Next Article in Journal
Preliminary Evaluation of Potential Impacts Associated with Small Cetacean Remote Biopsy Sampling by Controlled Testing on Stranded Common Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)
Previous Article in Journal
A Census of Federally Regulated Big Cat Populations within the United States as of December 2020
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Animal Training, Environmental Enrichment, and Animal Welfare: A History of Behavior Analysis in Zoos

by
Eduardo J. Fernandez
1,* and
Allison L. Martin
2
1
School of Animal and Veterinary Science, The University of Adelaide, Adelaide, SA 5005, Australia
2
Department of Psychological Science, Kennesaw State University, Kennesaw, GA 30144, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Zool. Bot. Gard. 2021, 2(4), 531-543; https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg2040038
Submission received: 30 August 2021 / Revised: 11 September 2021 / Accepted: 23 September 2021 / Published: 13 October 2021

Abstract

:
The modern zoo has been associated with two major behavioral welfare advances: (a) the use of training to increase voluntary husbandry care, and (b) the implementation of environmental enrichment to promote naturalistic behaviors. Both practices have their roots in behavior analysis, or the operant conditioning-centered, reward-based approach to behavioral psychology. Operant conditioning served as the foundation for the development of reinforcement-based training methods commonly used in zoos to make veterinary and husbandry procedures easier and safer for animals and their caregivers. Likewise, operant conditioning, with its focus on arranging environmental antecedents and consequences to change behavior, also provided a framework for successful environmental enrichment practices. In this paper, we outline the key individuals and events that shaped two of the cornerstones of the modern zoo: (1) the emergence of reward-based husbandry training practices, and (2) the engineering of environmental enrichment. In addition, we (3) suggest ways in which behavior analysis can continue to advance zoo welfare by (i) expanding the efficacy of environmental enrichment, (ii) using within-subject methodology, and (iii) improving animal-visitor interactions. Our goal is to provide a historical and contextual reference for future efforts to improve the well-being of zoo animals.

1. Introduction

Due to the many advancements in zoo animal welfare and management, those familiar with modern, accredited zoos might expect to find diverse, enriched exhibits focused on the needs of each species, knowledgeable zookeepers well-versed in animal welfare and training, and visitor education experiences with an emphasis on the conservation of the zoo animals. What is not as apparent is the important role behavior analysis, or the Skinnerian operant conditioning-focused (e.g., reward- or reinforcement-based) approach to behavioral psychology, played in the formation of the present-day zoo. The modern zoo itself can be defined by two major behavioral advances focused on improving welfare: (1) the use of animal training procedures to increase voluntary participation in husbandry or other veterinary procedures by the zoo animals [1,2,3], and (2) the implementation of environmental enrichment to decrease detrimental and increase species-typical behaviors [4,5,6]. These advances were developed through decades of research and practices that incorporated behavioral principles to identify desirable outcomes for animals and visitors alike.
For those working with or in zoos, the influence of operant conditioning on animal training may be obvious. In most cases, trainers or keepers deliver reinforcing consequences to modify an animal’s behavior, often for husbandry purposes [7,8]. While the connection between behavior analysis and environmental enrichment may be less apparent, the practice of environmental enrichment also began as a set of consequence-focused operant conditioning procedures in zoos. Early enrichment practices incorporated food delivered mechanically as a reinforcer for engaging in desired responses, such as primates swinging from parts of their exhibit or felids chasing and catching artificial prey [9,10,11]. These procedures often incorporated visual or auditory stimuli that were meant to elicit or set the occasion for those desired responses, thus functioning as conditional (respondent) or discriminative (operant) stimuli, respectively. The advent of environmental enrichment was therefore a behavioral engineering endeavor, meant to adjust environment-behavior contingencies in the most optimal manner. From these modest beginnings, the practice grew to its current use as the major tool to increase naturalistic behaviors of all the animals we see in the zoo, as well as now extended to other settings, like shelters, homes, and farms [12,13,14,15].
The following paper examines the influence of behavior analysis on the modern zoo. We do this in three parts, with the first two parts detailing (1) the emergence of reward-based zoo husbandry training practices, and (2) the engineering of environmental enrichment in zoos. Both will sufficiently detail how behavioral principles were involved, with several photos to provide some context. For the final point, we discuss (3) the future of behavior analysis in zoos, with attention to how behavior analysis can continue to improve the lives of zoos animals by (i) expanding the efficacy of environmental enrichment, (ii) using within-subject methodology, and (iii) improving animal-visitor interactions. Our goal is to therefore detail the important role behavior analysis has had in the formation of the modern zoo, as well as how such behavioral principles can guide continued welfare progress in zoos and similar settings.

2. The Emergence of Reward-Based Zoo Husbandry Training Practices

Contemporary animal training procedures are often associated with clicker training or similar uses of conditioned reinforcers paired with positive reinforcement [16,17,18]. These procedures and other behavior analytic principles to train animals are tied to two major events: (1) Skinner’s discovery of shaping, or the use of differentially reinforcing successive approximations to a target response [19,20,21], and (2) the creation of a field of Applied Animal Psychology by Keller and Marian Breland, two of Professor Skinner’s graduate students at the University of Minnesota [22,23]. Both events were also directly connected to Project Pelican (also known as “Project Pigeon”), a wartime effort in the early 1940s that involved training pigeons (Columba livia) to guide bombs [24,25,26,27]. The project was sponsored by government contracts and through General Mills, Inc., with much of the research conducted within the top floor of the General Mills flour mill building (see Figure 1) in downtown Minneapolis, Minnesota [19,26].
Until Project Pelican, most of Skinner’s research involved the use of laboratory rats (Rattus norvegicus), with all the research published in The Behavior of Organisms [28] using rats as subjects. It was through Project Pelican that Skinner and his colleagues first began examining behavioral principles with pigeons, as well as his first experience training animals outside of an operant chamber, or “by hand” [24,29], for a review, see [19]. These events resulted in the discovery of shaping, which was so profound that by 1943, Keller and Marian Breland, having worked with Dr. Skinner as University of Minnesota Psychology graduate students on Project Pelican, left academia and began Animal Behavior Enterprises (ABE), an organization dedicated to the training of animals for a variety of applied, profit-driven purposes, including commercials and coin-operated acts [30,31,32,33,34].
By the 1950s, the Brelands moved their business to Hot Springs, Arkansas, where they held a tourist attraction known as “IQ Zoo”, intended as both an entertaining and educational experience [25,35]. The Brelands also continued to train animals for coin-operated acts and other revenue-generating ventures (see Figure 2), as well as suggesting the use of naturalistic exhibits and visitor-focused learning opportunities within zoos and similar settings [36,37,38,39].
By 1955, the Brelands also began working with marine mammal parks to establish some of the first cetacean training shows [22,40]. Originally begun with the training of bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncates) at Marine Studios (now Marineland of Florida, see Figure 3), the use of operant conditioning procedures to train marine mammals would soon spread to other parks, including Marineland of the Pacific, Sea Life Park, and SeaWorld (for a review, see [40]).
Eventually, the success of operant conditioning to shape the behaviors of marine mammals would be popularized by books such as Karen Pryor’s Lads before the Wind [41] and Don’t Shoot the Dog! [17]. At the same time, zoos began to see the benefits of using such procedures to produce voluntary participation in veterinary husbandry practices [7,42]. For instance, San Diego Zoo implemented a shaping protocol that allowed a diabetic drill (Mandrillus leucophaeus) to choose to receive insulin injections [43]. Denver Zoo trained nyala (Tragelaphus angasi) and bongo (Tragelaphus eurycerus) to willingly enter crates to receive blood draws or other veterinary procedures [44,45]. Bloomsmith, Stone, and Laule [46] successfully used reward-based methods to train large groups of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) to elect to move (i.e., “shift”) from outdoor areas to an indoor portion of their enclosures. The use of reinforcement-based training procedures is now commonplace for many species within most accredited zoos, with some organizations requiring facilities to establish and use such protocols to receive accreditation [47,48,49,50].

3. The Engineering of Environmental Enrichment in Zoos

The implementation of environmental enrichment in zoos can be traced to Hal Markowitz, who served as Director of the Oregon Zoological Research Center, Associate Director of the Portland/Washington Park Zoo (now the Oregon Zoo), and Professor of Biological Science at San Francisco State University. While prior work in zoos and similar settings described the need for promoting the well-being of captive animals [51,52,53,54], Markowitz and his colleagues were among the first to promote a systematic, functional approach to the behavior of zoo animals through behavioral engineering [9,55,56,57,58,59]. The term “behavioral engineering” itself was taken directly from the application of Skinner’s operant conditioning procedures, or the field of Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) [24,60,61]. The term, “engineer,” emphasizes the real-world application of a science. In the same way that mechanical engineers apply basic physics principles to better society, behavioral engineers apply the science of respondent and operant conditioning to bring about positive change in the world and, in this case, zoos. Through the creation of contrived, reinforcement-based learning contingencies, Markowitz and his colleagues were able to produce mechanical levers that would allow white-handed gibbons (Hylobates lar) to swing across their enclosure to activate the levers and receive a food reward, mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx) to compete against zoo visitors in a computerized arcade-like reaction game, and polar bears (Ursus maritimus) to vocalize into a voice-operated relay system that would result in a frozen fish being launched into their exhibit (see Figure 4) [62,63,64]. All the above was carried out to produce desired behaviors (e.g., foraging) or reduce undesired responses (e.g., pacing) as a form of artificial, mechanized occupational therapy for the zoo animals.
Among the criticisms of such applications were the artificiality of the procedures involved, as well as the arbitrary distinction of what constituted ‘desired’ responses to be increased [65,66,67]. These critics argued that, rather than engineering environments through contrived contingencies, zoos should focus on creating naturalistic exhibits that increased spatial and temporal complexity, for instance, the exhibit arrangement or timing of feeding events [68,69]. Markowitz [70] responded to some of these criticisms by noting that, “the best interests of captive animals may not be served by making their state as ‘wild’ as possible.” (p. 12). Markowitz’s argument was that artificial and mechanical contingencies, such as those provided by enabling an elephant to pull a chain to receive part of their diet (see Figure 5), could improve the lives of exhibited animals. “Behavioral enrichment”, a term Markowitz began using synonymously with behavioral engineering, could benefit exhibited animals by giving the animals ‘something to do’ [10,70].
The term “enrichment” appeared to come directly from psychobiological and developmental neuroscience research, where comparisons were often made between animals raised in enriched versus impoverished environments (for a review, see [71]). Regardless, both the ideas of enriching and engineering environments were now being used interchangeably, with other authors arguing that these and the naturalistic/complexity concepts could be integrated to benefit the behavioral welfare of zoo animals [72]. Likewise, some of Markowitz and colleagues’ later efforts focused on naturalistic implementations of behavioral engineering endeavors, such as Asian small-clawed otters (Aonyx cinereus) hunting live crickets (Acheta domesticus) that visitors mechanically assisted in releasing into different parts of their exhibit, servals (Leptailurus serval) chasing artificial prey run through clear tubes in their enclosure (see Figure 6), and an African leopard (Panthera pardus) chasing bird sounds along a tree limb to receive a food reward [73,74,75].
The result has been the introduction of environmental enrichment as an approach that is both pragmatically focused and attends to the species-typical needs of the organisms involved. The use of enrichment is one of those rare events that requires simultaneous attention to both evolutionary and learning histories to be optimally implemented, which necessarily requires an integration between naturalistic and engineered exhibits [14,76]. Equally important for the zoo is how the visitor behaves in response to enriched animals, therefore driving the need for naturalistic devices and responses for and from the animals being enriched, respectively [77,78]. Environmental enrichment, because of its behavior analytic underpinnings, is now an animal welfare endeavor where all features of how an animal interacts with its environment are examined for their behavioral benefits [5,79].

4. The Future of Behavior Analysis in Zoos

Behavior analysis has had a profound influence on shaping the modern zoo; however, we have only begun to realize its full potential in animal settings. There have been many calls for the adoption of a behavior analytic framework to improve animal care [7,61,80,81,82,83,84,85,86]. While the science of behavior analysis grew out of basic animal studies, recent advances in applied behavior analysis have been developed and implemented primarily in human clinical settings. However, coming full circle, researchers are now successfully adapting and using behavioral protocols developed for use with people to impact animal welfare. For example, functional analysis protocols have been used to assess and treat problem behaviors in animals [87,88,89,90,91,92,93,94,95,96], and this function-based approach that emphasizes the identification and modification of existing behavior-environment relationships may help provide a framework that allows animal caregivers both a deeper understanding of behavior and the ability to move beyond the reliance of artificial reinforcers when modifying behavior [61]. In addition, empirical preference assessments have been successfully used in a variety of species [97,98,99,100,101,102,103,104,105,106,107,108,109,110,111] with promise for improving training effectiveness. By using a behavior analytic lens, adapting existing behavioral technologies, and developing new, animal-specific, behavioral protocols and methodologies, behavior analysts could play a considerable role in guiding the next advances in modern zoos. We outline just a few of the possibilities below, including (i) expanding the efficacy of environmental enrichment, (ii) using within-subject methodology, and (iii) improving animal-visitor interactions.
Applied behavior analysts have a long history with increasing behavioral repertoires to the benefit of the participants involved, and zoo animals should be no exception to this approach. Using both antecedent and consequence manipulations, behavior analysts in clinical settings have developed effective protocols to increase social and occupational skills with people [112,113]. While zoos use environmental enrichment to increase an animal’s behavioral repertoire, its application is often void of an underlying theoretical framework and is based on factors like appearance, novelty, cost, or availability/convenience [14,76]. A zoo-based behavior analyst could increase the effectiveness of these enrichment practices by learning theory-focused implementations that incorporate factors such as schedules of reinforcement, habituation, preference, variation, and choice (e.g., [86,109,111,114,115,116]). In doing so, they could help to solve issues such as animals who are unusually inactive, not using existing enrichment options, or not utilizing all areas of a habitat. Furthermore, training practices could be combined with environmental enrichment (e.g., [1]) to shape more complex behaviors that allow for increased engagement with enrichment devices. Enrichment could also play a key role in training adaptive behaviors that would aid in conservation and re-release programs (e.g., [117,118]). In addition to increasing an animal’s behavioral repertoire, another goal of enrichment is to decrease maladaptive behaviors, and behavior analysis likewise can provide guidance in this area. The competing stimuli framework is used in ABA to identify which items or activities effectively reduce problem behaviors by offering alternative sources of reinforcement [118,119]. By monitoring an individual animal’s (or group’s) enrichment use and problem behavior across different enrichment conditions using within-subjects designs, behavior analysts could make data-driven decisions regarding the best implementation of a variety of different types of potential enrichment items or events [75,80,82,117,120,121].
Within- or single-subject research designs such as some of the research mentioned above form the methodological foundation of behavior analysis [122,123,124]. These individual-focused designs allow researchers to experimentally determine the functional relationship between variables and effectively monitor a subject’s response to interventions using as few as one subject. In ABA, the goal is to make a meaningful change in the behavior of a particular individual in a specific circumstance or setting, and within-subject designs allow for this flexibility and specificity [123]. Similarly, every attempt to provide proper behavioral welfare for any zoo animal is ultimately a study with a sample size (n) = 1. Even if it is possible to find enough similar subjects to conduct a robust group study, knowing that the average animal responds to a particular reinforcer or treatment is of limited use when focusing on the treatment of an individual animal. For example, knowing that 60% of a sample of a particular species will forage for a particular food item will not aide a facility if the animal in an exhibit is in the 40% who will not. Animals’ responses to stimuli are based on both their species and individual histories as well as their current environment, so assessing and monitoring behavior at the level of the individual level is key. Behavior analytic methodologies and their focus on the overt behaviors of individuals is ideally suited for improving the lives of zoo animals [80,82,125].
Finally, zoos have become increasingly interested in understanding both direct and indirect animal-visitor interactions, with particular emphasis in minimizing adverse impacts that visitors may have on animals while also increasing visitor education and entertainment [77,126,127]. Factors that increase visitor engagement with conservation efforts are also of interest (see [128] for review). Zoo visitors respond favorably to animal interactions, training demonstrations, and environmental enrichment activities [128], and a zoo-based behavior analyst could optimize these events. For example, more interactive elements in the spirit of some of Markowitz’s early behavioral engineering endeavors (see ‘The Engineering of Environment Enrichment in Zoos’ section) could be developed and upgraded with modern technology (e.g., [129,130,131,132]) so that visitors could introduce interactive elements (e.g., movement, sound, or food) into animal exhibits. This would require careful arrangements and monitoring, of course, and behavior analysts have the expertise and tools to ensure its success. A large component of understanding these interactions should come from simultaneously observing the overt behaviors of both animals and visitors, an exercise all too common for many applied behavior analysts when equally working with caregivers and clients. While zoos have a primary responsibility to promote the welfare of the animals in their care, by focusing on learning contingencies involved for both the animals and the visitors, ABA practitioners in zoos could effectively provide win-win solutions that mutually improve the welfare of animals and the education/enjoyment of the visitor.

5. Conclusions

Many of the behavioral practices found in modern zoos can be traced back to pioneers in operant conditioning such as B. F. Skinner, Keller Breland, Marian Breland Bailey, and Hal Markowitz. Behavior analytic-driven advances in animal training and environmental enrichment have improved the welfare of zoo animals and have benefited animal care workers and zoo visitors. Nonetheless, behavior analysis still has untapped potential in this setting. While some operant-based techniques, such as clicker training and environmental enrichment, have become commonplace in zoos, these practices have in some instances become disconnected from their underlying learning principles. Further advancement of behavior analysis in animal settings necessitates individuals who are well-trained in the fundamentals of respondent and operant conditioning and can combine this theoretical background with practical knowledge of animal behavior to design habitats and arrange behavioral contingencies to optimize welfare [61]. This will require more animal care professionals with advanced training in behavior analysis and more collaborations between zoos and behavior analysts working in other settings, such as human clinical settings or academic institutions [82,84,85,133]. Integrative approaches to behavior analysis and animal behavior are rapidly increasing. We hope that this historical recognition of such work in zoos, as well as a potential guide for future research, helps foster such practices.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, E.J.F.; writing—original draft preparation, E.J.F., A.L.M.; writing—review and editing, E.J.F., A.L.M. Both authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Fernandez, E.J.; Kinley, R.C.; Timberlake, W. Training penguins to interact with enrichment devices for lasting effects. Zoo Biol. 2019, 38, 481–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Laule, G.; Desmond, T. Positive reinforcement training as an enrichment strategy. In Second Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals; Shepherdson, D.J., Mellen, J.D., Hutchins, M., Eds.; Smithsonian Books: Washington, DC, USA, 1998; pp. 302–313. ISBN 978-1560983972. [Google Scholar]
  3. Melfi, V.A.; Dorey, N.R.; Ward, S.J. (Eds.) Zoo Animal Learning and Training; Wiley & Sons, Ltd.: West Sussex, UK, 2020; ISBN 9781118968536. [Google Scholar]
  4. Fernandez, E.J.; Myers, M.; Hawkes, N.C. The effects of live feeding on swimming activity and exhibit use in zoo Humboldt penguins (Spheniscus humboldti). J. Zoo. Bot. Gard. 2021, 2, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Hoy, J.M.; Murray, P.J.; Tribe, A. Thirty years later: Enrichment practices for captive mammals. Zoo Biol. 2010, 29, 303–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Shepherdson, D.J.; Mellen, J.D.; Hutchins, M. (Eds.) Second Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals; Smithsonian Books: Washington, DC, USA, 1998; ISBN 978-1560983972. [Google Scholar]
  7. Forthman, D.L.; Ogden, J.J. The role of applied behavior analysis in zoo management: Today and tomorrow. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 1992, 25, 647–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  8. Lukas, K.E.; Marr, M.J.; Maple, T.L. Teaching operant conditioning at the zoo. Teach. Psychol. 1998, 25, 112–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Markowitz, H. Engineering environments for behavioral opportunities in the zoo. Behav. Anal. 1978, 1, 34–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Markowitz, H. Behavioral Enrichment in the Zoo; Van Nostrand Reinhold: New York, NY, USA, 1982; ISBN 978-0442251253. [Google Scholar]
  11. Snyder, R. Putting the wild back into the zoo. Int. Zoo News 1977, 24, 11–17. [Google Scholar]
  12. Bender, A.; Strong, E. Canine Enrichment for the Real World; Dogwise Publishing: Wenatchee, WA, USA, 2019; ISBN 9781617812682. [Google Scholar]
  13. Markowitz, H. Enriching Animal Lives; Mauka Press: Cromwell, NZ, USA, 2011; ISBN 978-0983357919. [Google Scholar]
  14. Mellen, J.; MacPhee, M.S. Philosophy of environmental enrichment: Past, present, and future. Zoo Biol. 2001, 20, 211–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Young, R.J. Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals; Blackwell Science Ltd.: Oxford, UK, 2003; ISBN 978-0632064076. [Google Scholar]
  16. Fernandez, E.J. Click or treat: A trick or two in the zoo. Am. Anim. Train. Mag. 2001, 2, 41–44. [Google Scholar]
  17. Pryor, K. Don’t Shoot the Dog!: The New Art of Teaching and Training; Bantam Books: New York, NY, USA, 1984; ISBN 9780671438425. [Google Scholar]
  18. Ramirez, K. Animal Training: Successful Animal Management through Positive Reinforcement; Shedd Aquarium Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1999; ISBN 978-0961107499. [Google Scholar]
  19. Peterson, G.B. A day of great illumination: BF Skinner’s discovery of shaping. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 2004, 82, 317–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Pierce, W.D.; Cheney, C.D. Behavior Analysis and Learning, 5th ed.; Psychology Press: East Sussex, UK, 2013; ISBN 978-1848726154. [Google Scholar]
  21. Skinner, B.F. How to teach animals. Sci. Am. 1951, 185, 26–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Bailey, R.E.; Gillaspy, J.A. Operant psychology goes to the fair: Marian and Keller Breland in the popular press, 1947–1966. Behav. Anal. 2005, 28, 143–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Breland, K.; Breland, M. A field of applied animal psychology. Am. Psychol. 1951, 6, 202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Capshew, J.H. Engineering behavior: Project pigeon, World War II, and the conditioning of BF Skinner. Technol. Cult. 1993, 34, 835–857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Gillaspy, J.A., Jr.; Bihm, E.M. Marian Breland Bailey (1920–2001). Am. Psychol. 2002, 57, 292–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Skinner, B.F. Pigeons in a pelican. Am. Psychol. 1960, 15, 28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Skinner, B.F. The Shaping of a Behaviorist: Part Two of an Autobiography; New York University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1979; ISBN 978-0814778449. [Google Scholar]
  28. Skinner, B.F. The Behavior of Organisms: An Experimental Analysis; Appleton-Century Press: New York, NY, USA, 1938; ISBN 978-0-9964539-0-5. [Google Scholar]
  29. Skinner, B.F. Reinforcement today. Am. Psychol. 1958, 13, 94–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Bailey, R.E.; Bailey, M.B. A view from outside the Skinner box. Am. Psychol. 1980, 35, 942–946. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Breland, K.; Breland, M. The misbehavior of organisms. Am. Psychol. 1961, 16, 681–684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Breland, K.; Breland, M. Animal Behavior; Macmillan: New York, NY, USA, 1966; ISBN 978-0988807921. [Google Scholar]
  33. Timberlake, W. Marian Breland Bailey: Many lives: (SQAB, May 25, 2002, Toronto, Canada). Behav. Processes 2003, 62, 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Yin, S. Bells and whistles: When operant conditioning clicked (and clucked). Barks Mag. 2012, 69, 74–78. [Google Scholar]
  35. Bihm, E.M.; Gillaspy, J.A.; Lammers, W.J.; Huffman, S.P. IQ Zoo and teaching operant concepts. Psychol. Rec. 2010, 60, 523–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Bailey, M.B.; Bailey, R.E. Animal exhibits as revenue producers. In Proceedings of the American Association of Zoological Parks and Aquariums Regional Workshop, Little Rock, AR, USA, May 1979; pp. 367–373. [Google Scholar]
  37. Breland, K. New strides in animal psychology bring about naturalistic behavior exhibits. Parks Recreat. 1962, 45, 80. [Google Scholar]
  38. Breland, K. The ‘Who’s Teaching Whom?’ Machine. Am. Psychol. 1963, 18, 261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Curtis, L. Coin-operated animal acts. Parks Recreat. 1957, 40, 18. [Google Scholar]
  40. Gillaspy, J.A., Jr.; Brinegar, J.L.; Bailey, R.E. Operant psychology makes a splash—In marine mammal training (1955–1965). J. Hist. Behav. Sci. 2014, 50, 231–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Pryor, K. Lads before the Wind: Adventures in Porpoise Training; Harper & Row Publishers: New York, NY, USA, 1975; ISBN 9780060134426. [Google Scholar]
  42. Desmond, T.; Laule, G. Use of positive reinforcement training in the management of species for reproduction. Zoo Biol. 1994, 13, 471–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Priest, G.M. Training a diabetic drill (Mandrillus leucophaeus) to accept insulin injections and venipuncture. Lab. Primate Newsl. 1991, 30, 1–4. [Google Scholar]
  44. Grandin, T.; Rooney, M.B.; Phillips, M.; Cambre, R.C.; Irlbeck, N.A.; Graffam, W. Conditioning of nyala (Tragelaphus angasi) to blood sampling in a crate with positive reinforcement. Zoo Biol. 1995, 14, 261–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Phillips, M.; Grandin, T.; Graffam, W.; Irlbeck, N.A.; Cambre, R.C. Crate conditioning of bongo (Tragelaphus eurycerus) for veterinary and husbandry procedures at the Denver Zoological Gardens. Zoo Biol. 1998, 17, 25–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Bloomsmith, M.A.; Stone, A.M.; Laule, G.E. Positive reinforcement training to enhance the voluntary movement of group-housed chimpanzees within their enclosures. Zoo Biol. 1998, 17, 333–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Association of Zoos and Aquariums. Animal Care Manuals. Available online: https://www.aza.org/animal-care-manuals (accessed on 20 November 2020).
  48. European Association of Zoos and Aquaria. Standards for the Accommodation and Care of Animals in Zoos and Aquaria. Available online: https://www.eaza.net/assets/Uploads/Standards-and-policies/2019-04-EAZA-Standards-for-Accomodation-and-Care.pdf (accessed on 25 April 2019).
  49. Mackie, J. The application of positive reinforcement training to enhance welfare of primates in zoological collections. In Zoo Animal Learning and Training; Melfi, V.A., Dorey, N.R., Ward, S.J., Eds.; Wiley & Sons, Ltd.: West Sussex, UK, 2020; pp. 211–215. ISBN 9781118968536. [Google Scholar]
  50. Savastano, G.; Hanson, A.; McCann, C. The development of an operant conditioning training program for New World primates at the Bronx Zoo. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2003, 6, 247–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Hediger, H. Wild Animals in Captivity; Butterworths Scientific Publications: Oxford, UK, 1950; ISBN 9780486212609. [Google Scholar]
  52. Hediger, H. Studies of the Psychology and Behavior of Captive Animals in Zoos and Circuses; Butterworths Scientific Publications: London, UK, 1955; ISBN 9780598574206. [Google Scholar]
  53. Morris, D. The response of animals to a restricted environment. Symp. Zool. Soc. Lond. 1964, 13, 99–118. [Google Scholar]
  54. Yerkes, R.M. Almost Human; Century: New York, NY, USA, 1925. [Google Scholar]
  55. Maple, T.L. Remembering Hal Markowitz. Zoo Biol. 2013, 32, 243–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Markowitz, H. Environmental enrichment and behavioral engineering for captive primates. In Captivity and Behavior: Primates in Breeding Colonies, Laboratories and Zoos; Erwin, J., Maple, T., Eds.; Van Nostrand Reinhold: New York, NY, USA, 1979; pp. 217–238. ISBN 978-0442223298. [Google Scholar]
  57. Markowitz, H.; Woodworth, G. Experimental analysis and control of group behavior. In Behavior of Captive Wild Animals; Markowitz, H., Stevens, V.J., Eds.; Nelson-Hall: Chicago, IL, USA, 1978; pp. 107–131. ISBN 978-0882293851. [Google Scholar]
  58. Markowitz, H.; Schmidt, M.; Nadal, L.; Squier, L. Do elephants ever forget? J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 1975, 8, 333–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Mellen, J.D.; Stevens, V.J.; Markowitz, H. Environmental enrichment for servals, Indian elephants and Canadian otters at Washington Park Zoo, Portland. Int. Zoo Yearb. 1981, 21, 196–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Homme, L.; C’de Baca, P.; Cottingham, L.; Homme, A. What behavioral engineering is. Psychol. Rec. 1968, 18, 425–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Martin, A.L. The primatologist as a behavioral engineer. Am. J. Primatol. 2017, 79, e22500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  62. Markowitz, H.; Schmidt, M.J.; Moody, A. Behavioural engineering and animal health in the zoo. Int. Zoo Yearb. 1978, 18, 190–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Schmidt, M.J.; Markowitz, H. Behavioral engineering as an aid in the maintenance of healthy zoo animals. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 1977, 171, 966–969. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  64. Yanofsky, R.; Markowitz, H. Changes in general behavior of two mandrills (Papio sphinx) concomitant with behavioral testing in the zoo. Psychol. Rec. 1978, 28, 369–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Hutchins, M.; Hancocks, D.; Calip, T. Behavioral engineering in the zoo: A critique. Int. Zoo News 1978, 25(Part I), 18–23. [Google Scholar]
  66. Hutchins, M.; Hancocks, D.; Calip, T. Behavioral engineering in the zoo: A critique. Int. Zoo News 1978, 25(Part II), 18–23. [Google Scholar]
  67. Hutchins, M.; Hancocks, D.; Calip, T. Behavioral engineering in the zoo: A critique. Int. Zoo News 1979, 26(Part III), 20–27. [Google Scholar]
  68. Hancocks, D. Bringing nature into the zoo: Inexpensive solutions for zoo environments. Int. J. Study Anim. Behav. Probl. 1980, 1, 170–177. [Google Scholar]
  69. Hutchins, M.; Hancocks, D.; Crockett, C. Natural solutions to the behavioral problems of captive animals. Zool. Gart. 1984, 54, 28–42. [Google Scholar]
  70. Markowitz, H.; Spinelli, J.S. Environmental engineering for primates. In Primates: The Road to Self-Sustaining Populations; Benirschke, W.K., Ed.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1986; pp. 489–498. ISBN 978-1-4612-9360-6. [Google Scholar]
  71. Uphouse, L. Reevaluation of mechanisms that mediate brain differences between enriched and impoverished animals. Psychol. Bull. 1980, 88, 215–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Forthman-Quick, D.L. An integrative approach to environmental engineering in zoos. Zoo Biol. 1984, 3, 65–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Foster-Turley, P.; Markowitz, H. A captive behavioral enrichment study with Asian small-clawed river otters (Aonyx cinerea). Zoo Biol. 1982, 1, 29–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Markowitz, H.; LaForse, S. Artificial prey as behavioral enrichment devices for felines. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1987, 18, 31–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Markowitz, H.; Aday, C.; Gavazzi, A. Effectiveness of acoustic “prey”: Environmental enrichment for a captive African leopard (Panthera pardus). Zoo Biol. 1995, 14, 371–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Newberry, R.C. Environmental enrichment: Increasing the biological relevance of captive environments. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1995, 44, 229–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Fernandez, E.J.; Tamborski, M.A.; Pickens, S.R.; Timberlake, W. Animal–visitor interactions in the modern zoo: Conflicts and interventions. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009, 120, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Learmonth, M.J. Human–animal interactions in zoos: What can compassionate conservation, conservation welfare and duty of care tell us about the ethics of interacting, and avoiding unintended consequences? Animals 2020, 10, 2037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Fernandez, E.J. Training as enrichment: A critical review. Anim. Welf. 2021, in press. [Google Scholar]
  80. Alligood, C.A.; Dorey, N.R.; Mehrkam, L.R.; Leighty, K.A. Applying behavior-analytic methodology to the science and practice of environmental enrichment in zoos and aquariums. Zoo Biol. 2017, 36, 175–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Bloomsmith, M.A.; Marr, M.J.; Maple, T.L. Addressing nonhuman primate behavioral problems through the application of operant conditioning: Is the human treatment approach a useful model? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 102, 205–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Fernandez, E.J.; Timberlake, W. Mutual benefits of research collaborations between zoos and academic institutions. Zoo Biol. 2008, 27, 470–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Kummrow, M. Diagnostic and therapeutic guidelines to abnormal behavior in captive nonhuman primates. Vet. Clin. N. Am. Exot. Anim. Pract. 2021, 24, 253–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Maple, T.L.; Perdue, B.M. Zoo Animal Welfare; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2013; ISBN 978-3-642-35954-5. [Google Scholar]
  85. Maple, T.L.; Segura, V.D. Advancing behavior analysis in zoos and aquariums. Behav. Anal. 2015, 38, 77–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  86. Tarou, L.R.; Bashaw, M.J. Maximizing the effectiveness of environmental enrichment: Suggestions from the experimental analysis of behavior. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 102, 189–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Dorey, N.R.; Rosales-Ruiz, J.; Smith, R.; Lovelace, B. Functional analysis and treatment of self-injury in a captive olive baboon. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2009, 42, 785–794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  88. Farmer-Dougan, V. Functional analysis of aggression in a black-and-white ruffed lemur (Varecia variegata variegata). J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2014, 17, 282–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Feuerbacher, E.N.; Wynne, C.D. Application of functional analysis methods to assess human–dog interactions. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2016, 49, 970–974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Franklin, A.N.; Martin, A.L.; Perlman, J.E.; Bloomsmith, M.A. Functional analysis and treatment of disruptive behavior in a rhesus macaque. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Hall, N.J.; Protopopova, A.; Wynne, C.D. The role of environmental and owner-provided consequences in canine stereotypy and compulsive behavior. J. Vet. Behav. 2015, 10, 24–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Martin, A.L.; Bloomsmith, M.A.; Kelley, M.E.; Marr, M.J.; Maple, T.L. Functional analysis and treatment of human-directed undesirable behavior exhibited by a captive chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2011, 44, 139–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Mehrkam, L.R.; Perez, B.C.; Self, V.N.; Vollmer, T.R.; Dorey, N.R. Functional analysis and operant treatment of food guarding in a pet dog. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2020, 53, 2139–2150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Morris, K.L.; Slocum, S.K. Functional analysis and treatment of self-injurious feather plucking in a black vulture (Coragyps atratus). J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2019, 52, 918–927. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Pfaller-Sadovsky, N.; Arnott, G.; Hurtado-Parrado, C. Using principles from applied behaviour analysis to address an undesired behaviour: Functional analysis and treatment of jumping up in companion dogs. Animals 2019, 9, 1091. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  96. Salmeron, M.C.; Payne, S.W.; Hegr, A.B. Functional analysis and treatment of feline aggression in an animal shelter. Behav. Anal. Pract. 2021, 21, 128–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Brox, B.W.; Edwards, K.; Buist, N.A.; Macaskill, A.C. Investigating food preference in zoo-housed meerkats. Zoo Biol. 2021, 21640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Cameron, K.E.; Bizo, L.A.; Starkey, N.J. Food preferences of the brushtail possum (Trichosurus vulpecula). Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 2013, 26, 324–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Clay, A.W.; Bloomsmith, M.A.; Marr, M.J.; Maple, T.L. Systematic investigation of the stability of food preferences in captive orangutans: Implications for positive reinforcement training. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2009, 12, 306–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  100. Clayton, M.; Shrock, T. Making a Tiger’s Day: Free-operant assessment and environmental enrichment to improve the daily lives of captive Bengal tigers (Panthera tigris tigris). Behav. Anal. Pract. 2020, 13, 883–893. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Dorey, N.R.; Mehrkam, L.R.; Tacey, J. A method to assess relative preference for training and environmental enrichment in captive wolves (Canis lupus and Canis lupus arctos). Zoo Biol. 2015, 34, 513–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Fay, C.; Miller, L.J. Utilizing scents as environmental enrichment: Preference assessment and application with Rothschild giraffe. Anim. Behav. Cogn. 2015, 2, 285–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Fernandez, E.J.; Dorey, N.; Rosales-Ruiz, J. A two-choice preference assessment with five cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2004, 7, 163–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  104. Fernandez, E.J.; Timberlake, W. The Functional Value of Enrichment: Determining Environmental Enrichment Effects in Lemurs through the Use of Paired-Choice Preference Assessments; Association for Behavior Analysis Conference: Chicago, IL, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  105. Fernandez, E.J.; Timberlake, W. Selecting and testing environmental enrichment in lemurs. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Gaalema, D.E.; Perdue, B.M.; Kelling, A.S. Food preference, keeper ratings, and reinforcer effectiveness in exotic animals: The value of systematic testing. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2011, 14, 33–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Martin, A.L.; Franklin, A.N.; Perlman, J.E.; Bloomsmith, M.A. Systematic assessment of food item preference and reinforcer effectiveness: Enhancements in training laboratory-housed rhesus macaques. Behav. Process. 2018, 157, 445–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Mehrkam, L.R.; Dorey, N.R. Is preference a predictor of enrichment efficacy in Galapagos tortoises (Chelonoidis nigra)? Zoo Biol. 2014, 33, 275–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Slocum, S.K.; Morris, K.L. Assessing preference in a paired-stimulus arrangement with captive vultures (Aegypius Monachus). J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2020, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  110. Vicars, S.M.; Miguel, C.F.; Sobie, J.L. Assessing preference and reinforcer effectiveness in dogs. Behav. Process. 2014, 103, 75–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  111. Woods, J.M.; Lane, E.K.; Miller, L.J. Preference assessments as a tool to evaluate environmental enrichment. Zoo Biol. 2020, 39, 382–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  112. Miltenberger, R.G. Behavior Modification: Principles and Procedures, 6th ed.; Cengage Learning: Boston, MA, UA, 2016; ISBN 978-1-305-10939-1. [Google Scholar]
  113. Fisher, W.W.; Piazza, C.C.; Roane, H.S. Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis, 2nd ed.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2021; ISBN 978-1-4625-4375-5. [Google Scholar]
  114. Hanratty, L.A.; Hanley, G.P. A preference analysis of reinforcer variation and choice. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2021, 54, 1062–1074. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  115. Learmonth, M.J.; Sherwen, S.; Hemsworth, P.H. Assessing preferences of two zoo-housed Aldabran giant tortoises (Aldabrachelys gigantea) for three stimuli using a novel preference test. Zoo Biol. 2021, 40, 98–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  116. Ringdahl, J.E.; Vollmer, T.R.; Marcus, B.A.; Roane, H.S. An analogue evaluation of environmental enrichment: The role of stimulus preference. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 1997, 30, 203–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  117. Sanders, K.; Fernandez, E.J. Behavioral implications of enrichment for golden lion tamarins: A tool for ex situ conservation. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2020, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Roberts, L.J.; Taylor, J.; Garcia de Leaniz, C. Environmental enrichment reduces maladaptive risk-taking behavior in salmon reared for conservation. Biol. Coserv. 2011, 144, 1972–1979. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Haddock, J.N.; Hagopian, L.P. Competing stimulus assessments: A systematic review. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2020, 53, 1982–2001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  120. Bashaw, M.J.; Gibson, M.D.; Schowe, D.M.; Kucher, A.S. Does enrichment improve reptile welfare? Leopard geckos (Eublepharis macularius) respond to five types of environmental enrichment. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 184, 150–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Carlstead, K.; Seidensticker, J.; Baldwin, R. Environmental enrichment for zoo bears. Zoo Biol. 1991, 10, 3–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Bailey, J.S.; Burch, M.R. Research Methods in Applied Behavior Analysis; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2017; ISBN 9781138685260. [Google Scholar]
  123. DeRosa, N.M.; Sullivan, W.E.; Roane, H.S.; Craig, A.R.; Kadey, H.J. Single-case experimental design. In Handbook of Applied Behavior Analysis, 2nd ed.; Fisher, W.W., Piazza, C.C., Roane, H.S., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2021; pp. 155–171. ISBN 978-1-4625-4375-5. [Google Scholar]
  124. Johnston, J.M.; Pennypacker, H.S. Strategies and Tactics of Behavioral Research, 3rd ed.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2010; ISBN 978-0-8058-5882-2. [Google Scholar]
  125. Fernandez, E.J. Appetitive search behaviors and stereotypies in polar bears (Ursus maritimus). Behav. Process. 2021, 182, 104299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Sherwen, S.L.; Hemsworth, P.H. The visitor effect on zoo animals: Implications and opportunities for zoo animal welfare. Animals 2019, 9, 366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  127. Fernandez, E.J.; Upchurch, B.; Hawkes, N.C. Public feeding interactions as enrichment for three zoo-housed elephants. Animals 2021, 11, 1689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Godinez, A.M.; Fernandez, E.J. What is the zoo experience? How zoos impact a visitor’s behaviors, perceptions, and conservation efforts. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 1746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  129. Carter, M.; Sherwen, S.; Webber, S. An evaluation of interactive projections as digital enrichment for orangutans. Zoo Biol. 2021, 40, 107–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Clay, A.W.; Perdue, B.M.; Gaalema, D.E.; Dolins, F.L.; Bloomsmith, M.A. The use of technology to enhance zoological parks. Zoo Biol. 2011, 30, 487–497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  131. Coe, J.; Hoy, J. Choice, control and computers: Empowering wildlife in human care. Multimodal Technol. Interact. 2020, 4, 92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  132. Perdue, B.M.; Clay, A.W.; Gaalema, D.E.; Maple, T.L.; Stoinski, T.S. Technology at the zoo: The influence of a touchscreen computer on orangutans and zoo visitors. Zoo Biol. 2012, 31, 27–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Gray, J.M.; Diller, J.W. Evaluating the work of applied animal behaviorists as applied behavior analysis. Behav. Anal. Res. Pract. 2017, 17, 33–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Colorized photo of Keller Breland (left) and B. F. Skinner on top of the General Mills building in Minneapolis, MN, USA, circa 1943. (Photo courtesy of Robert E. Bailey).
Figure 1. Colorized photo of Keller Breland (left) and B. F. Skinner on top of the General Mills building in Minneapolis, MN, USA, circa 1943. (Photo courtesy of Robert E. Bailey).
Jzbg 02 00038 g001
Figure 2. Colorized photo of Keller and Marian Breland training IQ Zoo’s Professor Punch, late 1950s. (Photo courtesy of Robert E. Bailey).
Figure 2. Colorized photo of Keller and Marian Breland training IQ Zoo’s Professor Punch, late 1950s. (Photo courtesy of Robert E. Bailey).
Jzbg 02 00038 g002
Figure 3. Colorized photo of Keller Breland training a dolphin at Marine Studios/Marineland of Florida, circa 1957. (Reproduced from [40] with permission of John Wiley and Sons, 2014).
Figure 3. Colorized photo of Keller Breland training a dolphin at Marine Studios/Marineland of Florida, circa 1957. (Reproduced from [40] with permission of John Wiley and Sons, 2014).
Jzbg 02 00038 g003
Figure 4. Colorized photo of a polar bear chasing a frozen fish catapulted into their pool at the Portland (Oregon) Zoo, circa mid-1970s. (Reproduced from [63] with permission of American Veterinary Medical Association, 1977).
Figure 4. Colorized photo of a polar bear chasing a frozen fish catapulted into their pool at the Portland (Oregon) Zoo, circa mid-1970s. (Reproduced from [63] with permission of American Veterinary Medical Association, 1977).
Jzbg 02 00038 g004
Figure 5. Colorized photo of an Asian elephant pulling a ring to obtain fruit at the Honolulu Zoo, circa late-1970s. (Reproduced from [10] with permission of John Wiley & Sons Limited, 1982).
Figure 5. Colorized photo of an Asian elephant pulling a ring to obtain fruit at the Honolulu Zoo, circa late-1970s. (Reproduced from [10] with permission of John Wiley & Sons Limited, 1982).
Jzbg 02 00038 g005
Figure 6. Colorized photo of a serval chasing artificial prey at the San Francisco Zoo, circa 1984. (Reproduced from [74] with permission of Elsevier, 1982).
Figure 6. Colorized photo of a serval chasing artificial prey at the San Francisco Zoo, circa 1984. (Reproduced from [74] with permission of Elsevier, 1982).
Jzbg 02 00038 g006
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Fernandez, E.J.; Martin, A.L. Animal Training, Environmental Enrichment, and Animal Welfare: A History of Behavior Analysis in Zoos. J. Zool. Bot. Gard. 2021, 2, 531-543. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg2040038

AMA Style

Fernandez EJ, Martin AL. Animal Training, Environmental Enrichment, and Animal Welfare: A History of Behavior Analysis in Zoos. Journal of Zoological and Botanical Gardens. 2021; 2(4):531-543. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg2040038

Chicago/Turabian Style

Fernandez, Eduardo J., and Allison L. Martin. 2021. "Animal Training, Environmental Enrichment, and Animal Welfare: A History of Behavior Analysis in Zoos" Journal of Zoological and Botanical Gardens 2, no. 4: 531-543. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg2040038

APA Style

Fernandez, E. J., & Martin, A. L. (2021). Animal Training, Environmental Enrichment, and Animal Welfare: A History of Behavior Analysis in Zoos. Journal of Zoological and Botanical Gardens, 2(4), 531-543. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg2040038

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop