Next Article in Journal
Ethnomedicinal Use, Phytochemistry, Pharmacology, and Toxicology of Euphorbia resinifera O. Berg. (B): A Review
Previous Article in Journal
Role of Endemism and Other Factors in Determining the Introduction Success of Rare and Threatened Species in Tashkent Botanical Garden
Previous Article in Special Issue
Improving Animal Wellbeing Using Behavior-Based Methodologies: A Discussion on Enrichment and Bears under Human Care
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Investigating Object Recognition Memory Using Sensory Enrichment with a North American River Otter (Lontra canadensis)

by
Jessica J. Wegman
and
Caroline M. DeLong
*
Department of Psychology, College of Liberal Arts, Rochester Institute of Technology, 18 Lomb Memorial Dr., Rochester, NY 14623, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Zool. Bot. Gard. 2023, 4(2), 335-363; https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg4020028
Submission received: 21 February 2023 / Revised: 31 March 2023 / Accepted: 5 April 2023 / Published: 12 April 2023

Abstract

:
Research studies that shed light on cognitive and perceptual abilities in otters can utilize tasks that provide environmental, structural, food-based, sensory, or cognitive enrichment. The current study examined the use of the novel object recognition task, a task commonly used to study memory in non-human animals, as a form of sensory enrichment. The subject of the current study was an adult male otter that resided at the Seneca Park Zoo in Rochester, NY, USA. The stimulus pairs for this task were multisensory (3D objects and odorants). In this study, three memory intervals were investigated: 10 min, 1 h, and 24 h (each memory interval included 10 sessions). The otter spent only 15% of his time near the stimulus pairs and engaged in very few explorations, suggesting that this was not an effective form of sensory enrichment and did not provide any evidence for long-term memory. These results contrast strongly with our previous studies with otters using a two-alternative forced-choice task that provided engaging cognitive enrichment. We suggest that cognitive enrichment, including enrichment via training (and food-based enrichment), may be more effective for otters than sensory enrichment. Future research should further investigate cognitive phenomena in otters using tasks involving cognitive enrichment. These types of studies can improve enrichment practices and promote positive welfare for otters in zoos, inform conservation efforts, and grow our limited knowledge of otter perception and cognition.

1. Introduction

The Association of Zoos and Aquariums defines enrichment as a process that improves and/or enhances animal environments and care, with the goal of increasing behavioral choices and providing opportunities for species-appropriate behaviors [1]. Enrichment is a principle that is meant to “enhance the quality of captive animal care by identifying and providing the environmental stimuli necessary for optimal psychological and physiological well-being” (p. 1) [2]. There are various types of enrichment, with environmental enrichment being the most common and often just referred to plainly as enrichment, given how routine it is for animals in human care [3]. Environmental enrichment can include foraging tasks, toys and objects that can be manipulated by the animal, physically altering the environment, and sensory stimuli. Various other subsets of enrichment include food-based enrichment (the animal gains food as part of the enrichment), structural enrichment (changes or additions are made to the environment for enrichment purposes), sensory enrichment (engages one or more senses as part of enrichment), and cognitive enrichment.
Cognitive enrichment promotes well-being by engaging cognitive skills in animals but is not frequently used in captive settings [3]. For example, computer game tasks that test cognitive skills using discrimination tasks via a computer touchscreen serve as cognitive enrichment [4]. Any task that requires training, such as a two-alternative forced-choice task or a match to sample task, can also be viewed as cognitive enrichment because the animals must learn behaviors by interacting with a human trainer and follow conceptual rules to gain a reward. Cognitive enrichment has been gaining more attention; however, it is likely not used as much due to the increased amount of time and effort from human caretakers and staff this type of enrichment can necessitate. Without proper enrichment, animals residing in human care can face negative consequences. For example, carnivores such as otters can display abnormal behaviors, such as pacing or hair plucking, when under human care if they expend too little effort to obtain their food [5]. However, when appropriate enrichment devices or techniques are used for the species, abnormal behavior patterns can decrease in otters [6,7].
When considering what kind of enrichment to use for an animal, it is important to understand their habitat and the naturalistic behaviors that species would display in that environment. North American river otters are members of the subfamily Lutrinae within the family Mustelidae. There are 13 otter species that vary in their habitat, diet, size, and foraging skills [8]. North American river otters are found throughout most of North America from the Atlantic to Pacific coasts in a wide range of habitats including rivers, streams, lakes, bogs, and rocky seacoasts [9]. North American river otters normally reside in social groups of females and their young or unrelated males that have overlapping home ranges with other males showing their gregariousness [9]. Their primary food source tends to be small, bottom-dwelling fish, but they also consume crustaceans and insects along with a variety of other species in more opportunistic situations such as birds, mammals, reptiles, and amphibians [10,11,12,13,14,15,16]. Therefore, otters forage both underwater and on land, depending on their prey type. North American river otters usually find their den sites on land, utilizing available structures such as tree logs and abandoned dens made by other animals [17]. Female otters will also prepare natal nests on land near small streams in a secluded location [17]. As semi-aquatic mammals, North American river otters spend time both in the water as well as on land, so they frequently use their senses both underwater and in the air.
Enrichment is frequently used in zoos to encourage species-specific, naturalistic behaviors and to reduce stereotypical behaviors, which is important for carnivores like river otters that can capture a diverse array of prey items [18]. There have been few studies examining enrichment practices with otters, and they have largely focused on foraging tasks and food-based enrichment. For example, Ross [7] introduced a simple feeding enrichment technique for Asian small-clawed otters (Aonyx cinerea) where two of their three daily meals were inserted into grapevine balls and then placed in a pool. Various stereotypical behaviors, including auto hair-plucking and door manipulations (trying to move/open doors), significantly decreased in the two otters when the feeding enrichment was used. Foster-Turley and Markowitz [19] also used a simple feeding enrichment task where Asian small-clawed otters were given the opportunity to hunt live crickets. Compared to when the otters were given dead crickets, cat food, and gelatin capsules, the otters were most engaged with hunting the live crickets. This foraging opportunity increased the activity of the otters, and they still chose to engage in the foraging activities even when other food items were easily available. In this study, the otters were very inquisitive and would seek out a challenge rather than have easy access to their food [19].
In another study, Nelson [6] introduced multiple environmental enrichment initiatives to a male North American river otter. These included live fish, frozen fish, a swim tube (a large tube the otter could swim through), and PVC scent tubes placed throughout the exhibit. The two food-based enrichment initiatives (live fish and frozen fish) were the most effective at decreasing stereotyped behaviors, and increasing species-appropriate behaviors, compared to the other two enrichment initiatives [6]. These three studies [6,7,19] show how effective food-based enrichment tasks appear to be for Asian small-clawed otters and river otters but do not explore other types of enrichment extensively.
Cognitive challenges are important for captive animals to experience and may be especially important for otters. Even when they fail to solve the task, the cognitive challenge itself may be enriching for otters [18]. This concept was supported in a study where two sea otters (Enhydra lutris) were given a novel tool-use task. The sea otters were not successful in solving the tool-use task; however, for one of the otters it seemed that the task was still enriching. The otter spent 55.4% of her time during each trial with the apparatus, compared to the other otter that spent only 6.2% of his time with the apparatus [18]. This study also highlights how it can be important for enrichment to be individualized. Some tasks may be engaging and enriching for certain individuals but not for others.
In addition to tasks that are designed with enrichment as the main goal, cognitive tasks designed to study the perceptual or cognitive abilities of animals can also be enriching. The animal must use their cognitive skills to engage in the task they are participating in, which could be classified as cognitive enrichment [3]. Two studies used a two-alternative forced-choice task to study the perceptual and cognitive abilities of North American river otters [20,21]. In this task, the otter is trained to choose between two different visual stimuli on each trial. A correct choice results in a food reward, whereas an incorrect choice results in the otter not receiving food for that trial (note that the otters were never deprived of food; they always received their full diet each day). The otters were often motivated to participate in this cognitive task even when it could be quite challenging for the otters to learn a conceptual rule. For example, although one otter’s performance was not significantly different than chance during the first test of the second experiment [20], the otter would still readily participate on every trial (performance on later tests was approximately 70% correct, with the other otter achieving approximately 80% correct in the first experiment). In addition to these cognitive tasks being enriching for the animal subject, it also allows researchers to learn more about the cognitive and perceptual abilities of many understudied species that are kept in zoos. For example, DeLong et al. [20] found preliminary evidence of categorization (the ability to group objects together based on defining features) in North American river otters. These scientific findings can in turn be applied to aid in creating better enrichment initiatives for the species studied across many zoos.
Various types of enrichment can be used to study cognitive phenomena in otters, such as memory. Saliveros et al. [22] studied spatial and long-term memory of Asian short-clawed otters (Aonyx cinereus) in a foraging context. Three groups of otters (n = 25 individuals) were presented with five novel extractive foraging tasks (which could be classified as either food-based or cognitive enrichment). The otters solved all five foraging puzzles significantly faster the second time they were introduced (M = 65.23 s) compared to the first time (M = 160.64 s), even when this time interval between sessions was greater than 100 days. This study shows support for otters possessing long-term memory capabilities in a foraging context. A simulated foraging task provided cognitive enrichment while investigating spatial memory in Asian small-clawed otters [23]. Thus far, no studies have examined memory of any kind in North American river otters (Lontra canadensis).
In the current study, we sought to examine long-term object recognition memory (the ability to recognize a familiar object over varying lengths of time; [24]) in a North American river otter using a technique that involved sensory enrichment. It is important to study memory in otters to gain knowledge about the species as well as inform enrichment programs for zoos with otters. The ability to remember objects could be useful and adaptive for otters, such as while foraging. There is evidence that otters use sight, as their primary sensory modality when foraging, as well as touch to catch their prey [25]. A foraging otter could use multiple sensory modalities along with object recognition memory to find the prey that will result in the greatest metabolic payoff [26]. Otters could also potentially remember aversive events, including conflicts with predators such as wolves, coyotes, bobcats, alligators, and/or killer whales [9], depending on where the otters reside. Otters could use object recognition memory to identify predators and avoid future conflicts.
In order to develop proper sensory enrichment, it is important to understand the perceptual abilities of the species the enrichment is being designed for. However, not much is known about the visual acuity of North American river otters. Balliet and Schusterman [27] found that in bright conditions, Asian small-clawed otters had equivalent visual acuity in the air (14–15 min of arc) as they did underwater (15–16 min of arc). However, in dim light conditions Asian small-clawed otters showed better visual acuity in the air (38–39 min of arc) than underwater (57–58 min of arc; [28]). Regarding color vision, two anatomical studies found that both sea otters and European river otters (Lutra lutra) have dichromatic color vision with S and M/L cones [28,29,30]. In a behavioral study, European river otters were able to discriminate blue and green from multiple shades of gray (Kasprzyk, 1990, cited in [31]). Svoke et al. [32] suggests that Asian small-clawed otters likely have dichromatic vision as well. Additionally, DeLong et al. [21] found preliminary evidence of dichromatic vision in North American river otters, though further research is necessary.
There have only been two studies that examined visual object discrimination in any species of otter and both investigated North American river otters [21,33]. Slack [33] presented two North American river otters, a male and female, with a variety of 2D stimulus pairs. In a two-alternative forced-choice task, one otter succeeded with all eleven stimulus pairs while the second otter succeeded with only two of the stimulus pairs. The features used by the otters to complete the task were not investigated. DeLong et al. [21] presented two North American river otters with 2D objects to explore salient visual features, including shape and color. Both otters successfully learned to discriminate between training stimuli using multiple features, including shape and color in combination with other possible features such as the size and brightness of the stimuli. One of the otters was able to discriminate between test stimuli equally well using either shape (when color was removed as a cue) or color (when shape was removed as a cue). The DeLong et al. [21] study showed that North American river otters are able to discriminate between 2D objects using multiple features and are potentially equally likely to rely on color or shape.
In addition to vision, olfaction is an important sensory modality in otters which can be engaged during sensory enrichment. Rostain et al. [34] showed that river otters perceive olfactory signals that communicate both sex and species identity. Signaling territory through scent cues is a behavior that otters frequently engage in. North American river otters tend to leave spraint sites, mostly consisting of large piles of scat, outside of their dens, next to freshwater pools along seacoasts, at junctions of streams, and near trees [9]. To date, there is no published research to suggest how often otters use olfaction to identify objects other than animals or individual otters. Similarly lacking is psychophysical research on olfactory detection and discrimination in otters.
We chose to investigate object recognition memory using sensory enrichment involving both visual and olfactory elements. Object recognition memory is a type of memory that may aid non-human animals, including otters, in avoiding predators, locating food, choosing mates, and selecting an appropriate habitat. Object recognition memory can be defined as the ability to discriminate between familiar and novel objects [35]. Object recognition memory has been studied in a variety of non-human animals such as dogs [36], domestic pigs [35], rodents [37,38,39], monkeys [40], and fish [41,42] as well as human infants (e.g., [43,44]). To date, there has not been any research examining object recognition memory in any species of otter using any of these tasks.
Object recognition memory is frequently assessed in non-human animals using the novel object recognition (NOR) task [42], which was the task used in the current study. The NOR task was originally developed for rats [45] but has been adapted and used to investigate object recognition memory in many different non-human animal species (e.g., monkeys, horses, birds, cats, and pigs). In this task, an animal is first exposed to two identical objects in the familiarization trial that they have never seen before (e.g., two balls that are exactly the same in shape, size, color, material, etc.). The animal is then removed from the test area for the memory interval that the researcher wants to test. The memory interval is the time between the end of the familiarization trial and the test trial. Finally, the animals reenter the test area where this time they are exposed to one familiar object from the previous familiarization trial, as well as a novel object (e.g., one of the balls they were shown in the familiarization trial and a water bottle that they have never seen before). The animal is then observed, and their behavior is recorded in both the familiarization and the test trials to see if they show a significant preference for either object, usually based on the amount of time spent close to and/or interacting with the object [24,41,42,46]. Significantly more time spent near or interacting with either the familiar or the novel object would demonstrate that the animal remembered the familiar object and thus is evidence of object recognition memory [42]. If no difference is found in the time spent with the familiar or novel object, this would demonstrate that the animal did not remember the familiar object, which would not provide evidence of object recognition memory.
The NOR task has many advantages that make it suitable to collect data on memory in otters while also serving as sensory enrichment. The NOR task is relatively simple in that it does not involve training (e.g., training the animal via operant conditioning to approach or avoid any stimulus for a food reward), so there is less involvement from zookeepers, and the results can be obtained relatively quickly. The NOR task is also easily adapted to a variety of species, making it quite a useful task in comparative psychology research [47]. Since the NOR task has been used in such a wide variety of species, is relatively simple to perform, and has ecological relevance for many species [47] this was a good task to use as a contrast to a highly time-intensive cognitive enrichment training task (as in [21]). The presentations of numerous different stimuli during the NOR task allow the otters to explore new objects using vision and olfaction, which provides opportunities for sensory enrichment.
The current study was the first study to utilize the NOR task with any species of otter. One goal of this study was to explore the usefulness of a relatively simple, less time-intensive sensory enrichment task in otters, and contrast the engagement and attention of the otters with highly time-intensive cognitive enrichment training tasks. Another goal was to search for evidence of object recognition memory in otters. When conducting cognitive research with zoo animals, it is best to provide tasks that can serve as some form of enrichment. It is beneficial to the animals participating in the research, and it fulfills a mission of the AZA by enhancing the welfare of animals under human care [48].
The current study provided sensory enrichment by utilizing multimodal stimulus pairs. The stimulus pairs in this study were 3D objects that were visually accessible paired with odorants that were contained in plastic bottles and presented next to the 3D objects. Visual stimuli are frequently used in studies examining object recognition memory, especially when the NOR task is used, e.g., [24,35,36,37,41,42]. However, olfaction seems to be a primary sensory modality in otters that is used throughout their daily lives [49]. In fact, olfaction is said to be one of the most frequently used senses in mustelids [50]. Since otters may frequently use both vision and olfaction, it seemed best to use multimodal stimulus pairs in this study to ensure that the otter could discriminate between the objects as well as encode multiple cues during the familiarization trial. Therefore, multiple cues were provided in two (potentially three if the otter chose to touch the stimulus pairs) different sensory modalities to maximize potential discrimination and enrichment opportunities.
The objects and the odorants were novel to the otter. New objects that the otter had never encountered were used for the visual component of the stimulus pairs. Odorants were selected based on several criteria: a food item the otter does not eat (e.g., no meat scents were used), the otter was unlikely to have encountered it before (e.g., artificial floral scents), and the zookeeper reported it was not used in any previous enrichment experiences. Another reason for using multimodal stimuli is multisensory facilitation, which is the phenomena where organisms benefit from using multiple senses to complete cognitive tasks [51]. The otter subject in our study could potentially use vision, olfaction, or both to remember the stimulus pairs.
This study investigated object recognition memory at three memory intervals (10 min, 1 h, and 24 h). These memory intervals were chosen to test a range of intervals while limiting the total number of sessions (10 sessions per interval) to fit in a constrained testing period. The familiarization trials were recorded in the current study to observe whether time spent with the stimuli and explorations of the stimuli decreased over time and then restrict analyses for the test trials to the appropriate time period. We hypothesized that the otter would show evidence of object recognition memory across all the memory intervals tested since Saliveros et al. [22] found evidence of long-term memory within a foraging context of Asian short-clawed otters after memory intervals of over 100 days. If the NOR task provides effective sensory enrichment it could inform future enrichment programs for the otters. In contrast, if this task does not provide effective sensory enrichment, it suggests other forms of enrichment may be more optimal for river otters.

2. Materials and Method

2.1. Subject

The subject was one male North American river otter who resided at the Seneca Park Zoo in Rochester, NY, USA since 2012. Sailor was 14 years old at the start of the study and did not have any abnormalities or diseases of the eyes or nose at his most recent veterinary exams. He was considered elderly for otters (individuals older than 13 years; [8]) and was on a prescription of arthritis medication due to some behavioral signs of the disease, but he had a normal range of motion and was given a clean bill of health. Prior to the current study, Sailor participated in three behavioral studies over the course of five years (2016–2021). The first study he participated in investigated shape and color discrimination [21], the second focused on global and local processing [52], and the third and most recent study examined visual object categorization [20]. All of these previous studies used the two-alternative forced-choice task with 2D objects as stimuli. None of the stimuli from these studies were used in the current study. We intended to include a 5-year-old female otter, Ashkii, but she was excluded from the task during the pilot study (see below).
Sailor’s daily diet was on a sliding scale based on his weight, so it could fluctuate slightly. At the beginning of the study, he received 250 g of fish in the morning and 200 g of meat in the afternoon. This diet was determined by the animal care staff at the Seneca Park Zoo. He was also sometimes fed various types of seafood (clams, cod, crab, crayfish, flounder, shrimps, and/or scallops) as part of the enrichment activities at the zoo. He was fed 100% of his morning meal of fish prior to all sessions in the current study. He received his entire daily diet regardless of performance during study sessions.
The otter’s habitat at the Seneca Park Zoo is divided into two separate areas, one for public exhibition and an off-exhibit area. The public exhibition area (33.5 m × 18.3 m) is a naturalistic habitat that contains a large upper pool (9.1 m × 2.4 m × 1.8 m) connected to a waterfall that leads down into the lower pool (9.1 m × 4.6 m × 1.5 m). These pools are surrounded by dirt banks that are covered in natural grasses and flowers, shrubbery, felled conifer trees, and solid and hollowed-out logs. The habitat also contains at different times various enrichment devices both in the water and on the land including plastic tunnels, plastic rafts, buoys, and a jellyfish float (a garbage can lid with mesh straps attached). There is a large observation window that is split into three panes (central window = 3.5 m in length, side windows = 1.7 m each in length) located at the upper pool where guests can view the otters both underwater and on the land portion of the habitat. The off-exhibit habitat is outdoors and includes two separate areas completely enclosed with chain-link fencing, covered by a metal roof, and containing cement floors. Both enclosures are divided into three pens that are connected by gates that the keepers can lift to move the otters to the various areas within the enclosures. The enclosures usually contained dens (plastic dog houses) with blankets, hammocks, plastic slides, and small plastic pools, though these were sometimes varied between the pens. Sailor and Ashkii were housed together with access to three adjacent pens (2 m × 1.6 m, 1 m × 1.5 m, and 2 m × 1.5 m).

2.2. Materials

All sessions were filmed using a GoPro® Hero Silver 4 camera. The camera was mounted onto the chain-link fence that surrounds the off-exhibit enclosure, at the back of the area, using a plastic clip attachment that came with the camera (Figure 1). The experimental area was divided into three sections (Figure 1 and Figure 2) using a Sharpie™ silver metallic permanent marker to aid in video coding. Section 1 and Section 2 were 91.4 cm by 74.3 cm and Section 3 was 182.8 cm by 148.6 cm. Zip tie loops were attached to the stimulus pairs using Honyear™ black, 20 cm zip ties and then both the object and odorant were hung on the chain-link fence using Gabbro™ black aluminum D-shaped carabiner clips that were 5 cm × 3 cm. The odorants were applied with a dropper onto a 4 cm × 2 cm strip of filter paper that was then put into a Cornucopia™ mini plastic spice jar (5.3 cm × 4.6 cm) with 11 holes (0.3 cm diameter each) on the top. This method of presentation allowed for the odorant to be concentrated near the 3D objects and ensured the otter was not able to easily distribute the odor around the enclosure [53]. Med Pride™ vinyl gloves made from latex-free rubber were used when handling all stimulus pairs so there were not any other odors contaminating the stimulus pairs. To clean all stimulus pairs before they are placed in the enclosure, due to COVID-19 precautions, 70% isopropyl alcohol was used with a paper towel to wipe the stimulus pairs. This was left to dry for at least one minute and also helped to remove any possible odors (such as the odor from my own hands, any scented soaps or perfumes on my hands, etc.) other than the odorants.

2.3. Stimulus Pairs

Each stimulus pair contained a visual and olfactory element. The otter was able to view the 3D objects and smell the odorants. He was always tested during the day and all stimuli were presented in the air, not underwater. The use of odorants in this study did not permit us to present the stimulus pairs underwater since odorants diffuse in water. It was essential to this study that the odorants stay concentrated and close to the stimulus pairs, which was only achievable in the air and not underwater.
Objects. The 3D objects included items such as pet toys, machined objects, objects constructed from PVC tubes, and water bottles (Figure 3). The pet toys were ordered from Petco®, PetSmart®, and Otto Environmental™. These objects were chosen because they are durable (unlikely to be damaged by the otters) and able to be cleaned for COVID-19 protocols at the zoo (and cleaned if Sailor were to soil them in any way during his explorations, though this was never observed). The objects were of ‘intermediate size’ (ranging from 7 cm to 37.5 cm in length, 4 cm to 27.5 cm in width, and 1 cm to 16.5 cm in depth) compared to the size of the otter. None of the objects were substantially smaller than the stimuli used in the DeLong and et al. [21] study where the otters showed success in object discrimination. All objects and their attachment gear (zip ties) were approved to be safe for the otters by the Seneca Park Zoo staff prior to use in this study. The objects were selected because they were thought to have equal levels of biological relevance to the otter. At the Seneca Park Zoo, similar objects are often used for enrichment such as puzzle feeders made of PVC, plastic rafts, and toys such as plastic balls. Therefore, we estimated our objects would all fall within this general category of potential enrichment objects and thus have intermediate levels of significance to the otter. None of the objects were associated with food, to our best knowledge, which would likely be of more biological relevance to the otter.
Odorants. Essential oils from Candles and Supplies, Inc., McCormick® food extracts, and OOOFlavors® flavored liquid concentrates were used. Only scents that Sailor had not been exposed to previously (as far as the experimenter and zookeepers knew) were included. All odorants were approved to be safe for the otters by the Seneca Park Zoo staff prior to use in this study. Odorants known to the zoo to be problematic for dogs and cats were avoided (e.g., anise, mint, and peppermint extract; and azalea, gardenia, honeysuckle, lavender, lily, and ylang ylang essential oils). Sailor did not show any aversive reactions to the selected odorants in the current study. Additionally, odorants (usually perfumes and colognes, none of which we used in the current study) have been used at the zoo with the otters for potential enrichment purposes.
Stimulus Pairings. The stimulus pairings of 3D objects and odorants were created semi-randomly. The objects in stimulus pairs that were paired together within a session (e.g., the novel and familiar objects) were approximately the same size, but differed in color, shape, and material to maximize visual discriminability (Figure 3). The side of the enclosure that the novel stimulus pair was placed on for each session was also semi-randomized. The novel stimulus pair appeared an equal number of times on the right as it did on the left for each memory interval (Table 1).

2.4. Experimental Setup

The experimental setup was in one of the pens in the off-exhibit habitat (1.6 m [height] × 2 m [width] × 1.8 m [depth]). Guests did not have access to this area, so the otter was less likely to be distracted during the sessions. The experimental area was located in the pen closest to the on-exhibit area where no previous studies have taken place. The otters normally used this section of their enclosure to sleep, so their dens with blankets and hay were kept in this enclosure but were removed prior to all of the sessions. This area of the enclosure was also used by the otters to enter the gate that leads to the public exhibition habitat. There were three gates (76.2 cm × 30.5 cm) in this part of the enclosure, though only two were used regularly by the otters (Figure 1). One gate (gate 3) connected to a tunnel that the otters used to access the on-exhibit portion of their habitat. The other two gates lead to an adjacent pen, though only one was used regularly as they both lead to the same area.

2.5. Procedure

Prior to each trial (both familiarization and test trials), the otter was moved to the off-exhibit pen farthest from the experimental area, and all items were removed from the experimental area. The floor was then hosed down with water and drained to remove any debris and/or odors that were present in the enclosure. Stimulus pairs were wiped with 70% isopropyl alcohol; then the experimenter connected the stimulus pair to carabiners and attached them to the chain-link walls inside the enclosure (35.6 cm up from the bottom of the mesh wall and 50.8 cm over from the side corner of the mesh wall). Sailor had direct access to the stimulus pair in that he could touch them since the stimuli were hanging inside the enclosure. However, Sailor could not move the stimulus pairs around the enclosure as they were anchored to the chain-link wall. The camera was then mounted near the ceiling onto the chain-link wall of the enclosure, so the entire experimental area was captured in the view. Once the stimulus pairs and camera were in place, the camera would begin recording and the door to the pen was closed and locked. Gate 1 (Figure 1) was then opened so Sailor was able to enter the experimental area. The other otter Ashkii would remain out on-exhibit while Sailor was being tested, and she was given an enrichment item (such as small pieces of fish scattered throughout the naturalistic on-exhibit enclosure or earthworms scattered in the large pool on-exhibit) to minimize her attempts to come through the tunnel and wait next to the gate by Sailor. Once Sailor was in the experimental area all three gates were closed and the experimenter and zookeeper left so they were not in the otter’s view. Sailor remained in the enclosure for the duration of the session, and he was monitored by both the experimenter and zookeeper via live video from the camera in his enclosure for any signs of distress, which he never displayed during the study. Once the session was complete the zookeeper would open the gate again so the otter could leave the experimental area. At this time, the camera stopped recording, the stimulus pairs and camera were removed from the experimental area, and the stimulus pairs were cleaned again with the 70% isopropyl alcohol spray. The enclosure was again hosed down with water in case any of the odorants were still present. For the 24-h memory intervals, all items (e.g., dens, enrichment devices) were placed back into the enclosure for the otters during the memory interval. During the sessions with the 1-h and 10-min memory intervals, the enclosure was still hosed down but none of the items were placed back in the enclosure until the end of the test session. Sessions were run on separate days to ensure there was no cross-contamination of different odorants used for different sessions.

3. Pilot Study

Three total pilot sessions were conducted before starting the experimental study over the course of two weeks. During the first pilot session we ran through the procedure for an entire session (familiarization trial, 10-min memory interval, and a test trial) first without any otters present, and then we attempted to include each of the otters in an entire session. However, Ashkii displayed behaviors (continuous pacing, climbing on the gate, and scratching at the door) indicating she could not complete a full session, which prevented her inclusion in the experiment and prompted her removal from the last two pilot sessions (per zoo staff recommendation). Sailor did not show the same behaviors as Ashkii in the first pilot session. During the second pilot session, 25 min into the familiarization session Sailor started clawing at the gate for more than a few seconds. Thus, we reduced the time in the familiarization trial from 30 min to 20 min and a small pool was added to the back of the experimental area in Section 3 (Figure 1) to provide an alternative activity for Sailor. With this addition, Sailor no longer showed problematic behaviors during the test trial of the third pilot session. A third pilot test was completed for Sailor with these changes in place and no problematic behaviors were observed.

4. Experimental Study

In this study, one session consisted of a familiarization trial (two of the same stimulus pairs were presented), a memory interval (the amount of time between the familiarization and test trials), and a test trial (one stimulus pair from the familiarization trial and a novel stimulus pair were presented [Figure 4]). A habituation session was not conducted before the familiarization trial, where the otter would be exposed to the area without any objects. It was not deemed necessary to do so since the otter already lived in the experimental area, so he should have already been habituated to the area. The pilot study could have also served as habituation sessions for the otter, though stimulus pairs were presented during these sessions. The experimental sessions were conducted over a period of three months.

4.1. Familiarization Trial

The purpose of the familiarization trial was for the otter to be exposed to two identical stimulus pairs for the first time. In the test trial, this became the familiar stimulus pair. The familiarization trial lasted 20 min, and the entire trial was recorded with the video camera. The otter entered the experimental area where two identical stimulus pairs (e.g., two green pet toys as seen in Figure 4, with sweet pea as the odorant) were presented on each side of the enclosure. The zookeeper then went to the right side of the enclosure (Section 2, Figure 1) and had Sailor target the stimulus pair twice where he had to touch the stimulus pair with his nose. Sailor was rewarded with a small piece of fish given to him through the chain-link in the area away from the stimulus pair (Section 3, Figure 1). The zookeeper then went to the left side of the enclosure (Section 1, Figure 1) and repeated the same process. This was done in order to give Sailor a reward for entering the enclosure and to encourage him to approach and pay attention to the stimulus pairs. The explorations Sailor made with the stimulus pairs while targeting were not included in later analyses as explorations with the stimulus pairs since he was given a command to touch the stimulus pair. Once the zookeeper finished this targeting procedure, all personnel left the area. After 20 min, the otter was moved from the experimental area (the zookeeper opened the gate) to go into the adjacent pens. The experimenter would then be able to enter the experimental area and remove the stimulus pairs for the duration of the memory interval.

4.2. Memory Interval

The memory interval began as soon as the otter finished the familiarization trial by exiting the experimental area. The exact time was noted as soon as the otter left the experimental area, and the gate was closed. Three memory intervals were tested in the current study: 10 min, 1 h, and 24 h. Given that there was a single subject available for testing, a power analysis indicated that adequate power would be achieved with 10 sessions per memory interval (30 total sessions). During the 10 min and 1 h memory intervals, Sailor had access to the two adjacent areas in the off-exhibit enclosure, but he did not have access to the experimental area nor the on-exhibit enclosure. This was done to ensure that Sailor would be ready to enter the experimental area the second that the memory interval ended. During the 24-h memory interval, Sailor had access to the entirety of the off-exhibit enclosure, including the experimental area where all his dens and enrichment items were placed back inside, as well as the on-exhibit enclosure. This was done because during a typical day the otters are able to go in-between the on- and off-exhibit enclosures as they please, which is what they prefer. The zoo staff also prefers for the otters to be on-exhibit as much as possible and not locked in the off-exhibit enclosure for extended periods of time if possible.

4.3. Test Trial

The test trials took place after the memory interval was completed. Near the end of the memory interval, approximately during the last five minutes, the experimenter began prepping the test stimulus pairs behind a gate and out of view of the otter. These consisted of one familiar stimulus pair (from the familiarization trial, e.g., the PVC object and the McCormick® pumpkin pie spice food extract from block 1 session 1 [Figure 3]) and one novel stimulus pair (e.g., the plastic machined “Z” and the magnolia essential oil from block 1 session 1 [Figure 3]). The experimenter placed the stimulus pairs in the enclosure no more than five minutes before the otter was able to enter the experimental area when the memory interval was complete. The otter then spent 15 min in the experimental area during the test trial. The entire trial was recorded on the camera. After the trial ended, the otter was moved from the experimental area (by opening a gate) and the stimulus pairs and camera were removed.

4.4. Session Order

Sailor completed a total of 30 sessions (three memory intervals, with each memory interval being tested ten times). The number of sessions was determined by a power analysis that indicated at an alpha of 0.05 and with a moderate effect size, a power value of 0.7 would be achieved if at least 27 total sessions were completed. Additionally, more than about 40 sessions was not feasible given the time frame of the study and the number of sessions per week we were allowed to run with the otter (usually no more than 4, though occasional exceptions were made to allow 5 sessions in one week in order to complete some of the 24-h memory intervals). To ensure there were not any order effects, the intervals were semi-randomized. Simple randomization was not used as there was a possibility the same memory interval could be completed many times in a row. Therefore, when randomizing the memory interval order, the sessions were divided into five blocks. Each block contained six sessions, with each memory interval repeated twice per block (Table 1). The first three blocks were then randomized individually. Blocks 4 and 5 repeated the order of memory intervals for blocks 1 and 2 but with different stimulus pairs. This allowed for a more equal distribution of the memory intervals throughout the study while still being able to semi-randomize the sessions. Additionally, otters can show seasonal effects in performance (e.g., [21]), and data were collected during the end of summer through late fall (August through October 2021). DeLong et al. [21] noted that otters tend to behave differently during hotter weather. An otter could have different results in the summer compared to the late fall if he did not want to move around the enclosure as much during the hotter summer weather. Therefore, it was important to try to have the three memory intervals evenly distributed throughout these seasons so that a single memory interval was more or less unimpacted by these potential seasonal differences in behavior.

5. Video Coding

Video coding was completed by three independent coders who were all blind to the identity of the stimulus pairs (whether they were novel or familiar) while coding. Since they were blind, the coders just referred to the stimulus pairs as right stimulus pair and left stimulus pair when coding. The experimenter trained the coders using two familiarization session videos as examples, though the experimenter did not serve as one of the three video coders. All of the test trials and the familiarization trials were coded by a primary coder. There were also two secondary coders. Each secondary coder coded 50% of the data, one of each memory interval within each test block (Table 1), instead of 100% of the data like the primary coder. The secondary coders did not have access to the primary coder’s data. The two secondary coders did have access to each other’s data, but they coded the opposite 50% of the videos (e.g., they never coded the same video). Therefore, 100% of the videos were coded by one of the secondary coders. The criterion for inter-rater reliability was set at 80% or higher [54]. This was not met at first when the familiarization videos were coded, so the five instances where there were large discrepancies between coders (differences in timing greater than 10 s, or any differences in the number of explorations) were highlighted. The procedures for video coding were reviewed with all three coders in a re-training meeting with the experimenter. The main coder and the reliability coder for that video then recoded that part of the video. After recoding the criterion was met for all familiarization sessions.
There were two dependent variables that were coded in each video of test trials: (1) the amount of time spent in each section of the enclosure, and (2) the number of explorations made to each stimulus pair with the nose or paw on either the 3D object or the odorant bottle. The time spent near each stimulus pair is almost always examined as the primary dependent variable in the NOR task, (e.g., [35,41,42]), and the number of touches (explorations) has been used as a secondary dependent variable [24]. To code the amount of time spent on each side of the enclosure, there were visible lines on the floor separating the areas close to each stimulus pair from the area not in close proximity to the stimulus pairs at the back of the enclosure (Figure 1). The video was watched in its entirety twice; once to code the time spent close to the right stimulus pair, and once to code the time spent close to the left stimulus pair. During the first time watching the video, the coder kept a tally of the number of explorations to both stimulus pairs. This was repeated the second time watching the video to ensure the correct number of explorations was coded. If necessary, it was permitted for the coders to watch the video for a third time, skipping to a certain section of the video where there was a potential exploration. This was done on occasion because it was sometimes difficult to determine if the otter was close enough to the stimulus pair (within a few centimeters) to count as an exploration.
To code the time spent with a stimulus pair, when the otter’s entire body crossed one of the lines (both his front and back legs, but not his tail), the coder would start a stopwatch. As soon as the otter’s head and front paws touched the line again as he was heading to the opposite side or the back of the enclosure, the coder would pause the stopwatch. Section 1 and Section 2 were coded separately and Section 3 was not coded as it was not necessary for any of the planned analyses. In the familiarization trials, the amount of time spent in Section 1 and Section 2 was coded in 5-min increments (Figure 1). The average amount of time spent near the stimulus pairs and number of explorations was compared across each 5-min increment during the familiarization trial. Lucon-Xiccato and Dadda (2014) only coded the first five minutes of their test trials after they found a decrease in time spent close to the stimulus after the first five minutes in the familiarization trials, and a similar pattern was found in the current study. After the first five minutes in the familiarization sessions, Sailor spent very little time with the stimulus pair on the right and hardly made any explorations (Figure 5). Therefore, when coding the test videos only the first five minutes were coded and included in the analyses.
To code the explorations, the coder kept a tally of the number of times the otter touched each stimulus pair while they were coding the time spent on each side. The video was coded for the number of explorations in one complete sitting. It was sometimes difficult to determine in the video if the otter made direct contact with the stimulus, or if he was just very close to the stimulus (within a few centimeters) but not actually touching it. Therefore, an exploration was defined as the otter’s nose being within a few centimeters from the stimulus. Sailor has been previously trained to touch stimuli with his nose, so whenever any touches were made to the stimuli he used his nose not his paws. This is why his nose was specified in the definition of an exploration for the purpose of the current study. Additionally, during video coding none of the coders ever saw Sailor touch the stimulus pairs with his paws.

6. Data Analyses

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS Statistics for Mac, version 28 (SPSS Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). All of the data were checked manually to ensure no data entry errors were made. There was no missing data since all sessions were completed in full. Since this was a single subject design, determining the normality of the familiarization and test data was important for choosing a statistical method. Normality of the familiarization data was assessed using a Shapiro–Wilk test and showed that the overall time spent near the two stimulus pairs did depart significantly from normality (W = 0.95, p = 0.004). The Normal Q-Q plot and histogram were examined to further assess normality. The Q-Q plot provided evidence of normality, but the histogram seemed to show that the data were of a more uniform distribution. Finally, the skewness was 0.27 (values between −1 and 1 are generally considered excellent and indicate the shape of the data are fairly normal, while values between −2 and 2 are generally acceptable; George & Mallery, 2020), indicating that the distribution was approximately symmetric, and the kurtosis was −1.12, indicating the distribution was more heavy-tailed compared to the normal distribution (the general rules for interpreting kurtosis follow the same as for skewness; [55]. We decided to use paired-samples t-tests to analyze the familiarization data because the familiarization data were only going to be used to determine the amount of time to code the test videos and not to form any conclusions regarding memory and because there was some evidence of normality.
Multiple paired-samples t-tests were used to analyze the familiarization sessions to determine the length of time test videos should be coded. Therefore, a Bonferroni correction was performed to ensure the alpha stayed at 0.05. Seven total t-tests were completed; therefore, the corrected alpha was 0.007. One paired-samples t-test was completed to compare the overall average time in Section 1 (near the left stimulus pair) to Section 2 (the area near the right stimulus pair). The remaining six paired-samples t-tests compared each of the four 5-min intervals to each other using the proportion of time spent in Section 2 (Figure 1) versus the time spent outside of that section. Only the time spent in Section 2, not Section 1, was used due to the otter’s side bias.
Normality of the test sessions was assessed using a Shapiro–Wilk test and showed that the dependent variable of overall time spent near each stimulus pair did not depart significantly from normality (W = 0.97, p = 0.54). A second Shapiro–Wilk test was performed and showed that the distribution of the dependent variable of overall explorations did significantly depart from normality (W = 0.92, p = 0.02). The Normal Q-Q plot and histogram were examined to further assess the normality of both variables. These plots for both overall time and explorations provided evidence that both variables were normally distributed. Finally, the skewness of the overall times was −0.49, indicating that the distribution was approximately symmetric, and the kurtosis was −0.02, indicating the distribution was less heavy-tailed compared to the normal distribution. The skewness of the overall explorations was 0.38, indicating that the distribution was approximately symmetric, and the kurtosis was 0.72, indicating the distribution was less heavy-tailed compared to the normal distribution. Based on the preponderance of evidence, we decided that the data for both the overall time and explorations approximated the normal distribution closely enough to proceed in using paired-samples t-tests in the analyses of test sessions instead of nonparametric tests. Additionally, much of the previous research with the NOR task used t-tests in at least some parts of their data analyses for memory, (e.g., [24,39,41,42]).
Multiple paired-samples t-tests were used to assess for evidence of memory and side bias in the test sessions, so again a Bonferroni correction was performed to ensure alpha stayed at 0.05. There were five t-tests completed for each dependent variable (including one for each of the three memory intervals, one for overall assessment across all intervals, and one testing for the side bias), so the corrected alpha was 0.01. For each memory interval, the data were pooled (e.g., there were ten 24-h memory interval sessions that were pooled together) for time spent with the familiar stimulus pair and novel stimulus pair. This was done to see if there was a significant difference in either the amount or time spent with, or the number of explorations with the novel versus the familiar stimulus pairs. The time spent with the familiar versus the novel stimulus pair in each of the three memory intervals were compared across three paired-samples t-tests. Additionally, all the times in all the memory intervals were pooled together and then a fourth paired-samples t-test was conducted to assess overall memory. This was all repeated for the dependent variable of explorations. If the otter spent significantly more time or made significantly more explorations with either the familiar or the novel stimulus pair, this would show evidence of object recognition memory.
To check for side biases in the test sessions, two total paired-samples t-tests (accounted for in the Bonferroni correction) were conducted for the dependent measures of overall time spent with each stimulus pair and overall explorations. A side bias occurs when the otter spends significantly more time or explorations with the stimulus pair on one side consistently over the other simply based on the side of the enclosure not based on the stimulus pairs themselves. A side bias would not indicate object preference just a side preference without regard for the stimulus pair. The amount of time spent on the right and left sides was compared. If a significant difference is found consistently for the same side (either right or left) this would indicate a side bias.
Exploration and discrimination of the stimulus pairs were assessed using formulas created by May et al. [42] (Table 2). Exploration in the familiarization trials (EF) was to be used to determine how long data should be coded in the test trials. Discrimination (D1) was calculated for each of the three memory intervals to determine if the otter explored one object significantly more than the other. D1 was compared to a theoretical mean of 0 in a total of three unpaired two-sample t-tests, as performed by May et al. [42]. A Bonferroni correction was performed, and the corrected alpha was 0.02. A significant difference from the theoretical means would indicate a preference for either the novel or the familiar object in the test phase. A positive value for D1 would indicate a preference for the familiar stimulus pair, while a negative value would indicate a preference for the novel stimulus pair (Table 2).
A Pearson correlation was computed to check the inter-rater reliability between the three independent coders by comparing both of the secondary coders together to the primary coder. For the familiarization sessions, a value of 0.8 or higher was acceptable and met. Initially, the criterion was not met so the three coders individually reviewed their training and recoded some of the familiarization sessions. For the time spent with the right or left stimulus pair, if there was a discrepancy greater than 10 s between the primary coder and one of the secondary coders, both coders recoded that session. For the explorations, if there was a discrepancy greater than 1 (usually where one coder said there was one exploration and the other coder said there were zero explorations) between the coders, both the main coder and secondary coder recoded that session. Once the recoding was complete, the criterion was met. Pearson correlations coefficients were between 0.84 and 0.97 (time on the left, r(71) = 0.93; time on the right, r(71) = 0.84; explorations on the left, r(71) = 0.89; and explorations on the right, r(71) = 0.97). For the test sessions, a value of 0.8 or higher was acceptable and met. Only minor recoding was necessary for the test sessions (16.67% of the test sessions were recoded). There were three sessions where the time spent close to the right stimulus pair was recoded, and two sessions where the number of explorations with the right stimulus pair was recoded. The same recoding procedures were used for test sessions as was used for familiarization sessions. Pearson correlation coefficients were between 0.93 and 0.99 (time on the left, r(29) = 0.98; time on the right, r(29) = 0.99; explorations on the left, r(29) = 0.94; and explorations on the right, r(29) = 0.93).

7. Results

7.1. Familiarization Sessions

The 20-min familiarization sessions were recorded and coded to determine the amount of time (the first 5 min, the first 10 min, or the entire 15 min) that should be coded in the 15-min test sessions, similar to Lucon-Xiccato and Dadda [41]. The familiarization exploration index, EF [42], was calculated to be 46.1 s. Both the time spent with the left and right stimulus pairs and the explorations with the left and right stimulus pairs were compared (Figure 5). A significant side bias was found for the dependent variable of time, with the otter spending significantly more time with the left stimulus pair than the right stimulus pair (t(71) = 12.82, p < 0.001). The otter often spent time sitting by gate 3 (Figure 1), which was coded as time spent in Section 1 of the enclosure. It was difficult to reliably determine when the otter was in Section 1 to explore the stimulus pair versus when he was just sitting at the gate. Since EF added the time in Section 1 and Section 2 together, it was not valuable to use EF to determine how long the data should be coded in the test sessions, as was planned. When assessing if there was a difference among the four different 5-min time intervals, only the time spent in Section 2 (the right side of the enclosure) was used in the analyses (time in Section 1 on the left was not analyzed). The otter spent significantly more time in Section 2 in the first 5-min period of the familiarization sessions (right side only) compared to the second (t(17) = 3.50, p = 0.001), third (t(17) = 3.62, p = 0.001), and fourth (t(17) = 3.01, p = 0.004) 5-min periods. The second, third, and fourth 5-min periods did not significantly differ from each other (all p-values ≥ 0.19).
The otter conducted the majority of his explorations with the stimulus pairs during the first five minutes of the familiarization sessions (Figure 5B). In fact, the otter only explored the right stimulus pair during the first five minutes. Figure 5B gives a clearer picture of what the otter was doing at different time intervals during the familiarization session as this dependent variable was potentially not as influenced by the otter sitting at the gate (Figure 5A). Taken together, time spent with each stimulus pair and explorations of the stimulus pairs in the familiarization sessions suggested that the video coding and analyses should be restricted to the first five minutes of the test sessions.

7.2. Test Sessions

7.2.1. Side Bias

The otter spent significantly more time on the left side of the enclosure versus the right side of the enclosure, (t(29) = 6.43, p < 0.001; Figure 6A). No significant difference was found between the number of explorations on the left side of the enclosure versus on the right side of the enclosure (t(29) = 0.34, p = 0.74; Figure 6B). The otter displayed a side bias for time spent in Section 1 vs. Section 2 but not for the number of explorations to each stimulus pair.

7.2.2. Time with Stimulus Pairs

There was no difference in the overall time spent with the familiar versus novel stimulus pairs averaged across all three memory intervals (t(29) = −1.02, p = 0.32; Figure 7A). In addition, there was no difference between the average time spent with the familiar stimulus pair versus the novel stimulus pair for any of the memory intervals (10 min: t(9) = −0.75, p = 0.47; 1 h: t(9) = −0.54, p = 0.60; or 24 h: t(9) = −0.40, p = 0.70; Figure 7A). The test exploration index, ET [42], was 46.5 s. This meant that Sailor spent 15% of his time, on average, with the stimulus pairs during test sessions. Discrimination indices [42] were calculated for each of the three memory intervals by subtracting the time spent with the novel stimulus pair from the time spent with the familiar stimulus pair (Table 2). For each memory interval, D1 was compared to a theoretical mean of 0 with an unpaired two-sample t-test, as performed by May et al. [42]. No significant differences were found for any of the memory intervals (10 min, t(9) = 0.75, p = 0.47; 1 h, t(9) = 0.54, p = 0.60; or 24 h, t(9) = 0.40, p = 0.70).

7.2.3. Stimulus Pair Explorations

There was no difference in the number of overall explorations with the familiar versus novel stimulus pairs across all memory intervals (t(29) = 1.39, p = 0.17; Figure 7B). In addition, there were no differences between the number of explorations with the familiar stimulus pair versus with the novel stimulus pair for any of the memory intervals (10 min: t(9) = −0.26, p = 0.80; 1 h: t(9) = 1.96, p = 0.08; or 24 h: t(9) = 1.00, p = 0.34; Figure 7B).

7.2.4. Session by Session Analysis

This analysis was done to determine if there were any factors (e.g., object identity, odorant identity, time of day, the zookeeper working with the otter, the weather, etc.) that could account for differences in explorations across sessions. First, the test sessions in which the otter engaged in a higher-than-average number of explorations of the stimulus pairs were examined (Table 3). All but one of the sessions where there was more than one (higher-than-average) exploration to a single stimulus pair occurred within the first two blocks of testing (session 1, session 5, session 6, session 9, and session 12). Session 1 had the highest number of total explorations (seven total explorations), with four explorations of the novel stimulus pair and three explorations of the familiar stimulus pair. Session 6 had the second highest number of total explorations as well as the greatest difference in novel vs. familiar explorations with three explorations of the novel stimulus pair and zero explorations of the familiar stimulus pair. In addition, the greater number of explorations was always with the novel stimulus pair, except for session 12. We examined the stimulus pairs (e.g., color and shape of the objects, and food odorant versus odorants from nature such as floral scents) that were used in these sessions but could not find any pattern that would explain why the otter seemed more interested in exploring these stimulus pairs compared to those in other sessions. For all of these sessions, the weather was relatively warm (21–31 °C) and either mostly or partly cloudy. It is possible that these weather conditions could have motivated Sailor to explore the stimulus pairs more compared to colder or sunnier days. It did not appear that the zookeeper who was working on the day of testing impacted Sailor’s explorations. Each of the three zookeepers that helped run the majority of the sessions in this study (there was a fourth zookeeper who very occasionally helped run some sessions) ran at least one of these five sessions with a higher number of explorations.
Test trials where there were zero total explorations across both novel and familiar stimulus pairs, perhaps indicating a lack of interest or motivation, were also examined. There were 11 of 30 sessions where this occurred. Blocks 1–3 had an equal number of sessions where Sailor made no explorations (two sessions each), block 4 had the greatest number of sessions with no explorations (four sessions), and block 5 had the fewest sessions with no explorations (one session). Thus, sessions with zero explorations were evenly spread between the first part of the test (six sessions in blocks 1–3) and the last part (five sessions in blocks 4–5). Within these sessions certain colors of objects seemed to be prevalent but not any certain types of odorants. Five of the seven green objects were presented in these sessions where Sailor made no explorations, as well as three of the four purple objects and two of the three pink objects. There was not a clear pattern for weather or zookeeper identity in these instances of zero total explorations. All test trials were conducted in the afternoon around the same time of day.

8. Discussion

The NOR task was not an effective form of sensory enrichment in the current study. Sailor spent approximately 15% of his time near both stimulus pairs during the test sessions and conducted very few explorations of the stimulus pairs. In about one third of the test sessions, Sailor did not explore either stimulus pair at all, showing very little engagement in the task. He spent most of his time in Section 3 of the enclosure (Figure 1). Sailor was allowed to move around the enclosure as he pleased, and he often spent much of his time swimming in the small pool that was present. Therefore, Sailor was active during these test sessions (he was not sleeping or sitting still) he just was not engaging with the multimodal stimuli.
Additionally, there was no evidence for memory retention in the otter for any of the three memory intervals (10 min, 1 h, and 24 h) in this study. Sailor did not spend more time with the familiar stimulus pair compared to the novel object pair, or vice versa. Sailor spent significantly more time on the left side of the enclosure, regardless of the location of the familiar or novel stimulus pair, showing a side bias. There was no side bias regarding the number of explorations to the familiar vs. novel stimulus pair, but Sailor did not explore the familiar stimulus pair significantly more often than the novel object pair (or vice versa). The lack of evidence for memory retention is likely a product of Sailor’s lack of interaction with and interest in the sensory stimuli in this task.
Many studies on enrichment in otters utilize food to some extent to keep the otter engaged or as a reward for participating in the task [7,19,20,21,22,56]. In comparison, the NOR task relies on the sensory stimuli alone to engage the animal. While this may be suitable for other species, it is possible that sensory enrichment may not be an engaging form of enrichment for otters. It is also possible that the NOR task and sensory enrichment may have simply not been effective for the individual otter that participated in the current study but could be effective for a different otter. This was the case for the two sea otters that participated in Hanna et al.’s [18] tool use task. Despite both otters being unable to complete the task, one otter was very engaged with the apparatus while the other otter was not. It is possible that another otter could be very engaged in the NOR task or other sensory enrichment unlike the otter that participated in the current study.
Personality may be relevant for selecting the right type of enrichment for an individual animal. Different otters could have responded differently to a sensory enrichment task compared to Sailor. For example, aspects of Sailor’s personality, such as curiosity, could have impacted his performance in the current study [57]. Curiosity has been defined as “the motivation towards acquisition of novel information” (p. 1) [58]. This can be determined by an animal’s exploration of novel stimuli when there is no immediate reward, as was the case in the current study. Given Sailor’s few explorations of the stimulus pairs, it appears that his curiosity was low (at least during this study), and perhaps he would have low curiosity in other contexts. Boldness is another factor that is often studied when investigating animal personality [57]). Boldness is characterized by animals that are more active, take more risks, and learn more quickly [59]. It is often measured by how much the animal is exploring their environment. During this study, Sailor spent most of his time in the pool that was in the enclosure and not much time with the stimulus pairs. Sailor only spent about 15% of his time during the test trials near the stimulus pairs. This is in comparison to another study that used the NOR task with non-laboratory mammals, pigs, where they spent over 25% of their time during test trials touching the stimuli [35]. Sailor may be low in boldness. If Sailor was low in curiosity and boldness, he may not have engaged much with stimuli or his environment compared to other otters. Having multiple subjects in a future study could ameliorate this issue since there would likely be a range of personality types.
Sailor participated in two previous studies [20,21] where he was motivated and engaged in a two-alternative forced-choice task which provided effective cognitive enrichment. During these tasks there was no issue getting Sailor to start a session, follow learned behavioral cues during multiple trials (up to 40 trials per day), and engage with the stimuli (viewing the stimuli and making a choice by touching one stimulus). This previous experience with a different type of task may have impacted Sailor’s behavior in the NOR task and his willingness to engage with sensory enrichment. In the two-alternative forced-choice task, Sailor would approach two stimuli, rear up onto his back legs, and touch one stimulus with his nose or paw. Sailor received a food reward each time he made a correct choice by touching the reinforced stimulus. In the current study with the NOR task, Sailor was only provided a food reward for entering the enclosure during test sessions (and for targeting the stimulus pairs in the beginning of the familiarization sessions). This was done to encourage Sailor to enter the enclosure promptly, but we did not want to bias Sailor during test sessions by feeding him for making his own explorations of the stimulus pairs (which is not a part of the classic NOR task). The majority of Sailor’s explorations occurred during the first two blocks of the study (twelve sessions). Therefore, it is possible that Sailor may have eventually realized he was not going to receive food for touching the stimuli (after the beginning a familiarization sessions) like he did in the previous study [21]. Sailor’s behavior of exploring and touching stimuli could have eventually been extinguished because he was not receiving an anticipated food reward. The food reward in the previous study was Sailor’s external motivation for touching the stimuli, but during the current study he would have had only his internal motivation to explore the stimuli throughout the sessions. It is possible that a different otter without this past reinforcement history may be more internally motivated to make explorations of the sensory stimuli in the NOR task.
When using sensory enrichment, the animals must be able to perceive the stimuli. Since there is limited data on otter visual and olfactory perception, we did our best to utilize objects that otters can perceive. All the objects were chosen for maximum visual discriminability based on size, shape, and color based on what we know about vision in otters [21,60]. In a previous study [21], Sailor himself was able to discriminate between two-dimensional stimuli when he had both shape and color as a cue. In that same study, another otter was able to further discriminate between stimuli using either shape or color alone as a cue when the other cue was removed. Therefore, there is evidence North American river otters can not only perceive but also discriminate between stimuli using shape and color as cues. Such cues were present with the three-dimensional objects used in the current study. There is a strong likelihood that Sailor could see the objects in the current study and perceive differences among them.
However, we do not know if the otter was able to perceive the odorants used in the current study. To date, there are no published psychophysical studies on odor detection and discrimination abilities in otters. The literature is focused on otter olfaction for identifying scents of other animals [34,49] not artificial scents like the essential oils and food extracts used in this study. Due to the large number of sessions (30) and thus the need for at least 60 unique odorants, it was not feasible to use scents of other animals for this study. It is possible Sailor did not detect some portion of our odorants. This is why multimodal stimuli (visual, olfactory, and tactile) were used to create stimulus pairs in the hopes of maximizing perception discriminability via multisensory facilitation. Another possibility is that Sailor detected the odorants but since they were not ecologically valid, they were unimportant and unengaging for him. Additionally, Sailor could touch the objects but not manipulate them. Therefore, tactile perception may not have been used much to help with recognition of the objects. Any sensory enrichment used for otters must be informed by gaining more knowledge about visual, olfactory, and tactile perception in otters.
The main limitation in this study was utilizing a single subject. There was a second otter (a 5-year-old female, Ashkii) residing at the zoo, and we planned to have her participate in the current study. This otter participated in a previous study on visual object categorization [20] similar to the DeLong et al. [21] study where a two-alternative forced-choice task was used. Ashkii was motivated in the previous study that involved training and performed well. During the pilot test of the current study, after viewing Ashkii in the experimental enclosure, the zoo staff decided that it would not be possible for Ashkii to participate. She exhibited problematic behaviors (scratching at and climbing on top of the gate, as well as pacing) after only a few minutes inside the experimental area. Therefore, it was necessary to exclude Ashkii from the study. This sensory enrichment task did not suit Ashkii.
Our single subject Sailor was both a male and elderly (otters older than 13 years are considered elderly; [8]). These factors could have impacted our results, such as by affecting his sensory systems, memory, motivation, physical abilities, etc. In otters, some studies have reported sex differences in cognitive abilities [33,60] while others have not found such differences [23]. One cognitive study on three otter species found an age difference where younger otters were more neophilic [61]. We do not know if sex or age played a role in the current study since Sailor was our only subject. There was no evidence that Sailor had issues seeing or smelling, and we ensured that the stimulus pairs were easily accessible for Sailor to touch. Additionally, Sailor was successfully visually categorizing two-dimensional objects approximately two weeks prior to the current study [20]. Stimulus pairs were hung at approximately the same height as in DeLong et al. [21], and Sailor easily touched those stimuli.
We compensated for having a single subject by conducting 30 sessions with the otter. Smith and Little [62]) discuss this aspect of low n (subject) designs as beneficial, as the individual becomes a replication unit when large samples of data are collected from individuals. Since there was just a single otter in this study, Smith and Little [62] would equate this to running a single study with 30 different otters that all completed one session (as is often the case in many psychology studies on humans). However, the results of the current study should still be interpreted with caution. It would be beneficial to present additional otters at other facilities with opportunities for sensory and cognitive enrichment to further develop a strategy for presenting optimal enrichment for this species.
It is possible that this sensory enrichment task may have more successfully engaged the otter’s attention if the stimuli had been presented in the larger on-exhibit area instead of in the off-exhibit area we used. Since we were using a traditional NOR task, it had to be located in a confined space where the otter could be closely monitored in marked areas near each stimulus pair. The placement of the gate within this area on the left side of the enclosure near where the left stimulus pair was placed (gate 3 in Figure 1) was an issue. The otter used this gate multiple times per day to access the on-exhibit area. In our experimental setup, this gate remained closed during a session. This resulted in Sailor sitting over by the gate for extended periods of time, likely because he wanted to leave the enclosure of his own accord as he typically would be able to do. It may be useful in the future with otters to use a sensory enrichment task similar to the modified NOR task, such as those used with baboons [63] and marmosets [64]. In this modified task, stimuli were presented in specific spots throughout the animals’ enclosure over a period of multiple days, where a novel stimulus was added each day. This way, the animals were not confined to a smaller space and could engage with the stimuli within their whole enclosure and not just an off-exhibit space.
One potential conclusion that could be drawn from this study is that the effectiveness of sensory enrichment depends on the personality, reinforcement history, and behavioral traits of the subject, along with the features of the stimuli and the placement of the stimuli within the animal’s enclosure. Providing novel objects and odorants for an individual to explore during sensory enrichment is not always as effective for otters as it may be for other species (e.g., olfactory enrichment in elephants; [65], and lions; [66], and multi-sensory enrichment in lemurs; [67]). Another potential conclusion is that despite all these factors, cognitive enrichment (and food-based enrichment) is simply more effective and engaging than sensory enrichment for otters. A previous study that investigated enrichment for a North American river otter found food-based enrichment to be the most effective at reducing stereotyped behaviors [6]. There was a sensory enrichment device used in Nelson’s [6] study, unfortunately conclusions about its effectiveness could not be properly compared to the food-based enrichment due to a low number of presentations of the sensory enrichment device.
We suggest that future research examining memory and other phenomena in otters utilize tasks that provide cognitive enrichment rather than sensory enrichment. The results of the current study are not in line with previous studies that successfully used the NOR task to show evidence of memory in many other animals (e.g., birds: [68]; fish: [42]; pigs: [35]; and rats: [24]). While it is possible that our results suggest that North American river otters do not retain long-term memories, it is more likely that this sensory enrichment task was not ideal for otters. Saliveros and colleagues (2020) used a task with more puzzle feeders (a well-known form of cognitive enrichment) baited with food and found some evidence of long-term memory in Asian short-clawed otters. Taken along with previous studies using cognitive enrichment successfully in North American river otters [20,21,32], this seems to be a more promising approach. We explored using simple and less time-intensive sensory enrichment in the current study, but it appeared to be inferior to cognitive enrichment. Although very beneficial to animals in zoos, laboratories, and farms, cognitive enrichment (enrichment that promotes wellbeing by engaging cognitive skills in animals) seems to be used the least of any of the enrichment types [3].
It is important to continue studying otters using different types of enrichment because there are many otters residing under human care who can benefit from effective enrichment practices. There are approximately 500 freshwater otters (4 species) in 162 Association of Zoos and Aquariums (AZA) accredited facilities across the United States (D. Hamilton, personal communication). This includes an estimated 274 North American river otters in 107 AZA accredited facilities in the United States, which are the most numerous of all species of otters in zoos, and the focal species in the current study (D. Hamilton, personal communication). Advancing animal welfare is a part of AZA’s mission [47], and providing appropriate and engaging enrichment is one of the main ways facilities often promote animal welfare. It is also important to remember that enrichment is not one-size-fits-all for every species within a zoo. The current study not only examined a new type of enrichment that had not been used with otters before, but it also pursued new scientific discoveries about these animals regarding their memory abilities.
Another reason it is beneficial to study cognitive phenomena in otters using various types of enrichment is because it can benefit conservation research and efforts for otters. North American river otters nearly went extinct in many parts of the United States by the mid-20th century, including western New York, until reintroduction efforts began in the 1990s [9]. Over the past 18 years, states have reported either increasing or stable river otter populations throughout the US [69]. While some river otter populations are increasing, it does seem that the species is approaching their maximum geographic distribution in the US. Otters still face issues today such as pollution, trapping, and degradation of their environments [8], so they may not have as many suitable habitats to continue to spread throughout the US in order to continue increasing their population numbers. Conservation efforts can be better informed about how best to support these animals if we know more about their cognitive abilities through utilizing tasks that provide cognitive enrichment and are engaging to this species.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.J.W. and C.M.D.; methodology, J.J.W. and C.M.D.; validation, J.J.W. and C.M.D.; formal analysis, J.J.W.; investigation, J.J.W.; resources, J.J.W. and C.M.D.; data curation, J.J.W.; writing—original draft preparation, J.J.W. and C.M.D.; writing—review and editing, J.J.W. and C.M.D.; visualization, J.J.W.; supervision, C.M.D.; project administration, C.M.D.; funding acquisition, J.J.W. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

Data collection was sponsored by the Rochester Institute of Technology College of Liberal Arts Student Research Award to J.J.W.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The animal study protocol was approved by both the Rochester Institute of Technology Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (proposal 2021-2, approved 21 June 2021) and by the Seneca Park Zoo Welfare Committee (proposal 2021-2, approved 30 June 2021). This research was conducted according to the ethical standards of the United States.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. None of the experiments were preregistered.

Acknowledgments

Portions of these findings were presented at the 2022 International Conference on Comparative Cognition (online conference). This study was completed as an M.S. thesis in the Experimental Psychology program at Rochester Institute of Technology. Thank you to the thesis committee members Jessamy Comer and Tina Sutton for their helpful feedback. We greatly appreciate the staff at the Seneca Park Zoo for their involvement; especially the General Curator David Hamilton, Assistant Curator John Adamski, Veterinarian Louis DiVincenti, and Zookeepers Catina Wright, Danielle Newport, Brian Sheets, and Ryan Statt. In addition, we thank the following undergraduate students in the RIT Comparative Cognition and Perception Lab for coding the videos for this study: Hunter Ansari, Kelsey Beers, and Kera Hampton. Thank you to Felicia Soto for her drawing of the experimental setup. Finally, thanks to Jack Mason for the use of the camera to record all the sessions.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no relevant financial or non-financial interest to disclose.

References

  1. Association of Zoos and Aquariums. The Accreditation Standards & Related Policies. 2021. Available online: https://assets.speakcdn.com/assets/2332/aza-accreditation-standards.pdf (accessed on 4 March 2021).
  2. Shepherdson, D.J. Tracing the path of environmental enrichment in zoos. In Second Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals; Sheperdson, D.J., Mellen, J.D., Hutchins, M., Eds.; Smithsonian Institution Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1998; pp. 1–14. [Google Scholar]
  3. Clark, F. Cognitive enrichment and welfare: Current approaches and future directions. Anim. Behav. Cogn. 2017, 4, 52–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Ross, S.R. How cognitive studies help shape our obligation for ethical care of chimpanzees. In The Mind of the Chimpanzees: Ecological and Experimental Perspectives; Lonsdorf, E.V., Ross, S.R., Matsuzawa, T., Eds.; The University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2010; pp. 309–319. [Google Scholar]
  5. Carlstead, K. Stereotypic behaviors in zoo carnivores. In Second Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals; Sheperdson, D.J., Mellen, J.D., Hutchins, M., Eds.; Smithsonian Institution Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  6. Nelson, K. Environmental Enrichment Effects on the Activity of Neartic River Otter. Unpublished. Master’s Thesis, Rochester Institute of Technology, Rochester, NY, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  7. Ross, S.R. The effect of a simple feeding strategy on the behavior of two Asian small-clawed otters (Aonyx cinerea). Aquat. Mamm. 2002, 28, 113–120. [Google Scholar]
  8. Yoxon, P.; Yoxon, G.M. Otters of the World; Whittles Publishing Ltd.: Dunbeath, UK, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  9. Kruuk, H. Otters: Ecology, Behvavior, and Conservation; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  10. Fretueg, G.R.; Martin, T.J.; Widga, C.; Ruez, D.R. Summer diet characteristics of North American River Otters (Lontra canadensis) in central Illinois. Am. Midl. Nat. 2015, 173, 294–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Guertin, D.A.; Harestad, A.S.; Elliott, J.E. Summer feeding habits of river otters inhabiting a contaminated coastal marine environment. Northwest Sci. 2010, 84, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Larsen, D.N. Feeding Habits of River Otters in Coastal Southeastern Alaska. J. Wildl. Manag. 1984, 48, 1446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Penland, T.F.; Black, J.M. Seasonal Variation in River Otter Diet in Coastal Northern California. Northwestern Nat. 2009, 90, 233–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Reid, D.G.; Code, T.E.; Reid, A.C.H.; Herrero, S.M. Food habits of the river otter in a boreal ecosystem. Can. J. Zool. 1994, 72, 1306–1313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Scordino, J.J.; Gearin, P.J.; Riemer, S.D.; Iwamoto, E.M. River Otter (Lontra canadensis) Food Habits in a Washington Coast Watershed: Implications for a Threatened Species. Northwestern Nat. 2016, 97, 36–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Toweill, D.E. Winter Food Habits of River Otters in Western Oregon. J. Wildl. Manag. 1974, 38, 107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Haar, A.K.R. Recommendations for the Successful Captive Breeding of North American River Otters (Lontra canadensis). Ph.D. Thesis, Colorado State University-Pueblo, Pueblo, CO, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  18. Hanna, P.; Frick, E.; Kuczaj, S. A Tool Use Task Proves Enriching for a Captive Sea Otter (Enhydra lutris). Anim. Behav. Cogn. 2016, 3, 88–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Foster-Turley, P.; Markowitz, H. A captive behavioral enrichment study with Asian small-clawed river otters (Aonyx cinerea). Zoo Biol. 1982, 1, 29–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. DeLong, C.M.; Wegman, J.J.; Wilcox, K.T.; Wright, C. Categorization of 2D objects in North American river otters (Lontra canadensis). Learn. Behav. 2023. manuscript in preparation. [Google Scholar]
  21. DeLong, C.M.; Wright, C.; Fobe, I.; Wilcox, K.T.; Morrison, E. North American river otters (Lontra canadensis) discriminate between 2D objects varying in shape and color. Learn. Behav. 2019, 47, 91–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  22. Saliveros, A.M.; Blyth, E.C.; Easter, C.; Hume, G.V.; McAusland, F.; Hoppitt, W.; Boogert, N.J. Learning strategies and long-term memory in Asian short-clawed otters (Aonyx cinereus). R. Soc. Open Sci. 2020, 7, 201215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Perdue, B.M.; Snyder, R.J.; Maple, T.L. Cognitive research in Asian small-clawed otters. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 2013, 26, 105–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Reger, M.L.; Hovda, D.A.; Giza, C.C. Ontogeny of Rat Recognition Memory measured by the novel object recognition task. Dev. Psychobiol. 2009, 51, 672–678. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Green, J. Sensory perception in hunting otters, Lutra lutra L. Otters J. Otter Trust. 1977, 13–16. [Google Scholar]
  26. Crimmins, S.M.; Roberts, N.M.; Hamilton, D.A. Effects of Prey Size on Scat Analysis to Determine River Otter Lontra canadensis Diet. Wildl. Biol. 2009, 15, 449–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Balliet, R.F.; Schusterman, R.J. Underwater and Aerial Visual Acuity in the Asian “Clawless” Otter (Amblonyx cineria cineria). Nature 1971, 234, 305–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Schusterman, R.J.; Barrett, B. Amphibious Nature of Visual Acuity in the Asian “Clawless” Otter. Nature 1973, 244, 518–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Levenson, D.H.; Ponganis, P.J.; Crognale, M.A.; Deegan, J.F.; Dizon, A.; Jacobs, G.H. Visual pigments of marine carnivores: Pinnipeds, polar bear, and sea otter. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2006, 192, 833–843. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  30. Peichl, L.; Behrmann, G.; Kröger, R.H.H. For whales and seals the ocean is not blue: A visual pigment loss in marine mammals*. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2001, 13, 1520–1528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  31. Griebel, U.; Peichl, L. Colour vision in aquatic mammals—Facts and open questions. Aquat. Mamm. 2003, 29, 18–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Svoke, J.T.; Snyder, R.J.; Elgart, J.B. Preliminary evidence for color stimuli discrimination in the Asian small-clawed otter (Aonyx cinerea). Learn. Behav. 2014, 42, 176–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Slack, R.D. Learning in the River Otter (Lutra canadensis). Unpublished. Master’s Thesis, Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA, 1966. [Google Scholar]
  34. Rostain, R.R.; Ben-David, M.; Groves, P.; Randall, J.A. Why do river otters scent-mark? An experimental test of several hypotheses. Anim. Behav. 2004, 68, 703–711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Gifford, A.K.; Cloutier, S.; Newberry, R.C. Objects as enrichment: Effects of object exposure time and delay interval on object recognition memory of the domestic pig. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 107, 206–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Callahan, H.; Ikeda-Douglas, C.; Head, E.; Cotman, C.W.; Milgram, N.W. Development of a protocol for studying object recognition memory in the dog. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2000, 24, 693–707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Lueptow, L.M. Novel Object Recognition Test for the Investigation of Learning and Memory in Mice. J. Vis. Exp. 2017, 2017, e55718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Silvers, J.M.; Harrod, S.B.; Mactutus, C.F.; Booze, R.M. Automation of the novel object recognition task for use in adolescent rats. J. Neurosci. Methods 2007, 166, 99–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Sutcliffe, J.; Marshall, K.; Neill, J. Influence of gender on working and spatial memory in the novel object recognition task in the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 2007, 177, 117–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Malkova, L.; Mishkin, M. One-Trial Memory for Object-Place Associations after Separate Lesions of Hippocampus and Posterior Parahippocampal Region in the Monkey. J. Neurosci. 2003, 23, 1956–1965. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Lucon-Xiccato, T.; Dadda, M. Assessing memory in zebrafish using the one-trial test. Behav. Process. 2014, 106, 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. May, Z.; Morrill, A.; Holcombe, A.; Johnston, T.; Gallup, J.; Fouad, K.; Schalomon, M.; Hamilton, T.J. Object recognition memory in zebrafish. Behav. Brain Res. 2016, 296, 199–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Kimura, A.; Wada, Y.; Yang, J.; Otsuka, Y.; Dan, I.; Masuda, T.; Kanazawa, S.; Yamaguchi, M.K. Infants’ recognition of objects using canonical color. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2010, 105, 256–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Reynolds, G.D.; Richards, J.E. Infant Visual Attention and Stimulus Repetition Effects on Object Recognition. Child Dev. 2019, 90, 1027–1042. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Ennaceur, A.; Delacour, J. A new one-trial test for neurobiological studies of memory in rats. 1: Behavioral data. Behav. Brain Res. 1988, 31, 47–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Aggleton, J.P.; Albasser, M.M.; Aggleton, D.J.; Poirier, G.L.; Pearce, J.M. Lesions of the rat perirhinal cortex spare the acquisition of a complex configural visual discrimination yet impair object recognition. Behav. Neurosci. 2010, 124, 55–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Blaser, R.; Heyser, C.J. Spontaneous object recognition: A promising approach to the comparative study of memory. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. AZA Strategic Plan. Available online: https://www.aza.org/strategic-plan (accessed on 4 March 2021).
  49. Mumm, C.A.S.; Knörnschild, M. Mustelid Communication. In Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior; Vonk, J., Shackelford, T., Eds.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2018; pp. 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Garvey, P.M.; Glen, A.; Clout, M.N.; Wyse, S.; Nichols, M.; Pech, R.P. Exploiting interspecific olfactory communication to monitor predators. Ecol. Appl. 2017, 27, 389–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Creighton, S.D.; Palmer, D.; Mitchnick, K.A.; Winters, B.D. Exploiting novelty and oddity exploratory preferences in rodents to study multisensory object memory and perception. Handb. Behav. Neurosci. 2018, 27, 103–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Pluck, R.; Wright, C.; Morrison, E.; Wegman, J.; Rachfal, T.; Jimenez, R.; DeLong, C.M. Do North American river otters have a global or local processing strategy? In Proceedings of the 26th Annual International Conference on Comparative Cognition, Melbourne Beach, FL, USA, 10–13 April 2019. [Google Scholar]
  53. Clark, F.; King, A.J. A critical review of zoo-based olfactory enrichment. In Chemical Signals in Vertebrates, 11; Hurst, J.L., Beynon, R.J., Roberts, S.C., Wyatt, T.D., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Neuman, J.; Chardine, J.W.; Porter, J.M. Approaches to Testing Inter-Observer Reliability of Field-Collected Behavioral Data. Waterbirds 1999, 22, 348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. George, D.; Mallery, P. IBM SPSS Statistics 26 Step by Step: A Simple Guide and Reference; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Saliveros, A.M.; Bowden-Parry, M.; McAusland, F.; Boogert, N.J. Captive Asian short-clawed otters (Aonyx cinereus) learn to exploit unfamiliar natural prey. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2022, 9, 211819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Skinner, M.; Miller, N. Aggregation and social interaction in garter snakes (Thamnophis sirtalis sirtalis). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2020, 74, 51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Christensen, J.W.; Ahrendt, L.P.; Malmkvist, J.; Nicol, C. Exploratory behaviour towards novel objects is associated with enhanced learning in young horses. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 1428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Sneddon, L.U. The bold and the shy: Individual differences in rainbow trout. J. Fish Biol. 2003, 62, 971–975. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Frick, E.E.; Friedman, L.; Peranteau, J.; Beacham, K.; Ii, S.A.K. Flexibility and use of a novel tool in Asian Small-Clawed Otters (Aonyx cinerea). Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 2016, 29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Gormley, R. Problem Solving and Tool Use in Three Species of Otter. Unpublished. Master’s Thesis, The University of Tennessee at Chattanooga, Chattanooga, TN, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  62. Smith, P.L.; Little, D.R. Small is beautiful: In defense of the small-N design. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2018, 25, 2083–2101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Joubert, A.; Vauclair, J. Reaction to novel objects in a troop of guinea baboons: Approach and manipulation. Behaviour 1986, 96, 92–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Menzel, E.; Menzel, C.R. Cognitive, Developmental and Social Aspects of Responsiveness To Novel Objects in a Family Group of Marmosets (Sag Uinus Fuscicollis). Behaviour 1979, 70, 251–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. LaDue, C.A.; Schulte, B.A. Pheromonal enrichment in the zoo: An empirical approach with Asian elephants (Elephas maximus). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2021, 235, 105228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Martínez-Macipe, M.; Lafont-Lecuelle, C.; Manteca, X.; Pageat, P.; Cozzi, A. Evaluation of an innovative approach for sensory enrichment in zoos: Semiochemical stimulation for captive lions (Panthera leo). Anim. Welf. 2015, 24, 455–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Browning, H.; Moro, L. A multi-sensory enrichment program for ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta) at Auckland Zoo, including a novel feeding device. In Proceedings of the 1st Australasian Regional Environmental Enrichment Conference, Melbourne, Australia, January 2006. [Google Scholar]
  68. Damphousse, C.C.; Miller, N.; Marrone, D.F. Reaction to novelty as a behavioral assay of recognition memory in homing pigeons and Japanese quail. Learn. Behav. 2021, 50, 167–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Roberts, N.M.; Lovallo, M.J.; Crimmins, S.M. River otter status, management, and distribution in the United States: Evidence of large-scale population increase and range expansion. J. Fish Wildl. Manag. 2020, 11, 279–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Experimental setup. There were four equal areas, though the two areas with the pool were combined into one section (Section 3). The enclosure measured 203 × 157.5 × 178 cm. The gates, indicated by the three gray rectangles, were 76 × 30.5 cm. See text for more details.
Figure 1. Experimental setup. There were four equal areas, though the two areas with the pool were combined into one section (Section 3). The enclosure measured 203 × 157.5 × 178 cm. The gates, indicated by the three gray rectangles, were 76 × 30.5 cm. See text for more details.
Jzbg 04 00028 g001
Figure 2. Photograph of the experimental setup. The image shows a still, captured during a familiarization session from block 2 session 11. Sailor is seen touching the stimulus on the left with his nose. This would be counted as an exploration during video coding.
Figure 2. Photograph of the experimental setup. The image shows a still, captured during a familiarization session from block 2 session 11. Sailor is seen touching the stimulus on the left with his nose. This would be counted as an exploration during video coding.
Jzbg 04 00028 g002
Figure 3. Stimulus pairings. The first object and odorant shown was the familiar stimulus pair, the second object and odorant listed was the novel stimulus pair. The line in the bottom left corner was 10 cm for size reference. The water bottles were made of aluminum (the red bottle was filled with water, the purple bottle was filled with sand, and the blue bottle was filled with gravel). For all the objects, the length range was 7 cm to 37.5 cm, the width range was 4 cm to 27.5 cm, and the depth range was 1 cm to 16.5 cm. They were purchased from three companies, notated by superscripts in the figure; 1 is McCormick®, 2 is OOOFLavors®, and 3 is Candles and Supplies Inc., Quakertown, PA, USA.
Figure 3. Stimulus pairings. The first object and odorant shown was the familiar stimulus pair, the second object and odorant listed was the novel stimulus pair. The line in the bottom left corner was 10 cm for size reference. The water bottles were made of aluminum (the red bottle was filled with water, the purple bottle was filled with sand, and the blue bottle was filled with gravel). For all the objects, the length range was 7 cm to 37.5 cm, the width range was 4 cm to 27.5 cm, and the depth range was 1 cm to 16.5 cm. They were purchased from three companies, notated by superscripts in the figure; 1 is McCormick®, 2 is OOOFLavors®, and 3 is Candles and Supplies Inc., Quakertown, PA, USA.
Jzbg 04 00028 g003aJzbg 04 00028 g003b
Figure 4. Example of an NOR task session. One session in the NOR task contains one familiarization trial, one memory interval, and one test trial. In this study the memory interval could be 10 min, 1 h, or 24 h.
Figure 4. Example of an NOR task session. One session in the NOR task contains one familiarization trial, one memory interval, and one test trial. In this study the memory interval could be 10 min, 1 h, or 24 h.
Jzbg 04 00028 g004
Figure 5. Results from the familiarization sessions. (A) There was a significant decrease in the time spent on the right after the first five minutes. Although Sailor spent greater amounts of time on the left after the first five minutes, it was likely because a gate was positioned on the left side of the enclosure which Sailor would sit near for extended lengths of time (Section 1, Figure 1). The error bars show standard error. (B) The majority of explorations with all stimulus pairs took place during the first five minutes. All explorations to the right stimulus pair took place during the first five minutes. There are no error bars because the graph shows the exact counts.
Figure 5. Results from the familiarization sessions. (A) There was a significant decrease in the time spent on the right after the first five minutes. Although Sailor spent greater amounts of time on the left after the first five minutes, it was likely because a gate was positioned on the left side of the enclosure which Sailor would sit near for extended lengths of time (Section 1, Figure 1). The error bars show standard error. (B) The majority of explorations with all stimulus pairs took place during the first five minutes. All explorations to the right stimulus pair took place during the first five minutes. There are no error bars because the graph shows the exact counts.
Jzbg 04 00028 g005
Figure 6. Time spent and number of explorations on the left versus right in the test sessions. (A) The otter spent significantly more time on the left versus the right, indicating a side bias for the dependent variable of time (* indicates a significant difference between the bars). The error bars show standard error. (B) There was no significant difference between number of explorations on the left versus the right, indicating there was no side bias for the dependent variable of explorations. There are no error bars because the graph shows the exact counts.
Figure 6. Time spent and number of explorations on the left versus right in the test sessions. (A) The otter spent significantly more time on the left versus the right, indicating a side bias for the dependent variable of time (* indicates a significant difference between the bars). The error bars show standard error. (B) There was no significant difference between number of explorations on the left versus the right, indicating there was no side bias for the dependent variable of explorations. There are no error bars because the graph shows the exact counts.
Jzbg 04 00028 g006
Figure 7. Time spent and number of explorations with the novel versus familiar stimulus pair in the test sessions. (A) There were no significant differences between the time spent with the novel versus the familiar stimulus pair averaged across all three memory intervals nor for any specific memory interval. The error bars show standard error. (B) There were no significant differences between the explorations with the novel versus familiar stimulus pair averaged across all three memory intervals nor for any specific memory interval. There are no error bars because the graph shows the exact counts.
Figure 7. Time spent and number of explorations with the novel versus familiar stimulus pair in the test sessions. (A) There were no significant differences between the time spent with the novel versus the familiar stimulus pair averaged across all three memory intervals nor for any specific memory interval. The error bars show standard error. (B) There were no significant differences between the explorations with the novel versus familiar stimulus pair averaged across all three memory intervals nor for any specific memory interval. There are no error bars because the graph shows the exact counts.
Jzbg 04 00028 g007aJzbg 04 00028 g007b
Table 1. Session Composition.
Table 1. Session Composition.
SessionBlockMemory IntervalNovel Stimulus Side
111 hL
2 10 minL
3 24 hR
4 10 minR
5 24 hL
6 1 hR
7224 hR
8 1 hL
9 10 minR
10 1 hR
11 24 hL
12 10 minL
13324 hR
14 1 hR
15 10 minL
16 24 hL
17 10 minR
18 1 hL
1941 hR
20 10 minR
21 24 hL
22 10 minL
23 24 hR
24 1 hL
25524 hL
26 1 hR
27 10 minL
28 1 hL
29 24 hR
30 10 minR
Note. The novel stimulus pair was not presented on the same side more than twice in a row to reduce the possibility that the otter could develop a side bias.
Table 2. Exploration and Discrimination Indices.
Table 2. Exploration and Discrimination Indices.
ExplorationDiscrimination
EF = A1 + A2D1 = A3 − B
EF = 46.1DT = 9.5
ET = 46.3D10 = 13.5
D1 = 10.5
D24 = 4.57
Note. Formulas were created by May et al. [42]. Exploration in the familiarization trials (EF) was calculated by adding the time spent around each identical object, A1 and A2. Exploration in the test trials (ET) was calculated by adding the time spent around the familiar object, A3, and the novel object, B. EF was used to help in determining how long data were coded in the test trial. DT = overall discrimination in test trials. D10 = discrimination for test sessions with 10-min memory interval. D1 = discrimination for test sessions with 1-h memory interval. D24 = discrimination for test sessions with 24-h memory interval.
Table 3. Number of Explorations Shown for Each Session.
Table 3. Number of Explorations Shown for Each Session.
BlockSessionNovelFamiliarMemory Interval
11431 h
20010 min
30124 h
40010 min
52024 h
6301 h
271024 h
8101 h
92010 min
10001 h
110024 h
120210 min
3131024 h
14001 h
151010 min
160124 h
170110 min
18001 h
419001 h
200110 min
210024 h
220010 min
231024 h
24001 h
5251124 h
26001 h
271010 min
28101 h
291124 h
301210 min
Note. The grey shading indicates a session with zero explorations of both the novel and familiar stimulus pairs. The bold font indicates a session with a difference in explorations between novel and familiar stimulus pairs of at least two explorations. See Figure 3 for pictures of the objects and the odorants for each session.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wegman, J.J.; DeLong, C.M. Investigating Object Recognition Memory Using Sensory Enrichment with a North American River Otter (Lontra canadensis). J. Zool. Bot. Gard. 2023, 4, 335-363. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg4020028

AMA Style

Wegman JJ, DeLong CM. Investigating Object Recognition Memory Using Sensory Enrichment with a North American River Otter (Lontra canadensis). Journal of Zoological and Botanical Gardens. 2023; 4(2):335-363. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg4020028

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wegman, Jessica J., and Caroline M. DeLong. 2023. "Investigating Object Recognition Memory Using Sensory Enrichment with a North American River Otter (Lontra canadensis)" Journal of Zoological and Botanical Gardens 4, no. 2: 335-363. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg4020028

APA Style

Wegman, J. J., & DeLong, C. M. (2023). Investigating Object Recognition Memory Using Sensory Enrichment with a North American River Otter (Lontra canadensis). Journal of Zoological and Botanical Gardens, 4(2), 335-363. https://doi.org/10.3390/jzbg4020028

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop