Next Article in Journal
Evaluating the Anesthetic and Physiologic Effects of Intramuscular and Intravenous Alfaxalone in Eastern Mud Turtles (Kinosternon subrubrum)
Next Article in Special Issue
Do Animal Welfare Education Campaigns Really Work? An Evaluation of the RSPCA’s #DogKind Campaign in Raising Awareness of Separation-Related Behaviours in UK Dog Owners
Previous Article in Journal
Antimicrobial Prescription Behavior in Equine Asthma Cases: An International Survey
Previous Article in Special Issue
What Do We Know about Dog Owners? Exploring Associations between Pre-Purchase Behaviours, Knowledge and Understanding, Ownership Practices, and Dog Welfare
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Potential Benefits of a ‘Trauma-Informed Care’ Approach to Improve the Assessment and Management of Dogs Presented with Anxiety Disorders

by
Claire Lorraine Corridan
1,*,
Susan E. Dawson
2 and
Siobhan Mullan
1
1
Animal Welfare & Ethics, UCD School of Veterinary Science, Belfield, D04 V1W8 Dublin, Ireland
2
Research Fellow in Psychology, University of Manchester, Manchester M13 9PL, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2024, 14(3), 459; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14030459
Submission received: 14 December 2023 / Revised: 10 January 2024 / Accepted: 13 January 2024 / Published: 31 January 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Companion Animal Welfare: Focusing on the Future)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Trauma-informed care (TIC) is an approach which has been utilised in human psychology for many years now. TIC considers how important early experience is in determining lifelong responses to challenging situations, how individuals respond to stress, how they overcome it, and their ability to develop and sustain resilience. There are a number of scientific publications which consider the importance of early experience in animals, both in utero and during their early development. This paper considers aspects of TIC approaches for humans which might be applied in dogs, focusing on both prevention of behavioural problems, by protecting puppies from adverse early experiences, and also, assessment of shelter dogs or those presented for problematic behaviours. A TIC approach for dogs would result in the following: the realisation that adverse early experience has significant consequences for canine welfare; recognising that where dogs respond in an apparently irrational or over the top manner, it may be the result of previous trauma; people involved in the care of these dogs must respond with empathy, understanding, and practical solutions to improve the welfare of the dog, while avoiding the need to re-traumatise them in as part of the diagnostic or treatment processes.

Abstract

Dog caregiver reporting on the spectrum of fearful–aggressive behaviours often describes ‘unpredictable’ or ‘exaggerated’ responses to a situation/animal/person. A possible explanation for these behavioural responses considers that the dog is reacting to triggered memories for which the dog has a negative association. For many dogs undergoing veterinary behavioural treatment or rehabilitation through a canine rescue organisation, the assessing clinician relies on “proxy” reporting of the history/background by a caregiver (dog owner, foster carer, or shelter personnel). Detailed information on the event or circumstances resulting in this negative association may be limited or absent altogether. Consideration of a trauma-informed care (TIC) approach, currently applied in a wide range of human psychology and social care fields, may be helpful in guiding the clinical approach taken. The literature relating to adverse early experience (AEE) and trauma-informed care (TIC) in puppies/dogs compared to children/adults was evaluated to identify common themes and conclusions identified across both species. In the absence of known/identifiable trauma, behavioural assessment and management should consider that a ‘problem’ dog may behave as it does, as the result of previous trauma. The dog can then be viewed through a lens of empathy and understanding, often lacking for dogs presenting with impulsive, reactive, or aggressive behaviours. Assessment must avoid re-traumatising the animal through exposure to triggering stimuli and, treatment options should include counselling of caregivers on the impact of adverse early experiences, consideration of the window of tolerance, and TIC behavioural modification techniques.

1. Introduction

Trauma is a term used to describe a range of experiences that can cause significant distress and have long-lasting negative effects on mental health [1].
Trauma-informed care (TIC) is widely accepted [2,3] as a useful approach in understanding the development of human health and mental health difficulties resulting from adverse childhood experience (ACE) [4]. TIC describes a framework for working with and relating to people presenting with both the physical and emotional signs associated with early trauma, often summarised using the 4 R’s approach: realise, recognise, respond, resist re-traumatisation [5]. TIC differs from previous approaches aimed at modifying harmful behaviours or ameliorating negative emotions because it recognises the importance of considering any underlying trauma experienced by the patient [6]. Instead of asking “what is wrong with you”, TIC considers, “what happened to you” [7]. This approach is a biopsychosocial model integrating research from neurobiology, attachment, trauma, and resilience [8,9].
A simplified explanation of this concept considers the “danger-response” of a patient [10]: when a patient experiences actual danger or the anticipation of danger, whether real or imagined, their body responds in a protective manner. This may be recognised as either “hyper- or hypo-arousal” as illustrated in the window of tolerance model [11] where the patient is experiencing autonomic dysregulation because of reminders of their original trauma. The behavioural responses associated with hyper-arousal may be interpreted as agitation, aggression, or sometimes perilous or risky behaviour. In contrast, hypo-arousal results in the patient appearing withdrawn, frozen, or apparently “vacant” behaviours. Both the hypo- and hyper-aroused states are the individual’s attempt to rebalance their autonomic nervous system, returning them to their window of tolerance, where they feel safe, able to think clearly, and make rational decisions. The nature of arousal experienced and demonstrated behaviourally can be situation specific and not necessarily consistent, fluctuating within the same situation and between occasions where the situation ‘appears’ similar. This may result in the patient initially appearing to be animated and aggressive, before seeming to shut down, or vice versa, for example, a seemingly subdued individual suddenly “flipping a switch” and lashing out.
The imbalance of ascending dopaminergic tracts results in rapid alterations in mood, arousal, and ultimately behavioural responses [12]. A TIC approach would consider that the exaggerated responses seen in an individual patient may be the result of previous trauma. Instead of considering “what is wrong with the individual,” making them behave in a certain way, it focuses on supporting both the patient and those around them, recognising the unpredictable, dynamic, and sometimes difficult to explain, responses to particular triggers and finding practical solutions to minimise the risk posed from future triggering events [5].
This paper considers the possibility that a trauma-informed care approach may be helpful in canine veterinary behavioural medicine; however, this does not exclude the possibility that similar post-traumatic effects occur in other species of animals, with species-specific variation in severity, the cognitive processes involved, and the resultant behavioural signs exhibited.

1.1. The Importance of Fear, Anxiety, and Stress as a Welfare Concern in Dogs

Canine behavioural pathologies contribute to both relinquishment [13,14,15,16,17,18] and elective or ‘convenience’ euthanasia in all dogs, but the prevalence in younger dogs is particularly high [19]. This is partially the result of them being less likely to suffer from other pathologies resulting in euthanasia (chronic DJD, neoplasia, etc.), and also the immaturity of the human:dog bond. As such, this constitutes a serious welfare concern for juvenile dogs (those who have not yet reached physical maturity). Caregiver reporting on the spectrum of fearful–aggressive behaviours often describes ‘unpredictable’ or excessive/over the top responses [20] to a situation/animal/person (herein described as the trigger), to which the behaviour is directed. Estimates of the prevalence of fearfulness and aggression vary across different areas and studies estimate it to be between 20 to 50% of the dog population [21,22,23,24].
Both fear and anxiety are unpleasant emotional reactions to the actual presence (fear) or potential presence (anxiety) of a threat [25]. Fear and anxiety are expressed by many of the same physiological and behavioural signs and there is no empirical data on distinguishing between them [26]. It is therefore difficult to determine whether the trigger is the direct source of the dog’s negative reaction or if the dog is associating another, apparently innocuous stimulus (trigger), with the original trauma and reacting as if they are anticipating trauma or danger to the same degree as they have experienced it before. This is an exaggerated form of a normal self-protective or defensive response, which becomes maladaptive for the patient when it prevents them from having normal or healthy interactions with their environment. Some examples might include: a dog who experienced either verbal or physical abuse by a man, and who then mistrusts all men; or a dog who was frightened by the sound of a firework as a puppy, and who will not leave their home or garden in the autumn or winter months, due to the multiple sensory triggers of the season, which they directly associate with their initial fearful exposure to fireworks; equally, one traumatic event associated with travelling to a veterinary setting for treatment, can result in animals being triggered by all future car travel.

1.2. Early Experience and Neurobiological Development

Canine behaviour has a significant genetic component and thus, behavioural genotype often correlates closely with the behavioural phenotype exhibited [27,28]. Where there is a genetic predisposition for fearfulness and anxiety, those individuals are at higher risk of experiencing difficulties when faced with potentially stressful or fear-inducing experiences [29]. Environmental factors, such as maternal care, owner experience, habituation, and training also have a significant impact on behavioural phenotype [30,31,32], so where any of these factors are inadequate for the developing puppy or juvenile dog, early experiences can be interpreted adversely. The behavioural phenotype displayed and the severity of any fearful response will be the combined result of that individual’s behavioural genotype, early environment, and epigenetic effects [33].
Trauma can be experienced by an individual at any time in their life. The developing puppy is, however, more vulnerable than an adult dog due to their behavioural immaturity and pliability [34,35] and the absence of resilience [36]. The experiences and behaviour of dogs during their first year of life are crucial in determining their later behaviour and temperament [37]. A maladaptive response to stress during puppyhood, referred to as a toxic stress response [38], plays an important role in the pathway from early adversity to physical and behavioural pathologies. Such toxic stress can have damaging effects on learning, behaviour, physical, and mental health across the lifespan of the animal [39].

1.3. Behavioural Resilience

The ability of an organism to adapt to its environment is integral to its survival [40]. Daily life involves confrontation with changing situations that can be physiologically or psychologically challenging. To cope with these ‘physical or perceived threats to homeostasis’ [41] the ability to alter the function and expression of neuronal genes has been developed in the form of molecular and behavioural stress responses [42]. This is advantageous because it allows rapid behavioural, autonomic, and cognitive CNS responses to stressful circumstances, followed by prompt re-establishment of homeostasis and a functional steady state. Both genetics and environmental factors will contribute to the individual’s ability to develop and sustain emotional and behavioural resilience [43].
The offspring of mammals exposed to stress whilst pregnant may experience several physiological consequences, as their bodies are prepared for entering a potentially hostile environment: accelerated growth to compensate for reduced maternal investment [44]; increased risk of psychiatric disorders [45]; and long-lasting changes in the white matter of the hippocampus [46].
While enhancing neuronal communication and promoting survival, this can impact neuronal function and integrity, both immediately and in the long-term. Consequences of stress, such as its negative influences on cognition and emotional stability, are an issue of major relevance in human health and the development of mental resilience, which is a necessity for re-establishment of emotional homeostasis. Chronic and/or severe activation of the stress response early in life has been shown to be potentially injurious in both humans and experimental animals [47,48,49].

1.4. Adverse Early Experience in Humans

Adverse childhood experiences have been described in human psychiatric medicine since 1995 [50]. Commonly referred to as ACEs, these are potentially traumatic events that occur in utero, childhood, and adolescence, with a spectrum that is extensive but includes physical, emotional, or sexual abuse; witnessing violence; having a family member attempt or die by suicide; and growing up in a household with substance use (or abuse), mental health problems, or instability due to parental separation, divorce, or incarceration [51,52]. In people, multiple ACEs can be experienced by the same individual, over time, or as a result of a single serious event. The long-term impact of such will be influenced by the severity of the ACE, the cumulative effect of multiple ACEs, the support system available to the patient, and their individual genetic and developed resilience.
Stress experienced by a pregnant woman has been shown to have a profound impact on their developing foetus. Prenatal maternal stress and depression can impact the developing foetus, the effects of which can be lifelong and impact future generations [53]. Maternal stress triggers a strong physiologic response that causes the release of cortisol and stress-related neurotransmitters. The release of these neurotransmitters can have a profound and often harmful impact on the growing foetus [54], which has been linked to subsequent stress reactivity and psychopathology later in life [55]. The placenta attempts to filter and protect the foetus from exposure to stress-related effects attributable to these neurotransmitters, but high levels, prolonged exposure, or a compromised placenta limit this protection. It has been suggested that this effect is in fact preparing the foetus for survival in a similarly stressful postnatal environment [56].
Several studies have explored the adverse influence of prenatal maternal depression on neurodevelopment in childhood, highlighting increased reactivity [57], more difficult temperament [58], greater negative affectivity [59,60], and high cry reactivity [61]. Furthermore, elevated temperamental domains of activity and emotional reactivity have been linked to depression, anxiety, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and conduct disorder later in childhood [62,63,64].
The purpose of ACE screening in human medicine is to identify the likelihood that a patient may be experiencing toxic stress physiology. In and of themselves, the ACEs are not the outcome of interest, it is the long term physiological and neurological effects that are important [65].
The trauma-informed care approach includes realisation of how ACEs affect both health and behaviour, encompassing a more holistic approach to medicine; recognition of clinical symptom presentation, particularly when it appears to be difficult to explain via any other aetiology; and adjustment of the health care provider’s approach to treatment to ensure they do not re-traumatise patients when delivering evidence-based care [2].

1.5. Adverse Early Experience in Dogs

Through comparable mammalian mechanisms to those described in humans, it is plausible that adverse early experiences in dogs that result in toxic stress physiology can have similarly long-term negative impacts on health, behaviour, and resilience. This could include chronic stress, in utero and into puppyhood, multiple “low impact” adverse experiences or a single event, the severity of which is such, that the HPA is activated in a heightened manner and with similar, long-lasting effects.
As yet, there are few studies on the impact of toxic stress physiology in puppies. Studies in rodents and human children have shown that chronic glucocorticoid excess, including pre- and perinatally, interferes with learning at a cellular level [66,67,68,69], compromising development of the hippocampus and amygdala; therefore, associational learning and fear modulation are altered. This results in memory impairment and compromises fear conditioning [69,70]. Early, pronounced stress has a direct impact on brain pathology and the consequent behavioural changes.
Adult dogs with naturally occurring hypercortisolism (Cushing’s disease) show disorientation, altered social interactions, disrupted sleep–wake cycles, house-soiling, compulsive behaviours, depressive behaviours, anxiety, and difficulties with memory and learning [71]. Iatrogenic hyperadrenocorticism, from use of steroid medications, results in dogs who are: significantly less playful, more nervous/restless, more fearful/less confident, more aggressive in the presence of food, more prone to barking, more prone to startle, more prone to reacting aggressively when disturbed, and more prone to avoiding people or unusual situations [72]. If these effects are consistently reported in adult dogs with fully developed brains exposed to high levels of blood cortisol, the impact of elevated blood cortisol on the rapidly growing and vulnerable brains of puppies could also be significant and worth further scientific scrutiny.
Puppies born to dams who have experienced significantly high or chronic levels of stress during their gestation period could be at risk, particularly if the stress results in reduced “parenting capacity” which is a risk in both pregnant humans and bitches [73,74]. During the first weeks of life, poor maternal behaviour has an impact on the puppies’ cognitive development [75]. As a result of the crucial nature of both the pre/post-partum care of canine dams, which corresponds with the sensitive neurodevelopmental landmark phases of puppy development, it is vital that puppies and their dams be exposed to humane conditions that minimise the risks of excess stress and fear [76]. As we learn more about epigenetics and the impact of environmental conditions resulting in gene expression, it is easier to understand why puppies raised in “less than ideal” conditions will almost certainly be more vulnerable [77]. Where the environment is stressful for the pregnant/nursing dam, and her puppies (noisy, strong-smelling enclosures, lacking space, lacking comfort, lacking optimal thermal conditions, lacking the optimal physical/mental/social stimulation) presence of any genetic predisposition for fear, anxiety, and stress, will be more likely to be activated [78]. Poor maternal care, where there is mismothering, illness, or absence of the dam, will impact intraspecific socialisation, whether there are appropriately matched littermates present or not [75]. The absence of, limited, or negative human–dog interactions and handling [79], including fear-inducing or painful situations commonly experienced by puppies under 12 weeks of age [80,81], tail docking, dew claw removal, separation from dam/siblings/familiar environment, transport, injections, and implanting of microchips [82] could negatively impact the puppies’ future relationships with people.
Providing an excellent environment and care of a pregnant dam through pregnancy, nursing of her pups, and then natural weaning, is a time-consuming task requiring skill, experience, and training, to mitigate the inherent risks described above. This may be difficult to achieve, cost-effectively, in larger scale puppy rearing units, with large numbers of breed stock housed in proximity and high animal: caregiver ratios [83].

1.6. Adverse Experiences or Traumas in Dogs

The study of adverse events, described as ‘traumatic’ in the canine literature, has focused on a combination of experimentally induced trauma, and naturally occurring traumatic events, such as experiencing natural disasters such as earthquakes [84]. Exposure to experimental traumas was unpredictable and uncontrollable, resulting in greater fear and defensive responses, agitation, tachyarrhythmias, phobias, generalised learning deficiency in relation to escape and active avoidance behaviours, passivity, impaired cognition and learning, and persistent signs of trauma such as tremor, anorexia, intermittent howling, excitement, and recurrent diarrhoea [85,86,87,88,89,90,91,92,93,94,95]. The signs continued over time, without the presence of triggers, demonstrating the long-lasting, chronic influence of the trauma, long after exposure to the traumatic incident.
Naturally occurring traumatic events and the resultant “post-traumatic stress disorder-like” signs in dogs have been hypothesised about for some considerable time. In 1979, Wagner [96] hypothesised that trauma-trigger responses in animals are intrusion signs, resulting in both an exaggerated and avoidance response to those triggers, which are likely to be expressions of them having re-experienced the original traumatic incident. The evidence to support the occurrence of post-traumatic physical/behavioural pathologies in dogs is, however, a newer development in veterinary behavioural medicine [97,98]. It has been investigated in working dogs utilised in bomb detection or post-disaster deployment [98,99,100] whereas their human-handler counterparts have been diagnosed and treated for PTSD for many years [101].
Studies in chimpanzees have shown that adverse early experience impacts on both development of social skills and resilience: orphaned juveniles lacking in the safe and facilitating social environment provided by their mother, were less equipped for fine-tuned social play [102] (assayed by a higher prevalence of aggression) and deciding whom they entrusted to keep their body clean [103] (grooming behaviours). Identification of evidence to support the presence of behavioural clusters similar to PTSD and depression in chimpanzees [104] has contributed to the argument that a diagnosis of complex PTSD in chimpanzees is consistent with descriptions of trauma-induced symptoms as described by the DSM-IV and human trauma research [105] and has led to the emergence of trans-species psychology inclusive of both humans and other species.

1.7. Application of a Trauma-Informed Care Approach in Dogs

Application of a trauma-informed care approach might be particularly useful in the assessment and rehabilitation of both shelter dogs and those presented with problematic behaviours (as presented by their caregiver), as companion dogs, or those with a working or assistance dog role.
Many shelter dogs are collected as strays or relinquished by caregivers who can no longer provide that care, for a multitude of reasons. Often the history accompanying these dogs is incomplete or non-existent and this provides additional challenges for those attempting to rationalise the behaviours they are exhibiting. The owners or caregivers of dogs presented for problem behaviours may have equal difficulty understanding why their dogs are reacting to what they (the caregiver) perceive to be a harmless stimulus, or with ‘apparent’ aggression (again, the caregiver’s interpretation).
Dog caregivers presenting their dog for behavioural assessment and therapy may be divided into four categories:
  • They have owned their dog from puppyhood (generally 6–12 weeks of age) and can recall a specific event or series of events that has resulted in their puppy/dog becoming anxious.
  • They have acquired their puppy or dog, beyond 12 weeks of age and are directly aware, or have been informed of a specific event or series of events that may have resulted in their puppy/dog becoming anxious.
  • They have owned their dog from puppyhood (generally 6–12 weeks of age) but are unaware of a specific event or series of events that has resulted in their puppy/dog becoming anxious.
  • They have acquired their puppy or dog beyond 12 weeks of age and are unaware of a specific event or series of events that has resulted in their puppy/dog displaying signs of anxiety or fearful behaviours.
Dogs in the first two categories have a plausible rationale for their anxiety, with or without the addition of early trauma (in utero and during the sensitive phases) to explain the aetiology of the behaviour. Even if the dog caregiver cannot specifically recall a traumatic event in their puppy’s history, it can be determined- with some certainty, by a clinician collecting a medical history exploring aspects of the breeding environment, social isolation, early events, etc. Behaviour exhibited by the last two categories of dog, could potentially be explained by early trauma the dog was exposed to themselves as a puppy, or where the dam had experienced trauma during pregnancy or whilst nursing her puppy/puppies during the sensitive phases. They could equally be the result of trauma experienced by the dog, without their caregiver being aware (either where the caregiver was absent altogether, distracted, or not behaviourally sensitive to the puppy or dog’s response). Irrespective of the category to which the puppy or dog can be categorised here, gestational trauma and early experience could have an influence on the behavioural phenotype of the adult dog. Information about what triggers an individual patient [105] may prove helpful in managing their behaviour. However, because of the cumulative nature of triggering and the individual differences associated with the response to trauma, they may serve as a guide, but not necessarily the solution to the problem. Adoption of a TIC approach for canine behavioural medicine should therefore focus on the management of the case and less on the inciting cause(s).
In the absence of a corroborative history, we propose that an approach which considers “that there is a likelihood that this puppy/dog has experienced trauma” would be more helpful and productive than “I don’t understand why this dog is behaving this way”. This is the lens of empathy which utilisation of a TIC approach provides.
Equally, when a dog is presented with a known history of trauma, neglect, or abandonment, it could be helpful to formulate strategies which consider the potential current and future impact this might have on their psychological and physical health, thereby setting the patient or animal up for success, rather than waiting for the inevitable signs of behavioural pathology when the dog is placed in a challenging situation.
By applying a TIC approach in assessing these animals, the following scenarios may be considered: the dog may have experienced in utero trauma due to the environment accommodating their dam; or they may have experienced trauma as a puppy, with or without the care provided by their dam or breeding establishment. This could include the effects of mismothering or the absence of positive habituation, in healthy, stimulating, and safe environmental conditions. TIC recognises the long-term impact of fear-inducing experiences in early life, in the absence of behavioural resilience which develops with time, stability, and supportive environmental and social conditions. TIC also acknowledges that the response to trauma is completely individual, based on their unique combination of genotype and early environmental conditions, so that exposure to any type of negative experience as a juvenile animal could result in long-term behavioural pathology.
In veterinary behavioural medicine there are currently several potential differential diagnoses which might be considered in response to trauma: fearful avoidance behaviour [106], fear aggression [107], generalised anxiety disorder [108], panic disorder [109], PTSD [110], PTS-like disorder [99,111], phobias [112], and also some compulsive disorders [113]. Differentiation between these conditions can be very difficult, and various researchers and clinicians may favour one over the other for a variety of reasons. What is consistent between them all is the underlying aetiology, where the patient has experienced trauma, on a spectrum of severity and with various behavioural manifestations.

1.8. Modification of Current Treatment Approaches for Anxious/Fearful Dogs to Incorporate TIC

Veterinary behavioural medicine currently uses a combination of medical interventions and behaviour modification techniques for treating fearful animals [114,115,116], which aim to reduce physiological responses to a stressful situation and over time, desensitise the patient to the triggering situation and ideally, ultimately countercondition, so that at worst, exposure to the trigger results in a manageable, neutral response for both the animal and their caregiver, and in the best case scenarios, the negative can be converted into a rewarding situation for that patient, depending on the severity of their baseline state. This approach relies heavily on pet caregiver empathy and compliance [117].
Assessing dogs in the care of an animal shelter or rescue organisation will have the additional challenges associated with the absence of social support from a permanent caregiver, balanced against the training, empathy, and capabilities of those working with rescue dogs as a vocation [118]. Nonetheless, would consideration of the 4 R’s approach—realise, recognise, respond, resist re-traumatisation [119]—be helpful in the management of anxious/fearful canine patients? This is not an approach currently documented for use in dogs and this paper will consider whether it is worthy of future consideration and development.

2. Exploring Use of Trauma-Informed Care Approaches in Dogs

2.1. Adverse Early Experiences (AEE)

Many of the examples of adverse childhood experience can also be experienced by dogs, whether living with an caregiver or as a stray/roaming, independent animal. Due to the differences in cognitive ability and behavioural adaptation, the responses will not be equivalent, but serve as examples or a guide in considering the impact of adverse early experiences (AEEs) in dogs. Table 1 describes the results of a search on adverse early experiences for children and dogs, indicating that few areas have previously been identified or explored in relation to canine welfare. The spectrum of ACE in children is huge and diverse in terms of severity. Instead of listing every category of ACE and whether the equivalent effects have been documented in the canine literature, Table 1 lists six of the main groups of ACE, which have an equivalent consideration in canine behaviour and welfare. All the groupings listed were considered to be highly likely or already supported by the literature, as being relevant to dogs.

2.2. Signs or Symptoms of Trauma in Humans and Dogs

A 2017 study [161] looking at the ordinal risk of a list of potential health outcomes associated with ACE in humans found a moderate risk (OR 2–3) of smoking, heavy alcohol use, poor self-rated health, cancer, heart disease, and respiratory disease, strong associations (OR 3–6) with sexual risk taking, mental ill health, and problematic alcohol usage, and the strongest risks (OR 7+) for problematic drug use and interpersonal and self-directed violence (to include self-harm and suicide). Systematic investigations of the impact of AEE on dog physical health have not been conducted here, but, understanding the correlations between trauma and physical illness is developing within veterinary behavioural medicine [162,163] and worthy of future consideration and research.
The behavioural and mental health impacts of ACE are shown in Table 2, along with the likely applicability to dogs.
Table 2 highlights that many of the symptoms of trauma demonstrated in humans have also been described in the canine literature as well; however, there are many that were not. This may be explained by the differences in cognitive capacities and the internal processing of trauma that humans are capable of, and dogs are either not known to be capable of, or where they are, it is to a lesser extent. There is however significant scope to explore this further. Consideration of the potential for adverse early experience to cause a range of “danger responses” in dogs, which cannot otherwise be explained rationally by either caregiver or clinician, would be a particular benefit supporting inclusion of TIC in canine behavioural management.

2.3. Treatment Strategies Utilising a Trauma-Informed Care (TIC) Approach and Potential Relevance for Canine Welfare

Current behavioural modification techniques for animals with a history of trauma/anxiety/fearful behaviour might include a combination of cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) and anti-anxiety or mood modifying medications.
A study evaluating the feasibility, acceptability, safety, tolerability, and potential clinical effectiveness of trauma-focused cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) in young people [202], reported that 85% of participants “found it helpful.” Many of the TIC-centred treatments described in the human literature would not appear to be suitable for integration in canine behaviour modification approaches, such as talking-based cognitive behavioural therapy, and structured sensory interventions like drawing, imagery, and other forms of expressive art [203]. However, there have been recent attempts to translate eye movement desensitisation and reprocessing (EMDR)—which requires the patient to identify multiple aspects of the traumatic memory and an alternate, desired, positive self-representation [204]—for use in dogs, utilising approaches developed for pre-verbal children [205,206].
Use of the TIC lens, whereby a canine caregiver or treating clinician may be aware of the effects of trauma, recognise its signs, understand how it informs individual responses, and actively try to prevent re-traumatisation are the tenets of trauma-informed care and could be integrated into canine behaviour modification techniques [207,208,209]. In addition, it is plausible that a TIC approach could resonate with owners more than traditional non-TIC approaches, thereby encouraging help-seeking and compliance with behavioural interventions [210].
Development of new TIC approaches for use in both shelter dogs and those presented by their caregivers for behavioural therapy, as well as adaptation of current assessment and treatment modalities, to remove the risk of re-traumatisation, would significantly improve canine welfare and potentially caregiver compliance, for both current and new dog caregivers.

3. Practical Application of the TIC Approach for Dog Management

Viewing problematic canine behaviours through the lens of early trauma does not excuse or trivialise them, instead it enriches our understanding of how canine behaviour develops, informs intervention strategies, and improves clinical outcomes.

3.1. Importance of Pre-Acquisition Advice for Dog Caregivers

Improved acknowledgement and understanding on the importance of the in utero environments and the care of the dam, both pre- and post-partum within the dog breeding, veterinary, and dog owning communities can only be beneficial for the welfare of the dogs in their care. Considering the importance of ‘behavioural health and prophylaxis’ will benefit dog caregivers and help ensure their puppies are better prepared for life in their new domestic environment.
In child psychology, there is an understanding that “it can be too late, but never too early” in safeguarding the mental health of babies and infants [211] (Acquarone, 2004), so too, this could be argued, for the safeguarding of canine mental health. When the mother of a human infant is experiencing acute stress during pregnancy, resulting in the foetus being “incubated in terror [212]”, often the causes of the stress are unavoidable or difficult to remove [213]. When a potential dog caregiver is making the decision about sourcing a puppy to join their family, the time and diligence invested in getting advice and researching breeders or other sources of puppies should be taken without any pressure at all [214].
Avoiding problems associated with traumatic early development, in utero and during the vital sensitive phase between birth and weaning, will help ensure the puppy has had the best possible start in life. This in turn improves outcomes for the breeder in terms of reputation: the new dog caregiver, because their puppy is less likely to have problems and ultimately, the puppy will have improved mental wellbeing and the best possible chance of developing healthy resilience. From there, the puppy’s continued development will rely on the care taken by their new caregiver and those involved directly and indirectly in their care [215,216]. Caution should also be considered about whether puppies who have experienced early trauma, contributing to behavioural issues in later life, will be covered by pet insurance providers [217].

3.2. Separation from the Dam, Litter, Care Giver, and Familiar Environment

Opinions vary on the ideal time for weaning, separation of pups from their dams and litter mates, and transport to their second or new home [84,218] (varying in the range of 6–16 weeks and generally recommended as 8–12 weeks). These practices are considered routine and, although there may be some anecdotal advice given to dog caregivers about minimising the associated stress for the puppy, there does not appear to be scientific evidence to inform the advice given. Breed-specific information on variation of timing in the sensitive phases [219] for the huge spectrum of dog sizes, types, and maturities, would help update the current advice given. Transport of puppies from the property they were born on to the home they are intended to stay in varies considerably, in terms of geography, duration, mode of transport, care pre/during/post transport. Information on the optimal age for transport, to minimise the detrimental effects on the puppy, appears to be lacking. There are a number of unknowns here, which could be explored for the advancement of canine welfare.

3.3. Procedures Routinely Carried out on Juvenile Dogs

In the UK and Ireland, dogs must be microchipped and registered on an authorised database before 12 weeks of age, or earlier if leaving the property they were born from 6 to 7 weeks onwards [220,221]. Equally, routine vaccination of puppies usually commences between 4 and 12 weeks of age [222] (NOAH, 2023), coinciding with normal puppy sensitive phases. The manner of handling puppies during these procedures, to include catching, lifting, restraint, skin tenting, insertion of the needle, and potential use of distractors [111] can potentially have long lasting effects on the puppy’s acceptance of restraint or any invasive procedures (e.g., annual booster vaccinations) and yet, this has traditionally been considered as routine and low risk and therefore not necessarily given the time, care, and investment of expertise and training that it deserves.
If potentially more invasive procedures are to be carried out, such as tail docking, dew claw removal, ear cropping, etc., depending on the breeds involved and the local legislation in place, even more attention should be given to use of appropriately trained personnel, local anaesthesia, sedation of the puppies, and adequate use of analgesia, to minimise the risks of both physical and mental/emotional damage to the puppies involved.

3.4. Assessment of Dogs in Animal Shelters

Various means have been developed of assessing dogs in animal rescue shelters/dog pounds, to triage animals for either euthanasia or potential rehoming [223,224,225]. Utilising a TIC approach would ensure that testing considers, and ideally avoids, the potential detrimental effects of exposure to triggers which could incite a “danger response” in the animal, resulting not only in acute stress for the dog but also an increased likelihood of the outcome or decision being unfavourable for the dog too.
Consideration for the fact that shelter dogs have, as a minimum, lost their previous caregiver, and in many cases experienced multiple other sources of trauma, means that basic empathy should be applied to every stage of their care. On arrival, they will require time to adjust to their new environment, diet, care givers, and proximity to other dogs or species of animals. Ideally their care should be kept stable, minimising the risk of triggering from novel stressors (new kennels, new handlers/trainers/care givers, and to other potential changes to their routine).
Assessments of “temperament” should be delayed until the dog has had the opportunity to recover from their previous trauma (e.g., living as strays, capture and transport to the shelter or foster facility, separation from their caregiver) before any “meaningful” behavioural assessment can be made, particularly when that decision could potentially be one of life or death (euthanasia).
Incorporating TIC into the assessment approach should pay particular attention to the euthanasia decision-making process. Where dogs exhibit signs consistent with previous trauma, they should universally be afforded appropriate empathy. Some will be suitable for, and responsive to rehabilitation, others will not. Where a dog displays significant stress when exposed to triggers, which cannot be avoided and either medical management is not feasible, or has not given the desired response, the option for euthanasing the dog, to avoid future suffering, should be available.
Instead of exposing a shelter dog to a sequential list of potentially triggering objects or triggers, to assess how they react, they should all be considered to be “potentially traumatised” and triggering experiences avoided altogether, to protect the welfare of the animal. Triggers may arise naturally during routine care of the dog, for example, reluctance to have their feet touched; fear of men with beards; white vans, similar to the one they were transported to the shelter in, and so on. Identifying these triggers and documenting them on the dog’s behavioural profile, will be valuable information in selecting suitable new caregivers and also for those new caregivers, in both avoidance and desensitisation to those triggers, where avoidance is either impossible or compromising for the welfare of the dog.
If the acts of relinquishment, abandonment, and loss of their caregiver result in a traumatic experience for dogs in shelters, this in combination with any of the other adverse events experienced by rescue dogs means that they should universally be treated with the appropriate TIC approaches and empathy, by both the rescue organisations and those becoming their new caregivers. This empathy and TIC approach might also be applied by companies providing pet insurance, so that despite a traumatic history, they are still eligible for all of the benefits afforded by being fully covered by pet insurance [226].

3.5. Procedures Carried out on Shelter Dogs

Many shelter dogs will require multiple, potentially stressful, procedures to happen as part of their care in the shelter and in preparation for rehoming. This might include vaccination, worming, microchipping, neutering, and grooming to various degrees of difficulty and potential discomfort, etc. These tend to occur right away and in quick succession, so that the dog is “ready to go” as soon as an alternative home has been secured. A TIC approach could be applied at various levels in the management of these procedures. Initially, nothing potentially scary or painful should be performed on the dog until they have had time to recover from their entry to the shelter. Except for acute veterinary pathological conditions, everything else can wai,t and be timed with sensitivity and planning to minimise the negative impact on the dog.
If there is any indication at all that a dog may be euthanased rather than rehabilitated, they should not be challenged unnecessarily. Additional stress from handling, restraint, and potentially painful or scary procedures increases the dog’s physiological stress and increases the risk of them “failing” an assessment of temperament. If microchipping, neutering, etc., can wait, then it should. This will improve the short-term welfare of the dog, improve their chances of “settling in” so they perform well if or when testing occurs, and also, saves the organisation money for procedures or medications “lost” on a dog who is then euthanased.
If sedation or a general anaesthetic is required for neutering, dental work, lump removals, or orthopaedic procedures, as many as possible of the “other” necessary procedures should be carried out while the dog is asleep and unaware (e.g., microchipping, grooming etc.). This reduces the stress for the dogs, caretakers, and veterinary teams.

3.6. Dogs Exhibiting Problematic Behaviours

Incorporating a TIC approach into the treatment protocols of dogs presented with problematic behaviours, whether owned or in a shelter, would be a novel concept which builds on current practices in veterinary behavioural medicine. These generally combine the following: pharmaceutical management of the physiological stress response, both chronic, as in anxiety or acute, in response to exposure to triggers and; behaviour modification techniques. The TIC-focused strategies would focus on treating the animal for underlying anxiety and any unexplained or “over the top” responses to triggers, irrespective of whether there is a plausible explanation for their behaviour, or not.
Current, potential, or new dog caregivers should have TIC and the influence of AEEs explained to them, potentially using visual aids such as the “window of tolerance” [11] so that they understand: why their dog is behaving as it is; how medication may help widen the window of tolerance; how reframing exposure to a particular trigger, through desensitisation and counter-conditioning can help them reduce and/or redirect their stress response and; how relaxation exercises, EMDR, and other stress-reduction techniques can alleviate an acute stress response, improve dog caregiver empowerment, and strengthen the human–dog bond.
In the same manner as the shelter dogs, a TIC approach would consider whether avoidance of “triggering situations” is possible at all, and if not, is it beneficial or detrimental to the dog’s welfare? Forcing exposure in an attempt to desensitise and counter condition needs to be planned carefully, sensitively, and slowly to ensure both dog and caregiver(s) are coping with the treatment plan. This may result in delays until underlying medical issues are resolved, pain management has stabilised, and if anti-anxiety medications are utilised, that therapeutically effective blood titres and the desired anti-anxiety effects are reached, before challenging the dog.

4. Limitations of the TIC Approach

There are two main limitations to the TIC approach: firstly, differentiating stress from trauma and secondly, differentiating traumatised versus non-traumatised canine patients. These are the same arguments that are explored in the human literature, where definitions of stress, crisis, and trauma overlap [227]. Children who have experienced ACEs are less able to discriminate between threatening and non-threatening stimuli in later life, through impaired threat discrimination and deficient reward processing [228].
Individual people and/or animals do not respond to stress in the same way and their response will be determined by numerous factors: personality, temperament, exposure to concurrent stressors, protective factors, adaptability to change, presence of suitable support systems, as well as the intensity and duration of the stressor [229]. Early adverse experience will however have a long-term impact on the development of a number of these variable factors in response to stress, which is any stimulus, internal state, situation, or event with an observable individual reaction, usually in the form of positively or negatively adapting to a new or different situation in one’s environment [230].
If we accept that an individual’s response to a particular stressor or trigger is unique, and determined by a range of factors, almost of all of which are outside of the individual’s control, then differentiating between stress and a life-threatening trauma becomes less important. If the individual perceives a situation to be dangerous or a predictor of a dangerous situation to come, it is no less real for that individual, than a truly life-threatening situation or event. Empathetic management of the individual is the same, whether the threat is real or imagined and as such, use of a TIC approach is still valid.
Differentiating between traumatised and non-traumatised individuals becomes important when “over diagnosis” masks or prevents treatment of a concurrent aetiology or pathological state, therefore compromising the welfare of the individual overall. The risk of this can be minimised by ensuring that physical parameters are tested either before or during psychological assessment to ensure that any underlying medical conditions, particularly those associated with pain, are identified, or ruled out. This provides further evidence for the need to consider veterinary behavioural patients holistically, using a “whole animal approach” and ensuring that underlying veterinary pathology is addressed first, to maximise the potential for behavioural management to prove helpful for the patient.
Equally, one might ask whether the differentiation between traumatised or non-traumatised individuals is the important question, or rather, should it be differentiating between dogs who appear able to “cope with” their environment (and the triggers contained within it) versus those who “appear to struggle to cope with” their environment [231]. Is the previous trauma the important factor, or the ability to cope with stress? If we accept that the combination of genetics, experience, epigenetics, developed resilience, and physiological state at the time of exposure, all influence a dog’s ability to cope with stress, use of a TIC approach, with recognition, response and avoidance of re-traumatising still has scope to improve the situation for the dog, irrespective of their behavioural history.
Similarly, it is believed that many veterinary behavioural patients experience chronic pain, to varying degrees of severity. This is often overlooked, because it is difficult to conduct a meaningful pain assessment on an overtly stressed animal and use of sedation or anaesthesia limits the ability to assess the animal’s response to palpation or manipulation of painful areas of the body [163]. Mills et al. (2020) concluded that “in general, it is better for veterinarians to treat suspected pain first, rather than considering its significance only when the animal does not respond to behaviour therapy”. If it is accepted that trauma is diverse its source, its severity, and the resultant effects on the individual experiencing it, then it could similarly be argued that treating a veterinary behavioural patient utilising a TIC approach routinely would be more beneficial for more dogs, than the few cases where it can categorically be proved that the individual has never been exposed to trauma at all.

5. Conclusions and Animal Welfare Implications

Use of a trauma-informed care approach in the management of dogs in rescue shelter scenarios or presented for veterinary behavioural treatment, as either companion or working dogs, is a concept worthy of consideration and more investigation. The benefits of coaching new or potential dog caregivers on the importance of minimising early adverse experiences in puppies would help support the need for responsible breeding practices and careful management of both the sale and transport of young dogs. The management of stray or relinquished dogs would benefit from implementation of TIC to reduce the risk of re-traumatisation and aid in the counselling of both relinquishing dog caregivers, as well as those hoping to adopt a rescue dog, to better understand why dogs sometimes do the things that they do. Over-utilisation of the TIC approach must be a consideration, but where comprehensive physical veterinary diagnostics are used, and the behavioural assessment is carried out by a suitably trained and experienced individual, the risks posed by utilising TIC can be minimised.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.L.C. and S.E.D.; methodology, C.L.C. and S.M.; original draft, C.L.C.; review and editing S.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

No data collection or storage was involved in this study.

Acknowledgments

Susan Dawson’s expertise was crucial in giving legitimacy to the idea for this paper and she skillfully bridged the gap between human paediatric and veterinary psychology. The authors gratefully acknowledge Andrew Dawson, who gave permission for this project to carry on, despite his wife Sue’s death in December 2022. Gary Landsberg, Kersti Seksel, and Daniel Mills are also gratefully acknowledged for their support and advice in the writing of this paper.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Mind. What is Trauma?—Mind. Available online: https://www.mind.org.uk/information-support/types-of-mental-health-problems/trauma/about-trauma/ (accessed on 21 November 2023).
  2. Goddard, A. Adverse Childhood Experiences and Trauma-Informed Care. J. Pediatr. Health Care 2021, 35, 145–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Wathen, C.N.; Varcoe, C. (Eds.) Implementing Trauma-and Violence-Informed Care: A Handbook; University of Toronto Press: Toronto, ON, Canada, 2023; ISBN 978-1487529260. [Google Scholar]
  4. Felitti, V.J.; Anda, R.F.; Nordenberg, D.; Williamson, D.F.; Spitz, A.M.; Edwards, V.; Marks, J.S. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults: The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. Am. J. Prev. Med. 1998, 14, 245–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Harris, M.E.; Fallot, R.D. Using Trauma Theory to Design Service Systems; Jossey-Bass/Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  6. Levenson, J.S.; Willis, G.M.; Prescott, D. Incorporating principles of trauma-informed care into evidence-based sex offending treatment. In New Frontiers in Offender Treatment: The Translation of Evidence-Based Practices to Correctional Settings; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2018; pp. 171–188. [Google Scholar]
  7. Van der Kolk, B. The Body Keeps the Score: Brain, Mind, and Body in the Healing of Trauma; Viking Press: New York, NY, USA, 2014; ISBN 978-0670785933. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bath, H. The three pillars of traumawise care: Healing in the other 23 hours1. Reclaiming Child. Youth 2015, 23, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Brendtro, L.K.; Mitchell, M.L. Powerful outcomes: Delivering what works. Reclaiming Child. Youth 2014, 22, 5. [Google Scholar]
  10. Bath, H. The three pillars of trauma-informed care. Reclaiming Child. Youth 2008, 17, 17–21. [Google Scholar]
  11. Corrigan, F.; Fisher, J.; Nutt, D. Autonomic dysregulation and the Window of Tolerance model of the effects of complex emotional trauma. J. Psychopharmacol. 2011, 25, 17–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Ogden, P.; Minton, K.; Pain, C. Trauma and the Body: A Sensorimotor Approach to Psychotherapy; Norton: New York, NY, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  13. Scarlett, J.M.; Salman, M.D.; New, J.G., Jr.; Kass, P.H. Reasons for relinquishment of companion animals in US animal shelters: Selected health and personal issues. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 1999, 2, 41–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Patronek, G.J.; Glickman, L.T.; Beck, A.M.; McCabe, G.P.; Ecker, C. Risk factors for relinquishment of dogs to an animal shelter. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 1996, 209, 572–581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Salman, M.D.; Hutchison, J.; Ruch-Gallie, R.; Kogan, L.; New, J.C., Jr.; Kass, P.H.; Scarlett, J.M. Behavioral reasons for relinquishment of dogs and cats to 12 shelters. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2000, 3, 93–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Shore, E.R. Returning a recently adopted companion animal: Adopters’ reasons for and reactions to the failed adoption experience. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2005, 8, 187–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Corridan, C.L.; Mills, D.S.; Pfeffer, K. Comparison of the prevalence of behavioural problems in samples of owned and relinquished companion dog populations in the UK. In Proceedings of the BSAVA Congress 2010: Scientific Proceedings: Veterinary Programme, Birmingham, UK, 8 April 2010; BSAVA Publications: Gloucester, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  18. Diesel, G.; Brodbelt, D.; Pfeiffer, D.U. Characteristics of relinquished dogs and their owners at 14 rehoming centers in the United Kingdom. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2010, 13, 15–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Fighera, R.A.; Souza, T.M.; Silva, M.C.; Brum, J.S.; Graça, D.L.; Kommers, G.D.; Irigoyen, L.F.; Barros, C.S. Causes of death and reasons for euthanasia in dogs from the midland region of the Midwest of Rio Grande do Sul State, Brazil (1965–2004). Pesqui. Veterinária Bras. 2008, 28, 223–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Guy, N.; Luescher, U.; Dohoo, S.; Spangler, E.; Miller, J.; Dohoo, I.; Bate, L. A case series of biting dogs: Characteristics of the dogs, their behaviour, their victims. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2001, 74, 43–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Czerwinski, V.H.; Smith, B.P.; Hynd, P.I.; Hazel, S.J. The influence of maternal care on stress-related behaviors in domestic dogs: What can we learn from the rodent literature? J. Vet. Behav. 2016, 14, 52–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Fatjó, J.; Ruiz-De-La-Torre, J.; Manteca, X. The epidemiology of behavioural problems in dogs and cats: A survey of veterinary practitioners. Anim. Welf. 2006, 15, 179–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Bamberger, M.; Houpt, K.A. Signalment factors, comorbidity, and trends in behavior diagnoses in dogs: 1,644 cases (1991–2001). J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 2006, 229, 1591–1601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Col, R.; Day, C.; Phillips, C.J. An epidemiological analysis of dog behavior problems presented to an Australian behavior clinic, with associated risk factors. J. Vet. Behav. 2016, 15, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Dinwoodie, I.R.; Dwyer, B.; Zottola, V.; Gleason, D.; Dodman, N.H. Demographics and comorbidity of behavior problems in dogs. J. Vet. Behav. 2019, 32, 62–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Rhudy, J.L.; Meagher, M.W. Fear and anxiety: Divergent effects on human pain thresholds. Pain 2000, 84, 65–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Overall, K.L.; Dunham, A.E.; Juarbe-Diaz, S.V. Phenotypic determination of noise reactivity in 3 breeds of working dogs: A cautionary tale of age, breed, behavioral assessment, and genetics. J. Vet. Behav. 2016, 16, 113–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Arvelius, P. Genetic Evaluation of Behaviour in Dogs. Ph.D. Thesis, Department of Animal Breeding and Genetics, Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences, Uppsala, Sweden, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  29. Ilska, J.; Haskell, M.J.; Blott, S.C.; Sánchez-Molano, E.; Polgar, Z.; Lofgren, S.E.; Clements, D.N.; Wiener, P. Genetic characterization of dog personality traits. Genetics 2017, 206, 1101–1111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Rosado, I.R.; Da Silva, J.G.; De Freitas, D.M.; Dos Reis, A.S.; Picelli, J.P.; Alves, E.G.L.; Martins, I. Compulsive Disorder in Border Collie Bitch. Acta Sci. Vet. 2019, 47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Tiira, K.; Lohi, H. Early life experiences and exercise associate with canine anxieties. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0141907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Bennett, P.C.; Rohlf, V.I. Owner-companion dog interactions: Relationships between demographic variables, potentially problematic behaviours, training engagement and shared activities. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 102, 65–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Edwards, P.T.; Hazel, S.J.; Browne, M.; Serpell, J.A.; McArthur, M.L.; Smith, B.P. Investigating risk factors that predict a dog’s fear during veterinary consultations. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0215416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Sanders, A.R.R.; Bhongir, N.; vonHoldt, B.; Pellegrini, M. Association of DNA methylation with energy and fear-related behaviors in canines. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 1025494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Hughes, B.O.; Duncan, I.J.H. The notion of ethological ‘need’, models of motivation and animal welfare. Anim. Behav. 1988, 36, 1696–1707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Kleiman, D.G. Canid mating systems, social behavior, parental care and ontogeny: Are they flexible? Behav. Genet. 2011, 41, 803–809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Tiira, K. Resilience In Dogs? Lessons From Other Species. Vet.Med. Res. Rep. 2019, 10, 159–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Kutzler, M. Puppy Behavior, Socialization, and Personality. Adv. Small Anim. Med. Surg. 2014, 27, 1–3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. McMillan, T.; Spaulding, K. Handling Shelter Dogs. In Animal Behavior for Shelter Veterinarians and Staff; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2022; pp. 177–204. [Google Scholar]
  40. Bucci, M.; Marques, S.S.; Oh, D.; Harris, N.B. Toxic stress in children and adolescents. Adv. Pediatr. 2016, 63, 403–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. McEwen, B.S. Stress and hippocampal plasticity. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 1999, 22, 105–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Appleby, D.; Pluijmakers, J. Separation anxiety in dogs: The function of homeostasis in its development and treatment. Vet. Clin. Small Anim. Pract. 2003, 33, 321–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Bhatia, V.; Tandon, R.K. Stress and the gastrointestinal tract. J. Gastroenterol. Hepatol. 2005, 20, 332–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Chen, A. (Ed.) Stress Resilience: Molecular and Behavioral Aspects; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  45. Berghänel, A.; Heistermann, M.; Schülke, O.; Ostner, J. Prenatal stress accelerates offspring growth to compensate for reduced maternal investment across mammals. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2017, 114, E10658–E10666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Clarke, M.; Brannigan, R.; Tanskanen, A.; Huttunen, M.; Leacy, F.; Cannon, M. O6. 3. Evidence for an Association between Maternal Stress during Pregnancy and the Later Development of Psychiatric Disorders in the Exposed Offspring. Schizophr. Bull. 2019, 45 (Suppl. S2), S176–S177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Wong, B.K.Y.; Murry, J.B.; Ramakrishnan, R.; He, F.; Balasa, A.; Stinnett, G.R.; Pedersen, S.E.; Pautler, R.G.; Van den Veyver, I.B. Maternal stress in Shank3ex4-9 mice increases pup-directed care and alters brain white matter in male offspring. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0224876. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Sanchez-Carrion, R.; Gomez, P.V.; Junque, C.; Fernández-Espejo, D.; Falcon, C.; Bargallo, N.; Roig-Rovira, T.; Enseñat-Cantallops, A.; Bernabeu, M. Frontal hypoactivation on functional magnetic resonance imaging in working memory after severe diffuse traumatic brain injury. J. Neurotrauma 2008, 25, 479–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. McEwen, B.S. The neurobiology and neuroendocrinology of stress: Implications for post-traumatic stress disorder from a basic science perspective. Psychiatr. Clin. 2002, 25, 469–494. [Google Scholar]
  50. Brunson, K.L.; Avishai-Eliner, S.; Hatalski, C.G.; Baram, T.Z. Neurobiology of the stress response early in life: Evolution of a concept and the role of corticotropin releasing hormone. Mol. Psychiatry 2001, 6, 647–656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Anda, R.F.; Croft, J.B.; Felitti, V.J.; Nordenberg, D.; Giles, W.H.; Williamson, D.F.; Giovino, G.A. Adverse childhood experiences and smoking during adolescence and adulthood. JAMA 1999, 282, 1652–1658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Jones, C.M.; Merrick, M.T.; Houry, D.E. Identifying and preventing adverse childhood experiences: Implications for clinical practice. JAMA 2020, 323, 25–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Goddard, A.; Janicek, E.; Etcher, L. Trauma-informed care for the pediatric nurse. J. Pediatr. Nurs. 2022, 62, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Wadhwa, P.D.; Culhane, J.F.; Rauh, V.; Barve, S.S.; Hogan, V.; Sandman, C.A.; Hobel, C.J.; Chicz-DeMet, A.; Dunkel-Schetter, C.; Garite, T.J.; et al. Stress, infection and preterm birth: A biobehavioural perspective. Paediatr. Perinat. Epidemiol. 2001, 15, 17–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Conradt, E.; Lester, B.M.; Appleton, A.A.; Armstrong, D.A.; Marsit, C.J. The roles of DNA methylation of NR3C1 and 11β-HSD2 and exposure to maternal mood disorder in utero on newborn neurobehavior. Epigenetics 2013, 8, 1321–1329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Sandman, C.A.; Davis, E.P.; Buss, C.; Glynn, L.M. Exposure to prenatal psychobiological stress exerts programming influences on the mother and her fetus. Neuroendocrinology 2012, 95, 8–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Glover, V. Annual research review: Prenatal stress and the origins of psychopathology: An evolutionary perspective. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2011, 52, 356–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Davies, P.T.; Sturge-Apple, M.L.; Cicchetti, D.; Cummings, E.M. The role of child adrenocortical functioning in pathways between interparental conflict and child maladjustment. Dev. Psychol. 2007, 43, 918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Austin, M.-P.; Hadzi-Pavlovic, D.; Leader, L.; Saint, K.; Parker, G. Maternal trait anxiety, depression and life event stress in pregnancy: Relationships with infant temperament. Early Hum. Dev. 2005, 81, 183–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Blair, C.; Ursache, A.; Mills-Koonce, R.; Stifter, C.; Voegtline, K.; Granger, D.A. Emotional reactivity and parenting sensitivity interact to predict cortisol output in toddlers. Dev. Psychol. 2015, 51, 1271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Zhang, W.; Rajendran, K.; Ham, J.; Finik, J.; Buthmann, J.; Davey, K.; Pehme, P.M.; Dana, K.; Pritchett, A.; Laws, H.; et al. Prenatal exposure to disaster-related traumatic stress and developmental trajectories of temperament in early childhood: Superstorm Sandy pregnancy study. J. Affect. Disord. 2018, 234, 335–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Werner, K.H.; Roberts, N.A.; Rosen, H.J.; Dean, D.L.; Kramer, J.H.; Weiner, M.W.; Miller, B.L.; Levenson, R.W. Emotional reactivity and emotion recognition in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neurology 2007, 69, 148–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. De Pauw, S.S.; Mervielde, I. Temperament, personality and developmental psychopathology: A review based on the conceptual dimensions underlying childhood traits. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2010, 41, 313–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Musser, E.D.; Galloway-Long, H.S.; Frick, P.J.; Nigg, J.T. Emotion regulation and heterogeneity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2013, 52, 163–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Pagliaccio, D.; Luby, J.L.; Bogdan, R.; Agrawal, A.; Gaffrey, M.S.; Belden, A.C.; Botteron, K.N.; Harms, M.P.; Barch, D.M. Amygdala functional connectivity, HPA axis genetic variation, and life stress in children and relations to anxiety and emotion regulation. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2015, 124, 817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Guyon-Harris, K.L.; Humphreys, K.L.; Zeanah, C.H. Adverse caregiving in early life: The trauma and deprivation distinction in young children. Infant Ment. Health J. 2021, 42, 87–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Tam, E.W.; Chau, V.; Ferriero, D.M.; Barkovich, A.J.; Poskitt, K.J.; Studholme, C.; Fok, E.D.; Grunau, R.E.; Glidden, D.V.; Miller, S.P. Preterm cerebellar growth impairment after postnatal exposure to glucocorticoids. Sci. Transl. Med. 2011, 3, 105ra105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Amugongo, S.K.; Hlusko, L.J. Impact of maternal prenatal stress on growth of the offspring. Aging Dis. 2014, 5, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. O’Donnell, K.J.; Meaney, M.J. Fetal origins of mental health: The developmental origins of health and disease hypothesis. Am. J. Psychiatry 2017, 174, 319–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Anderson, R.M.; Johnson, S.B.; Lingg, R.T.; Hinz, D.C.; Romig-Martin, S.A.; Radley, J.J. Evidence for similar prefrontal structural and functional alterations in male and female rats following chronic stress or glucocorticoid exposure. Cereb. Cortex 2020, 30, 353–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Green, B.L.; Saunders, P.A.; Power, E.; Dass-Brailsford, P.; Schelbert, K.B.; Giller, E.; Wissow, L.; Hurtado de Mendoza, A.; Mete, M. Trauma-informed medical care: Patient response to a primary care provider communication training. J. Loss Trauma 2016, 21, 147–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Schofield, I.; Brodbelt, D.C.; Niessen, S.J.; Church, D.B.; Geddes, R.F.; O’Neill, D.G. Frequency and risk factors for naturally occurring Cushing’s syndrome in dogs attending UK primary-care practices. J. Small Anim. Pract. 2022, 63, 265–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Notari, L.; Burman, O.; Mills, D. Behavioural changes in dogs treated with corticosteroids. Physiol. Behav. 2015, 151, 609–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Huizink, A.; Menting, B.; De Moor, M.; Verhage, M.L.; Kunseler, F.; Schuengel, C.; Oosterman, M. From prenatal anxiety to parenting stress: A longitudinal study. Arch. Women’s Ment. Health 2017, 20, 663–672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Santos, N.R.; Beck, A.; Fontbonne, A. A review of maternal behaviour in dogs and potential areas for further research. J. Small Anim. Pract. 2020, 61, 85–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Santos, N.R.; Beck, A.; Maenhoudt, C.; Billy, C.; Fontbonne, A. Profile of Dogs’ Breeders and Their Considerations on Female Reproduction, Maternal Care and the Peripartum Stress—An International Survey. Animals 2021, 11, 2372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Overall, K.L. Interventions for Early Puppy and Kitten Trauma and Neglect. Adv. Small Anim. Med. Surg. 2017, 30, 1–3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Wauthier, L.M.; Williams, J.M. Using the mini C-BARQ to investigate the effects of puppy farming on dog behaviour. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2018, 206, 75–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Sundman, A.-S.; Van Poucke, E.; Holm, A.-C.S.; Faresjö, Å.; Theodorsson, E.; Jensen, P.; Roth, L.S. Long-term stress levels are synchronized in dogs and their owners. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 7391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Gazzano, A.; Mariti, C.; Notari, L.; Sighieri, C.; McBride, E.A. Effects of early gentling and early environment on emotional development of puppies. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 110, 294–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Noonan, G.; Rand, J.; Blackshaw, J.; Priest, J. Behavioural observations of puppies undergoing tail docking. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1996, 49, 335–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Mills, K.E.; von Keyserlingk, M.A.G.; Niel, L. A review of medically unnecessary surgeries in dogs and cats. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 2016, 248, 162–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Laurence, C. Concurrent tail docking and microchipping in puppies. Vet. Rec. 2007, 160, 347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. McMillan, F.D. Behavioral and psychological outcomes for dogs sold as puppies through pet stores and/or born in commercial breeding establishments: Current knowledge and putative causes. J. Vet. Behav. 2017, 19, 14–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Yamamoto, T. An Unusual Behavior and Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD)-like Syndrome in Dogs After the Vigorous Earthquake with Seismic Scale of 5+ Degree. J. Vet. Med. 2003, 984, 535–541. [Google Scholar]
  86. Maier, S.F.; Albin, R.W.; Testa, T.J. Failure to learn to escape in rats previously exposed to inescapable shock depends on nature of escape response. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1973, 85, 581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Hollis, K.L. Contemporary research on Pavlovian conditioning: A new functional analysis. Am. Psychol. 1997, 52, 956. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Brennan, F.X.; Beck, K.D.; Ross, R.J.; Servatius, R.J. Stress-induced increases in avoidance responding: An animal model of post-traumatic stress disorder behavior? Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2005, 1, 69–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Osborne, F.H.; Mattingly, B.A.; Redmon, W.K.; Osborne, J.S. Factors affecting the measurement of classically conditioned fear in rats following exposure to escapable versus inescapable signaled shock. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 1975, 1, 364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Baker, A.G. Learned irrelevance and learned helplessness: Rats learn that stimuli, reinforcers, responses are uncorrelated. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 1976, 2, 130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Fanselow, M.S. Signaled shock-free periods and preference for signaled shock. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 1980, 6, 65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Rush, B.F., Jr. Irreversibility in the post-transfusion phase of hemorrhagic shock. In The Fundamental Mechanisms of Shock, Proceedings of a Symposium, Oklahoma City, OK, USA, 1–2 October 1971; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 1972; pp. 215–234. [Google Scholar]
  93. Mineka, S.; Cook, M.; Miller, S. Fear conditioned with escapable and inescapable shock: Effects of a feedback stimulus. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 1984, 10, 307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Abbott, B.B.; Schoen, L.S.; Badia, P. Predictable and unpredictable shock: Behavioral measures of aversion and physiological measures of stress. Psychol. Bull. 1984, 96, 45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Maier, S.F.; Keith, J.R. Shock signals and the development of stress-induced analgesia. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 1987, 13, 226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Foa, E.B.; Zinbarg, R.; Rothbaum, B.O. Uncontrollability and unpredictability in post-traumatic stress disorder: An animal model. Psychol. Bull. 1992, 112, 218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Roemer, L.; Litz, B.T.; Orsillo, S.M.; Wagner, A.W. A preliminary investigation of the role of strategic withholding of emotions in PTSD. J. Trauma. Stress 2001, 14, 149–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Lagutchik, M.; Baker, J.; Balser, J.; Burghardt, W.; Enroth, M.; Flournoy, S.; Giles, J.; Grimm, P.; Hiniker, J.; Johnson, J.; et al. Trauma management of military working dogs. Mil. Med. 2018, 183, 180–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Patt, M.W.; Oh, J.T.; Branstetter, J.G.; Benavides, J.M. Overuse Injury in Military Working Dog Handlers: A Pattern of Behavior and a Path to Prevention. Mil. Med. 2023, 188, e1338–e1340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Salden, S.; Wijnants, J.; Baeken, C.; Saunders, J.H.; De Keuster, T. Disaster Response and its Aftermath: A Systematic Review of the Impact of Disaster Deployment on Working Dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2023, 265, 105987. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Cobb, M.L.; Otto, C.M.; Fine, A.H. The animal welfare science of working dogs: Current perspectives on recent advances and future directions. Front. Vet. Sci. 2021, 8, 1116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Van Leeuwen, E.J.C.; Bruinstroop, B.M.C.; Haun, D.B.M. Early Trauma Leaves No Social Signature in Sanctuary-Housed Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Animals 2022, 13, 49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Ferdowsian, H.R.; Durham, D.L.; Kimwele, C.; Kranendonk, G.; Otali, E.; Akugizibwe, T.; Mulcahy, J.B.; Ajarova, L.; Johnson, C.M. Signs of mood and anxiety disorders in chimpanzees. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e19855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Bradshaw, G.A.; Capaldo, T.; Lindner, L.; Grow, G. Building an inner sanctuary: Complex PTSD in chimpanzees. J. Trauma Dissociation 2008, 9, 9–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Hunt, M.; Otto, C.M.; Serpell, J.A.; Alvarez, J. Interactions between handler well-being and canine health and behavior in search and rescue teams. Anthrozoös 2012, 25, 323–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. de Santana, N.G.; Malm, C.; Maia, M.Z.; Megda, T.; Beier, S.L.; Mamão, L.D.; Franco, T.C. Evaluation of Post-Operative Pain and Stress in Dogs After Elective Ovariohysterectomy Under Hospitalization. Braz. J. Vet. Res. Anim. Sci. 2020, 57, e162908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Stellato, A.C.; Flint, H.E.; Widowski, T.M.; Serpell, J.A.; Niel, L. Assessment of Fear-Related Behaviours Displayed by Companion Dogs (Canis Familiaris) in Response to Social and Non-Social Stimuli. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017, 188, 84–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Squair, C. Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD) in a Teaching Laboratory Beagle: Presentation, Relative Contributions, and Treatment. J. Vet. Behav. 2023, 64–65, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Overall, K.L. Crate Training can do Harm to Dogs with Panic Disorders. DVM 1998, 29, 10S. [Google Scholar]
  110. Alupo, C. Canine PTSD. Master’s Thesis, SLU Skara, Skara, Sweden, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  111. Riemer, S.; Heritier, C.; Windschnurer, I.; Pratsch, L.; Arhant, C.; Affenzeller, N. A Review on Mitigating Fear and Aggression in Dogs and Cats in a Veterinary Setting. Animals 2021, 11, 158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. McCobb, E.; Brown, E.; Damiani, K.; Dodman, N. Thunderstorm Phobia in Dogs: An Internet Survey of 69 Cases. J. Am. Anim. Hosp. Assoc. 2001, 37, 319–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Scheifele, P.M.; Sonstrom, K.E.; Dunham, A.E.; Overall, K.L. Is noise reactivity reflected in auditory response variables, including those that measure cognition, in dogs? Initial findings. J. Vet. Behav. 2016, 16, 65–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Horwitz, D.; Mills, D. BSAVA Manual of Canine and Feline Behavioural Medicine; University of Lincoln: Lincoln, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  115. Overall, K. Manual of Clinical Behavioral Medicine for Dogs and Cats-E-Book; Elsevier Health Sciences: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  116. Landsberg, G.; Radosta, L.; Ackerman, L. Behavior Problems of the Dog and Cat, 4th ed.; Elsevier: St. Louis, MO, USA, 2024. [Google Scholar]
  117. Daniels, J.T.; Busby, D.; Chase-Topping, M.; Brown, S.M. I Wish he’d Listen: Client-Centered Interviewing Approaches are Associated with Higher Compliance with Behavioral Modification Advice in Pet Dog Owners. J. Vet. Behav. 2023, 63, 22–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Diesel, G.; Brodbelt, D.; Pfeiffer, D.U. Reliability of Assessment of Dogs’ Behavioural Responses by Staff Working at a Welfare Charity in the UK. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 115, 171–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Fischer, K.R.; Bakes, K.M.; Corbin, T.J.; Fein, J.A.; Harris, E.J.; James, T.L.; Melzer-Lange, M.D. Trauma-informed care for violently injured patients in the emergency department. Ann. Emerg. Med. 2019, 73, 193–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Munro, H.M.C.; Thrusfield, M.V. ‘Battered Pets’: Features that Raise Suspicion of Non-Accidental Injury. J. Small Anim. Pract. 2001, 42, 218–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Munro, H.M.C.; Thrusfield, M.V. ‘Battered Pets’: Munchausen Syndrome by Proxy (Factitious Illness by Proxy). J. Small Anim. Pract. 2001, 42, 385–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Arluke, A. Bystander Apathy in Animal Abuse Cases: Exploring Barriers to Child and Adolescent Intervention. Anthrozoös 2012, 25, 5–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Almeida, D.C.; Torres, S.M.F.; Wuenschmann, A. Retrospective Analysis of Necropsy Reports Suggestive of Abuse in Dogs and Cats. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 2018, 252, 433–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Luke, C.; Arluke, A. Physical cruelty toward animals in Massachusetts, 1975–1996. Soc. Anim. 1997, 5, 195–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Morais, H.R.; Março, K.S.; Bregano, L.C.; Grassi, T.L.M.; Seraguci, T.F.; Jussiani, G.; de Melo, G.D.; Cipriano, R.; Ponsano, E.H.G.; Machado, G.F. Bone marrow fat, visceral fat, and body conditions as parameters of possible neglect in dogs with leishmaniasis. Forensic Sci. Int. Anim. Environ. 2022, 2, 100049. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Munro, H.M.C.; Thrusfield, M.V. ‘Battered pets’: Sexual abuse. J. Small Anim. Pract. 2001, 42, 333–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Tong, L.J. Fracture characteristics to distinguish between accidental injury and non-accidental injury in dogs. Vet. J. 2014, 199, 392–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Juodžentė, D.; Karvelienė, B.; Riškevičienė, V. The influence of the duration of the preoperative time spent in the veterinary clinic without the owner on the psychogenic and oxidative stress in dogs. J. Vet. Med. Sci. 2018, 80, 1129–1133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Lefman, S.H.; Prittie, J.E. Psychogenic stress in hospitalized veterinary patients: Causation, implications, and therapies. J. Vet. Emerg. Crit. Care 2019, 29, 107–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Learn, A.; Landsberg, G. Reducing fear, anxiety, and stress in veterinary clinics. In Behavior Problems of the Dog and Cat-E-Book; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2023; 269p. [Google Scholar]
  131. Doukas, D. Non-accidental injuries in dogs and cats: Review of post-mortem forensic evaluations and the social significance of small animal practice. J. Hell. Vet. Med. Soc. 2022, 73, 3543–3552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  132. Bartelink, E.J.; Clinkinbeard, S.; Spessard, C.; Kilmartin, A.; Spangler, W.L. Documenting non-accidental injury patterns in a dog abuse investigation: A collaborative approach between forensic anthropology and veterinary pathology. J. Forensic Sci. 2022, 67, 756–765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Thompson, O.F.; Wales, D. A qualitative study of experiences, actions and motivations during accidental dwelling fires. Fire Mater. 2015, 39, 453–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  134. Zeleny, M.; Grusova, K. A car accident involving a restrained dog within the vehicle: A case report. Veterinární Medicína 2015, 60, 399–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  135. Warner, G.S. Increased Incidence of Domestic Animal Bites following a Disaster Due to Natural Hazards. Prehospital Disaster Med. 2010, 25, 187–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  136. Surridge, L. Dogs’/Bodies, Women’s Bodies: Wives as Pets in Mid-Nineteenth-Century Narratives of Domestic Violence. Victorian Rev. 1994, 20, 1–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Wood, D.S.; Weber, C.V.; Ascione, F.R. The abuse of animals and domestic violence: A national survey of shelters for women who are battered. Soc. Anim. 1997, 5, 205–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  138. Maiuro, R.D.; Eberle, J.A.; Rastaman, P.; Snowflake, B. Pet abuse: Relationships to psychobiology, attachment processes, and domestic violence. In The International Handbook of Animal Abuse and Cruelty: Theory, Research, and Application; Purdue University: West Lafayette, IN, USA, 2008; pp. 133–153. [Google Scholar]
  139. Fielding, W.J. Domestic violence and dog care in New Providence, The Bahamas. Soc. Anim. 2010, 18, 183–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  140. Tiplady, C.M.; Walsh, D.B.; Phillips, J.C. The ongoing impact of domestic violence on animal welfare. Anim. Stud. J. 2015, 4, 116–139. [Google Scholar]
  141. Rothschild, J.; Maroney, P.; Hitch, D. Evaluating the benefits and challenges of owning a pet dog at a residential Psychiatric rehabilitation facility from the perspective of staff and clients. Occup. Ther. Ment. Health 2019, 35, 92–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  142. Mubanga, M.; Byberg, L.; Egenvall, A.; Ingelsson, E.; Fall, T. Dog ownership and survival after a major cardiovascular event: A register-based prospective study. Circ. Cardiovasc. Qual. Outcomes 2019, 12, e005342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  143. Canady, B.; Sansone, A. Health care decisions and delay of treatment in companion animal owners. J. Clin. Psychol. Med. Settings 2019, 26, 313–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  144. Merkouri, A.; Graham, T.M.; O’Haire, M.E.; Purewal, R.; Westgarth, C. Dogs and the good life: A cross-sectional study of the association between the dog–owner relationship and owner mental wellbeing. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 903647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  145. Williamson, L.E.; Dell, C.A.; Chalmers, D.; Cruz, M.; De Groot, P. Strengthening Zooeyia: Understanding the Human-Animal Bond between Veterans Living with Comorbid Substance Use and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and their Service Dogs. Hum. Anim. Interact. Bull. 2022, 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  146. Salmon, A.J.; Pachana, N.A. A systematic review of the effect of assistance dog ownership on practical support, health, and wellbeing: Implications for older adults. Clin. Gerontol. 2023, 46, 669–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  147. Kerr-Little, A.; Bramness, J.G.; Newberry, R.C.; Biong, S. Exploring dog ownership in the lives of people with substance use disorder: A qualitative study. Addict. Sci. Clin. Pract. 2023, 18, 57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  148. Fallani, G.; Previde, E.P.; Valsecchi, P. Behavioral and physiological responses of guide dogs to a situation of emotional distress. Physiol. Behav. 2007, 90, 648–655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  149. Furst, G. Without words to get in the way: Symbolic interaction in prison-based animal programs. Qual. Sociol. Rev. 2007, 3, 96–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  150. Müller, C.A.; Riemer, S.; Rosam, C.M.; Schößwender, J.; Range, F.; Huber, L. Brief owner absence does not induce negative judgement bias in pet dogs. Anim. Cogn. 2012, 15, 1031–1035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  151. Welch, M. Animal Law. In Veterinary Forensic Medicine and Forensic Sciences; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2020; pp. 435–460. [Google Scholar]
  152. Kaufman, K.R.; Kaufman, N.D. And then dog died. Death Stud. 2006, 30, 61–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  153. Mondelli, F.; Previde, E.P.; Verga, M.; Levi, D.; Magistrelli, S.; Valsecchi, P. The bond that never developed: Adoption and relinquishment of dogs in a rescue shelter. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2004, 7, 253–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  154. Corridan, C. The Role of Dog Owner Expectation and Development of a Successful Human: Dog Bond. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  155. Cardoso, S.D.; Pereira, G.d.G.; de Sousa, L.; Faraco, C.B.; Piotti, P.; Pirrone, F. Factors behind the Relinquishment of Dogs and Cats by their Guardians in Portugal. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2022, 13, 243. [Google Scholar]
  156. Eagan, B.H.; Gordon, E.; Protopopova, A. Reasons for guardian-relinquishment of dogs to shelters: Animal and regional predictors in British Columbia, Canada. Front. Vet. Sci. 2022, 9, 857634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  157. Orihel, J.S.; Fraser, D. A note on the effectiveness of behavioural rehabilitation for reducing inter-dog aggression in shelter dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 112, 400–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  158. Casey, R.A.; Loftus, B.; Bolster, C.; Richards, G.J.; Blackwell, E.J. Inter-dog aggression in a UK owner survey: Prevalence, co-occurrence in different contexts and risk factors. Vet. Rec. 2013, 172, 127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  159. Flint, H.E.; Coe, J.B.; Serpell, J.A.; Pearl, D.L.; Niel, L. Identification of fear behaviors shown by puppies in response to non- social stimuli. J. Vet. Behav. 2018, 28, 17–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  160. Wormald, D.; Lawrence, A.J.; Carter, G.; Fisher, A.D. Physiological stress coping and anxiety in greyhounds displaying inter-dog aggression. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 180, 93–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  161. Hughes, K.; Bellis, M.A.; Hardcastle, K.A.; Sethi, D.; Butchart, A.; Mikton, C.; Jones, L.; Dunne, M.P. The effect of multiple adverse childhood experiences on health: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Public Health 2017, 2, e356–e366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  162. Fagundes, A.L.L.; Hewison, L.; McPeake, K.J.; Zulch, H.; Mills, D.S. Noise sensitivities in dogs: An exploration of signs in dogs with and without musculoskeletal pain using qualitative content analysis. Front. Vet. Sci. 2018, 5, 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  163. Mills, D.S.; Demontigny-Bédard, I.; Gruen, M.; Klinck, M.P.; McPeake, K.J.; Barcelos, A.M.; Hewison, L.; Van Haevermaet, H.; Denenberg, S.; Hauser, H.; et al. Pain and problem behavior in cats and dogs. Animals 2020, 10, 318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  164. Cameron, D.B. Differing views on the cause of aggression in dogs. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 2009, 234, 39–40. [Google Scholar]
  165. Schilder, M.B.H.; van der Borg, J.A.M.; Vinke, C.M. Intraspecific killing in dogs: Predation behavior or aggression? A study of aggressors, victims, possible causes, and motivations. J. Vet. Behav. 2019, 34, 52–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  166. Wallis, L.J.; Szabó, D.; Kubinyi, E. Cross-Sectional Age Differences in Canine Personality Traits; Influence of Breed, Sex, Previous Trauma, and Dog Obedience Tasks. Front. Vet. Sci. 2020, 6, 493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  167. Baslington-Davies, A.; Howell, H.; Hogue, T.E.; Mills, D.S. An assessment of scientific evidence relating to the effect of early experience on the risk of human-directed aggression by adult dogs. Animals 2023, 13, 2329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  168. Sherman, B.L.; Mills, D.S. Canine anxieties and phobias: An update on separation anxiety and noise aversions. Vet. Clin. North Am. Small Anim. Pract. 2008, 38, 1081–1106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  169. Dreschel, N.A. The effects of fear and anxiety on health and lifespan in pet dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2010, 125, 157–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  170. Rooney, N.J.; Clark, C.C.; Casey, R.A. Minimizing fear and anxiety in working dogs: A review. J. Vet. Behav. 2016, 16, 53–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  171. Vas, J.; Topál, J.; Péch, E.; Miklósi, A. Measuring attention deficit and activity in dogs: A new application and validation of a human ADHD questionnaire. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 103, 105–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  172. Hoppe, N.; Bininda-Emonds, O.R.P.; Gansloßer, U. Correlates of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)-like behavior in domestic dogs: First results from a questionnaire-based study. Vet. Med. Open J. 2017, 2, 95–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  173. Sulkama, S.; Puurunen, J.; Salonen, M.; Mikkola, S.; Hakanen, E.; Araujo, C.; Lohi, H. Canine hyperactivity, impulsivity, and inattention share similar demographic risk factors and behavioural comorbidities with human ADHD. Transl. Psychiatry 2021, 11, 501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  174. González-Martínez, Á.; Muñiz de Miguel, S.; Graña, N.; Costas, X.; Diéguez, F.J. Serotonin and Dopamine Blood Levels in ADHD-Like Dogs. Animals 2023, 13, 1037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  175. Black, A.H. Heart rate changes during avoidance learning in dogs. Can. J. Psychol. Rev. Can. Psychol. 1959, 13, 229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  176. Seligman, M.E.P. Learned helplessness. Annu. Rev. Med. 1972, 23, 407–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  177. Appleby, D.L.; Bradshaw, J.W.S.; Casey, R.A. Relationship between aggressive and avoidance behaviour by dogs and their experience in the first six months of life. Vet. Rec. 2002, 150, 434–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  178. Morrow, M.; Ottobre, J.; Ottobre, A.; Neville, P.; St-Pierre, N.; Dreschel, N.; Pate, J.L. Breed-dependent differences in the onset of fear-related avoidance behavior in puppies. J. Vet. Behav. 2015, 10, 286–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  179. Luna, D.; Carrasco, C.; Álvarez, D.; González, C.; Egaña, J.I.; Figueroa, J. Exploring Anhedonia in Kennelled Dogs: Could Coping Styles Affect Hedonic Preferences for Sweet and Umami Flavours? Animals 2020, 10, 2087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  180. Harvey, N.D.; Moesta, A.; Kappel, S.; Wongsaengchan, C.; Harris, H.; Craigon, P.J.; Fureix, C. Could greater time spent displaying waking inactivity in the home environment be a marker for a depression-like state in the domestic dog? Animals 2019, 9, 420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  181. Alvarez, R.D.; Figueroa, H.J. Anhedonia. Effect of stress on eating behaviour and its possible implication in domestic dogs. Av. En. Cienc. Vet. 2015, 30, 38–53. [Google Scholar]
  182. Edwards, P.T.; Smith, B.P.; McArthur, M.L.; Hazel, S.J. Fearful Fido: Investigating dog experience in the veterinary context in an effort to reduce distress. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2019, 213, 14–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  183. Collins, K.; Miller, K.; Zverina, L.; Patterson-Kane, E.; Cussen, V.; Reid, P. Behavioral rehabilitation of extremely fearful dogs: Report on the efficacy of a treatment protocol. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2022, 254, 105689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  184. Haq, A.U. Canine psychiatry: Addressing animal psycho-pathologies. Behaviour 2017, 6, 1126–1128. [Google Scholar]
  185. Fuller, J.L.; Gillum, E. A study of factors influencing performance of dogs on a delayed response test. Pedagog. Semin. J. Genet. Psychol. 1950, 76, 241–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  186. Wright, J.C.; Nesselrote, M.S. Classification of behavior problems in dogs: Distributions of age, breed, sex and reproductive status. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1987, 19, 169–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  187. Sforzini, E.; Michelazzi, M.; Spada, E.; Ricci, C.; Carenzi, C.; Milani, S.; Luzi, F.; Verga, M. Evaluation of young and adult dogs’ reactivity. J. Vet. Behav. 2009, 4, 3–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  188. Gácsi, M.; Maros, K.; Sernkvist, S.; Faragó, T.; Miklósi, Á. Human analogue safe haven effect of the owner: Behavioural and heart rate response to stressful social stimuli in dogs. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e58475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  189. Siniscalchi, M.; McFarlane, J.; Kauter, K.; Quaranta, A.; Rogers, L. Cortisol levels in hair reflect behavioural reactivity of dogs to acoustic stimuli. Res. Vet. Sci. 2013, 94, 49–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  190. Dollion, N.; Paulus, A.; Champagne, N.; St-Pierre, N.; St-Pierre, É.; Trudel, M.; Plusquellec, P. Fear/Reactivity in working dogs: An analysis of 37 years of behavioural data from the Mira Foundation’s future service dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2019, 221, 104864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  191. Somppi, S.; Törnqvist, H.; Koskela, A.; Vehkaoja, A.; Tiira, K.; Väätäjä, H.; Surakka, V.; Vainio, O.; Kujala, M.V. Dog–owner relationship, owner interpretations and dog personality are connected with the emotional reactivity of dogs. Animals 2022, 12, 1338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  192. Guardini, G.; Bowen, J.; Mariti, C.; Fatjó, J.; Sighieri, C.; Gazzano, A. Influence of maternal care on behavioural development of domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) living in a home environment. Animals 2017, 7, 93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  193. Lezama-García, K.; Mariti, C.; Mota-Rojas, D.; Martínez-Burnes, J.; Barrios-García, H.; Gazzano, A. Maternal behaviour in domestic dogs. Int. J. Vet. Sci. Med. 2019, 7, 20–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  194. Hecht, J.; Miklósi, Á.; Gácsi, M. Behavioral assessment and owner perceptions of behaviors associated with guilt in dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2012, 139, 134–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  195. Passalacqua, C.; Marshall-Pescini, S.; Merola, I.; Palestrini, C.; Previde, E.P. Different problem-solving strategies in dogs diagnosed with anxiety-related disorders and control dogs in an unsolvable task paradigm. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2013, 147, 139–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  196. van Herwijnen, I.R.; van der Borg, J.A.M.; Naguib, M.; Beerda, B. Dog ownership satisfaction determinants in the owner-dog relationship and the dog’s behaviour. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0204592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  197. Serpell, J.A.; Duffy, D.L. Aspects of juvenile and adolescent environment predict aggression and fear in 12-month-old guide dogs. Front. Vet. Sci. 2016, 3, 49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  198. Dietz, L.; Arnold, A.-M.K.; Goerlich-Jansson, V.C.; Vinke, C.M. The importance of early life experiences for the development of behavioural disorders in domestic dogs. Behaviour 2018, 155, 83–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  199. Lee, G.-H.; Jo, W.; Kang, T.-K.; Oh, T.; Kim, K. Assessment of Stress Caused by Environmental Changes for Improving the Welfare of Laboratory Beagle Dogs. Animals 2023, 13, 1095. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  200. Höglund, O.V.; Lövebrant, J.; Olsson, U.; Höglund, K. Blood pressure and heart rate during ovariohysterectomy in pyometra and control dogs: A preliminary investigation. Acta Vet. Scand. 2016, 58, 80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  201. Custance, D.; Mayer, J. Empathic-like responding by domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) to distress in humans: An exploratory study. Anim. Cogn. 2012, 15, 851–859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  202. Peters, W.; Rice, S.; Cohen, J.; Murray, L.; Schley, C.; Alvarez-Jimenez, M.; Bendall, S. Trauma-focused cognitive–behavioral therapy (TF-CBT) for interpersonal trauma in transitional-aged youth. Psychol. Trauma Theory Res. Pract. Policy 2021, 13, 313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  203. Steele, W.; Malchiodi, C.A. Trauma-Informed Practices with Children and Adolescents; Taylor & Francis: Abingdon, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  204. Chemtob, C.M.; Tolin, D.F.; van der Kolk, B.A.; Pitman, R.K. Eye movement desensitization and reprocessing. In Effective Treatments for PTSD: Practice Guidelines from the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  205. Kapteijn, C.M.; van der Borg, J.A.M.; Vinke, C.M.; Endenburg, N. On the applicability of eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (EMDR) as an intervention in dogs with fear and anxiety disorders after a traumatic event. Behaviour 2021, 158, 1471–1487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  206. Kapteijn, C.M.; van der Borg, J.A.M.; Vinke, C.M.; Endenburg, N. Recent Findings from University of Utrecht Provides New Insights into Behavior Research on the Applicability of Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing (Emdr) as an Intervention in Dogs with Fear and Anxiety Disorders after a Traumatic; University of Utrecht: Utrecht, The Netherlands, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  207. Arya, S.; Zutshi, A. Trauma-informed care in the neonatal intensive care unit: Through the lens of the COVID-19 pandemic. Cureus 2022, 14, e30307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  208. Amateau, G.; Gendron, T.L.; Rhodes, A. Stress, strength, and respect: Viewing direct care staff experiences through a trauma-informed lens. Gerontol. Geriatr. Educ. 2023, 44, 380–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  209. Langley-Brady, D.L.; Shutes, J.; Vinson, J.J.; Zadinsky, J.K. Aromatherapy through the lens of trauma-informed care: Stress-reduction practices for healthcare professionals. J. Interprof. Educ. Pract. 2023, 30, 100602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  210. Tedeschi, P.; Sisa, M.L.; Olmert, M.D.; Parish-Plass, N.; Yount, R. Treating human trauma with the help of animals: Trauma informed intervention for child maltreatment and adult post-traumatic stress. In Handbook on Animal-Assisted Therapy; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2015; pp. 305–319. [Google Scholar]
  211. Acquarone, S. Infant-Parent Psychotherapy: A handbook; Routledge: Abingdon, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  212. Perry, B.D. Incubated in terror: Neurodevelopmental factors in the cycle of violence. In Children in a Violent Society; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  213. Guardino, C.M.; Schetter, C.D. Coping during pregnancy: A systematic review and recommendations. Health Psychol. Rev. 2014, 8, 70–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  214. Packer, R.M.A.; Brand, C.L.; Belshaw, Z.; Pegram, C.L.; Stevens, K.B.; O’neill, D.G. Pandemic puppies: Characterising motivations and behaviours of UK owners who purchased puppies during the 2020 COVID-19 pandemic. Animals 2021, 11, 2500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  215. Seksel, K.; Mazurski, E.J.; Taylor, A. Puppy socialisation programs: Short and long term behavioural effects. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1999, 62, 335–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  216. Serpell, J.; Duffy, D.L.; Jagoe, J.A. Becoming a dog: Early experience and the development of behavior. In The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behavior and Interactions with People; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2016; Volume 2, pp. 93–117. [Google Scholar]
  217. Becker, M.; Volk, H.; Kunzmann, P. Is Pet Health Insurance Able to Improve Veterinary Care? Why Pet Health Insurance for Dogs and Cats Has Limits: An Ethical Consideration on Pet Health Insurance. Animals 2022, 12, 1728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  218. Chamberlain, T. Do age and conditions at rehoming affect behaviour in puppies? Plymouth Stud. Sci. 2012, 5, 23–37. [Google Scholar]
  219. Bornstein Marc, H. Sensitive periods in development: Structural characteristics and causal interpretations. Psychol. Bull. 1989, 105, 179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  220. Gov.ie. Gov.ie: Rules on the Sale, Supply and Advertising of Pet Animals. Available online: https://www.gov.ie (accessed on 18 November 2023).
  221. Gov.uk. Get Your Dog or Cat Microchipped—GOV.UK. Available online: https://www.gov.uk (accessed on 18 November 2023).
  222. Noah Compendium. Available online: https://www.noahcompendium.co.uk/?id=-455506 (accessed on 18 November 2023).
  223. Christensen, E.; Scarlett, J.; Campagna, M.; Houpt, K.A. Aggressive behavior in adopted dogs that passed a temperament test. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 106, 85–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  224. De Meester, R.H.; Pluijmakers, J.; Vermeire, S.; Laevens, H. The use of the socially acceptable behavior test in the study of temperament of dogs. J. Vet. Behav. 2011, 6, 211–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  225. Barnard, S.; Kennedy, D.; Watson, R.; Valsecchi, P.; Arnott, G. Revisiting a previously validated temperament test in shelter dogs, including an examination of the use of fake model dogs to assess conspecific sociability. Animals 2019, 9, 835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  226. Dunn, L. Small animal practice: Billing, third-party payment options, pet health insurance. Vet. Clin. Small Anim. Pract. 2006, 36, 411–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  227. Yeager, K.R.; Roberts, A.R. Differentiating Among Stress, Acute Stress Disorder, Crisis Episodes, Trauma, and PTSD: Paradigm and Treatment Goals. Brief Treat. Crisis Interv. 2003, 3, 3–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  228. Lamers-Winkelman, F.; Willemen, A.M.; Visser, M. Adverse childhood experiences of referred children exposed to intimate partner violence: Consequences for their wellbeing. Child Abus. Negl. 2012, 36, 166–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  229. Feelings, I. Welfare, stress, and the evolution of feelings. Adv. Study Behav. Stress Behav. 1998, 27, 371. [Google Scholar]
  230. Chu, Y.C.; Wang, H.H.; Chou, F.H.; Hsu, Y.F.; Liao, K.L. Outcomes of trauma-informed care on the psychological health of women experiencing intimate partner violence: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Psychiatr. Ment. Health Nurs. 2023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  231. Sadock, B.J.; Sadock, V.A. Kaplan & Sadock’s Concise Textbook of Clinical Psychiatry; Lippincott Williams & Wilkins: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
Table 1. Types of adverse childhood experiences with high likelihood of applicability to dogs.
Table 1. Types of adverse childhood experiences with high likelihood of applicability to dogs.
Type of Early Adverse Experiences in Children (REFs)Equivalent Adverse Early Experience in DogsReferences Identified in the Canine Literature
Deliberate psychological harm Verbal abuse, neglect
  • Dogs react fearfully towards human shouting [120];
  • Effects of fictitious illness on animals, Munchausen syndrome by proxy [121];
  • Exploring the complexities of animal hoarding and children who abuse animals [122];
  • Understanding the psychological outcomes of neglect, abuse, and harsh environments in dogs [123];
  • Postmortem evidence of neglect [124];
  • The nature of, and criminal justice response to, shooting, beating, stabbing, or throwing of dogs and cats [125];
  • Determining neglect by assessing bone-marrow fat reserves [126].
Physical harm
(Non-accidental injury or pain, sexual abuse)
Physical harm
(Non-accidental injury or pain, sexual abuse)
  • Considers factors raising suspicion of non-accidental injury in animals: vaginal and anorectal injuries and damage to genitalia from sexual abuse [127];
  • Fracture characteristics distinguishing accidental and non-accidental injury in dogs [128];
  • Postmortem evidence of non-accidental injuries (blunt-force or sharp-force trauma, gunshots, burns, drowning, asphyxiation, or suspicious intoxications) [129];
  • Traumatic medical procedures and stressful visits to the veterinary clinic [130];
  • Considering the evidence of animal cruelty in dogs presented both dead and alive [131];
  • Collaboration between forensic anthropology and veterinary pathology to investigate non-accidental injury in dogs [132].
Accidental injuries/accidents
  • Dogs experiencing the trauma of home fires [133];
  • Dogs injured during car crash incidents [134];
  • Acts of nature: wind and storms, rain, and flooding [135].
Family psychopathology Challenges within the home environment
(mental health challenges, substance abuse, inter-personal violence)
  • ‘Spousal violence’ and implications for animal cruelty in the same household [136];
  • A survey of ‘battered’ women in shelters identifying links with cruelty to dogs [137];
  • Comorbidity between pet abuse and family violence [138];
  • Domestic violence and animal cruelty in New Providence [139];
  • Incidents of animal abuse and neglect in dogs belonging to domestic violence victims [140];
  • Challenges of owning a dog for psychiatric patients resulting in delays in dogs being treated [141];
  • Dog ownership and survival following cardiovascular events, where owners’ compromised health results in degrees of neglect and instability for the dog [142];
  • Dog owners are more likely to delay medical procedures, depending on the availability of social support [143];
  • Strong dog–owner relationship associated with poorer mental health, anxiety, and depression [144];
  • Difficulties forming a bond for veterans with their services dogs with comorbid substance abuse and PTSD [145];
  • Disadvantages and barriers of dog ownership for owners of assistance dogs [146];
  • Dog ownership can hinder access to services for owners with substance abuse [147].
Unintentional caregiver absence Owner or caregiver absence due to the following:
hospital stays, imprisonment,
owner death.
  • Response of guide dogs to separation from their owner [148];
  • Examining how imprisoned dog owners relate to their pets [149];
  • Anxiety in dogs caused by owner absence [150];
  • Dog owners imprisoned [151];
  • Effects of death of dog owners on their dogs following cardiovascular incidents [152].
Intentional removal from caregiver Relinquishment, abandonment or change of owner in dogs.
  • Risk factors for relinquishment of dogs in the US [153];
  • Under-developed human: dog bonds resulting in relinquishment [18];
  • Dogs in UK shelters are more likely to have reported behavioural problems [154];
  • Factors resulting relinquishment of dogs in Portugal [155];
  • Factors resulting in relinquishment of dogs in Canada [156].
Bullying or peer violence (threat of or actual physical violence) Intraspecific, inter-dog aggression
  • Rehabilitating shelter dogs with reported inter-dog aggression [157];
  • Risk factors for inter-dog aggression in dogs in the UK [158];
  • Understanding fear and aggression in dogs [159];
  • Physiological stress, coping, and anxiety in greyhounds displaying inter-dog aggression [160].
Table 2. Potential application of human signs of trauma to dogs.
Table 2. Potential application of human signs of trauma to dogs.
Likelihood of Application of Human Signs to DogsMental or Emotional Symptoms or Signs of Trauma Identified in HumansRelevance to Canine Behavioural Medicine
Likely to apply to dogsAggressive behaviours
  • Effect of owners challenging already anxious dogs may worsen their tendency to bite [164];
  • Where a dog has been attacked by another dog in earlier life, they are more likely to mount a vigorous and fast attack [165];
  • Considering canine personality and previous trauma and their impact on fearfulness and aggression in dogs [166];
  • Effects of early experience on human-directed aggression in dogs [167].
Anxiety
  • Separation anxiety behaviours in dogs [168];
  • The stress of living with fear and anxiety has negative effects on health and life-span in dogs [169];
  • Multifactorial nature of fear and anxiety in dogs to include learned experiences during sensitive periods [170].
Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder
  • Age and training experience of dogs showed significant effect when ADHD and non-ADHD dogs are compared [171];
  • Early social and physical parameters influence the development of ADHD in dogs [172];
  • Demographic, environmental, and behavioural factors contributing to aggression, ADHD-like behaviour, and repetitive behaviours in Finnish dogs [173];
  • Serum serotonin and dopamine are lower in dogs with behavioural signs consistent with ADHD-like disorders [174].
Avoidance behaviours [110]
  • Trauma-related thoughts or feelings;
  • Trauma-related external reminders;
  • Avoidance of triggers;
  • Psychological numbing such as disassociation.
  • Intensity of heart rate responses and the occurrence of avoidance behaviours in dogs [175];
  • Escape, avoidance, and learned helplessness behaviour in dogs [176];
  • Early experience and later avoidance behaviours in dogs [177];
  • Breed differences in the onset of fear-related avoidance behaviours in dogs [178].
Depression
  • Exploring anhedonia in kennelled dogs [179];
  • Greater periods of waking inactivity of dogs in their home environment [180];
  • Anhedonia: effect of stress on eating behaviour in domestic dogs [181].
Memories: recurrent, involuntary, and intrusive memories
  • Traumatic nightmares;
  • Dissociative reactions, flashbacks;
  • Intense and prolonged distress;
  • Marked physiological reactivity after exposure to trauma-related triggers.
  • Emotional distress resulting in calm behavioural responses but higher cardiac activation in guide dog [182];
  • Fearful behaviours exhibited by dogs with memory of previous fear inducing/negative experiences [148];
  • Dogs subjected to neglect, abuse, or severe under-socialisation may develop moderate–severe fearfulness [183].
PTSD or PTSD-like behaviours
  • PTSD has been demonstrated in dogs following both experimental and natural trauma [110];
  • PTSD demonstrated in both household pets and military canines [184].
Reactivity [110]:
  • Irritable or aggressive behaviour;
  • Self-destructive or reckless behaviour;
  • Hyper-vigilance;
  • Exaggerated startle response;
  • Problems concentrating;
  • Sleep disturbance.
  • Temperament influences reactivity in dogs [185];
  • Stimulus reactivity in dogs presented with behavioural problems [186];
  • Reactivity of puppies towards tolerance of new situations [187];
  • Effect of owner presence on reactive behaviours of dogs during a stressful experience [188];
  • Cortisol levels in hair reflect dog’s chronic state of emotional reactivity [189];
  • Fear and reactivity in working dogs remains consistent between 6 and 12 months of age [190];
  • Dogs’ emotional reactivity and the dog–owner relationship modulate each other [191].
Reduced parenting capacity
  • Influences on maternal care on stress in dogs and rodents [192];
  • The quality of maternal care contributes to the adaptation of puppies to environmental stimuli [21];
  • The level of interactions with the dam influences the physiological, cognitive, and behavioural development of their puppies [193];
  • The peripartum period is stressful, so minimising stress and improving reassurance of the bitch improves her mothering ability [75].
Rule-breaking behaviours
  • Interactions between demographics, problematic behaviours, and trainability in dogs [32];
  • Owner perceptions of guilt as a resultant of canine misbehaving [194];
  • Problem-solving capabilities in dogs diagnosed with anxiety disorders [195];
  • Dog-directed parenting skills predicting the resultant behaviour of their dogs [196].
Stress
  • Early experience modulates stress in German Shepherds [197];
  • Effect of maternal care, attachment, and socialisation on responses to stress in dogs [198];
  • Assessing stress caused by environmental changes in laboratory dogs [199];
  • Stress responses in dogs exposed to repeated noxious stimuli [200].
Likely but not documentedDistressing thoughts, feelings, or sensations
  • Empathic-like responding of dogs to distress in their owners [201].
Difficulty forming emotional attachments
Low morale
Mistrusting
Psychological distress
Unlikely to apply to dogsSubstance abuse disorders
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Corridan, C.L.; Dawson, S.E.; Mullan, S. Potential Benefits of a ‘Trauma-Informed Care’ Approach to Improve the Assessment and Management of Dogs Presented with Anxiety Disorders. Animals 2024, 14, 459. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14030459

AMA Style

Corridan CL, Dawson SE, Mullan S. Potential Benefits of a ‘Trauma-Informed Care’ Approach to Improve the Assessment and Management of Dogs Presented with Anxiety Disorders. Animals. 2024; 14(3):459. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14030459

Chicago/Turabian Style

Corridan, Claire Lorraine, Susan E. Dawson, and Siobhan Mullan. 2024. "Potential Benefits of a ‘Trauma-Informed Care’ Approach to Improve the Assessment and Management of Dogs Presented with Anxiety Disorders" Animals 14, no. 3: 459. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14030459

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop