Next Article in Journal
Exploring Children’s Self-Reports of Victimisation Experiences and Solitary, Prosocial, and Aggressive Behaviours
Previous Article in Journal
Punching up the Fun: A Comparison of Enjoyment and In-Task Valance in Virtual Reality Boxing and Treadmill Running
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Emotional Intelligence and University Students’ Happiness: The Mediating Role of Basic Psychological Needs’ Satisfaction

by
Aikaterini Vasiou
1,*,
Eleni Vasilaki
1,
Konstantinos Mastrothanasis
2 and
Evangelia Galanaki
3
1
Department of Primary Education, University of Crete, 74100 Rethymno, Greece
2
School of Medicine, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, 15772 Athens, Greece
3
Department of Pedagogy and Primary Education, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, 10680 Athens, Greece
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Psychol. Int. 2024, 6(4), 855-867; https://doi.org/10.3390/psycholint6040055
Submission received: 12 September 2024 / Revised: 13 October 2024 / Accepted: 15 October 2024 / Published: 17 October 2024

Abstract

:
Given the increasing importance of adjusting to university life and achieving happiness, identifying the effective role of emotional intelligence and psychological needs’ satisfaction in enhancing students’ well-being is crucial. This study investigated the relation between emotional intelligence, psychological need satisfaction, and university students’ happiness. Data were collected from 205 university students (mean age: 23.35; predominantly female) at the University of Crete. Participants completed the Emotional Intelligence Scale, the Oxford Happiness Questionnaire, and the Basic Psychological Needs Satisfaction Scale. The analysis began with a correlation matrix to explore preliminary relations among the key variables. A multiple-linear regression analysis was then conducted to predict happiness levels based on observed correlations. Following this, a multiple-mediation analysis examined how emotional intelligence affects happiness through psychological needs satisfaction. The results indicated a positive association between emotional intelligence and happiness, with psychological needs’ satisfaction also positively correlating with happiness. Specifically, the use and regulation of emotion, competence, and autonomy significantly predicted happiness. Path analysis revealed that emotional intelligence indirectly influences happiness, particularly through competence. The results indicate that high emotional intelligence, through the satisfaction of basic psychological needs, is significantly associated with university students’ happiness. The study suggests that institutions should guide students in emotional intelligence and competence to increase happiness during their studies.

1. Introduction

Adjusting to university life and feeling happy have become a critical area of research in higher education and student well-being [1]. To this end, it is imperative to identify crucial and effective personal sources and educational practices that enhance university students’ happiness. Using both emotional intelligence [2] and self-determination theory [3], the current study investigated the relation between happiness and its antecedents among undergraduate students.
Happiness is defined in various ways [4]. In positive psychology, happiness is recognized as an emotion; it is a mental evaluation of events with positive emotions [5]. Happiness is negatively related to stress, anxiety, and depression, while happier individuals are less prone to mental disorders [6,7]. Even during the COVID-19 (Coronavirus Disease 2019) pandemic, people who reported higher levels of subjective happiness experienced flourishing mental health [8]. University students’ happiness has received attention in recent research examining its relationship with motivation [9], self-efficacy [1], leisure [10], perfectionism [11], and literal therapy [12].
Emotional intelligence (EI), as a multidimensional construct involving mental skills and abilities, enables individuals to accurately evaluate, regulate, and express their emotions, as well as understand the emotions of others [2]. Two common constructs differentiated in the EI literature, trait EI (which involves individuals’ perceptions of their emotional world and their emotional self-efficacy [13], measured through self-report assessments [14]) and ability EI (which refers to the actual cognitive skills that enable individuals to recognize, understand, and manage emotions [15], and which uses performance-based assessments [16]). Both constructs have a positive impact on academic settings, including psychological well-being [17].
EI has been linked to happiness with mixed results. For instance, no significant correlation was found between the Mayer–Salovey–Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test [18] (MSCEIT) and life satisfaction [19] (SWLS) in a study of 202 Israeli adolescents [20]. However, a positive relationship between EI and happiness was observed in studies with 188 male adolescents in Tehran [21], 412 Malaysian high-school students [22], and 400 university students in Spain [23]. Although a positive association between EI and happiness was revealed through a recent meta-analyses, in both Western [24] and Chinese cultures [25], rather less attention has been paid to the mechanisms or processes underlying this relation.
In academic settings, basic psychological needs theory [26,27] (BPNT) posits that all students have inherent psychological needs that must be fulfilled to enhance learning, well-being, and adaptive coping with changes [28]. Specifically, students have a set of three inherent, motivational, and universal basic psychological needs [26,27], namely autonomy (controlling their own actions and decisions), competence (interacting effectively with the learning environment), and relatedness (feeling connected with their instructors and fellow students). The fulfillment of these inherent needs during learning activities allows students to thrive and to function optimally [3]. Conversely, when these needs are not fulfilled, students are more likely to experience mental health problems, which can then subsequently affect achievement [29].
Recent research has explored the mediating role of psychological needs’ satisfaction in studies examining students’ happiness. For example, in a sample of Chinese adolescents, psychological need satisfaction was the mediator in the relationship between school-related social support and school-related happiness [30]. In a structural equation modeling study (n = 1961), Froiland et al. [31], examining teacher–student relationships, psychological need satisfaction, and happiness among diverse students, proposed that teacher–student relationships could promote happiness by meeting psychological needs. In addition, in a study examining between- and within-person-level associations [32], grit was positively associated with adolescents’ subjective well-being, and needs’ satisfaction was a mediator, while daily grit promoted happiness by satisfying basic psychological needs. However, it remains unclear whether psychological needs’ satisfaction mediates the link between EI and happiness.
Our study focused on university students–emergent adults [33,34] because, during this period, these individuals navigate the complex transition to adulthood [35,36], facing various challenges, including academic stress, life transitions, and career planning, which can intensely impact their happiness, affecting their academic performance as well [4]. Several other studies have explored factors affecting happiness, such as academic satisfaction [37], parental support [38], romantic relationships [39], and emotional stability [40], revealing its prevalence in this population sample. Therefore, the current study had two specific aims: (a) to examine whether students’ emotional intelligence abilities are related to their happiness and (b) to explore whether students’ basic psychological needs’ satisfaction mediates the relations between students’ emotional intelligence and happiness.
We hypothesized that both EI and psychological needs’ satisfaction would have a positive relationship with happiness (H1 and H2). Among predictors, EI and psychological needs’ satisfaction would predict happiness (H3 and H4, respectively). Finally, the satisfaction of needs concerning competence, autonomy, and relatedness would mediate the relationship between EI and happiness (H5).

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants and Procedure

The study received approval from the University of Crete research ethics committee (15/25-1-2024). A web-based survey was conducted in Greece during the spring semester of the 2022–2023 academic year. 205 university students from the University of Crete were selected through simple random sampling to participate in the research, and they were recruited by the researchers during class time. The students were asked to voluntarily complete an online questionnaire forwarded by the first two authors to the prospective participants. The home page of the electronic questionnaire provided information on the purpose of the study. Participants gave their consent after they were informed that the questionnaire was anonymous and that they had the opportunity to withdraw at any time.
Statistical power and sample size calculations were performed using G*Power software [41] (version 2.3.17), developed by the University of Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany. For the multiple linear regression analysis that was used, the required sample size was determined based on an anticipated effect size, f2, of 0.15, an alpha error probability of 0.05, and a power of 0.95. The analysis indicated that a minimum of 107 participants would be necessary (F(2, 98) = 3.089; noncentrality parameter λ = 16.05, actual power = 0.95). Additionally, for the mediation analyses that were used, a sample size of 129 was required to achieve similar power levels and statistical confidence (F(4, 124) = 2.445; noncentrality parameter λ = 19.35; actual power = 0.95). The participants’ ages ranged from 18 to 51 years, with a mean age of 23.35 years (SD = 6.73) and a median age of 21 years. The sample’s gender composition was predominantly female (86%, n = 176), with males representing 13% (n = 27) and other gender identities accounting for 1% (n = 2).

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Emotional Intelligence

Students’ emotional intelligence was assessed using the Greek version of the Wong and Law Emotional Intelligence Scale (WLEIS) [42,43], consisting of 16 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = totally disagree; 5 = totally agree). Following the components of emotional intelligence proposed by Mayer and Salovey [44], the scale includes four subscales: Self-Emotion Appraisal (SEA; e.g., “I always know whether or not I am happy”), Appraisal of Others’ Emotion (AOE; e.g., “I am sensitive to the feelings of others”), Use of Emotion (UOE; e.g., “I always tell myself I am competent”), and regulation of emotion (ROE; e.g., “I am capable of controlling my emotions”). Reliability was satisfactory (α = 0.86, ω = 0.87), with subscale reliabilities ranging from α = 0.68 to 0.79 and ω = 0.72 to 0.82. Confirmatory factor analysis (Relative chi-square χ2/df = 2.08, Comparative fit index CFI= 0.91, Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) = 0.89, standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) = 0.07, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.06) confirmed the scale’s validity for measuring emotional intelligence in a Greek-speaking sample.

2.2.2. Happiness

In the current study, we used the Oxford Happiness Inventory [45] (OHQ), which is one of the most used measures to investigate happiness [46], and which has been translated and validated into many languages, including Greek [47]. Using this measure, EI was revealed as a predictive factor for happiness [48,49,50]. The Oxford Happiness Questionnaire [45] (OHQ; a = 0.92), consisting of 8 items (e.g., “I feel that life is very rewarding”), was used to assess students’ happiness. The participants rated each item on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The scale has a unidimensional structure [45]. Recent studies have shown that this questionnaire has good validity and reliability (e.g., Pákozdy et al., [11]; α =  0.78).
The scale demonstrated satisfactory reliability and validity in this study. The scale’s reliability was confirmed with a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.74 and a McDonald’s omega of 0.74, indicating good internal consistency. A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) provided support for the scale’s construct validity. The model fit indices were robust: the Chi-square-to-degrees of freedom ratio (χ2/df) was 1.48, p = 0.091, indicating a reasonable fit. The comparative fit index (CFI) was 0.97, the Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) was 0.96, and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) was 0.04, which all suggest an excellent model fit. The root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) was 0.05 with a 90% confidence interval ranging from 0.00 to 0.09, further confirming the adequacy of the model.

2.2.3. Basic Psychological Needs’ Satisfaction

The 21-item scale, measuring the satisfaction of needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness based on self-determination theory [3,27], was also used. The participants indicated on a scale from 1 (not true at all) to 7 (very true) the extent to which items were true for them. The scores on this scale demonstrated adequate reliability for autonomy (e.g., “I am free to express my ideas and opinions”; seven items), competence (e.g., “Most days I feel a sense of accomplishment from my schoolwork”; six items), and relatedness (e.g., “I really like the people I go to school with”; eight items).
According to preliminary analyses of the scale, satisfactory levels of reliability were revealed (α = 0.87; ω = 0.88). The subscales’ reliability was also robust (from 0.70 to 0.79). The confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) confirmed the correspondence of the obtained model to the experimental one (autonomy: χ2/df = 2.18, CFI = 0.95, TLI = 0.92, SRMR = 0.05, and RMSEA = 0.08; competence: χ2/df = 2.03, CFI = 0.96, TLI = 0.93, SRMR = 0.04, and RMSEA = 0.07; and relatedness: χ2/df = 1.94, CFI = 0.95, TLI = 0.93, SRMR = 0.04, and RMSEA = 0.07).

2.3. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using Jamovi 2.3.17, developed by The Jamovi Project Pty Ltd, Tighes Hill, New South Wales, Australia. We ensured data accuracy and checked assumptions, confirming no missing data. Descriptive statistics were examined, and latent variables were created. A correlation matrix explored the relations among these variables, followed by a multiple linear regression with happiness (OHQ) as the dependent variable, which was influenced by SEA, AOE, UOE, ROE, autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Four mediation analyses assessed the indirect effects of SEA, AOE, UOE, and ROE on happiness through autonomy, competence, and relatedness, providing insights into emotional intelligence’s mediated influence on well-being.

3. Results

The analysis started with a correlation matrix, showing significant relations among the main variables (see Table 1).
In Table 2, multiple linear regression was used to predict happiness based on variables identified from earlier correlations. Predictors included SEA, AOE, UOE, ROE, autonomy, competence, and relatedness. The Durbin–Watson statistic (2.15; p = 0.264) showed no significant autocorrelation. The Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) values were below 3, indicating no multicollinearity. The Shapiro–Wilk test confirmed a normal distribution of residuals (W = 0.99; p = 0.734). The model explained 64% of the variance in happiness (R2 = 0.64; adjusted R2 = 0.63) and was a good fit (root mean square error, RMSE = 0.47), with F (8, 196) = 43.84, and p < 0.001.
Competence was the strongest predictor of happiness (B = 0.26, β = 0.32, and p < 0.001). Autonomy also significantly predicted happiness (B = 0.21, β = 0.25, and p < 0.001). UOE (B = 0.12, β = 0.16, and p = 0.006) and ROE (B = 0.10, β = 0.14, and p = 0.013) were positively related to happiness, indicating that emotional usage and regulation contribute to enhanced happiness. SEA did not significantly predict happiness (B = −0.01, β = −0.01, and p = 0.823), while AOE (B = 0.06, β = 0.06, and p = 0.241) and relatedness (B = 0.10, β = 0.11, and p = 0.065) showed positive but non-significant relations.
Subsequently, a multiple-mediation analysis was conducted to examine the indirect effects of SEA on happiness (OHQ) via three psychological mediators: autonomy (AUTO), competence (COMP), and relatedness (RELAT) (see Figure 1).
The mediation model showed that SEA positively predicted autonomy (β = 0.45, SE = 0.07, and p < 0.001), competence (β = 0.48, SE = 0.07, and p < 0.001), and relatedness (β = 0.39, SE = 0.06, and p < 0.001), which in turn predicted happiness (see Table 3). Specifically, autonomy (β = 0.25, SE = 0.05, and p < 0.001), competence (β = 0.42, SE = 0.05, and p < 0.001), and relatedness (β = 0.14, SE = 0.05, and p = 0.015) were significantly linked to happiness. The indirect effects of SEA on happiness through autonomy (β = 0.12, SE = 0.03, 95% CI [0.04, 0.16], and p < 0.001), competence (β = 0.20, SE = 0.03, 95% CI [0.11, 0.24], and p < 0.001), and relatedness (β = 0.06, SE = 0.02, 95% CI [0.01, 0.09], and p = 0.024) were significant, suggesting that SEA enhances happiness by boosting autonomy, competence, and social relatedness. SEA also had a direct effect on happiness (β = 0.11, SE = 0.04, 95% CI [0.01, 0.19], and p = 0.028). The total effect of SEA on happiness was substantial (β = 0.49, SE = 0.05, 95% CI [0.32, 0.52], and p < 0.001).
Further mediation analyses examined AOE’s indirect effects on happiness (OHQ) through autonomy, competence, and relatedness as potential mediators (see Figure 2).
The analysis revealed that AOE significantly predicted autonomy (β = 0.19, z = 2.79, and p = 0.005), competence (β = 0.20, z = 2.94, and p = 0.003), and relatedness (β = 0.37, z = 5.77, and p < 0.001). These predictors, in turn, influenced happiness: autonomy (β = 0.29, z = 4.42, and p < 0.001) and competence (β = 0.45, z = 7.83, and p < 0.001) had significant effects, while relatedness showed a non-significant positive trend (β = 0.11, z = 1.87, and p = 0.062). Indirect effects of AOE via autonomy (β = 0.05, z = 2.36, and p = 0.018) and competence (β = 0.09, z = 2.75, and p = 0.006) were significant, but the path through relatedness was not (β = 0.04, z = 1.78, and p = 0.076). AOE also had a direct effect on happiness (β = 0.10, z = 2.18, and p = 0.029), with a total effect of β = 0.29 (z = 4.35; p < 0.001) (see Table 4).
Building on the analyses of SEA and AOE, the third mediation analysis examined UOE’s indirect effects on happiness (OHQ) through autonomy, competence, and relatedness as mediators (see Figure 3).
UOE significantly predicted autonomy (β = 0.49; p < 0.001), competence (β = 0.57; p < 0.001), and relatedness (β = 0.34; p < 0.001). These needs, in turn, significantly predicted happiness, showing how effective emotional use promotes well-being (See Table 5). The indirect effects were significant: autonomy (β = 0.11; p = 0.001), competence (β = 0.21; p < 0.001), and relatedness (β = 0.05; p = 0.012). These findings highlight UOE’s role in enhancing happiness by supporting psychological well-being. Additionally, UOE had a direct effect on happiness (β = 0.22; p < 0.001), reinforcing its importance in emotional well-being. The total effect was substantial (β = 0.59; p < 0.001).
The final mediation analysis examined ROE’s indirect effects on happiness (OHQ) through autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Each need was analyzed as a mediator between ROE and happiness, along with a comprehensive model integrating all pathways (see Figure 4).
The mediation analysis showed that ROE significantly influenced autonomy (β = 0.31; p < 0.001), competence (β = 0.44; p < 0.001), and relatedness (β = 0.35; p < 0.001), highlighting the role of emotional regulation in enhancing psychological needs. Each mediator impacted happiness: autonomy (β = 0.29; p < 0.001), competence (β = 0.39; p < 0.001), and relatedness (β = 0.12; p = 0.037). Significant indirect effects were found for autonomy (β = 0.09; p = 0.001) and competence (β = 0.17; p < 0.001), while relatedness showed a near-significant effect (β = 0.04; p = 0.052). ROE also had a direct effect on happiness (β = 0.18; p < 0.001), contributing independently to happiness. The total effect of ROE on happiness was substantial (β = 0.48; p < 0.001), including both direct and mediated effects through psychological needs (see Table 6).

4. Discussion

Aiming to explore university students’ happiness, the current study indicated that EI was positively and significantly related to happiness (H1). Specifically, SEA had the strongest correlation with happiness, followed by UOE and ROE. Our findings suggest that understanding, using, and regulating emotions are important contributors to increased happiness, assuring that highly emotionally intelligent people are happier [23,25,51]. Similarly, psychological needs’ satisfaction also showed strong positive correlations with happiness (H2). These relationships clearly reveal that university students who are autonomous, competent, and connected with others tend to experience higher levels of happiness. Therefore, our results highlight the beneficial role of psychological needs’ satisfaction [3].
Among predictors, UOE and ROE significantly predicted happiness (H3), corresponding with studies proposing that these specific EI components, as affective–cognitive structures or emotion schemas, promote adaptive functioning [52]. Nevertheless, a recent systematic review and meta-analysis by Quílez-Robres et al. [53] suggested that setting up programs to stimulate EI in academic settings can improve both personal development and academic performance. Following this suggestion, our findings could suggest further implications for institutions, such as activating achievement emotions of enjoyment, hope, and pride to enhance students’ motivation [54], as well as counseling and psychological services, focused on emotional regulation techniques such as mindfulness, cognitive-behavioral strategies, and stress management [55].
In addition, competence and autonomy significantly predicted university students’ happiness (H4). This understanding suggests that, when students satisfy their needs for competence and autonomy, they feel happier. These findings align with previous research by Deci and Ryan [3], who also identified competence and autonomy as key predictors of well-being in academic settings. Based on the above, several implications for institutions can be suggested to help students satisfy both psychological needs, such as enhancing students’ sense of competence through mentorship programs and effective feedback systems [56] and promoting autonomy by allowing students to choose individual or group work and engage in optional tests [57].
Finally, the current study indicated indirect effects of competence on the relation between EI and university students’ happiness (H5). The study revealed a clear causal relationship between competence and happiness, with competence mediating the relationship between emotional intelligence and happiness. Specifically, the mediation results showed that EI influences happiness not only directly but also indirectly through the satisfaction of psychological need for competence. Therefore, we can say that highly intelligent individuals are more capable of fulfilling this need, which in turn increases their happiness. This finding is in line with previous studies that reveal competence as the dominant variable among the three types of basic psychological needs and suggest enhancing students’ competence in the context of higher education [57,58]. Taken together, the study suggests that having high EI through competence is a promising path toward university students’ happiness. Given that studying at university is a crucial period for personal growth, these findings offer valuable insights for professors and stakeholders to foster happiness among university students. Evidence-based intervention programs, aiming at improving emergent adolescents’ well-being, could facilitate their smooth transition to adulthood [59].
However, we are cautious about stating causal relations because of the self-reported instruments and the cross-sectional design of the study. Future studies could apply multiple assessment methods (e.g., focus groups and diary studies) and use longitudinal patterns to strengthen the validity of the findings. Additionally, given that female university students score higher in EI than their male counterparts (e.g., Fida et al., [60]; Meshkat & Nejati, [61]), while females are less happy than males (e.g., Barattucci et al., [46]; Gawas, [62]), future research should strive for a more gender-balanced sample. We should also mention the specific cultural context of our study, as all participants were Greek university students. This cultural specificity may have introduced potential bias, limiting the generalizability of the findings to students from different cultural backgrounds. Future research should aim to replicate the study in more diverse cultural settings to assess the validity of the results. All in all, the current study provides useful suggestions for institutions that can have a positive effect on university students’ happiness and, therefore, on their academic performance.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.V. and E.V.; methodology, A.V. and K.M.; formal analysis, K.M.; data curation, A.V., E.V. and K.M.; writing—original draft preparation, A.V., E.V. and K.M.; writing—review and editing, A.V., E.V., K.M. and E.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the University of Crete Ethics Committee (approval number: 15/25-1-2024).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data can be obtained upon request from the first author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. van Zyl, Y.; Dhurup, M. Self-Efficacy and Its Relationship with Satisfaction with Life and Happiness among University Students. J. Psychol. Afr. 2018, 28, 389–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Mayer, J.D.; Salovey, P.; Caruso, D.R. Emotional Intelligence as Zeitgeist, as Personality, and as a Mental Ability. In The Handbook of Emotional Intelligence: Theory, Development, Assessment, and Application at Home, School, and in the Workplace; Bar-On, R., Parker, J.D.A., Eds.; Jossey-Bass: San Francisco, CA, USA; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 2000; pp. 92–117. [Google Scholar]
  3. Ryan, R.M.; Deci, E.L. Self-Determination Theory: Basic Psychological Needs in Motivation, Development, and Wellness; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2017; ISBN 9781462538966. [Google Scholar]
  4. Chen, D.-F.; Huang, K.-W.; Ho, W.-S.; Cheng, Y.-C. Savoring Belief, Resilience, and Meaning in Life as Pathways to Happiness: A Sequential Mediation Analysis among Taiwanese University Students. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Argyle, M.; Martin, M.; Crossland, J. Happiness as a Function of Personality and Social Encounters. In Recent Advances in Social Psychology: An International Perspective; Forgas, J.P., Innes, J.M., Eds.; Elsevier Science Publishers: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1989; pp. 189–203. [Google Scholar]
  6. Mahakud, G.C.; Yadav, R. Effects of Happiness on Mental Health. Int. J. Indian Psychol. 2015, 2, 106–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Makki, N.; Mohanty, M.S. Mental Health and Happiness: Evidence From the U.S. Data. AM Econ. 2019, 64, 197–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Torales, J.; Barrios, I.; Melgarejo, O.; Ruiz Díaz, N.; O’Higgins, M.; Navarro, R.; Amarilla, D.; Almirón-Santacruz, J.; González-Urbieta, I.; Caycho-Rodríguez, T.; et al. Hope, Resilience and Subjective Happiness among General Population of Paraguay in the Post COVID-19 Pandemic. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 2024, 70, 489–497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Zhang, Z.; Lv, J.; Jiang, T.; Chen, X.; Luo, Y. Motivation Matters: Autonomous Motivation Sustains the Happiness from Prosocial Behavior. Motiv. Sci. 2024, 10, 100–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Liu, H.; Yu, B. Serious Leisure, Leisure Satisfaction and Subjective Well-Being of Chinese University Students. Soc. Indic. Res. 2015, 122, 159–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Pákozdy, C.; Askew, J.; Dyer, J.; Gately, P.; Martin, L.; Mavor, K.I.; Brown, G.R. The Imposter Phenomenon and Its Relationship with Self-Efficacy, Perfectionism and Happiness in University Students. Curr. Psychol. 2024, 43, 5153–5162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Mao, F.; Yang, X. Literary Therapy Based on Positive Psychology Impact on College Students’ Happiness. J. Poet. Ther. 2024, 37, 16–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Petrides, K.V.; Mikolajczak, M.; Mavroveli, S.; Sanchez-Ruiz, M.-J.; Furnham, A.; Pérez-González, J.-C. Developments in Trait Emotional Intelligence Research. Emot. Rev. 2016, 8, 335–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Mavroveli, S.; Petrides, K.V.; Rieffe, C.; Bakker, F. Trait Emotional Intelligence, Psychological Well-being and Peer-rated Social Competence in Adolescence. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2007, 25, 263–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Bar-On, R. Emotional Intelligence: An Integral Part of Positive Psychology. S. Afr. J. Psychol. 2010, 40, 54–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Petrides, K.V.; Pita, R.; Kokkinaki, F. The Location of Trait Emotional Intelligence in Personality Factor Space. Br. J. Psychol. 2007, 98, 273–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Malinauskas, R.; Malinauskiene, V. The Relationship between Emotional Intelligence and Psychological Well-Being among Male University Students: The Mediating Role of Perceived Social Support and Perceived Stress. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 1605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Mayer, J.D.; Salovey, P.; Caruso, D.R.; Sitarenios, G. Measuring Emotional Intelligence with the MSCEIT V2.0. Emotion 2003, 3, 97–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Diener, E.; Emmons, R.A.; Larsen, R.J.; Griffin, S. The Satisfaction with Life Scale. J. Pers. Assess. 1985, 49, 71–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Zeidner, M.; Olnick-Shemesh, D. Emotional Intelligence and Subjective Well-Being Revisited. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2010, 48, 431–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Abdollahi, A.; Abu Talib, M.; Motalebi, S.A. Emotional Intelligence and Depressive Symptoms as Predictors of Happiness Among Adolescents. Iran J. Psychiatry Behav. Sci. 2015, 9, e2268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Abdollahi, A.; Hosseinian, S.; Panahipour, H.; Najafi, M.; Soheili, F. Emotional Intelligence as a Moderator between Perfectionism and Happiness. Sch. Psychol. Int. 2019, 40, 88–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Blasco-Belled, A.; Rogoza, R.; Torrelles-Nadal, C.; Alsinet, C. Emotional Intelligence Structure and Its Relationship with Life Satisfaction and Happiness: New Findings from the Bifactor Model. J. Happiness Stud. 2020, 21, 2031–2049. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Sánchez-Álvarez, N.; Extremera, N.; Fernández-Berrocal, P. The Relation between Emotional Intelligence and Subjective Well-Being: A Meta-Analytic Investigation. J. Posit. Psychol. 2016, 11, 276–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Xu, X.; Pang, W.; Xia, M. Are Emotionally Intelligent People Happier? A Meta-analysis of the Relationship between Emotional Intelligence and Subjective Well-being Using Chinese Samples. Asian J. Soc. Psychol. 2021, 24, 477–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Deci, E.L.; Ryan, R.M. Self-Determination Theory: A Macrotheory of Human Motivation, Development, and Health. Can. Psychol. 2008, 49, 182–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Deci, E.L.; Ryan, R.M. The “What” and “Why” of Goal Pursuits: Human Needs and the Self-Determination of Behavior. Psychol. Inq. 2000, 11, 227–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Vansteenkiste, M.; Niemiec, C.P.; Soenens, B. The Development of the Five Mini-Theories of Self-Determination Theory: An Historical Overview, Emerging Trends, and Future Directions. In The Decade Ahead: Theoretical Perspectives on Motivation and Achievement; Urdan, T.C., Karabenick, S.A., Eds.; Emerald Group Publishing: Leeds, UK, 2010; pp. 105–165. [Google Scholar]
  29. Dasinger, T.M.; Gibson, D.J. Perceptions of Mental Health and Need Satisfaction/Frustration among Rural University Students. J. Am. Coll. Health 2024, 72, 253–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Tian, L.; Tian, Q.; Huebner, E.S. School-Related Social Support and Adolescents’ School-Related Subjective Well-Being: The Mediating Role of Basic Psychological Needs Satisfaction at School. Soc. Indic. Res. 2016, 128, 105–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Froiland, J.M.; Worrell, F.C.; Oh, H. Teacher–Student Relationships, Psychological Need Satisfaction, and Happiness among Diverse Students. Psychol. Sch. 2019, 56, 856–870. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Jiang, F.; Lu, S.; Jiang, T.; Jia, H. Does the Relation between Humor Styles and Subjective Well-Being Vary across Culture and Age? A Meta-Analysis. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 2213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Arnett, J.J. New Horizons in Research on Emerging and Young Adulthood. In Early Adulthood in a Family Context; Booth, A., Brown, S.L., Landale, N.S., Manning, W.D., McHale, S.M., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 231–244. [Google Scholar]
  34. Arnett, J.J. Emerging Adulthood: A Theory of Development from the Late Teens through the Twenties. Am. Psychol. 2000, 55, 469–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Galanaki, E.; Sideridis, G. Dimensions of Emerging Adulthood, Criteria for Adulthood, and Identity Development in Greek Studying Youth: A Person-Centered Approach. Emerg. Adulthood 2019, 7, 411–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Vleioras, G.; Galanaki, E.P. Comparison of Adulthood Criteria Endorsed by Emerging Adults and Their Parents in Greece: A Mixed-Method Study. J. Genet. Psychol. 2024, 185, 366–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Silva, R.G.; Figueiredo-Braga, M. Evaluation of the Relationships among Happiness, Stress, Anxiety, and Depression in Pharmacy Students. Curr. Pharm. Teach. Learn. 2018, 10, 903–910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Kins, E.; Beyers, W.; Soenens, B.; Vansteenkiste, M. Patterns of Home Leaving and Subjective Well-Being in Emerging Adulthood: The Role of Motivational Processes and Parental Autonomy Support. Dev. Psychol. 2009, 45, 1416–1429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Gómez-López, M.; Viejo, C.; Ortega-Ruiz, R. Well-Being and Romantic Relationships: A Systematic Review in Adolescence and Emerging Adulthood. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 2415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Milić, J.; Škrlec, I.; Milić Vranješ, I.; Podgornjak, M.; Heffer, M. High Levels of Depression and Anxiety among Croatian Medical and Nursing Students and the Correlation between Subjective Happiness and Personality Traits. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 2019, 31, 653–660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Şahin, M.; Aybek, E. Jamovi: An Easy to Use Statistical Software for the Social Scientists. Int. J. Assess. Tools Educ. 2019, 6, 670–692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Kafetsios, K.; Zampetakis, L.A. Emotional Intelligence and Job Satisfaction: Testing the Mediatory Role of Positive and Negative Affect at Work. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2008, 44, 712–722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Wong, C.S.; Law, K.S. Wong and Law Emotional Intelligence Scale (WLEIS) [Database Record]; APA PsycTests: Washington, DC, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  44. Mayer, J.D.; Salovey, P. What Is Emotional Intelligence? In Emotional Development and Emotional Intelligence: Educational Implications; Salovey, P., Sluyter, D.J., Eds.; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1997; pp. 3–34. [Google Scholar]
  45. Hills, P.; Argyle, M. The Oxford Happiness Questionnaire: A Compact Scale for the Measurement of Psychological Well-Being. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2002, 33, 1073–1082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Barattucci, M.; Brugnera, A.; Ramaci, T.; Kuvačić, G.; De Giorgio, A. An 8-Item Scale for the Measurement of Happiness: Validation and Application of the Oxford Happiness Questionnaire in an Italian Sample. Curr. Psychol. 2024, 43, 11092–11101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Grigoriadou, E.; Kolias, P.; Theocharidou, M.; Gkioka, M. Psychometric Properties of the Oxford Happiness Questionnaire Short Form (OHQ-SF) in a Greek Student Sample. Discov. Psychol. 2024, 4, 53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Platsidou, M. Trait Emotional Intelligence Predicts Happiness, but How? An Empirical Study in Adolescents and Young Adults. Int. J. Wellbeing 2013, 3, 197–209. [Google Scholar]
  49. Ghahramani, S.; Jahromi, A.T.; Khoshsoroor, D.; Seifooripour, R.; Sepehrpoor, M. The Relationship between Emotional Intelligence and Happiness in Medical Students. Korean J. Med. Educ. 2019, 31, 29–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Hatamnejad, M.R.; Hosseinpour, M.; Shiati, S.; Seifaee, A.; Sayari, M.; Seyyedi, F.; Lankarani, K.B.; Ghahramani, S. Emotional Intelligence and Happiness in Clinical Medical Students: A Cross-sectional Multicenter Study. Health Sci. Rep. 2023, 6, e1745. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Szczygieł, D.; Mikolajczak, M. Why Are People High in Emotional Intelligence Happier? They Make the Most of Their Positive Emotions. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2017, 117, 177–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Izard, C.; Stark, K.; Trentacosta, C.; Schultz, D. Beyond Emotion Regulation: Emotion Utilization and Adaptive Functioning. Child Dev. Perspect. 2008, 2, 156–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Quílez-Robres, A.; Usán, P.; Lozano-Blasco, R.; Salavera, C. Emotional Intelligence and Academic Performance: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Think. Ski. Creat. 2023, 49, 101355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Frumos, F.-V.; Leonte, R.; Candel, O.S.; Ciochină-Carasevici, L.; Ghiaţău, R.; Onu, C. The Relationship between University Students’ Goal Orientation and Academic Achievement. The Mediating Role of Motivational Components and the Moderating Role of Achievement Emotions. Front. Psychol. 2024, 14, 1296346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. González-Valero, G.; Zurita-Ortega, F.; Ubago-Jiménez, J.L.; Puertas-Molero, P. Use of Meditation and Cognitive Behavioral Therapies for the Treatment of Stress, Depression and Anxiety in Students. A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 4394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Fountain, J.; Newcomer, K.E. Developing and Sustaining Effective Faculty Mentoring Programs. J. Public Aff. Educ. 2016, 22, 483–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Wang, C.; Cho, H.J.; Wiles, B.; Moss, J.D.; Bonem, E.M.; Li, Q.; Lu, Y.; Levesque-Bristol, C. Competence and Autonomous Motivation as Motivational Predictors of College Students’ Mathematics Achievement: From the Perspective of Self-Determination Theory. Int. J. STEM Educ. 2022, 9, 41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Wang, C.; Zhang, Y.; Moss, J.D.; Bonem, E.M.; Levesque-Bristol, C. Multilevel Factors Affecting College Students’ Perceived Knowledge Transferability: From the Perspective of Self-Determination Theory. Res. High Educ. 2020, 61, 1002–1026. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. de la Fuente, R.; Parra, Á.; Sánchez-Queija, I.; Lizaso, I. Flourishing During Emerging Adulthood from a Gender Perspective. J. Happiness Stud. 2020, 21, 2889–2908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Fida, A.; Ghaffar, A.; Zaman, A.; Satti, A.N. Gender Comparison of Emotional Intelligence of University Students. J. Educ. Educ. Dev. 2018, 5, 172–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Meshkat, M.; Nejati, R. Does Emotional Intelligence Depend on Gender? A Study on Undergraduate English Majors of Three Iranian Universities. Sage Open 2017, 7, 215824401772579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Gawas, A.G.A. Effect of Optimism, Happiness and Self-Esteem on Quality of Life among Yemeni University Students. Indones. Psychol. Res. 2024, 6, 17–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Self-emotion appraisal and indirect and total effects on happiness. SEA: self-emotion appraisal; OHQ: Oxford Happiness Questionnaire; AUTO: autonomy; COMP: competence; RELAT: relatedness.
Figure 1. Self-emotion appraisal and indirect and total effects on happiness. SEA: self-emotion appraisal; OHQ: Oxford Happiness Questionnaire; AUTO: autonomy; COMP: competence; RELAT: relatedness.
Psycholint 06 00055 g001
Figure 2. Appraisal of others’ emotion and indirect and total effects on happiness. SEA: self-emotion appraisal; AOE: appraisal of others’ emotion; UOE: use of emotion; ROE: regulation of emotion; OHQ: Oxford Happiness Questionnaire; AUTO: autonomy; COMP: competence; RELAT: relatedness.
Figure 2. Appraisal of others’ emotion and indirect and total effects on happiness. SEA: self-emotion appraisal; AOE: appraisal of others’ emotion; UOE: use of emotion; ROE: regulation of emotion; OHQ: Oxford Happiness Questionnaire; AUTO: autonomy; COMP: competence; RELAT: relatedness.
Psycholint 06 00055 g002
Figure 3. Use of emotion and indirect and total effects on happiness. UOE: use of emotion; OHQ: Oxford Happiness Questionnaire; AUTO: autonomy; COMP: competence; RELAT: relatedness.
Figure 3. Use of emotion and indirect and total effects on happiness. UOE: use of emotion; OHQ: Oxford Happiness Questionnaire; AUTO: autonomy; COMP: competence; RELAT: relatedness.
Psycholint 06 00055 g003
Figure 4. Regulation of emotion and indirect and total effects on happiness. ROE: regulation of Emotion; OHQ: Oxford Happiness Questionnaire; AUTO: autonomy; COMP: competence; RELAT: relatedness.
Figure 4. Regulation of emotion and indirect and total effects on happiness. ROE: regulation of Emotion; OHQ: Oxford Happiness Questionnaire; AUTO: autonomy; COMP: competence; RELAT: relatedness.
Psycholint 06 00055 g004
Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations of the latent variables.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations of the latent variables.
SEAAOEUOEROEOHQAUTOCOMPRELAT
SEA
AOE0.42 *
UOE0.49 *0.31 *
ROE0.55 *0.23 *0.47 *
OHQ0.49 *0.29 *0.59 *0.48 *
AUTO0.45 *0.19 **0.49 *0.31 *0.66 *
COMP0.48 *0.20 **0.57 *0.44 *0.71 *0.63 *
RELAT0.39 *0.37 *0.34 *0.35 *0.56 *0.63 *0.49 *
Mean5.265.555.114.604.174.874.615.32
SD0.910.821.011.040.790.960.990.88
Note. ** p < 0.01, and * p < 0.001. SEA: self-emotion appraisal; AOE: appraisal of others’ emotion; UOE: use of emotion; ROE: regulation of emotion; OHQ: Oxford Happiness Questionnaire; AUTO: autonomy; COMP: competence; RELAT: relatedness, SD: Standard Deviation.
Table 2. Model coefficients for the happiness index.
Table 2. Model coefficients for the happiness index.
95% CI 95% CI
PredictorBSELLULtpβLLUL
Intercept0.170.36−0.550.880.460.649
SEA−0.010.05−0.110.09−0.220.823−0.01−0.130.10
AOE0.060.05−0.040.151.180.2410.06−0.040.16
UOE0.120.040.040.212.770.0060.160.050.27
ROE0.100.040.020.192.490.0130.140.030.25
AUTO0.210.050.100.313.77<0.0010.250.120.38
RELAT0.100.05−0.010.211.860.0650.11−0.010.23
COMP0.260.050.150.364.95<0.0010.320.190.45
Note. SEA: self-emotion appraisal; AOE: appraisal of others’ emotion; UOE: use of emotion; ROE: regulation of emotion; OHQ: Oxford Happiness Questionnaire; AUTO: autonomy; COMP: competence; RELAT: relatedness, CI: Confidence Interval, SE: Standard Error, LL: Lower Limit, UL: Upper Limit, t: t-value, p: p-value, β: Standardized Beta Coefficient.
Table 3. Self-emotion appraisal and indirect and total effects on happiness.
Table 3. Self-emotion appraisal and indirect and total effects on happiness.
95% CI
TypeEffectsEstimateSELLULβzp
IndirectSEA ⇒ AUTO ⇒ OHQ0.100.030.040.160.123.44<0.001
SEA ⇒ COMP ⇒ OHQ0.180.030.110.240.205.27<0.001
SEA ⇒ RELAT ⇒ OHQ0.050.020.010.090.062.260.024
ComponentSEA ⇒ AUTO0.480.070.350.600.457.25<0.001
AUTO ⇒ OHQ0.210.050.100.310.253.90<0.001
SEA ⇒ COMP0.520.070.390.650.487.86<0.001
COMP ⇒ OHQ0.340.050.240.430.427.10<0.001
SEA ⇒ RELAT0.380.060.260.500.396.13<0.001
RELAT ⇒ OHQ0.130.050.020.230.142.440.015
DirectSEA ⇒ OHQ0.100.040.010.190.112.200.028
TotalSEA ⇒ OHQ0.420.050.320.520.498.00<0.001
Note. Confidence intervals computed with the standard method (delta method). Betas are completely standardized effect sizes. SEA: self-emotion appraisal; OHQ: Oxford Happiness Questionnaire; AUTO: autonomy; COMP: competence; RELAT: relatedness, z = z-value.
Table 4. Appraisal of others’ emotion and indirect and total effects on happiness.
Table 4. Appraisal of others’ emotion and indirect and total effects on happiness.
95% CI
TypeEffectsEstimateSELLULβzp
IndirectAOE ⇒ AUTO ⇒ OHQ0.050.020.010.100.052.360.018
AOE ⇒ COMP ⇒ OHQ0.090.030.030.150.092.750.006
AOE ⇒ RELAT ⇒ OHQ0.040.02−0.000.090.041.780.076
ComponentAOE ⇒ AUTO0.220.080.070.380.192.790.005
AUTO ⇒ OHQ0.230.050.130.340.294.42<0.001
AOE ⇒ COMP0.240.080.080.410.202.940.003
COMP ⇒ OHQ0.360.050.270.450.457.83<0.001
AOE ⇒ RELAT0.400.070.270.540.375.77<0.001
RELAT ⇒ OHQ0.100.05−0.010.210.111.870.062
DirectAOE ⇒ OHQ0.100.050.010.190.102.180.029
TotalAOE ⇒ OHQ0.280.060.150.410.294.35<0.001
Note. Confidence intervals computed with the standard method (delta method). Betas are completely standardized effect sizes. Note. AOE: appraisal of others’ emotion; OHQ: Oxford Happiness Questionnaire; AUTO: autonomy; COMP: competence; RELAT: relatedness.
Table 5. Use of emotion and indirect and total effects on happiness.
Table 5. Use of emotion and indirect and total effects on happiness.
95% CI
TypeEffectsEstimateSELLULβzp
IndirectUOE ⇒ AUTO ⇒ OHQ0.090.030.030.140.113.230.001
UOE ⇒ COMP ⇒ OHQ0.160.030.100.220.215.11<0.001
UOE ⇒ RELAT ⇒ OHQ0.040.020.010.080.052.510.012
ComponentUOE ⇒ AUTO0.470.060.350.580.498.00<0.001
AUTO ⇒ OHQ0.180.050.080.290.233.54<0.001
UOE ⇒ COMP0.560.060.450.670.579.97<0.001
COMP ⇒ OHQ0.290.050.190.380.365.96<0.001
UOE ⇒ RELAT0.300.060.180.410.345.14<0.001
RELAT ⇒ OHQ0.140.050.050.240.162.870.004
DirectUOE ⇒ OHQ0.170.040.090.250.224.12<0.001
TotalUOE ⇒ OHQ0.460.040.370.550.5910.44<0.001
Note. Confidence intervals computed with the standard method (delta method). Betas are completely standardized effect sizes. UOE: use of emotion; OHQ: Oxford Happiness Questionnaire; AUTO: autonomy; COMP: competence; RELAT: relatedness.
Table 6. Regulation of emotion and indirect and total effects on happiness.
Table 6. Regulation of emotion and indirect and total effects on happiness.
95% CI
TypeEffectsEstimateSELLULβzp
IndirectROE ⇒ AUTO ⇒ OHQ0.070.020.030.110.093.230.001
ROE ⇒ COMP ⇒ OHQ0.130.030.080.180.174.79<0.001
ROE ⇒ RELAT ⇒ OHQ0.030.02−0.000.060.041.940.052
ComponentROE ⇒ AUTO0.280.060.160.400.314.60<0.001
AUTO ⇒ OHQ0.230.050.130.340.294.52<0.001
ROE ⇒ COMP0.420.060.300.530.447.02<0.001
COMP ⇒ OHQ0.310.050.220.400.396.55<0.001
ROE ⇒ RELAT0.300.060.190.400.355.35<0.001
RELAT ⇒ OHQ0.110.050.010.210.122.080.037
DirectROE ⇒ OHQ0.140.040.070.210.183.80<0.001
TotalROE ⇒ OHQ0.370.050.280.460.487.92<0.001
Note. Confidence intervals computed with the standard method (delta method). Betas are completely standardized effect sizes. ROE: regulation of emotion; OHQ: Oxford Happiness Questionnaire; AUTO: autonomy; COMP: competence; RELAT: relatedness.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Vasiou, A.; Vasilaki, E.; Mastrothanasis, K.; Galanaki, E. Emotional Intelligence and University Students’ Happiness: The Mediating Role of Basic Psychological Needs’ Satisfaction. Psychol. Int. 2024, 6, 855-867. https://doi.org/10.3390/psycholint6040055

AMA Style

Vasiou A, Vasilaki E, Mastrothanasis K, Galanaki E. Emotional Intelligence and University Students’ Happiness: The Mediating Role of Basic Psychological Needs’ Satisfaction. Psychology International. 2024; 6(4):855-867. https://doi.org/10.3390/psycholint6040055

Chicago/Turabian Style

Vasiou, Aikaterini, Eleni Vasilaki, Konstantinos Mastrothanasis, and Evangelia Galanaki. 2024. "Emotional Intelligence and University Students’ Happiness: The Mediating Role of Basic Psychological Needs’ Satisfaction" Psychology International 6, no. 4: 855-867. https://doi.org/10.3390/psycholint6040055

APA Style

Vasiou, A., Vasilaki, E., Mastrothanasis, K., & Galanaki, E. (2024). Emotional Intelligence and University Students’ Happiness: The Mediating Role of Basic Psychological Needs’ Satisfaction. Psychology International, 6(4), 855-867. https://doi.org/10.3390/psycholint6040055

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop