Exploring the Effects of Additional Vibration on the Perceived Quality of an Electric Cello
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Method
2.1. Apparatus
2.1.1. Haptic E-Cello Prototype
2.1.2. Experimental Setup
- (1)
- A noise signal convolved with the IR preferred by the expert, with further processing to make it more coherent with the sound of the cello (details are given below).
- (2)
- The bridge signal convolved with the IR preferred by the expert.
- A pitch detection algorithm tracked the pitch of the bridge signal in real-time, and accordingly set the cutoff frequency of a high-pass filter applied to the noise signal. This allowed the noise signal to be consistent with the bridge output in terms of frequency content.
- An envelope follower algorithm analysing the bridge signal scaled the amplitude of the noise signal accordingly. This made the noise signal consistent with the bridge output in terms of intensity.
2.2. Test Design, Procedure, and Participants
- Preference: which setup is better?
- Power: which setup is more powerful?
- Liveliness: which setup feels more alive as an instrument?
- Feel: which setup has a better feel?
3. Results
3.1. Descriptive Analysis
3.2. Statistical Analysis
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- O’Modhrain, S.; Gillespie, B.R. Once More, with Feeling: The Dynamics of Performer-Instrument Interaction. In Musical Haptics; Papetti, S., Saitis, C., Eds.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Théberge, P. Any Sound You Can Imagine: Making Music/Consuming Technology; University Press of New England: Hanover, NH, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Cambourian, P.; Gal, O.; Pate, A.; Benacchio, S.; Vasseur, J. Understanding the vibrotactile feedback of the electric guitar: Methodology for a physical and perceptual study. In Proceedings of the 16th International Audio Mostly Conference, Trento, Italy, 1–3 September 2021; AM ’21. pp. 268–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marshall, K.; Genter, B. The musician and the vibrational behavior of a violin. J. Catgut Acoust. Soc. 1986, 45, 28–33. [Google Scholar]
- Suzuki, H. Vibration and sound radiation of a piano soundboard. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1986, 80, 1573–1582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Askenfelt, A.; Jansson, E.V. On Vibration Sensation and Finger Touch in Stringed Instrument Playing. Music Percept. 1992, 9, 311–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Papetti, S.; Saitis, C. (Eds.) Musical Haptics; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Galembo, A.; Askenfelt, A. Quality assessment of musical instruments—Effects of multimodality. In Proceedings of the ESCOM Conference, Hanover, Germany, 8–13 September 2003; pp. 441–444. [Google Scholar]
- Keane, M.; Dodd, G. Subjective Assessment of Upright Piano Key Vibrations. Acta Acust. United Acust. 2011, 97, 708–713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wollman, I.; Fritz, C.; Poitevineau, J. Influence of vibrotactile feedback on some perceptual features of violins. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2014, 136, 910–921. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Järveläinen, H.; Papetti, S.; Larrieux, E. Accuracy of Musical Pitch Control Through Finger Pushing and Pulling. In Haptic and Audio Interaction Design, Proceedings of the 11th International Workshop, HAID 2022, London, UK, 25–26 August 2022; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2022; pp. 125–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Papetti, S.; Jarvelainen, H.; Schiesser, S. Interactive Vibrotactile Feedback Enhances the Perceived Quality of a Surface for Musical Expression and the Playing Experience. IEEE Trans. Haptics 2021, 14, 635–645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fontana, F.; Avanzini, F.; Järveläinen, H.; Papetti, S.; Klauer, G.; Malavolta, L. Rendering and subjective evaluation of real vs. In synthetic vibrotactile cues on a digital piano keyboard. In Proceedings of the Sound and Music Computing Conference, SMC 2015, Maynooth, Ireland, 25 July–1 August 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verrillo, R.T. Vibration sensation in humans. Music Percept. 1992, 9, 281–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jack, R.H.; Mehrabi, A.; Stockman, T.; Mcpherson, A. Action-sound Latency and the Perceived Quality of Digital Musical Instruments: Comparing Professional Percussionists and Amateur Musicians. Music Percept. Interdiscip. J. 2018, 36, 109–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Occelli, V.; Spence, C.; Zampini, M. Audiotactile interactions in temporal perception. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2011, 18, 429–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wilson, E.C.; Reed, C.M.; Braida, L.D. Integration of auditory and vibrotactile stimuli: Effects of phase and stimulus-onset asynchrony. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2009, 126, 1960–1974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gift, A.G. Visual Analogue Scales. Nurs. Res. 1989, 38, 286–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kruschke, J.K. Doing Bayesian Data Analysis—A Tutorial with R, JAGS, and Stan, 2nd ed.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Liu, F.; Kong, Y. zoib: An R package for Bayesian inference for beta regression and Zero/one inflated beta regression. R J. 2015, 7, 34–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ospina, R.; Ferrari, S.L. Inflated beta distributions. Stat. Pap. 2008, 51, 111–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bürkner, P.C. brms: An R Package for Bayesian Multilevel Models Using Stan. J. Stat. Softw. 2017, 80, 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Vehtari, A.; Ojanen, J. A survey of Bayesian predictive methods for model assessment, selection and comparison. Stat. Surv. 2012, 6, 142–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Järveläinen, H.; Larrieux, E. Vibrotactile feedback enhances perceived arousal and listening experience in music. In Proceedings of the Sound and Music Computing Conference, SMC 2023, Stockholm, Sweden, 12–17 June 2023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Russell, J.A. A circumplex model of affect. J. Personal. Soc. Psych. 1980, 39, 1161–1178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zentner, M.; Eerola, T. Self-report measures and models. In Handbook of Music and Emotion: Theory, Research, Applications; Juslin, P., Sloboda, J., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
Acoustic Cello | RMS Amplitude [dB] | ||
---|---|---|---|
Strings | X | Y | Z |
C | 132 | 140 | 126 |
G | 134 | 132 | 123 |
D | 142 | 136 | 132 |
A | 124 | 117 | 108 |
E-Cello | RMS Amplitude [dB] | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Strings | X | Diff. X Cello | Y | Diff. Y Cello | Z | Diff. Z Cello |
C | 115 | 113 | 111 | |||
G | 136 | 135 | 120 | |||
D | 129 | 127 | 119 | |||
A | 120 | 120 | ||||
Tot. diff. across strings | ||||||
E-Cello w/ vibration | RMS amplitude [dB] | |||||
Strings | X | Diff. X cello | Y | Diff. Y cello | Z | Diff. Z cello |
C | 132 | 0 | 131 | 119 | ||
G | 139 | 135 | 122 | |||
D | 131 | 128 | 121 | |||
A | 136 | 131 | 126 | |||
Tot. diff. across strings | 0 |
Attribute | Vibration | Estimate | l-95% CI | u-95% CI |
---|---|---|---|---|
preference | bridge | 0.48 | 0.38 | 0.59 |
preference | noise | 0.45 | 0.35 | 0.56 |
feel | bridge | 0.54 | 0.44 | 0.63 |
feel | noise | 0.52 | 0.43 | 0.62 |
power | bridge | 0.56 | 0.46 | 0.66 |
power | noise | 0.49 | 0.39 | 0.58 |
liveliness | bridge | 0.55 | 0.47 | 0.64 |
liveliness | noise | 0.55 | 0.47 | 0.64 |
(a) Model: | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Effect | Estimate | Est. Error | Q2.5 | Q97.5 |
preference_Intercept | 1.00 | 0.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 |
power_Intercept | −0.16 | 0.44 | −0.89 | 0.73 |
liveliness_Intercept | 0.27 | 0.42 | −0.65 | 0.90 |
feel_Intercept | −0.27 | 0.40 | −0.89 | 0.61 |
(b) Raw data: | ||||
preference | power | liveliness | feel | |
preference | 1.00 | −0.03 | 0.35 | −0.01 |
power | −0.03 | 1.00 | 0.10 | 0.13 |
liveliness | 0.35 | 0.10 | 1.00 | −0.22 |
feel | −0.01 | 0.13 | −0.22 | 1.00 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Järveläinen, H.; Papetti, S.; Larrieux, E. Exploring the Effects of Additional Vibration on the Perceived Quality of an Electric Cello. Vibration 2024, 7, 407-418. https://doi.org/10.3390/vibration7020021
Järveläinen H, Papetti S, Larrieux E. Exploring the Effects of Additional Vibration on the Perceived Quality of an Electric Cello. Vibration. 2024; 7(2):407-418. https://doi.org/10.3390/vibration7020021
Chicago/Turabian StyleJärveläinen, Hanna, Stefano Papetti, and Eric Larrieux. 2024. "Exploring the Effects of Additional Vibration on the Perceived Quality of an Electric Cello" Vibration 7, no. 2: 407-418. https://doi.org/10.3390/vibration7020021
APA StyleJärveläinen, H., Papetti, S., & Larrieux, E. (2024). Exploring the Effects of Additional Vibration on the Perceived Quality of an Electric Cello. Vibration, 7(2), 407-418. https://doi.org/10.3390/vibration7020021