Next Article in Journal
Using the Food Stress Index for Emergency Food Assistance: An Australian Case Series Analysis during the COVID-19 Pandemic and Natural Disasters
Next Article in Special Issue
Subjective Evaluation on the Annoyance of Environmental Noise Containing Low-Frequency Tonal Components
Previous Article in Journal
Can Positive Mindsets Be Protective Against Stress and Isolation Experienced during the COVID-19 Pandemic? A Mixed Methods Approach to Understanding Emotional Health and Wellbeing Needs of Perinatal Women
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Effect of an Alternative Definition of “Percent Highly Annoyed” on the Exposure–Response Relationship: Comparison of Noise Annoyance Responses Measured by ICBEN 5-Point Verbal and 11-Point Numerical Scales
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Effects of Nocturnal Aircraft Noise and Non-Acoustical Factors on Short-Term Annoyance in Primary School Children

1
Department of Sleep and Human Factors Research, Institute of Aerospace Medicine, German Aerospace Center (DLR), 51147 Cologne, Germany
2
Department of Medical Biometry, Informatics and Epidemiology (IMBIE), Faculty of Medicine, University of Bonn, 53127 Bonn, Germany
3
Faculty of Medicine, University of Bonn, 53127 Bonn, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Authors contributed in equal shares (shared first authorship).
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(13), 6959; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18136959
Submission received: 31 May 2021 / Revised: 23 June 2021 / Accepted: 25 June 2021 / Published: 29 June 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Environmental Noise and Annoyance)

Abstract

:
Children are considered at higher risk for harmful noise effects due to their sensitive development phase. Here, we investigated the effects of nocturnal aircraft noise exposure on short-term annoyance assessed in the morning in 51 primary school children (8–10 years) living in the surrounding community of Cologne-Bonn Airport. Child-appropriate short-term annoyance assessments and associated non-acoustical variables were surveyed. Nocturnal aircraft noise exposure was recorded inside the children’s bedrooms. Exposure–response models were calculated by using random effects logistic regression models. The present data were compared with those from a previous study near Cologne-Bonn Airport in adults using very similar methodology. Short-term annoyance reaction in children was not affected by the nocturnal aircraft noise exposure. Non-acoustical factors (e.g., the attitude that “aircraft are dangerous” or noise sensitivity), however, significantly impacted on children’s short-term annoyance. In contrast to children, the probability of moderate to high annoyance in adults increased with the number of aircraft flyovers during the time in bed. It is concluded that short-term annoyance from nocturnal aircraft noise in children is mainly determined by non-acoustical factors. Unlike in adults, acoustical factors did not play a significant role.

1. Introduction

Rising demand for air, rail, and road travel implies that a growing number of people is being exposed to environmental noise, and noise exposure is increasingly being regarded as a prominent public health problem [1,2]. Noise is a main cause of environmental annoyance, and it negatively influences quality of life for large parts of the population. Environmental noise is generally defined as any kind of unwanted sound resulting from human activities, including noise emitted by all modes of transport [3]. Negative effects of traffic noise exposure on adults with respect to cardiovascular diseases [4,5,6,7,8] mental health [9,10,11,12], annoyance [5,13,14], cognitive performance and communication [5,15], and sleep disturbances [4,5,16,17,18] are empirically very well-documented. With regard to the number of affected residents, sleep disturbances manifested in changes in sleep depth and sleep continuity, awakening reactions and reduced (self-rated) sleep quality are considered to be the most serious impact of environmental noise [19,20,21].
Recently, noise effects research has increasingly focused on “groups at risk” or “susceptible groups” [22], i.e., the parts of the population who show particular vulnerability to harmful noise impacts. These so-called vulnerable groups include elderly and sick people, shift workers, as well as children [5]. It can be assumed that the majority of children living in urbanized regions are more or less affected by traffic noise throughout the day. Negative effects of chronic noise exposure on children’s learning and cognitive performance [5,12,15,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36], motivation [24,37,38], and quality of life including annoyance [15,24,28,30,31,39,40,41,42,43,44] have been extensively described in the literature so far. Only few well-founded studies on noise impacts on sleep in children have been available up to now [45,46,47] even though their sleep is expected to be particularly sensitive to traffic noise impacts [48,49,50,51]. Different physiological and psychological approaches have been used to explain the adverse effects of noise exposure on children [12,24,25,39,41,52,53,54,55,56,57,58]. An example is the theory of environmental stress [27,59]. The central aspect of this approach assumes that noise represents an environmental stressor, i.e., persons living in noisy environments see noise as a threat and perceive it stressful. This supposition is particularly important given that children’s ability to estimate or judge threats from environmental stressors is not fully developed. Thus, they have less control over threatening situations [60] which may result in increased susceptibility to adverse noise impacts.
Annoyance is the primary psychological parameter used to describe the impact of environmental noise on populations [61]. Annoyance primarily follows the noise-induced disturbance of (intended) activities [3] and includes negative emotional reactions to noise events [39,62]. Activities such as communication, relaxation, and recuperation are considered to be especially sensitive to noise-induced disturbances [3,61,62,63]. Sleep disturbances are the most common reason for noise complaints [64,65,66]. It is assumed that children’s and adults’ noise annoyance may be explained on the basis of the same psychological construct given that the emotional responses to disturbing noise events are similar [27,60]. Only a few studies have developed exposure–response models for noise-induced annoyance in children. In the cross-sectional EU project RANCH (Road traffic noise and Aircraft Noise exposure and children’s Cognition and Health), a curvilinear exposure–response relationship with chronic aircraft noise exposure both at school and at home was found for annoyance response in children (aged 9–11 years) when adjusted for confounding variables [42,67,68,69,70]. This model showed that children chronically exposed to aircraft noise had a significantly higher noise annoyance than children attending low aircraft noise exposed in both settings. Van Kempen et al. [70] documented that the exposure–response curves for aircraft noise annoyance in children and their parents were similar. However, children scored lower on annoyance at the top of the scale, and slightly higher at the lower end. Further studies on children’s long-term annoyance from chronic road [4] and aircraft noise [22,42,67,68,69,70] exposure indicated that children were noticeably less annoyed by traffic noise exposure than adults.
An established rule of thumb in noise effects research is that about one third of annoyance in adults is due to acoustical variables, another third caused by non-acoustical factors, while the last third has not been specified so far [3,13,39,63,71,72,73,74,75,76,77]. The individual noise sensitivity is an important personal variable [18,72,73,77,78,79]. There are a variety of psychological factors related to residents’ views on air transport [13,77,80,81,82,83]. They include, for instance, residents’ beliefs about how air traffic may affect them (including perceived social and economic advantages/disadvantages and its evaluation regarding the harm to one’s health), attitudes towards air traffic, feelings of helplessness in controlling air traffic and aircraft noise exposure, negative expectations regarding future aircraft noise development, perceived fairness and consideration of local residents’ interests in decision-making processes of the airport management, adaptation to long-term aircraft noise exposure, and worries about safety (e.g., fear of aircraft crashes). Though, studies examining the impact of non-acoustical influence variables with respect to children’s annoyance are sparse (e.g., [39,84]).
Recently, we investigated the acute physiological effects of night-time aircraft noise on sleep and associated psychological reactions (i.e., short-term annoyance, self-rated sleep quality, and cognitive performance) of primary school children. Results of the aircraft noise-induced short-term annoyance during the past night in children are presented here. A new children’s annoyance model was developed. For adult’s annoyance, the influence of acoustical and non-acoustical parameters has been widely demonstrated [13,39,71,72,73,75,76,77,80,81,83]. As noise-induced annoyance response in children is based on the same psychological construct used in adults [27,60], it was hypothesized that both acoustical and non-acoustical factors contribute to children’s short-term noise annoyance. Exposure–response models for annoyance due to night-time aircraft noise exposure were calculated by means of random effects logistic regression. The selection of potential acoustical and psychological predictors was based on previous studies on (aircraft) noise impact on annoyance in adults. To allow for a cross-sectional comparison of aircraft noise annoyance in children and adults, we also included data from a previous study (STRAIN; Study on Human Specific Response to Aircraft Noise) conducted by the German Aerospace Center (DLR) near Cologne-Bonn Airport in 2001/2002 in 64 adult residents. Data from the present and the STRAIN study [85] were compared on the basis of exposure–response models in order to reveal potential differences in the annoyance assessments between children and adults.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Fifty-one healthy and normal-hearing children aged 8 to 10 years participated in the study. There were 23 females (45%). All children passed a multi-stage selection procedure including questionnaires screening for major medical or intrinsic sleep disorders as well as an audiometric screening. Both children and parents volunteered to participate in the study and gave written informed consent in accordance with the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki prior to the study. The study protocol was approved by the ethics board of the North Rhine Chamber of Physicians. Participating children received age-adequate remuneration in the form of a voucher for a toy online shop or an amusement park. Parents received EUR 50.

2.2. Study Design

All measurements were implemented in a field study, i.e., at the children’s home under real-life condition. It was conducted during two measurement periods from June 2016 to November 2017 in the vicinity of Cologne-Bonn Airport. Cologne-Bonn Airport is an important passenger and cargo hub airport with the highest traffic densities at night mainly caused by freight traffic. About a quarter of daily flight movements take place between 10:00 p.m. and 6:00 a.m. [81]. Measurement sites were not exposed to major transportation or ambient noise sources other than air traffic. Bedroom windows of the participating children were not facing main roads or railways.
All children were studied for four consecutive nights. Measurements always started on a Wednesday. The time in bed of the study children corresponded to the age-appropriate sleep duration between 8 and 10 h [86]. Children slept with their usual window position in the bedroom. But they were asked not to change window position during the night. The first study night was not considered in the analysis as it served as adaptation to the polysomnographic devices used for objective sleep assessments [87]. To measure the short-term psychological effects of nocturnal aircraft noise, in the morning, children retrospectively rated the previous night with respect to their self-rated aircraft noise annoyance.

2.3. Acoustical Measurements

During all nights, sound pressure levels and sound files for the entire time in bed (=period between going to bed and getting up) were recorded inside the bedroom next to the children’s ear and in addition outside of the bedroom window. Recordings were performed by a Class-1 sound level meter XL2 from NTI Audio. The sound pressure levels were logged with an A-weighting and a slow-response (LAS) in the interval of one second. For subsequent noise identifying, wav-files were consistently recorded with 24 kHz sampling rate. The following acoustical parameters were determined based on the acoustical measurements inside the bedroom during each individual’s time in bed:
Number of aircraft (NAC) and number of aircraft overflights above threshold:
  • NAC: Number of aircraft in total;
  • NAT30NAT65: Number of aircraft with a maximum level ≥ 30 dB, NAT was computed in 5 dB steps.
Maximum sound pressure levels (LAmax) and statistical metrics (LX):
  • L1: Sound pressure level in dB which is exceeded during 1% of the AC time;
  • L10: Sound pressure level in dB which is exceeded during 10% of the AC time;
  • max LAmax,AC: Maximum sound pressure level of aircraft noise in dB (=maximum of the LAmax of all overflights);
  • mean LAmax,AC: Mean of the LAmax values in dB of all single overflights per time in bed.
Energy equivalent sound pressure level:
  • LAeq,AC: A-weighted energy equivalent sound pressure level related to aircraft noise.
Aircraft to background noise ratio:
  • SNR: Signal to noise ratio, ratio between LAeq,AC and the background sound pressure level, i.e., the LAeq for all sounds during bed time except aircraft sounds and noise produced by the participants themselves (e.g., coughing, snoring).
Time with aircraft noise:
  • Total AC time: Overall time in seconds influenced by aircraft noise;
Statistical parameters of the aircraft noise metrics used in the present study are shown in Table 1.

2.4. Assessment of Annoyance and Non-Acoustical Factors

All surveys were linguistically adapted to the age of the children. Mostly standardized semantic five-point scales with un-/happy smileys as graphical anchors were applied. Children received a detailed explanation of the scales and questions.
Short-term annoyance from nocturnal aircraft noise exposure was measured retrospectively in the morning, 30 min after rising time, using survey software running on a netbook (Lime-Survey, Version 2.0, [88]). Information on relevant psychological factors which may have an influence on the relationship between aircraft noise and annoyance was surveyed in computer-assisted personal interviews with the children on the first study day. Besides demographical data, relevant psychological influence variables such as children’s noise sensitivity, attitudes towards air traffic, the application of measures to cope with the noise, and residential satisfaction were inquired in the interviews. In addition, further potential influence factors of the children’s annoyance response were assessed by a parent, such as the child’s estimated capability to adapt to the aircraft noise, socioeconomic status and housing situation of the family, or the parent’s own long-term aircraft noise annoyance during the past 12 months.
Long-term and short-term annoyance was measured by a question recommended by the International Commission on Biological Effects of Noise (ICBEN) [89,90]. For the purpose of comparability, instead of the original ICBEN scale, a similar standardized verbal five-point scale [91] was used which had already been applied in a prior aircraft noise study with adults around Cologne-Bonn Airport. This scale ranged from “1 = not” to “5 = very annoyed”. The answering categories were presented to the (random) half of the children in descending order and the other half in ascending order to control the influence of the order of scale categories on response.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

Logistic regression analysis (LRA) is a standard statistical procedure in noise effects research for the derivation of exposure–response curves [83,85,92,93,94]. Thus, LRA was used to determine the probability to be (highly and moderately) annoyed by nocturnal aircraft noise exposure as a function of the different acoustical and non-acoustical variables. The LRA is similar to linear regression analysis except that the predicted variable is binary, i.e., the dependent variable can be defined as an event that either takes place (1) or does not take place (0). In the present study, each child was examined repeatedly. Hence, the random effects model accounted for the non-independency of annoyance ratings by enclosing a random subject effect through a random intercept [95].
For LRA, a binary variable was determined. The Schultz criterion [96] regards individuals, whose ratings are distributed in the upper 25% to 30% of an annoyance scale (e.g., on a five-point scale the categories 4 and 5) as “highly annoyed”. However, a restriction on the percentage of persons who are highly annoyed by noise is questionable since the (likewise quantitatively important) proportion of those, whose noise annoyance falls into the intermediate range of the answering scale, is not considered. Therefore, we determined a binary variable via summarizing the response categories 3, 4, and 5 of the original annoyance five-point scale in order to differentiate between “moderate to high annoyance” (value 1) and “no or low annoyance” (value 0) represented by categories 1 and 2.
The modelling was done on the theoretical basis according to [60] and [27], under consideration of findings of noise-induced annoyance response in adults. Accordingly, potential acoustical and non-acoustical predictors of aircraft noise-induced annoyance in children were pre-selected. In addition to the aircraft noise metrics described in Table 1, the following non-acoustical parameters were considered in the modelling:
Variables obtained from the children sample:
a.
Variables related to aircraft noise:
  • Evaluation of the residential area by means of an open question. The children’s answers were assessed by content analysis with respect to aircraft noise reference. Accordingly, answers were dichotomized (“1 = aircraft noise reference”, “0 = no reference”);
  • Perception of the sound level of aircraft in the home environment (“1 = not” to “5 = very loud”);
  • Long-term annoyance from chronic aircraft noise exposure of the previous 12 months (“1 = not” to “5 = very annoyed”);
  • Short-term annoyance from aircraft noise exposure of the previous day (“1 = not” to “5 = very annoyed”);
  • General attitude towards air traffic (“1 = very good” to “5 = very bad”);
  • Attitude towards air traffic with respect to its necessity, preventability, dan-ger/unsafety, health hazard, and fear of plane crashes (“1 = not true” to “5 = very true”);
  • Need for change in neighbourhood: “If you were a magician, what would you change in your neighbourhood, so that you and all the other children feel well here?”). Answers were dichotomized with respect to aircraft noise reference according to content analysis (“1 = aircraft noise reference”, “0 = no reference”);
  • Coping strategy in dealing with aircraft noise (e.g., closing windows or changing to a quieter room when aircraft are passing during homework is done) (“1 = never” to “5 = every day”).
b.
Personal variables:
  • Age;
  • Gender;
  • Noise sensitivity (“1 = not” to “5 = very sensitive to noise”);
  • Presence of stressful events the day before (“1 = not” to “5 = very present”);
  • Fatigue in the evening before starting the measurements (“1 = very awake” to “5 = very tired”);
  • Fatigue in the morning (“1 = very awake” to “5 = very tired”);
  • Self-rated sleep quality (“1 = very good” to “5 = very bad”);
Variables obtained from the parents of the children sample.
a.
Variables related to aircraft noise.
  • Children’s adaptation to chronic aircraft noise exposure (“1 = not” to “5 = very adapted”);
  • Parent’s long-term annoyance from chronic aircraft noise exposure of the previous 12 months (“1 = not” to “5 = very annoyed”).
b.
Sociodemographic and personal variables.
  • Housing situation (i.e., residential characterization, house type, tenant or home ownership, occupancy in months);
  • Sociodemographic status of the main breadwinner in the family as measured by the Scheuch–Winkler-Index (SWI) [97].
For each of the acoustical and non-acoustical predictors, univariate analyses were carried out first. The selection of a prediction model for short-term aircraft noise-induced annoyance was achieved on the basis of expert experience and published literature, considering formal selection procedures. The decision as to whether a predictor was included was based on the p-value and the AIC (Akaike Information Criterion), which is an indicator of the goodness of statistical models [98]. Forward selection was carried on until there was no improvement with regard to the AIC. Only variables were included that showed at least a trend for an effect on annoyance (i.e., p ≤ 0.10). All factors were also tested for interactions and multicollinearity.
Data were analyzed using SPSS 21 (IBM Statistics, Armonk, NY, USA). Final regression models are presented in tabular form.

3. Results

In the following section, the effect of nocturnal aircraft noise exposure and non-acoustical factors on short-term annoyance ratings in children are reported. First, we provided descriptive results on the nocturnal aircraft noise exposure and annoyance ratings (Section 3.1 and Section 3.2.1). In a second step, we investigated the association between a broad range of acoustical and non-acoustical variables, respectively, and the annoyance rating (Section 3.2.2). Subsequently, exposure–response models and curves were derived showing the association between the nocturnal aircraft noise exposure (described by the NAC and the LAeq,AC, respectively) and annoyance and considering the effect of relevant non-acoustical factors (Section 3.2.2). As a final step, we compared the exposure–response curves (Section 3.3.1) as well as the effect size of the predictors (Section 3.3.2) between children and adults.

3.1. Aircraft Noise Exposure during the Study Nights

Forty-eight children of the original sample were included in the data analysis. Data of three participants could not be considered due to missing data mainly in the personal interview. In total, data from 134 nights were used. Aircraft noise exposure assessed inside the bedroom at the sleeper’s ear during these study nights is summarized in Table 1. For the calculation of most of the acoustical parameters, all aircraft noise events recorded during the time in bed were considered, including those overlapping with sounds from other sources, e.g., road and rail noise events or sounds from the participant. An exception was made for the parameters L1 and L10 as well as the highest and the averaged maximum noise levels, as these four parameters describe different kinds of maximum levels of aircraft noise events and, thus, can easily be biased by other loud (indoor) ambient sounds. For the calculation of these parameters, only aircraft noise events were included that did not overlap with other ambient sounds.

3.2. Short-Term Annoyance from Nocturnal Aircraft Noise Exposure in Children

3.2.1. Descriptive Statistics

Figure 1 shows the percentage frequency distribution of the answer to the question “How much have you been disturbed or bothered by aircraft noise of the last night?” by using a five-point answering scale. As shown by Figure 1, 78.4% of the children felt “not” or “a little” (categories 1 and 2 of the original scale) annoyed. 21.6% of the ratings were in the range of “moderately and very” annoyed (categories 3 to 5).

3.2.2. Exposure–Response Models

The univariate analyses on acoustical parameters indicated that none of the aircraft noise metrics proved statistically significant (see Supplementary Table S1). Table 2 presents the results of the univariate analyses for those non-acoustical predictors that were significant at least on a trend level (p ≤ 0.10. Since the final p-values of the regression parameters may be biased due to the model selection process [99], OR and the 95% CI are also presented. The effect of a predictor is considered significant if the OR is different from 1 and the 95% CI is not covering 1.
As a first predictor in the null model, noise sensitivity was included as substantial evidence exists for its impact on transportation noise-induced annoyance [72,77]. In a second step, attitudes were regarded as potential predictor, since attitudes towards air traffic are also viewed as important determinants of noise annoyance [72,77]. Attitude variables (e.g., AC are dangerous, AC are useful, fear of plane crashes) were added one by one to the modelling. Afterwards, additional non-acoustical factors were tested for a significant contribution to annoyance and an improvement of the model fit. The best model based on AIC included the variables “noise sensitivity” and “AC are dangerous” and the application of measure to cope with the noise, short: “coping” (Table 3).
In the next step, acoustical predictors were integrated into the annoyance model LR1. Since the univariate regression analyses did not show any significant effects of the examined aircraft noise parameters, two established noise metrics were selected: the number of aircraft (NAC) and the energy equivalent sound pressure level for aircraft noise during the time in bed (LAeq,AC). However, neither of the two metrics proved significant in combination with model LR1. As shown by Table 4 and Table 5, only noise sensitivity retained its significant increasing influence on children’s short-term annoyance (LR2: OR = 2.171, 95% CI 1.142–4.127, p = 0.018; LR3: OR = 2.204, 95% CI 1.153–4.211, p = 0.017). The attitude variable “AC are dangerous” and coping behavior were significant in tendency (p ≤ 0.10) (LR2: OR = 1.721, 95% CI 0.957–3.096, p = 0.069; LR3: OR = 1.739, 95% CI 0.971–3.113, p = 0.062).
Figure 2 and Figure 3 depict the probability for annoyance in children depending on the energy equivalent noise level LAeq,AC (model LR2) and the number of nocturnal aircraft (model LR3) during time in bed, respectively. There was neither a significant increase of annoyance probability with rising LAeq,AC nor with growing number of nocturnal overflights. For both figures, the non-acoustical variables in the models were set to the median in the sample (i.e., noise sensitivity = 2, AC are dangerous = 2, coping = 1).

3.3. Comparison of Aircraft Noise-Induced Short-Term Annoyance in Children and Adults

3.3.1. Exposure–Response Models

From 2001 to 2002 the DLR carried out the field study STRAIN with 64 adult residents aged 19 to 61 years (35 female) in the surrounding of Cologne-Bonn Airport. Study areas, design and methods [85,100] corresponded to those in the present study in children. Data of the present study in children and the STRAIN study were compared via applying the exposure–response model for short-term annoyance from nocturnal aircraft noise that was found in the STRAIN study [85]. For this purpose, the two data sets were pooled. In order to make the data sets comparable, a random sample of 48 subjects was selected from the STRAIN study including only measurements from the second to the fourth night. Table 6 summarizes the regression-based comparison of short-term aircraft noise annoyance in the two studies based on the STRAIN model [85]. As evidenced by the indicator variable “study”, the two studies differed significantly with respect to short-term noise annoyance (LR4: OR = 0.117, 95% CI 0.017–0.808, p = 0.030) even when controlled for the effects of age and adaptation to aircraft noise. The exposure–response curve is depicted in Figure 4. Adaptation to chronic aircraft noise exposure was set to 4. Medians for age were 8.5 years (children) and 40 years (adults). Annoyance probability in the STRAIN study was significantly higher than in the present children’s study. For adults, but not for children, the probability to be moderately to highly annoyed increased with rising numbers of aircraft flyovers during the time in bed.

3.3.2. Comparison of the Predictor’s Effect Sizes by Using Forest Plots

To compare the influence of the predictors between children and adults, the annoyance model [85] originally developed for the STRAIN study were separately recalculated for children’s and adults’ data. Similar to the pooled model, only 48 subjects from the STRAIN study with three measurement nights were considered. None of the acoustical and non-acoustical predictors in model LR4 reached statistical significance in a children-only model. For the STRAIN data, the acoustical aircraft noise parameter NAC (OR = 1.025, 95% CI 1.005–1.045, p = 0.013,) as well as age (OR = 1.051, 95% CI 1.003–1.101, p = 0.037) were statistically significant. However, as illustrated in Figure 5, the direction of effects in children and adults, with the exception of age, was the same. With respect to the acoustical predictors, there were no marked differences between children and adults in terms of OR, but the confidence intervals were much larger in children.

4. Discussion

Studies dedicated to the short-term psychological effects of nocturnal aircraft noise exposure on children are rare. So far, there have been hardly any exposure–response models describing the relationship between aircraft noise exposure and the resulting short-term annoyance reactions in children. The present study was intended to close this research gap by means of a field study conducted around Cologne-Bonn Airport with 51 children aged 8 to 10 years. In order to rule out answer biases in the recall of noise (annoyance situations), annoyance was assessed every morning in the home environment and referring to the past night instead of simply asking for retrospective ratings for the past months. We developed new exposure–response models to predict aircraft noise-induced short-term annoyance considering a broad range of acoustical and non-acoustical factors as potential predictors of annoyance. The modelling assumed that—similar to adults—both acoustical and non-acoustical factors affect children’s annoyance. The contribution of both kinds of determinants to both short-term and long-term noise annoyance has been extensively validated in adults [13,39,71,72,73,75,76,77,80,81,83,85] whereas only few studies have been done in children [39,84].
Exposure–response models for the aircraft noise-induced short-term annoyance in children were calculated by means of random effects logistic regression considering repeated measurements for the same individual. Pre-selection of potential acoustical and non-acoustical predictors for the new children’s annoyance model was based on literature research. Univariate analyses indicated that none of the aircraft noise metrics proved statistically significant. It is possible that the current sample size (N = 51, 134 nights), which was derived from similar studies in adults, was not large enough to detect more subtle effects of aircraft noise in children. However, noise sensitivity [18,73,77,78,79] and attitude variables [13,77,80,81,82,83] were significant psychological determinants. After pre-selection, the modelling was performed by using literature-based forward selection. The best model (LR1) according to the AIC integrated the self-rated noise sensitivity, the attitude variable “AC are dangerous” and the children’s coping behavior. Integration of established aircraft noise metrics such as LAeq,AC (model LR2) and NAC (model LR3), however, did not improve the quality of the final (non-acoustical) annoyance model.
From the present results, it can be concluded that children’s short-term annoyance response to night-time aircraft noise was primarily influenced by non-acoustical variables rather than the noise exposure itself. In particular, the noise sensitivity played a central role. According to Stansfeld and Clark [43] children may be more sensitive to noise than adults because they are in an important phase of physical and mental development. Van Kamp and Davies [22] assumed that noise sensitivity-related effects (in children) may be an expression of a more general vulnerability, which could originate from psychological and/or physiological factors. Children may also cope with noise in a different (possibly less effective) way than adults [28,30,31,43,60,100]. For instance, Bartels et al. [81] found an increasing effect of coping on long-term annoyance in adult airport residents, i.e., carrying out coping measures was related to a higher aircraft noise annoyance. The range of strategies that children employ to cope with noise exposure depends on the perceived amount of control they have over the noise source. However, children are assumed to be less capable of assessing threat from environmental stressors and thus have very little control over threatening situations [60]. As a consequence, children may not yet have developed suitable coping strategies [22,43]. Thus, children may not be more vulnerable to noise effects per se, but may be more at risk due to less-developed coping strategies and susceptible developmental processes.
The present cross-sectional comparison that was based on a model originally developed by Quehl and Basner [85] in the framework of the STRAIN study revealed that annoyance probability was significantly higher in adults than in children. An exposure–response curve revealed that at lower exposure, the probability for moderate to high annoyance was similar between children and adults whilst the probability for annoyance at higher exposure was lower in children. The result is reminiscent of previous studies on long-term annoyance from chronic aircraft noise exposure [22,42,67,68,70] indicating that children were less annoyed than adults. Moreover, we found that in adults but not in children annoyance probability depended on aircraft noise metrics. Accordingly, the percentage of moderately to highly annoyed adults grew with the frequency of overflights, whereas such a relationship was absent in children. Children may have fewer experiences to compare with, whereas adults more likely have lived in different places with different degrees of noise exposure that have shaped their experience and, thus, their expectation or reference level of environmental noise exposure. Moreover, the lesser role of acoustical factors in children may be due to their lower probability to awaken from aircraft noise during the night [101]. Thus, the experience of noise-induced sleep disruption—a prominent factor in adults—may contribute less to the overall perception of aircraft noise of children; other experiences, e.g., aircraft-noise situations during the daytime, most likely have a greater influence on children’s individual attitudes and behavior and, thus, on their annoyance ratings. This assumption is supported by the significant positive associations among the short-term annoyance during the past night, the annoyance reported for the preceding day, and long-term annoyance of the past 12 months. However, a comprehensive examination of such interrelationships requires longitudinal data and, hence, cannot be examined here.
The present work shows that the extent and etiology of annoyance due to transport noise can differ substantially among subgroups of the population. The reason for the lower annoyance probability in children is not yet fully understood and deserves more research. In general, there is a need to focus on transport noise effects in vulnerable people, of which children and adolescents represent important but understudied subgroups. Although children seem to be less annoyed by aircraft noise, they still need to be considered vulnerable in that continued noise exposure may interfere with their development. Future research on transport noise will benefit from establishing subgroup-specific exposure–response relationships for various domains of health, hence enabling to derive adequate noise protection for those that are vulnerable.

5. Conclusions

In contrast to adults, children’s short-term annoyance response to aircraft noise exposure during the previous night is not determined by acoustical parameters primarily. Personal factors play a more important role, including noise sensitivity, the use of coping measures, the attitude that aircraft are dangerous, as well as the capability to adapt to the noise. Annoyance ratings of children and adults differ depending on the level of noise exposure (represented by the number of aircraft during the time in bed); whereas, at lower exposure the probability of moderate to high annoyance is similar for the two groups, and children show a lower probability than adults at higher exposure. With regard to short-term annoyance due to aircraft noise, children do not seem to be a group at higher risk, but this must not be generalized to other noise-related health effects.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ijerph18136959/s1, Table S1: Univariate logistic regression analyses with random effects of several acoustical predictors for aircraft noise-induced short-term annoyance at night in children. Odds Ratios (OR) with 95% Confidence Intervals (CI) and p-values. (N = 134 nights, 48 subjects).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.Q., S.B., and D.A.; methodology, J.Q., S.B., and D.A.; formal analysis, J.Q., S.B., and R.F.; investigation, J.Q. and S.B., writing—Original draft preparation, J.Q.; writing—Review and editing, S.B. and D.A.; supervision, D.A. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of Medical Association North Rhine (protocol number: 2015108, date of approval: 10 June 2015).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the colleagues of the DLR Institute of Aerospace Medicine, Department of Sleep and Human Factors Research and all student assistants, very much for their excel-lent work and their tremendous support in the implementation of this challenging study. Special thanks go to Uwe Müller (DLR) for his effort in the noise calculations and Matthias Putzke (currently Maria Hilf Klinikum, Bad Neuenahr-Ahrweiler) for his support in preparation of the survey software. Moreover, we would like to thank Moritz Berger (IMBIE) for his valuable advice data preparation and analysis. Finally, the authors thank all participating families for their willingness to contribute to our research. This study was part of the project MIDAS and funded by the DLR Aeronautics Program.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. EEA. Good Practice Guide on Noise Exposure and Potential Health Effects; European Environment Agency: Luxembourg, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  2. WHO. Guidelines for Community Noise; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  3. Guski, R. Lärm—Wirkungen Unerwünschter Geräusche; Hans Huber Bern: Bern, Switzerland, 1987. [Google Scholar]
  4. Babisch, W.; Swart, W.; Houthuijs, D.; Selander, J.; Bluhm, G.; Pershagen, G.; Dimakopoulou, K.; Haralabidis, A.S.; Katsouyanni, K.; Davou, E.; et al. Exposure modifiers of the relationships of transportation noise with high blood pressure and noise annoyance. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2012, 132, 3788–3808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Basner, M.; Babisch, W.; Davis, A.; Brink, M.; Clark, C.; Janssen, S.; Stansfeld, S. Auditory and non-auditory effects of noise on health. Lancet 2014, 383, 1325–1332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Jarup, L.; Babisch, W.; Houthuijs, D.; Pershagen, G.; Katsouyanni, K.; Cadum, E.; Dudley, M.L.; Savigny, P.; Seiffert, I.; Swart, W.; et al. Hypertension and exposure to noise near airports: The HYENA study. Environ. Health Perspect 2008, 116, 329–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Liu, C.; Fuertes, E.; Tiesler, C.M.T.; Birk, M.; Babisch, W.; Bauer, C.P.; Koletzko, S.; Heinrich, J. The association between road traffic noise exposure and blood pressure among children in Germany: The GINIplus and LISAplus studies. Noise Health 2013, 15, 165–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Van Kempen, E.; Babisch, W. The quantitative relationship between road traffic noise and hypertension: A meta-analysis. J. Hypertens. 2012, 30, 1075–1086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. EEA. Noise in Europe 2014; European Environment Agency: Luxembourg, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  10. Orban, E.; McDonald, K.; Sutcliffe, R.; Hoffmann, B.; Fuks, K.B.; Dragano, N.; Viehmann, A.; Erbel, R.; Jockel, K.H.; Pundt, N.; et al. Residential road traffic noise and high depressive symptoms after five years of follow-up: Results from the Heinz Nixdorf recall study. Environ. Health Perspect 2016, 124, 578–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  11. Schreckenberg, D.; Moehler, U.; Liepert, M. Gilt der Schienenbonus auch während der Abendzeit? Lärmbekämpfung 2010, 5, 57–63. [Google Scholar]
  12. Stansfeld, S.A.; Haines, M.M.; Burr, M.; Berry, B.; Lercher, P. A review of environmental noise and mental health. Noise Health 2000, 2, 1–8. [Google Scholar]
  13. Janssen, S.A.; Vos, H.; van Kempen, E.E.; Breugelmans, O.R.; Miedema, H.M. Trends in aircraft noise annoyance: The role of study and sample characteristics. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2011, 129, 1953–1962. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Weidenfeld, S.; Sanok, S.; Fimmers, R.; Puth, M.-T.; Aeschbach, D.; Elmenhorst, E.-M. Short-Term Annoyance Due to Night-Time Road, Railway, and Air Traffic Noise: Role of the Noise Source, the Acoustical Metric, and Non-Acoustical Factors. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 4647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Clark, C.; Stansfeld, S.A. The effect of transportation noise on health and cognitive development: A review of recent evidence. Int. J. Comp. Psychol 2007, 20, 145–158. [Google Scholar]
  16. Basner, M.; Griefahn, B.; Berg, M. Aircraft noise effects on sleep: Mechanisms, mitigation and research needs. Noise Health 2010, 12, 95–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Elmenhorst, E.M.; Pennig, S.; Rolny, V.; Quehl, J.; Müller, U.; Maaß, H.; Basner, M. Examining nocturnal Railway Noise and Aircraft Noise in the Field: Sleep, Psychomotor Performance, and Annoyance. Sci. Total Environ. 2012, 424, 48–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Miedema, H.M.E.; Vos, H. Noise sensitivity and reactions to noise and other environmental conditions. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2003, 113, 1492–1504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Griefahn, B. Sleep Disturbance Related to Environmental Noise. Noise Health 2002, 4, 57–60. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  20. Michaud, D.S.; Fidell, S.; Pearsons, K.; Campbell, K.C.; Keith, S.E. Review of field studies of aircraft noise-induced sleep disturbance. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2007, 121, 32–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. WHO. Burden of Disease from Environmental Noise: Quantification of Healthy Years Life Lost in Europe; World Health Organisation: Copenhagen, Denmark, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  22. Van Kamp, I.; Davies, H. Noise and health in vulnerable groups: A review. Noise Health 2013, 15, 153–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Evans, G.W.; Hygge, S. Noise and performance in adults and children. In Noise and Its Effects; Prasher, L., Ed.; Whurr Publishers: London, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  24. Evans, G.W.; Hygge, S.; Bullinger, M. Chronic noise and psychological stress. Psychol. Sci. 1995, 6, 333–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Evans, G.W.; Lepore, S. Non-auditory effects of noise on children: A critical review. Child. Environ. 1993, 10, 42–72. [Google Scholar]
  26. Evans, G.W.; Maxwell, L. Chronic noise exposure and reading deficits: The mediating effects of language acquisition. Environ. Behav. 1997, 29, 638–656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Haines, M.M.; Brentnall, S.; Stansfeld, S.A.; Klineberg, E. Qualitative response of children to environmental noise. Noise Health 2003, 5, 19–30. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  28. Haines, M.M.; Stansfeld, S.A.; Brentnall, S.; Head, J.; Berry, B.; Jiggins, M.; Hygge, S. The West London Schools Study: The effects of chronic aircraft noise exposure on child health. Psychol. Med. 2001, 31, 1385–1396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Haines, M.M.; Stansfeld, S.A.; Head, J.; Job, R.F. Multilevel modelling of aircraft noise on performance tests in schools around Heathrow Airport London. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2002, 56, 139–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Haines, M.M.; Stansfeld, S.A.; Job, R.F.; Berglund, B.; Head, J. Chronic aircraft noise exposure, stress responses, mental health and cognitive performance in school children. Psychol. Med 2001, 31, 265–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Haines, M.M.; Stansfeld, S.A.; Job, R.F.; Berglund, B.; Head, J. A follow-up study of effects of chronic aircraft noise exposure on child stress responses and cognition. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2001, 30, 839–845. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Hygge, S. Noise Exposure and Cognitive Performance—Children and the Elderly as Possible Risk Groups; WHO: Brussels, Belgium, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  33. Hygge, S.; Evans, G.W.; Bullinger, M. The Munich airport noise study—Effects of chronic airport noise on children‘s perception and cognition. In Proceedings of the Internoise 2000, Nice, France, 27–30 August 2000. [Google Scholar]
  34. Hygge, S.; Evans, G.W.; Bullinger, M. A prospective study of some effects of aircraft noise on cognitive performance in schoolchildren. Psychol. Sci. 2002, 13, 469–474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Lercher, P.; Evans, G.W.; Meis, M. Ambient noise and cognitive processes among primary school-children. Environ. Behav. 2003, 35, 725–735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Van Kempen, E.; van Kamp, I.; Lebret, E.; Lammers, J.; Emmen, H.; Stansfeld, S. Neurobehavioral effects of transportation noise in primary schoolchildren: A cross-sectional study. Environ. Health 2010, 9, 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Cohen, S.; Evans, G.W.; Krantz, D.S.; Stokols, D. Physiological, motivational, and cognitive effects of aircraft noise on children: Moving from the laboratory to the field. Am. Psychol 1980, 35, 231–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Evans, G.W.; Stecker, R. Motivational consequences of environmental stress. J. Environ. Psychol. 2004, 24, 143–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Grelat, N.; Houot, H.; Pujol; S.; Levain, J.P.; Defrance, J.; Mauny, F. Noise annoyance in urban children: A cross-sectional population-based study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13, 1056. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  40. Hjortebjerg, D.; Andersen, A.M.N.; Christensen, J.S.; Ketzel, M.; Raaschou-Nielsen, O.; Sunyer, J.; Julvez, J.; Forns, J.; Søren-sen, M. Exposure to road traffic noise and behavioral problems in 7-year-old children: A cohort study. Environ. Health Perspect 2015, 124, 228–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Paunovic, K. Noise and children‘s health: Research in central, eastern and south-eastern Europe and newly independent states. Noise Health 2013, 15, 32–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Stansfeld, S.A.; Berglund, B.; Clark, C.; Lopez-Barrio, I.; Fischer, P.; Ohrstrom, E.; Haines, M.M.; Head, J.; Hygge, S.; van, K., I; et al. Aircraft and road traffic noise and children’s cognition and health: A cross-national study. Lancet 2005, 365, 1942–1949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Stansfeld, S.A.; Clark, C. Health effects of noise exposure in children. Curr. Environ. Health Rep. 2015, 2, 171–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Van Kempen, E.; van Kamp, I.; Fischer, P.; Davies, H.; Houthuijs, D.; Stellato, R. Noise exposure and children’s blood pressure and heart rate: The RANCH project. Occup. Environ. Med. 2006, 63, 632–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Eberhardt, J.L. The influence of road traffic noise on sleep. J. Sound Vib. 1988, 127, 449–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Eberhardt, J.L. The disturbance by road traffic noise of the sleep of prepubertal children as studied in the home. In Proceedings of the 5th International Congress on Noise as a Public Health Problem, Stockholm, Sweden, 21–25August 1988; pp. 65–74. [Google Scholar]
  47. Van Kamp, I.; Persson Waye, K.; Gidlöf-Gunnarsson, A. The effects of noise disturbed sleep in children on cognitive development and long term health. J. Child. Adolesc. Behav. 2015, 3, 179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Basner, M.; McGuire, S. WHO environmental noise guidelines for the European region: A systematic review on environmental noise and effects on sleep. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  49. Hume, K.I. Research over the last and next five years. In Proceedings of the ICBEN, Foxwoods, CT, USA, 21–25 July 2008. [Google Scholar]
  50. Pierra, S.; De Valck, E.; Cluydts, R. Nocturnal road traffic noise: A review on its assessment and consequences on sleep and health. Environ Int. 2010, 36, 492–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Weyde, K.V.; Krog, N.H.; Ofteda, l.B.; Evandt, J.; Magnus, P.; Øverland, S.; Clark, C.; Stansfeld, S.; Aasvang, G.M. Nocturnal road traffic noise exposure and children’s sleep duration and sleep problems. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Babisch, W. Stress Hormones in the Research on Cardiovascular Effects of Noise. Noise Health 2003, 5, 1–11. [Google Scholar]
  53. Babisch, W. Kinder-Umwelt-Survey (KUS) 2003/06, Lärm, Daten und Materialiensammlung, Deskription und Zusammenhangsanalysen; Umwelt & Gesundheit 01/2009; Umwelt & Gesundheit: Ostrava, Czech Republic, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  54. Evans, G.W.; Lercher, P.; Meis, M.; Ising, H.; Kofler, W.W. Community noise exposure and stress in children. J. Acoust. Soc. Am 2001, 109, 1023–1027. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  55. Ising, H.; Ising, M. Chronic cortisol increases in the first half of the night caused by road traffic noise. Noise Health 2002, 4, 13–20. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  56. Lazarus, R.S.; Folkman, S. Stress, Appraisal, and Coping; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1984. [Google Scholar]
  57. Ising, H.; Lange-Asschenfeldt, H.; Lieber, G.F.; Weinhold, H.; Eilts, M. Auswirkungen langfristiger Expositionen gegenüber Straßenverkehrsimissionen auf die Entwicklung von Haut- und Atemwegserkrankungen bei Kindern. Bundesgesundheitsblatt Gesundheitsforschung Gesundheitsschutz 2002, 45, 807–814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Ising, H.; Pleines, F.; Meis, M. Beeinflussung der Lebensqualität von Kindern Durch Militärischen Fluglärm; UBA-Forschungsbericht 98—506 01 003; Umweltbundesamt: Dessau-Roßlau, Germany, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  59. Cohen, S.; Evans, G.W.; Stokols, D.; Krantz, D.S. Behavior, Health and Environmental Stress; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
  60. Bruni, L.; Novelli, R.; Ferri, R. Sleep disturbance in children by noise. In Encyclopedia of Environmental Health; Elsevier Science and Technology: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2011; pp. 88–94. [Google Scholar]
  61. Hellbrück, J.; Guski, R. Lauter Schall: Wie Lärm in Unser Leben Eingreift; Wissenschaftliche Buchgesellschaft: Darmstadt, Germany, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  62. Guski, R.; Schreckenberg, D.; Schuemer, R. The WHO evidence review on noise annoyance 2000-2014. In Proceedings of the 45th International Congress and Exposition on Noise Control Engineering (Internoise 2016), Hamburg, Germany, 21–24 August 2016; pp. 2564–2570. [Google Scholar]
  63. Guski, R. Personal and social variables as co-determinants of noise annoyance. Noise Health 1999, 1, 45–56. [Google Scholar]
  64. Halperin, D. Environmental noise and sleep disturbances: A threat to health? Sleep Sci. 2014, 7, 209–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Muzet, A. The Need for a Specific Noise Measurement for Population Exposed to Aircraft Noise during Night-time. Noise Health 2002, 4, 61–64. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  66. WHO. Night Noise Guidelines for Europe; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  67. Clark, C.; Martin, R.; van Kempen, E.; Alfred, T.; Head, J.; Davies, H.W.; Haines, M.M.; Lopez, B., I; Matheson, M.; Stansfeld, S.A. Exposure-effect relations between aircraft and road traffic noise exposure at school and reading comprehension: The RANCH project. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2006, 163, 27–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Stansfeld, S.A.; Clark, C.; Cameron, R.M.; Alfred, T.; Head, J.; Haines, M.M. Aircraft and road traffic noise exposure and children’s mental health. J. Environ. Psychol. 2009, 29, 203–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Stansfeld, S.A.; Hygge, S.; Clark, C. Night time aircraft noise exposure and children’s cognitive performance. Noise Health 2010, 12, 255–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Van Kempen, E.; van Kamp, I.; Stellato, R.K.; Lopez-Barrio, I.; Haines, M.M.; Nilsson, M.E.; Clark, C.; Houthuijs, D.; Brune-kreef, B.; Berglund, B.; et al. Children‘s annoyance reactions to aircraft and road traffic noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2009, 125, 895–904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  71. Brink, M. A review of explained variance in exposure-annoyance relationships in noise annoyance surveys. In Proceedings of the 11th International Congress on Noise as a Public Health Problem (ICBEN 2014), Nara, Japan, 1–5 June 2014. [Google Scholar]
  72. Fields, J.M. Effect of personal and situational variables on noise annoyance in residential areas. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1993, 93, 2753–2763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Gille, L.A.; Marquis-Favre, C.; Morel, J. Testing of the European Union exposure-response relationships and annoyance equivalents model for annoyance due to transportation noises: The need of revised exposure-response relationships and annoyance equivalents model. Environ. Int. 2016, 94, 83–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Guski, R. Der Referenten-Entwurf zum Fluglärmgesetz aus der Sicht eines Lärmwirkungsforschers. Zeitschrift für Lärmbekämpfung 2001, 48, 130–131. [Google Scholar]
  75. Job, R.F.S. Community response to noise: A review of factors influencing the relationship between noise exposure and reaction. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1988, 83, 991–1001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Miedema, H.M.E.; Oudshoorn, C.G.M. Annoyance from transportation noise: Relationships with exposure metrics DNL and DENL and their confidence intervals. Environ. Health Perspect. 2001, 109, 409–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Miedema, H.M.E.; Vos, H. Demographic and attitudinal factors that modify annoyance from transportation noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1999, 105, 3336–3344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Job, R.F.S. Noise sensitivity as a factor influencing human reaction to noise. Noise Health 1999, 1, 57–68. [Google Scholar]
  79. Weinstein, N.D. Individual differences in reactions to noise: A longitudinal study in a college dormitory. J. Appl. Psychol 1978, 63, 458–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Bartels, S.; Marki, F.; Muller, U. The influence of acoustical and non-acoustical factors on short-term annoyance due to aircraft noise in the field - The COSMA study. Sci. Total Environ. 2015, 538, 834–843. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Bartels, S.; Rooney, D.; Müller, U. Assessing aircraft noise-induced annoyance around a major German airport and its predictors via telephone survey – The COSMA study. Transp. Res. Part D Transp. Environ. 2018, 59, 246–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Kroesen, M.; Molin, E.J.E.; van Wee, B. Testing a theory of aircraft noise annoyance: A structural equation analysis. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2008, 123, 4250–4260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  83. Quehl, J.; Müller, U.; Mendolia, F. Short-term annoyance from nocturnal aircraft noise exposure: Results of the NORAH and STRAIN sleep studies. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 2017, 90, 765–778. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Lercher, P.; Brauchle, G.; Kofler, W.; Widmann, U.; Meis, M. The assessment of noise annoyance in schoolchildren and their mothers. In Proceedings of the 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering, Nice, France, 27–30 August 2000. [Google Scholar]
  85. Quehl, J.; Basner, M. Annoyance from nocturnal aircraft noise exposure: Laboratory and field-specific dose-response curves. J. Environ. Psychol. 2006, 26, 127–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Kast-Zahn, A.; Morgenroth, H. Jedes Kind Kann Schlafen Lernen; GU: München, Germany, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  87. Bartels, S.; Quehl, J.; Aeschbach, D. Effects of nocturnal aircraft noise on objective and subjective sleep quality in primary school children. In Proceedings of the 23rd International Congress on Acoustics, Aachen, Germany, 9–13 September 2019. [Google Scholar]
  88. LimeSurvey. Available online: https://www.limesurvey.org/de/ (accessed on 28 June 2021).
  89. Felscher-Suhr, U.; Guski, R.; Schuemer, R. Internationale Standardisierungsbestrebungen zur Erhebung von Lärmbelästigung. Zeitschrift für Lärmbekämpfung 2000, 2, 68–70. [Google Scholar]
  90. Fields, J.M.; de Jong, R.G.; Gjestland, T.; Flindell, I.; Job, R.F.S.; Kurra, L.; Lercher, P.; Vallet, M.; Yano, T.; Guski, R.; et al. Standardized general-purpose noise reaction questions for community noise surveys: Research and a recommendation—Community response to noise team of ICBEN. J. Sound Vib. 2001, 242, 641–679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  91. Rohrmann, B. Empirische Studien zur Entwicklung von Antwortskalen für die sozialwissenschaftliche Forschung. Z. Soz. 1978, 9, 222–245. [Google Scholar]
  92. Brink, M.; Wirth, K.E.; Schierz, C. Annoyance responses to stable and changing aircraft noise exposure. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2008, 124, 2930–2941. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  93. Keith, W.D.; Valente, D.; Nykaza, E.T.; Pettit, C.L. Information-criterion based selection of models for community noise annoyance. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2013, 133, EL195–EL201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  94. Sung, J.H.; Lee, J.; Park, S.J.; Sim, C.S. Relationship of Transportation Noise and Annoyance for Two Metropolitan Cities in Korea: Population Based Study. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0169035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Diggle, P.J.; Heagerty, P.; Liang, K.Y.; Zeger, S.L. Analysis of Longitudinal Data; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  96. Schultz, T.J. Synthesis of social surveys on noise annoyance. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1978, 64, 377–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  97. Winkler, J. Die Messung des sozialen Status mit Hilfe eines Index in den Gesundheitssurveys der DHP. RKI Schriften 1998, 1, 69. [Google Scholar]
  98. Pinheiro, J.; Bates, D. Mixed-Effects Models in S and S-Plus; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  99. Harrell, F.E. Regression Modeling Strategies: With Applications to Linear Models, Logistic and Ordinal Regression, and Survival Analysis; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  100. Basner, M.; Isermann, U.; Samel, A. Aircraft noise effects on sleep: Application of the results of a large polysomnographic field study. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2006, 119, 2772–2784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Bartels, S.; Quehl, J.; Fimmers, R.; Aeschbach, D. Exposure response-relationships between nocturnal aircraft noise and sleep disturbances in primary school children. In Proceedings of the 13th ICBEN Congress on Noise as a Public Health Problem, Stockholm, Sweden, 14–17 June 2021. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Percentage distribution of aircraft noise-induced short-term annoyance due to exposure of the previous night (N = 48 subjects, 134 nights).
Figure 1. Percentage distribution of aircraft noise-induced short-term annoyance due to exposure of the previous night (N = 48 subjects, 134 nights).
Ijerph 18 06959 g001
Figure 2. Probability for annoyance (categories ≥ 3) by aircraft noise of the previous night as predicted by model LR2 depending on the energy equivalent noise level for aircraft noise (LAeq,AC) during time in bed. The hatched area shows the 95% CI.
Figure 2. Probability for annoyance (categories ≥ 3) by aircraft noise of the previous night as predicted by model LR2 depending on the energy equivalent noise level for aircraft noise (LAeq,AC) during time in bed. The hatched area shows the 95% CI.
Ijerph 18 06959 g002
Figure 3. Probability for annoyance (categories ≥ 3) by aircraft noise of the previous night as predicted by model LR3 depending on the number of aircraft (NAC) during time in bed. The hatched area shows the 95% CI.
Figure 3. Probability for annoyance (categories ≥ 3) by aircraft noise of the previous night as predicted by model LR3 depending on the number of aircraft (NAC) during time in bed. The hatched area shows the 95% CI.
Ijerph 18 06959 g003
Figure 4. Probability for annoyance (categories ≥ 3) by aircraft noise of the previous night as predicted by model LR4 depending on NAC with corresponding 95% CI (hatched area).
Figure 4. Probability for annoyance (categories ≥ 3) by aircraft noise of the previous night as predicted by model LR4 depending on NAC with corresponding 95% CI (hatched area).
Ijerph 18 06959 g004
Figure 5. Forest plot depicting the OR and 95% CI for children (N = 48, red) and adults from the STRAIN study (N = 48, blue) for the acoustical and non-acoustical predictors of annoyance in model LR4.
Figure 5. Forest plot depicting the OR and 95% CI for children (N = 48, red) and adults from the STRAIN study (N = 48, blue) for the acoustical and non-acoustical predictors of annoyance in model LR4.
Ijerph 18 06959 g005
Table 1. Aircraft noise parameters averaged across study nights and subjects (N = 48) based on the indoor measurements related to the individual bed times. Mean values (M) and standard deviation (SD) were calculated across study nights and across subjects.
Table 1. Aircraft noise parameters averaged across study nights and subjects (N = 48) based on the indoor measurements related to the individual bed times. Mean values (M) and standard deviation (SD) were calculated across study nights and across subjects.
ParameterMinimumMaximumMSD
NAC4.0152.071.525.1
NAT300.0125.056.627.9
NAT350.0119.043.929.3
NAT400.0114.028.327.2
NAT450.0113.016.523.7
NAT500.0106.08.418.5
NAT550.090.03.411.5
NAT600.020.00.62.5
NAT650.04.00.10.4
L118.056.435.27.7
L1017.538.523.04.5
max LAmax,AC28.771.147.47.9
mean LAmax,AC24.458.939.47.2
LAeq,AC18.543.326.35.2
SNR0.491.00.90.8
Total AC time [s]195.910,537.74841.72045.7
All acoustical parameters were calculated for the time in bed (=the period between going to bed and getting up).
Table 2. Univariate logistic regression analyses with random effects of the non-acoustical predictors for aircraft noise-induced short-term annoyance at night in children. Odds Ratios (OR) with 95% Confidence Intervals (CI) and p-values. Illustrated predictor variables were measured using five-point scales.
Table 2. Univariate logistic regression analyses with random effects of the non-acoustical predictors for aircraft noise-induced short-term annoyance at night in children. Odds Ratios (OR) with 95% Confidence Intervals (CI) and p-values. Illustrated predictor variables were measured using five-point scales.
ORCIp-Value
Annoyance from aircraft noise exposure during daytime of the previous day2.8381.622–4.9650.000
Self-rated sleep quality2.2671.385–3.7120.001
Long-term annoyance from chronic aircraft noise exposure of the previous 12 months2.4561.360–4.4360.003
Noise sensitivity assessed by the child2.3561.328–4.1780.004
AC are dangerous1.8801.108–3.1910.020
Fatigue in the morning1.7651.071–2.9100.026
Coping1.5561.044–2.3180.030
Fear of plane crashes1.7261.075–2.7690.024
AC are useful0.6340.402–1.0000.050
Children’s adaptation to chronic aircraft noise exposure assessed by parents0.5510.315–0.9620.036
Table 3. Multiple logistic regression model LR1 with random effects for the prediction of aircraft noise-induced short-term annoyance at night in children. Adjusted Odds Ratios (OR) with 95% Confidence Intervals (CI) and p-values, AIC = 652.
Table 3. Multiple logistic regression model LR1 with random effects for the prediction of aircraft noise-induced short-term annoyance at night in children. Adjusted Odds Ratios (OR) with 95% Confidence Intervals (CI) and p-values, AIC = 652.
EstimateStandard ErrorORCIp-Value
Intercept−5.4371.2570.0040.000–0.0520.000
Noise sensitivity0.7410.3132.0991.131–3.8970.019
AC dangerous0.5410.2911.7180.965–3.0580.066
Coping0.3630.2151.4380.939–2.2020.094
Table 4. Multiple logistic regression model LR2 with random effects for the prediction of aircraft noise-induced short-term annoyance of children. Acoustical variable: LAeq,AC during time in bed. Adjusted Odds Ratios (OR) with 95% Confidence Intervals (CI) and p-values, AIC = 659.
Table 4. Multiple logistic regression model LR2 with random effects for the prediction of aircraft noise-induced short-term annoyance of children. Acoustical variable: LAeq,AC during time in bed. Adjusted Odds Ratios (OR) with 95% Confidence Intervals (CI) and p-values, AIC = 659.
EstimateStandard ErrorORCIp-Value
Intercept−6.2392.0370.0020.000–0.1100.003
LAeq,AC0.0260.0521.0260.926–1.1380.617
Noise sensitivity0.7750.3252.1711.142–4.1270.018
AC dangerous0.5430.2971.7210.957–3.0960.069
Coping0.3700.2191.4480.939–2.2340.093
Table 5. Multiple logistic regression model LR3 with random effects for the prediction of aircraft noise-induced short-term annoyance at night of children. Acoustical variable: NAC during time in bed. Adjusted Odds Ratios (OR) with 95% Confidence Intervals (CI) and p-values, AIC = 661.
Table 5. Multiple logistic regression model LR3 with random effects for the prediction of aircraft noise-induced short-term annoyance at night of children. Acoustical variable: NAC during time in bed. Adjusted Odds Ratios (OR) with 95% Confidence Intervals (CI) and p-values, AIC = 661.
EstimateStandard ErrorORCIp-Value
Intercept−5.9211.5430.0030.000–0.0570.000
NAC0.0050.0091.0050.987–1.0230.586
Noise sensitivity0.7900.3272.2041.153–4.2110.017
AC dangerous0.5530.2941.7390.971–3.1130.062
Coping0.3500.2201.4190.919–2.1920.114
Table 6. Logistic regression model LR4 with random effects for the prediction of aircraft noise-induced short-term annoyance at night measured in the children’s and STRAIN (adults’) studies, with NAC during time in bed, age and the adaptation to aircraft noise as independent variables (AIC = 1349).
Table 6. Logistic regression model LR4 with random effects for the prediction of aircraft noise-induced short-term annoyance at night measured in the children’s and STRAIN (adults’) studies, with NAC during time in bed, age and the adaptation to aircraft noise as independent variables (AIC = 1349).
EstimateStandard ErrorORCIp-Value
Intercept−0.7080.8210.4930.098–2.4790.389
NAC0.0100.0061.0100.998–1.0220.091
Age0.0430.0241.0440.993–1.0980.089
Adaptation to aircraft noise−0.5090.1910.6010.412–0.8770.009
Study 1−2.1460.8920.1170.017–0.8080.030
1 Reference: Adult sample of the STRAIN study.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Quehl, J.; Bartels, S.; Fimmers, R.; Aeschbach, D. Effects of Nocturnal Aircraft Noise and Non-Acoustical Factors on Short-Term Annoyance in Primary School Children. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 6959. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18136959

AMA Style

Quehl J, Bartels S, Fimmers R, Aeschbach D. Effects of Nocturnal Aircraft Noise and Non-Acoustical Factors on Short-Term Annoyance in Primary School Children. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(13):6959. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18136959

Chicago/Turabian Style

Quehl, Julia, Susanne Bartels, Rolf Fimmers, and Daniel Aeschbach. 2021. "Effects of Nocturnal Aircraft Noise and Non-Acoustical Factors on Short-Term Annoyance in Primary School Children" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 13: 6959. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18136959

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop