Next Article in Journal
Exploring High vs. Low Burnout amongst Public Sector Educators: COVID-19 Antecedents and Profiles
Next Article in Special Issue
Picturing Mental Health on Instagram: Insights from a Quantitative Study Using Different Content Formats
Previous Article in Journal
The Health and Environmental Impact of Plastic Waste Disposal in South African Townships: A Review
Previous Article in Special Issue
Development of a Scale Measuring Emotional Catharsis through Illness Narratives
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Positive Adult Education, Learned Helplessness and the Pygmalion Effect

by
David Cobos-Sanchiz
1,
Manuel-Jesús Perea-Rodriguez
2,*,
Juan-Agustín Morón-Marchena
1 and
María-Carmen Muñoz-Díaz
1
1
Department of Education and Social Psychology, Universidad Pablo de Olavide, 41013 Seville, Spain
2
Department Lifelong and Adult Education, Universidad Popular Dos Hermanas, 41700 Seville, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(2), 778; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19020778
Submission received: 26 November 2021 / Revised: 7 January 2022 / Accepted: 8 January 2022 / Published: 11 January 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Positive Education and Mental Health)

Abstract

:
Positive education is seen as a transformative methodological approach capable of improving the act of teaching and learning and, above all, essential for the development of students’ personal skills and competences. However, few studies have been carried out on this topic in the field of adult and continuing education; instead, they have been published mainly in the field of formal education and at school age. This study works with a sample of 399 people over 16 years of age and students of the Universidad Popular de Dos Hermanas in order to show the relationship between the Pygmalion effect and learned helplessness in the process of acquiring knowledge in adulthood. In this way, three tools were used: one questionnaire that showed the teachers’ perceptions of the students’ qualities and behaviour and two that provided information on self-concept, self-esteem, personal and social skills and other variables directly related to emotional intelligence and positive education. It shows how exposure to negative operational constraints hinders the psychosocial and socio-educational development of learners in all possible ways, while, on the other hand, it indicates the importance of positive education to compensate for this phenomenon by enhancing the development and growth of those who study and participate in non-formal education through positive reinforcement. Likewise, the factorial interrelation of both positive and negative conditioning factors and their incidence on learning is shown; the importance of neutralising the negative components and strengthening the positive reinforcement and the role played by the community and education professionals as catalysts and behavioural modulators at any stage of learning and age group for the achievement of the objectives of the student and of education itself in a broad sense.

1. Introduction

The aim of this study is to provide keys for the improvement of the teaching–learning process, both in terms of the competences included in each of the modules and in terms of personal and social skills, based on two basic ideas: learned helplessness and the Pygmalion effect. Both are juxtaposed situations that interfere in the results of the learner and from which education professionals can establish mechanisms for educational improvement.
With this, the aim is to show the value of positive education as a guiding and transforming element, not only in the teaching–learning process, but also in the quality of personal and community life, as well as in social cohesion. That is, taking into account the evidence of how emotional intelligence and personal and social skills training in turn have a positive impact on the social climate, the classroom, interpersonal relationships, inclusion, motivation, prevention of disruptive behaviour and attitudes, etc. [1,2,3,4,5]. In addition, following the postulates of operant conditioning which holds that behaviour can be modified by reinforcement determining their effects on learning and response behaviour [6]; we find the basis for a methodological model that allows the modulation of those elements involved in the cognitive and academic processes that are affected by internal [7] and external variables [8,9] favouring or neutralising personal growth. This shows the symbiotic relationship between emotions and the teaching–learning process [10].
In the 1960s, [11] Seligman coined the term learned helplessness to show how the use of negative reinforcement was key to overriding the capacity for reaction and behavioural shaping in animals, which was later tested in humans [12,13]. In this way, it was observed that those who had been exposed to aversive situations tended towards avoidance and ultimately to the belief that no matter how they operated, no positive outcome would be obtained. Thus, it is a model of uncontrollable stress that produces emotional distress [14,15], and as a coping deficit with characteristics of depression [14,16] in the face of aversive but avoidable situations [17].
Its conceptualisation has evolved over the years, resulting in several theses over time. In its beginnings, this theory was used to expose both animal behaviour [18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25] and human behaviour in learning and coping processes, being part of an experiment in behavioural situations, passing to the present day as a model that generates many other theories in areas such as education or health and related to psychological problems in human beings.
For [26] Peterson, Maier and Seligman, the theory of learned helplessness constitutes three essential elements: contingency, cognition and behaviour.
-
Contingency: is the objective correspondence between an individual’s action and the outcomes derived from experience. Contingency can be described in two constructs: controllability and lack of control.
-
Cognition: is understood as the way in which a person perceives, explains and extrapolates contingency [27]. The process of cognition is composed of several stages in which the person first understands the contingency.
-
Behaviour: the observable consequences of non-contingency and the individual’s cognitions about it [27].
Considering this, by definition, learned helplessness studies measure the passivity of an individual’s activity in a situation different from the one in which uncontrollability was first encountered.
On the other hand, this model argues that there are other consequences of a person’s expectations of future helplessness: cognitive retardation, low self-esteem, loss of aggression, immunological changes and physical illness [26].
Its opposite is the Pygmalion effect, based on the Pygmalion or fulfilled prophecy myth, taken by cognitivism to explore how positive reinforcement and realistically displayed expectations of success influence the learner to achieve success, thereby improving outcomes. From the above, we can see the importance of any type of conditioning and emotional learning for the development of the person, taking into account the transversal axis: motivation–cognition–emotion, which in turn connects with brain dysfunctions caused by the stimulus.
This approach received impetus from the experiment conducted by [28] Robert Rosenthal and Lenore Jacobson, where the expectation of an experimenter strongly influenced the way subjects behaved in different situations.
For [29] Rosenthal, there are three factors that influence the Pygmalion effect:
-
Climate: a warmer climate is created for those students from whom more is expected.
-
Input: teachers teach more things to students from whom they expect more.
-
Feed-back: the more is expected from students, the more praise and positive reinforcement they receive: there is a direct relationship between more praise and positive reinforcement.
Sánchez Hernández and López Fernández [30] define it as follows: the Pygmalion effect requires three aspects: firmly believing in a fact, having the expectation that it will be fulfilled and accompanying it with messages that encourage its achievement.
We know the Pygmalion effect as the prophecy of self-realisation, which can have both an external and an internal origin. Success, with respect to this subject, lies in the capacity that both subjects, as well as the people around them, have to be able to create positive expectations, firmly believing in them and transmitting them with the same intensity, turning them into a powerful stimulus.
Therefore, and on the basis of positive education in which the present analysis is embedded, we show how to approach the teaching–learning act with adults, taking into account the different conditioning models that the learner brings internalised, as well as the elements contemplated in the model of Goleman, Mayer et al. [31] such as: emotional intelligence, self-esteem, resilience, self-efficacy and empathic attitudes; and on the other hand, symptoms such as anxiety and stress [32,33,34,35,36]. At the same time, it is evident how the power of contextual elements [37,38], reinforcement [39], motivation and expectations about reinforcement [40,41,42,43,44] will significantly influence meaningful learning [45], especially positive feedback [46].
Education based on emotional intelligence is necessary for students to be able to positively use and control their emotions, with positive coping strategies for stressful situations [47] and triggers [48], improving their psychological well-being [49]; which in turn promotes a feeling of belonging to a group [50,51,52], greater personal, organisational and social success [53,54], with a strong positive impact on the community [55].
The study was carried out at the Universidad Popular de Dos Hermanas, an institution created in the city in 1992 and whose area of work is adult education on the basis of “Education for all” and “throughout life”; working in turn in different areas of non-formal education: socio-cultural animation and personal and community development [56]. It should be noted that its general objectives include, among others: facilitating access to education for those who for various reasons have been left out of the education system; improving the socio-labour profile of those who attend; promoting education in democratic values; developing transversal strategies for inclusive education, attending to diversity and from a gender perspective; enhancing personal and social skills through learning; and promoting the empowerment and awareness of the individual to improve the quality of life at all stages of adult life and old age [57].

2. Materials and Methods

In order to gather the relevant information on the degree of learned helplessness and the Pygmalion effect, this research provides clues for the improvement of the teaching–learning process of the knowledge in which the adults in this centre are enrolled and of personal and social skills, with three specific objectives:
-
To expose whether there are deficiencies related to social skills and self-esteem and, therefore, learned helplessness.
-
To show whether behaviour correlates with learned helplessness or whether it can be modified to improve learning and reduce psychosocial risks.
-
To show the keys of the Pygmalion effect to compensate the negative effects of learned helplessness as a scaffolding for the improvement and development of personal and social skills and empowerment.
Regarding the methodology established to carry out the research, in order to extract all the necessary information related to such broad study questions, we agreed on a combination of methods. Thus, the methodology is mixed qualitative and quantitative [58].
The research is made up of three compilations, on the one hand, a specific questionnaire for teachers (28 persons), where the behaviour of students was collected, with a total of 15 items, gathering the information related to the 399 persons that made up the sample. This was completed by all the teaching staff at the centre, in order to ascertain the influence of these variables in an objective and triangulated manner.
On the other hand, a questionnaire on self-esteem and social skills, consisting of 15 items each, was reformulated for the students of the Universidad Popular de Dos Hermanas. The questionnaire is a reformulation and adaptation of the questionnaire for women by Castillo, S., et al. [59], with the addition of other questions divided into appendices. It has tried to combine personal skills and experiences, while questionnaires on self-esteem, social skills, social participation and interests and habits have been introduced. In this way, the same tool could be used to assess their concerns, well-being and health, aptitudes, shortcomings and opportunities.
The questionnaire explained the objective of the research and specified that the answers would be anonymous. Of the 1356 people enrolled in the 2020–2021 academic year, a total of 399 were included in the sample, in accordance with the representative characteristics of this population, who successfully completed the questionnaire, the sample having a confidence level of 95% in relation to the total population. Thus, the sample was made up of 399 people over 16 years of age, proportionally representing the institution’s target public, following quotas of gender, age, experience at the university, areas of knowledge and origin of the students, in order to maintain coherence with the reality of the centre itself.
For data processing and analysis, all the information in the database was entered into the statistical programme SPSS, version 24 [60].
The validity of the questionnaires is supported by previous research by their creators; likewise, as it is a reformulation, we evaluated the coherence and internal consistency of the tool. On the other hand, Cronbach’s alpha coefficient [61] was used to study the reliability of the tool, with values of 0.941, 0.831 and 0.805, respectively. Following the general recommendation of George and Mallery [62], based on the scores obtained in each of the questionnaires, according to Cronbach’s alpha, it can be concluded that the instrument used was very suitable for the study, as the items measured the same construct and were highly correlated.
In this process of data collection, triangulation was fundamental in carrying out the research. Firstly, the data provided by the Universidad Popular itself in terms of statistics and reports were taken into account. Based on this, the identification and analysis of triggering situations was carried out, as well as the selection and adaptation of techniques, tools and instruments for the collection and elucidation of data.
The third component of the study was the unstructured interviews with teachers, which have been key throughout the analysis. With them it was possible to evidence and affirm ideas that had been revealed through the rest of the techniques.
We agreed on this methodological process as we considered it the most appropriate to obtain answers to the objectives set out. It was necessary to know the perception of the pupils themselves, as well as that of the teaching staff and the pooling of these objective results with the data obtained through the semi-structured interviews, in order to avoid any type of bias, and to be able to triangulate the different information to be dealt with.

3. Results

We are now in the central phase of the process, where, after using the tools mentioned above, the necessary information has been compiled. In order to elucidate the data in line with the proposed objectives, descriptive analyses were carried out to find out the socio-demographic characteristics, social skills, self-esteem and behaviour in the teaching–learning scenario. Within these descriptive analyses, the frequency and cumulative percentages were verified, as well as cross-checking tables with the chi-square test to clarify possible relationships between variables. The reliability of the internal consistency of the study factors was calculated by means of Cronbach’s alpha coefficient.
The sample consisted of 28% men and 72% women, with the widest age range being 45 to 54 years for women and 55 to 64 years for men. Of these, 36.1% were currently working and only 13.2% were retired or pensioners; 50.7% were unemployed. With regard to the level of studies, 20.1% had no qualifications, 19.5% had primary education, 28% had secondary education or BUP, 23% had studied vocational training and 10% had higher education.
The first set of data, relating to social skills, showed significant and revealing frequencies regarding the pupils’ self-perception, as shown below (Figure 1 and Figure 2):
-
Of the pupils, 65.4% said they did not recognise their chances of success, compared to 100% who claimed to recognise their limitations.
-
A proportion of 80.2% did not have a positive self-concept and said that they did not know how to identify their abilities and aptitudes. At the same time, 91.2% said they did not know how other people perceived them.
-
One hundred percent showed willingness to make an effort to train their social skills, respect their rights and the rights of others, knew that they should behave differently depending on the situation and were able to put themselves in other people’s shoes.
-
A proportion of 75% said they do not clearly communicate their wishes, opinions, feelings and goals; 94.1% said that they achieved their goals to the extent that they depend on them; 98% said that they tried to maintain or improve their relationship with others in extreme situations. A further 64% were not able to refuse requests and 91% did not express their feelings without difficulty.
With regard to the self-esteem questionnaire, high frequencies showing weakness and learned helplessness, as well as those that become basic pillars for positive reinforcement are noteworthy, as shown in Figure 3, Figure 4 and Figure 5.
Taking into account the nature and impact of the variables, we segregated between risk factors and safety factors to summarize the influence of both groups in the teaching–learning process, as indicated in Figure 6.
In relation to risk factors, 30% did not feel that they were treated with the same dignity as the rest, 26.3% of the participants stated that they did not maintain a positive attitude towards themselves, while 60.5% did not feel satisfied with themselves. A proportion of 87.6% strongly agreed that they should be valued, in addition to the 12.4% who claimed to agree. On the other hand, 84.9% agreed that sometimes they felt that they were worthless and the remaining 15.1% strongly agreed.
Regarding the perception of their rights, 96.6% agreed and strongly agreed that they felt that they had fewer rights than other people.
Attending to assertiveness we found that:
-
92.9% found it difficult to ask another person to change her behavior if it was bothering them;
-
94.4% hid their feelings for fear of rejection;
-
83.9% hid ideas and opinions for fear of losing the sympathy of other people;
-
92.9% feared rejection reactions from people;
-
24.6% believed they received praise for pity or disability.
On the other hand, we show the data related to the behaviour of the students provided by the teaching staff in Figure 7, Figure 8, Figure 9 and Figure 10. In the items “disposition”, “desire”, “attitude”, “intention”, “interest” and “motivation” we found the same frequencies obtaining 80% values of almost always and always. Regarding “understanding” and “expression” we observed a cumulative frequency between almost never and regularly of 34.6%; as well as 49% participation in the same scores. Regarding “aptitude” we found 34.45% with almost always and 65.6% always; and “behaviour” obtained 43.4% almost always and 45.9% always; showing the item “learning” more frequently, obtaining 33.7% almost always and 66.3% always.

4. Discussion

This study focuses on the results of the questionnaires, where a ranking of perceived problems was obtained and contrasted with the teachers’ assessment. Taking into account the data presented in the results, we postulate as main problems and/or needs, the factors that were shown as variables of learned helplessness (unknown personal and social skills, lack of self-regulation, self-esteem and self-concept, inequality and lack of empowerment) to which we add proposals of attention for their resolution such as training in personal and social skills, training and coaching for the promotion of empowerment, recognition of capacities and aptitudes, learning in equal opportunities.
These problems are the key elements in order of impact in the frequency tables after data processing, factors that are interrelated by their nature and that are framed within the same phenomenon with different stages or branches. Therefore, and taking into account the amount of data processed (both qualitative and quantitative), for the general overview it should be noted that the lack of formal training, role appropriation, diminished personal and social skills, low self-concept, self-esteem and self-regulation and low level of assertiveness for change are factors to be improved in order to achieve optimal levels of health and growth. In this way it is shown that, despite high levels of learned helplessness, the teacher’s assessment of the items “willingness”, “desire”, “attitude”, “intention”, “interest”, “motivation”, “attitude” and “aptitude” are crucial, based on those factors of counter security, to ensure that 100% of the students achieve a positive value in learning.
This research, through the literature and the data obtained, highlights the importance of motivation and the elements that comprise it that have been previously reflected: both personal and environmental, expectations, self-efficacy, results, environment and social relations [63,64,65,66].
We also found that refocusing based on interests and external reinforcement improves student motivation in all facets of their lives [65]. With regard to conditioning factors [67], we should focus on those that will promote extrinsic motivation in accordance with positive education. Thus, we find that motivational, emotional and social elements are directly related to academic performance [68,69].
On the one hand, it is shown that the maintenance of a positive self-concept [70,71,72] and self-esteem [73,74,75] are vital because of their direct impact on motivation.
In other words, the positive relationship between self-concept [76,77,78] and academic performance, as well as between self-esteem and performance [74,79,80], is ratified.
On the other hand, with regard to the emotional factors [81,82,83] understood as emotional intelligence, emotional competence and emotional well-being; added to the social characteristic of the human being and the act of teaching–learning itself, it highlights the need to maintain a trained personal and social interaction where the control of the internal locus allows the self-control of emotions and feelings [84,85]. The training and improvement of social intelligence, social competence and social skills are also necessary [86,87,88,89]. Similarly, there is a directly proportional relationship between academic performance and emotional intelligence [90,91,92].
As for the limitations of the research, on the one hand, the size of the sample, despite being representative and reliable, cannot be transferred to the bulk of the population as it represents the universe of students at the Universidad Popular. On the other hand, we cannot know the aetiology of the factors triggering learned helplessness in each of the cases in detail, but only verify that such dysfunction exists. Similarly, the longitudinal effect has to be taken into account, i.e., given the time of the research and the specificity of the analysis, we cannot show in this article the comparative pretest and post-test or control groups showing how the Pygmalion effect may stimulate change from baseline to completion.
However, these difficulties and limitations encountered are the threshold for future research. In such a way, the research was intended to find out in depth what the triggers of learned helplessness in adults are, and to know systematically how positive education affects behavioural modification, taking into account other variables.

5. Conclusions

Both the literature and the data extracted through the present research assert the importance of the impact of continued exposure to factors that inhibit intrinsic motivation. In the same way, we show how learned helplessness can be acquired independently of the personal and psychosocial profile, so that life history and the situational elements that affect personality development are key to growth and development in any environment. Therefore, it is shown that in the same way that external motivation influences internal motivation in a negative way, hindering and/or limiting development, the Pygmalion effect, i.e., positive support, becomes an engine of change for restructuring and cognitive refocusing for the optimal acquisition of tools and skills for life.
It is noteworthy that a large number of people are able to recognise their limitations but not their possibilities for success and claim not to have a positive self-concept and show low levels of assertiveness. On the other hand, they claim not to value themselves enough, which shows a diminished self-esteem and reveal in their answers how some social skills corresponding to participation and personal empowerment are dysfunctional. However, it is shown that the teachers’ perception of the students does not correspond to the initial idea that the students themselves have about themselves, and that through positive reinforcement, participation in educational actions and their learning achievements are fruitful. In other words, we find that it is common to show such learned helplessness in adulthood as a framework learned throughout life through conditioning and reinforcement.
Therefore, taking into account the association between emotions and attention, willingness to learn or behavioural self-regulation [93]; the relationship between personal well-being and emotional intelligence and academic performance [94], as well as the importance of satisfactory social relationships and social skills and performance [95,96], it is necessary to join efforts towards a new methodological approach based on positive education from childhood to neutralise these factors, thus achieving that in the evolutionary cycle, the adult person not only has access to new knowledge and learning, but that it is configured as a continuum of constant improvement and growth in all spheres of life, thus achieving a society whose emotional health is optimal and where the role of education is, in a broad sense, the perfectibility of the human being.

Author Contributions

M.-J.P.-R. and D.C.-S. designed the research; M.-J.P.-R., D.C.-S., J.-A.M.-M., and M.-C.M.-D. collected the data; all the authors analysed the data and wrote the manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research arises from the execution of the 001 program, of competitive attendance of IRPF 2019, with execution 2020: Development of experiences of social and labour integration, of values of solidarity, tolerance and equality, aimed at young people and adults. It was co-financed by the Ministry of Health, Social Services and Equality, the European Social Fund, the Spanish Federation of Popular Universities (FEUP), and the Popular University of Dos Hermanas. 13491. Royal Decree-Law 33/2020, of 3 November, adopting urgent measures to support entities of the Third Sector of Social Action at the state level. Official State Gazette. Wednesday 4 November 2020. No. 291. Sec. I, p. 95820.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data available on request due to privacy and ethical restrictions. The primary data are contained within the article.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Diekstra, R.F.W. Effectiveness of school-based social and emotional education programmes around the world. In Social and Emotional Education: An International Analysis; Foundation Marcelino Botín: Santander, Spain, 2008; pp. 255–312. [Google Scholar]
  2. Durlak, J.A.; Weissberg, R.P.; Dymnicki, A.B.; Taylor, R.D.; Schellinger, K.R. The impact of improving students’ social and emotional learning: A meta-analysis of universal school-based interventions. Child Dev. 2011, 82, 405–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Filella-Guiu, G.; Pérez-Escoda, N.; Agulló, M.J.; Oriol, X. Results of the application of an emotional education programme in primary education. Estud. Sobre Educ. 2014, 26, 125–147. [Google Scholar]
  4. Lübke, L.; Pinquart, M.; Schwinger, M. The role of flexibility in the realisation of inclusive education. Sustainability 2021, 13, 4452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Mahoney, J.L.; Durlak, J.A.; Weissberg, R.P. An Update on Social and Emotional Learning Outcome Research. Phi Delta Kappan 2018, 100, 18–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Oriola, S.; Tey, A.; Pérez-Escoda, N. Emotions and time management in learning. In Time Management in Higher Education: Practices of Efficiency and Procrastination; Calderón, C., Gustems, J., Eds.; Edicions de la Universitat de Barcelona: Barcelona, Spain, 2020; pp. 11–20. [Google Scholar]
  7. Edel Navarro, R. Academic Achievement: Concept, Research and Development. REICE Rev. Iberoam. Sobre Calid. Efic. Cambio Educ. 2003, 1, 16. [Google Scholar]
  8. Mayorquín, E.A.; Zaldívar, A. Parental involvement in the academic achievement of primary school students. Literature review. PASEO Rev. Iberoam. Investig. Desarro. Educ. 2019, 9, 868–896. [Google Scholar]
  9. Pros, R.C.; Muntada, M.C.; Martín, M.B.; Busquets, C.G. Extracurricular activities and academic performance in primary school pupils. Eur. J. Investig. Psicol. Salud. Educ. 2015, 3, 87–97. [Google Scholar]
  10. Cohen, J. (Ed.) Educating Hearts and Minds; ASCD: Alexandria, VA, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  11. Seligman, M.E.; Maier, S.F. Failure to escape traumatic shock. J. Exp. Psychol. 1967, 74, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Hiroto, D.S. Locus of control and learned helplessness. J. Exp. Psychol. 1974, 102, 187–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Hiroto, D.S.; Seligman, M.E. Generality of learned helplessness in man. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1975, 31, 311–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Jiménez, A.T.; Robert, A.; Tejero, A.; Boget, T.; de los Cobos, J.P. Indefensión aprendida y dependencia de sustancias. Learned helplessness and substance dependence. Trastor. Adict. 2006, 8, 168–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Velbinger, K.; De Vry, J.; Jentzsch, K.; Eckert, A.; Henn, F.; Müller, W.E. Acute stress induced modifications of calcium signaling in learned helpess rats. Pharmacopsychiatry 2000, 33, 132–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Keefe, F.J.; Kashikar-Zuck, S.; Robinson, E.; Salley, A.; Beaupre, P.; Caldwell, D.; Baucom, D.; Haythornthwaite, J. Pain coping strategies that predict patients’ and spouses’ ratings of patients’ self-efficacy. Pain 1997, 73, 191–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Vollmayr, B.; Gass, P. Learned helplessness: Unique features and translational value of a cognitive depression model. Cell Tissue Res. 2013, 354, 171–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Braud, W.; Wepman, B.; Russo, D. Task and species generality of the “helplessness” phenomenon. Psychon. Sci. 1969, 16, 154–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Maier, S.F.; Testa, T.J. Failure to learn to escape by rats previously exposed to inescapable shock is partly produced by associative interference. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1975, 88, 554–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Overmier, J.B. Interference with avoidance behavior: Failure to avoid traumatic shock. J. Exp. Psychol. 1968, 78, 340–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Overmier, J.B.; Seligman, M.E. Effects of inescapable shock upon subsequent escape and avoidance responding. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1967, 63, 28–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Padilla, A.M.; Padilla, C.; Ketterer, T.; Giacalone, D. Inescapable shocks and subsequent escape/avoidance conditioning in goldfish, Carassius auratus. Psychon. Sci. 1970, 20, 295–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Seligman, M.E.P. Helplessness: On Depression Development and Death; W. H. Freeman: San Francisco, CA, USA, 1975. [Google Scholar]
  24. Seward, J.P.; Humphrey, G.L. Avoidance learning as a function of pretraining in the cat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1967, 63, 338–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Maier, S.F.; Seligman, M.E. Desamparo aprendido: Teoría y evidencia. Rev. Psicol. Exp. Gen. 1976, 105, 3–46. [Google Scholar]
  26. Peterson, C.; Maier, S.F.; y Seligman, M.E. Learned Helplessness: A Theory for the Age of Personal Control; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
  27. John-Langba, J. The Relationship of Sexual and Gender-Based Violence (SGBV) to Sexual-Risk Behaviors among Refugee Women in Botswana: The Mediating Role of Depression; University of Pittsburgh: Pittsburgh, PA, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  28. Rosenthal, R.; Jacobson, L. Pygmalion in the classroom. Urban Rev. 1968, 3, 16–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Rosenthal, R. The Pygmalion effect and its mediating mechanisms. In Improving Academic Achievement: Impact of Psychological Factors on Education; Aronson, J., Ed.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2002; pp. 25–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Sánchez, M.; y López, M. Pigmalión; Editorial Universidad Autonómica de la Ciudad de México: Mexico City, Mexico, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  31. Goleman, D. Emotional Intelligence; Bantam: New York, NY, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  32. Cañero, M.; Mónaco, E.; Montoya, I. Emotional intelligence and empathy as predictors of subjective well-being in university students. EUR. J. Investig. Sanar. Psychol. Educ. 2019, 9, 19–29. [Google Scholar]
  33. Extremera, N.; Durán, A.; Rey, L. The moderating effect of trait meta-state of mind and perceived stress on life satisfaction. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2009, 47, 116–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Augusto-Landa, J.M.; López-Zafra, E.; Pulido-Martos, M. Perceived emotional intelligence and stress coping strategies in primary school teachers: Proposal of an explanatory model with structural equations. (SEM)). Rev. Psicol. Soc. 2011, 26, 413–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Tiana, A. Analysis of basic competences as a curricular nucleus in Spanish compulsory education/Analysis of key competences as a curricular nucleus of compulsory education in Spain. Bordón 2011, 63, 63–75. [Google Scholar]
  36. Atienza, F.M. Self-Esteem, Emotional Intelligence, Motivation and Psychological Well-Being of the Students of the University of Las Palmas de Gran Canaria. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Las Palmas de Gran Canaria, Gran Canaria, Spain, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  37. Bercovitz, J.; Feldman, M. Academic entrepreneurs: Organisational change and the individual level. Organ. Sci. 2008, 19, 69–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Rizzo, U. Why do scientists create academic spin-offs? The influence of context. J. Technol. Transf. 2015, 40, 198–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Burde, W.; Blankertz, B. Is the reinforcement locus of control a predictor of brain-computer interface performance? In Proceedings of the 3rd International Brain-Computer Interface Workshop and Training Course, Graz University of Technology, Graz, Austria, 21–24 September 2006; pp. 76–77. [Google Scholar]
  40. Domjan, M. The Basics of Conditioning and Learning, 4th ed.; AAP: Washington, DC, USA, 2018; ISBN 978-1-4338-2778-5. [Google Scholar]
  41. Flaherty, C.F. Incentive testing for CF: A review of behavior changes after reward changes. Anim. Aprender. Behav. 1982, 10, 409–440. [Google Scholar]
  42. Flaherty, C.F. Incentive Relativity; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1999; ISBN 978-0-521-65863-8. [Google Scholar]
  43. Hamilton, A.; Stellar, J.; Hart, E. Reward, performance and the method of response strength in self-stimulated rats: Validation and neuroleptics. Physiol. Behav. 1985, 35, 897–904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Cohen, M.X.; Elger, C.E.; Ranganath, C. Reward expectancy modulates feedback-related negativity and EEG spectra. NeuroImage 2007, 35, 968–978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Cole, P.M.; Martin, S.E.; Dennis, T.A. Emotion Regulation as a Scientific Construct: Methodological Challenges and Directions for Child Development Research. Child Dev. 2004, 75, 317–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Siniatchkin, M.; Kropp, P.; Gerber, W.-D. Neurofeedback: The importance of reinforcement and the search for an appropriate strategy for successful self-regulation. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 2000, 25, 167–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Wong, C.S.; Law, K.S. The effects of leader and follower emotional intelligence on performance and attitude: An exploratory study. Leadersh. Q. 2002, 13, 243–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Shin, E.J.; Park, Y.S. Emotional intelligence, ego resilience, stress in clinical practice of nursing students. J. Korea Acad.-Ind. Coop. Soc. 2013, 14, 5636–5645. [Google Scholar]
  49. Sora, B.; Höge, T.; Caballer, A.; Peiró, J.M. Employment contract, job insecurity and affective well-being of employees: The role of self-efficacy and collective efficacy. Econ. Ind. Democr. 2019, 40, 193–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Cottingham, M.D. Theorizing emotional capital. Theory Soc. 2016, 45, 451–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Gkonou, C.; Miller, E.R. An exploration of the language teacher’s reflection, emotional work, and emotional capital. TESOL Q. 2021, 55, 134–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Piri, S.; Rasekh, Z.E.; Pishghadam, R. Emotional capital within the framework of cultural dimensions. Cross Cult. Comun. 2017, 13, 1–13. [Google Scholar]
  53. Gendron, B. Why is Emotional Capital Important in Education and at Work? Towards an Optimal Exploitation of Human Capital and Knowledge Management, Cahiers de la Maison des Sciences Économiques; Cahiers de la MSE: Paris, Francia, 2004; Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/5179590_Why_emotional_capital_matters_in_education_and_in_labour_toward_anOptimal_exploitation_of_human_capital_and_knowledge_management (accessed on 14 November 2021).
  54. Liu, C.C.; Chen, J.C. Development and prioritization of emotional capital measures in public service organizations. Int. J. Inf. Syst. Serv. Sect. 2012, 4, 52–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Gendron, B. Emotional capital: A crucial capital for a citizen society with personal, social and economic benefits. In Education for Citizenship in Society: Proceedings of the Ninth Conference of the Thematic Network on Identity and Citizenship of Children in Europe; CiCe: London, UK, 2007; pp. 401–416. [Google Scholar]
  56. Morón-Marchena, J.A. The Popular University, an educational agent, en Andreu Abrio, Rosario. In Popular Education in the XXI century; Díaz Sánchez, J., y Camacho Herrera, A., Eds.; Instituto Andaluz de la Juventud-Junta de Andalucía: Seville, Spain, 1998; pp. 283–293. [Google Scholar]
  57. Muñoz Díaz, M.C.; Perea Rodriguez, M.J. Educación de personas adultas mayores: Mujeres +45. Hekademos 2008, 25, 24–33. [Google Scholar]
  58. Sánchez Flores, F.A. Fundamentos epistémicos de la investigación cualitativa y cuantitativa: Consensos y disensos. Rev. Digit. Investig. Docencia Univ. 2019, 13, 102–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Cabrerizo, J.; Castillo, S.; y Roldán, J. The Practice of Evaluation in Socio-Educational Intervention. Materials and Instruments (Vademecum of the Social Educator); Pearson: Madrid, Spain, 2011; pp. 275–276. [Google Scholar]
  60. IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows Version 24.0; IBM Corp: Armonk, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  61. Cronbach, L.J. Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika 1951, 16, 297–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Georg, D.; Mallery, P. SPSS for Windows Step by Step: A Simple Guide and Reference. 11.0 Update, 4th ed.; Allyn & Bacon: Boston, MA, USA, 2003; pp. 1–63. [Google Scholar]
  63. Cook, D.A.; Artino, A. Motivation to learn: An overview of contemporary theories. Med. Educ. 2016, 50, 997–1014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Kusurkar, R.A.; Ten Cate, T.J.; Vos, C.; Westers, P.; Croiset, G. How Motivation Affects Academic Performance: A Structural Equation Modeling Analysis. Adv. Health Sci. Educ. 2012, 18, 57–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  65. Ryan, R.M.; Deci, E.L. Theory of self-determination and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development and well-being. Soy. Psychol. 2000, 55, 68–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Weiner, B. The Development of an Attribution-Based Theory of Motivation: A History of Ideas. Educ. Psychol. 2010, 45, 28–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Ainley, M.; Ainley, J. Student Engagement with Science in Early Adolescence: The Contribution of Enjoyment to Students’ Continuing Interest in Learning About Science. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2011, 36, 4–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Najimi, A.; Sharifirad, G.; Amini, M.M.; Meftagh, S.D. Academic Failure and the Students’ Point of View: The Influence of Individual, Internal, and External Organizational Factors. J. Educ. Promot. salud. 2013, 2, 22. [Google Scholar]
  69. Ursin, J. Transforming of Finnish Higher Education: Institutional Mergers and Conflicting Academic Identities. Rev. Investig. Educ. 2017, 35, 307–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Van Soom, C.; Donche, V. Profiling freshmen in STEM programs based on autonomous motivation and academic self-concept and relationship to academic achievement. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e112489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Celada, F.J. Is suicide a consequence of low self-concept and low self-esteem? Researchgate 2013, 10, 1–23. [Google Scholar]
  72. Marsh, H.W.; Seaton, M. Academic self-concept. In International Guide to Student Achievement; Hatie, J., Anderman, E.M., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 62–63. [Google Scholar]
  73. Quirk, M.; Schwanenflugel, P.J.; Webb, M.-Y. A short-term longitudinal study of the relationship between motivation to read and reading fluency ability in second grade. J. Lit. Res. 2009, 41, 196–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  74. Rahmani, P. The relationship between self-esteem, achievement goals, and academic achievement among elementary school students. Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 2011, 29, 803–808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Walgermo, B.R.; Foldnes, N.; Uppstad, P.H.; Solheim, O.J. Dynamics of development of early reading ability, interest in literacy and self-concept of readers during the first year of formal education. Read. Writ. 2018, 31, 1379–1399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Guay, F.; Marsh, H.W.; Boivin, M. Academic self-concept and academic performance: Development perspectives on their causal ordering. J. Educ. Psychol. 2003, 95, 124–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Marsh, H.W.; Craven, R. Reciprocal effects of self-concept and performance from a multidimensional perspective: Beyond seductive pleasure and one-dimensional perspectives. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 1, 133–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Marsh, H.W.; Martin, A. Academic self-concept and academic achievement: Relationships and causal ordering. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2011, 81, 59–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Hong, E.; Peng, Y.; Rowell, L.L. Homework self-regulation: Grade, gender, and achievement-level differences. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2009, 19, 269–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Rastegar, A.; Jahromi, R.G.; Haghighi, A.S.; Akbari, A.R. The Relationship of Epistemological Beliefs and Mathematical Performance: The Mediating Role of Performance Goals, Mathematical Self-Efficacy, and Cognitive Engagement. Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 2010, 5, 791–797. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  81. Billings, C.E.; Downey, L.A.; Lomas, J.E.; Lloyd, J.; Stough, C. Emotional Intelligence and scholastic achievement in pre-adolescent children. Pers. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2014, 65, 14–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Franco, M.D.G.; Beja, M.J.; Candeias, A.; Santos, N. Emotion Understanding, Social Competence and School Achievement in Children from Primary School in Portugal. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Lv, B.; Zhou, H.; Guo, X.; Liu, C.; Liu, Z.; Luo, L. The relationship between academic achievement and the emotional well-being of elementary school children in China: The moderating role of parent-school communication. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 948. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  84. Alonso, M.S.L. Analysis of the order in which self-concept, self-esteem and self-image should appear in the personal maturation process to achieve emotional well-being. Rev. INFAD Psicol. 2018, 1, 257–264. [Google Scholar]
  85. Franco, M.D.G.S.D.C.; Santos, N.N. Desenvolvimento da Compreensão Emocional Development of Emotional Understanding. Psicol. Teor. Pesqui. 2015, 31, 339–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  86. Garner, P.W. Emotional competence and its influences on teaching and learning. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2010, 22, 297–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Gustavsen, A.M. Longitudinal relationship between social skills and academic performance from a gender perspective. Cogent Educ. 2017, 4, 1411035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Jovarini, N.V.; Leme, V.B.R.; Correia-Zanini, M.R.G. Influence of social skills and stressors on academic performance in sixth grade. Paidéia 2018, 28, e2819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Selimović, Z.; Selimović, H.; Opić, S. Development of social skills among elementary school children. Int. J. Cogn. Res. Sci. Eng. Educ. 2018, 6, 17–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  90. Pulido-Acosta, F.; Herrera-Clavero, F. Predicting Children’s Academic Performance through Emotional Intelligence. Educ. Psychol. 2018, 25, 23–30. [Google Scholar]
  91. Pulido-Acosta, F.; Herrera-Clavero, F. Emotional intelligence as a predictor of academic performance in childhood: The multicultural context of Ceuta. Rev. Complut. Educ. 2017, 28, 1251–1265. [Google Scholar]
  92. Downey, L.A.; Lomas, J.; Billings, C.; Hansen, K.; Stough, C. Scholastic Success: Fluid intelligence, personality and emotional intelligence. Poder. J. Sch. Psychol. 2014, 29, 40–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Rhoades, B.L.; Warren, H.K.; Domitrovich, C.E.; Greenberg, M.T. Examining the link between preschool socio-emotional competence and first grade academic achievement: The role of attention skills. Niño temprano. Res. Q. 2011, 26, 182–191. [Google Scholar]
  94. Ferragut, M.; Fierro, A. Emotional intelligence, personal well-being and academic performance in preadolescents. Rev. Latinoam. Psicol. 2012, 44, 95–104. Available online: http://www.scielo.org.co/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S0120-05342012000300008 (accessed on 8 January 2020).
  95. Trentacosta, C.J.; Izard, C.E.; Mostow, A.J.; Bien, S.E. Emotional competence and attention span of children in early primary school. Sch. Psychol. Q. 2006, 21, 148–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. McKown, C.; Russo-Ponsaran, N.M.; Allen, A.; Johnson, J.K.; Warren-Khot, H.K. Socio-emotional factors and academic results among elementary-age children. Desarro. Infant. Infant. 2015, 25, 119–136. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Percentage of “Yes” obtained from the social skills questionnaire. For the understanding of Figure 1 and Figure 2, the items observed in them correspond to: Items that are part of the social skills questionnaire. A: Possibilities of success. B: Personal limitations. C: Self-concept. D: Hetero concept. E: Skills. F: Interests. G: Social skills. H: Rights. I: Communication. J: Objectives. K. Extreme situations. L: Behaviour. M: Empathy. N: Reject requests. O: Express feelings.
Figure 1. Percentage of “Yes” obtained from the social skills questionnaire. For the understanding of Figure 1 and Figure 2, the items observed in them correspond to: Items that are part of the social skills questionnaire. A: Possibilities of success. B: Personal limitations. C: Self-concept. D: Hetero concept. E: Skills. F: Interests. G: Social skills. H: Rights. I: Communication. J: Objectives. K. Extreme situations. L: Behaviour. M: Empathy. N: Reject requests. O: Express feelings.
Ijerph 19 00778 g001
Figure 2. Percentage of “No” obtained from the social skills questionnaire.
Figure 2. Percentage of “No” obtained from the social skills questionnaire.
Ijerph 19 00778 g002
Figure 3. Percentage of “Disagree” from the self-esteem questionnaire. For the understanding of Figure 3, Figure 4 and Figure 5, the items observed in them correspond to: Items that are part of the self-esteem questionnaire. A: Appreciation. B: Failure. C: Qualities. D: Dignity. E: Pride. F: Positive attitude. G: Satisfaction. H: Value. I: Undervalue. J: Inequality. K: Change of attitude. L: Hide feelings. M: Discomfort. N: Fear of losing sympathy. O: Fear of rejection.
Figure 3. Percentage of “Disagree” from the self-esteem questionnaire. For the understanding of Figure 3, Figure 4 and Figure 5, the items observed in them correspond to: Items that are part of the self-esteem questionnaire. A: Appreciation. B: Failure. C: Qualities. D: Dignity. E: Pride. F: Positive attitude. G: Satisfaction. H: Value. I: Undervalue. J: Inequality. K: Change of attitude. L: Hide feelings. M: Discomfort. N: Fear of losing sympathy. O: Fear of rejection.
Ijerph 19 00778 g003
Figure 4. Percentage of “Agree” from the self-esteem questionnaire.
Figure 4. Percentage of “Agree” from the self-esteem questionnaire.
Ijerph 19 00778 g004
Figure 5. Percentage of “Strongly agree” from the self-esteem questionnaire.
Figure 5. Percentage of “Strongly agree” from the self-esteem questionnaire.
Ijerph 19 00778 g005
Figure 6. Percentage safety factors.
Figure 6. Percentage safety factors.
Ijerph 19 00778 g006
Figure 7. Percentage of “Rarely” from the teacher perception questionnaire. For the understanding of Figure 7, Figure 8, Figure 9 and Figure 10, the items observed in them correspond to: Items that are part of the teacher perception questionnaire. A: Available. B: You win. C: Attitude. D: Intent. E: Interest. F: Motivation. G: Compression. H: Expression. I: Aptitude. J: Behaviour. K: Inventiveness. L: Restlessness. M: Communication. N: Over traction. O: Participation. P: Learning.
Figure 7. Percentage of “Rarely” from the teacher perception questionnaire. For the understanding of Figure 7, Figure 8, Figure 9 and Figure 10, the items observed in them correspond to: Items that are part of the teacher perception questionnaire. A: Available. B: You win. C: Attitude. D: Intent. E: Interest. F: Motivation. G: Compression. H: Expression. I: Aptitude. J: Behaviour. K: Inventiveness. L: Restlessness. M: Communication. N: Over traction. O: Participation. P: Learning.
Ijerph 19 00778 g007
Figure 8. Percentage of “Regular” from the teacher perception questionnaire.
Figure 8. Percentage of “Regular” from the teacher perception questionnaire.
Ijerph 19 00778 g008
Figure 9. Percentage of “Usually” from the teacher perception questionnaire.
Figure 9. Percentage of “Usually” from the teacher perception questionnaire.
Ijerph 19 00778 g009
Figure 10. Percentage of “Always” from the teacher perception questionnaire.
Figure 10. Percentage of “Always” from the teacher perception questionnaire.
Ijerph 19 00778 g010
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Cobos-Sanchiz, D.; Perea-Rodriguez, M.-J.; Morón-Marchena, J.-A.; Muñoz-Díaz, M.-C. Positive Adult Education, Learned Helplessness and the Pygmalion Effect. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 778. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19020778

AMA Style

Cobos-Sanchiz D, Perea-Rodriguez M-J, Morón-Marchena J-A, Muñoz-Díaz M-C. Positive Adult Education, Learned Helplessness and the Pygmalion Effect. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(2):778. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19020778

Chicago/Turabian Style

Cobos-Sanchiz, David, Manuel-Jesús Perea-Rodriguez, Juan-Agustín Morón-Marchena, and María-Carmen Muñoz-Díaz. 2022. "Positive Adult Education, Learned Helplessness and the Pygmalion Effect" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 2: 778. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19020778

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop