movements effects on pro-sociality. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1798. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cross, L., Michael, J., Wilsdon, L., Henson, A., & Atherton, G. (2020). Still want to help? Interpersonal coordination’s effects on helping behaviour after a 24 h delay. Acta Psychologica, 206, 103062. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cross, L., Turgeon, M., & Atherton, G. (2019a). How moving together binds us together: The social consequences of interpersonal entrainment and group processes. Open Psychology, 1(1), 273–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cross, L., Turgeon, M., & Atherton, G. (2019b). Moving with the in-crowd: Cooperation and interpersonal entrainment in in-vs. outgroups. Current Psychology, 1–8. [Google Scholar]
- Cross, L., Wilson, A. D., & Golonka, S. (2016). How moving together brings us together: When coordinated rhythmic movement affects cooperation. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1983. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Crossey, B. P., Atherton, G., & Cross, L. (2021). Lost in the crowd: Imagining walking in synchrony with a crowd increases affiliation and deindividuation. PLoS ONE, 16(7), e0254017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Frith, U., & Frith, C. (2011). Reputation management: In autism, generosity is its own reward. Current Biology, 21(24), R994–R995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gebauer, L., Skewes, J., Westphael, G., Heaton, P., & Vuust, P. (2014). Intact brain processing of musical emotions in autism spectrum disorder, but more cognitive load and arousal in happy vs. sad music. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8(8), 192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldman, A. I. (2006). Simulating minds: The philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience of mindreading. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Heaton, P. (2009). Assessing musical skills in autistic children who are not savants. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1522), 1443–1447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hove, M. J., & Risen, J. L. (2009). It’s all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Social Cognition, 27(6), 949–961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huron, D. (2008). Sweet anticipation: Music and the psychology of expectation. MIT press. [Google Scholar]
- Isabella, R. A., & Belsky, J. (1991). Interactional synchrony and the origins of infant-mother attachment: A replication study. Child Development, 62(2), 373–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jones, S. C., Gordon, C. S., Akram, M., Murphy, N., & Sharkie, F. (2022). Inclusion, exclusion and isolation of autistic people: Community attitudes and autistic people’s experiences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 52(3), 1131–1142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keller, P. E., Novembre, G., & Hove, M. J. (2014). Rhythm in joint action: Psychological and neurophysiological mechanisms for real-time interpersonal coordination. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1658), 20130394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kirschner, S., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Joint music making promotes pro-social behaviour in 4-year-old children. Evolution and Human Behaviour, 31(5), 354–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koban, L., Ramamoorthy, A., & Konvalinka, I. (2019). Why do we fall into sync with others? Interpersonal synchronisation and the brain’s optimisation principle. Social Neuroscience, 14(1), 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Koehne, S., Hatri, A., Cacioppo, J. T., & Dziobek, I. (2016). Perceived interpersonal synchrony increases empathy: Insights from autism spectrum disorder. Cognition, 146, 8–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levinson, J. (2006). Contemplating art: Essays in aesthetics. Clarendon Press. [Google Scholar]
- Rabinowitch, T.-C., & Knafo-Noam, A. (2015). Synchronous rhythmic interaction enhances children’s perceived similarity and closeness towards each other. PLoS ONE, 10(4), e0120878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rabinowitch, T.-C., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2017). Synchronised movement experience enhances peer cooperation in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 160, 21–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reddish, P. (2012). Why sing and dance? An examination of the cooperative effects of group synchrony [Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, Victoria University of Wellington]. [Google Scholar]
- Reddish, P., Fischer, R., & Bulbulia, J. (2013). Let’s dance together: Synchrony, shared intentionality and cooperation. PLoS ONE, 8(8), e71182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Repp, B. H., & Su, Y. H. (2013). Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of recent research (2006–2012). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 403–452. [Google Scholar]
- Strogatz, S. (2003). Sync: The emerging science of spontaneous order. Penguin Books. [Google Scholar]
- Tarr, B., Launay, J., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2014). Music and social bonding: “Self-Other” merging and neurohormonal mechanisms. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1096. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tunçgenç, B., & Cohen, E. (2016). Movement synchrony forges social bonds across group divides. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 782. [Google Scholar]
- Wiltermuth, S., & Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and cooperation. Psychological Science, 20(1), 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zentner, M., & Eerola, T. (2010). Rhythmic engagement with music in infancy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(13), 5768–5773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Comparisons | Pro-Other vs. Fair | Pro-Other vs. Pro-Self | Fair vs. Pro-Self |
---|---|---|---|
Asynchronous vs. Synchronous | Χ2(1) = 11.92, p < 0.001 | Χ2(1) = 0.489, p > 0.999 | Χ2(1) = 20.326, p < 0.001 |
Asynchronous vs. Musical | Χ2(1) = 0.244, p > 0.999 | Χ2(1) = 1.331, p > 0.999 | Χ2(1) = 2.611, p = 0.954 |
Synchronous vs. Musical | Χ2(1) = 9.600, p = 0.018 | Χ2(1) = 1.351, p > 0.999 | Χ2(1) = 9.956, p = 0.018 |
Comparisons | Inferential’s | |
---|---|---|
Exterior or Middle Island Game | Alone or Together Island Game | |
Asynchronous vs. Synchronous | Χ2(1) = 0.6, p = 0.878 | Χ2(1) = 3.60, p = 0.116 |
Asynchronous vs. Musical | Χ2(1) = 4.271, p = 0.078 | Χ2(1) = 13.509, p < 0.001 |
Synchronous vs. Musical | Χ2(1) = 2.063, p = 0.302 | Χ2(1) = 5.912, p = 0.03 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Published by MDPI on behalf of the University Association of Education and Psychology. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Cross, L.; Pares, N.; Gali, O.; Ercan, S.B.; Sayis, B.; Heaton, P.; Atherton, G. The Moving Mandala: Exploring the Pro-Social Effects of Musical and Non-Musical Synchrony in Children in a Virtual World. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2025, 15, 39. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe15030039
Cross L, Pares N, Gali O, Ercan SB, Sayis B, Heaton P, Atherton G. The Moving Mandala: Exploring the Pro-Social Effects of Musical and Non-Musical Synchrony in Children in a Virtual World. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education. 2025; 15(3):39. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe15030039
Chicago/Turabian StyleCross, Liam, Narcis Pares, Olga Gali, Sena Beste Ercan, Batuhan Sayis, Pamela Heaton, and Gray Atherton. 2025. "The Moving Mandala: Exploring the Pro-Social Effects of Musical and Non-Musical Synchrony in Children in a Virtual World" European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education 15, no. 3: 39. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe15030039
APA StyleCross, L., Pares, N., Gali, O., Ercan, S. B., Sayis, B., Heaton, P., & Atherton, G. (2025). The Moving Mandala: Exploring the Pro-Social Effects of Musical and Non-Musical Synchrony in Children in a Virtual World. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education, 15(3), 39. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe15030039