Next Article in Journal
Contrasting Attitudes towards Animal Welfare Issues within the Food Chain
Previous Article in Journal
Nature of Pre-Earthquake Phenomena and their Effects on Living Organisms
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

A Longitudinal Study on Feeding Behaviour and Activity Patterns of Released Chimpanzees in Conkouati-Douli National Park, Republic of Congo

1
HELP Congo (Habitat Ecologique et Liberté des Primates), BP 335, Pointe Noire, Congo
2
Centre for Research in Evolutionary Anthropology, Department of Life Sciences, Roehampton University, Holybourne Avenue, London SW15 4JD, UK
3
Danau Girang Field Centre, c/o Sabah Wildlife Department, Wisma Muis, 88100 Kota Kinabalu, Sabah, Malaysia
4
Organisms and Environment Division, Cardiff School of Biosciences, Cardiff University, Biomedical Sciences Building, Museum Avenue, Cardiff CF10 3AX, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2013, 3(2), 532-550; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani3020532
Submission received: 26 April 2013 / Revised: 30 May 2013 / Accepted: 3 June 2013 / Published: 7 June 2013

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Wild chimpanzee populations are dramatically declining due to anthropogenic pressure. One way of increasing wild population numbers and/or repopulating areas where local extinction has occurred is to release captive animals. HELP Congo was the first project to successfully release wild-born orphan chimpanzees in their natural environment. We studied the behaviour of eight released chimpanzees over eight years. Over time, they modified their behaviour, suggesting long-term behavioural and ecological adaptations. This suggests that successful release programmes may reinforce existing populations of endangered species.

Abstract

Wild chimpanzee populations are still declining due to logging, disease transmission and hunting. The bushmeat trade frequently leads to an increase in the number of orphaned primates. HELP Congo was the first project to successfully release wild-born orphan chimpanzees into an existing chimpanzee habitat. A collection of post monitoring data over 16 years now offers the unique opportunity to investigate possible behavioural adaptations in these chimpanzees. We investigated the feeding and activity patterns in eight individuals via focal observation techniques from 1997–1999 and 2001–2005. Our results revealed a decline in the number of fruit and insect species in the diet of released chimpanzees over the years, whereas within the same period of time, the number of consumed seed species increased. Furthermore, we found a decline in time spent travelling, but an increase in time spent on social activities, such as grooming, as individuals matured. In conclusion, the observed changes in feeding and activity patterns seem to reflect important long-term behavioural and ecological adaptations in wild-born orphan released chimpanzees, demonstrating that the release of chimpanzees can be successful, even if it takes time for full adaptation.

Graphical Abstract

1. Introduction

The great apes have experienced a significant population reduction in the past 20 to 30 years and they continue to be threatened by poaching and habitat destruction [1,2]. Great apes are also frequently found in the pet trade, leading to large numbers of orphan animals arriving in sanctuaries. Attempts have been made to release wild-born orphan primates back into their natural environment [3,4,5,6]. Although long-term data on release programs are rare [5,6], such studies are vital if we are to assess the value of releases for either conservation or animal welfare [7].
Reintroduction has been defined as “an attempt to establish a species in an area which was once part of its historic range, but from which it has been extirpated or become extinct” [2]. Reintroducing wild animals in natural environments can be a useful tool for conservation because it can restore biodiversity [8], provide an important demographic and genetic reservoir for endangered species, and decrease the threat of extinction in endangered species [9]. In addition, releasing animals into areas where there is a wild population may also be done for a variety of other reasons, including animal welfare and supplementing wild populations where numbers are low; such cases may be referred to as reinforcement, supplementation or restocking [2]. Below we refer to both reintroduction and reinforcement programmes as release programmes.
Since the 1960s, several attempts have been made to release great apes into the wild [3,4,5,6,10,11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19]. Only a few projects have resulted in continued survival and successful reproduction of released individuals; in the 1960s in Rubondo forest (Tanzania—here chimpanzees were released onto islands, not in forested areas), and in the 1990s onwards in Conkouati-Douli National Park (Republic of Congo), and, more recently, in 2008, in Haut Niger National Park (HNNP) in Guinea (both latter projects released chimpanzees into forested areas) [3,6,12,13]. However, post monitoring of released primates is poorly documented in the literature [4,12,14,15,16,17,18] and it is not always possible to determine the reasons why success in release varies.
One of the released projects that has had positive results and has been described as a successful release was initiated in 1996 by HELP Congo, in the Conkouati-Douli National Park, Republic of Congo [14]. A 2005 study found that 37 released chimpanzees had an average survival rate of 62%, with several released females reproducing, mating with wild as well as with released males [14]. A study of the same population showed that, following release, the chimpanzees adjusted their social structure to become stabilized into a single community with a fission–fusion system [18]. Between 1996 and 2012, a total of 53 chimpanzees were released (17 are still monitored, 15 are dead, one has been returned to one of the sanctuary islands, 20 are no longer monitored but are thought to have integrated into wild communities). Of the 17 monitored individuals, five females had two infants aged between two and 10 years.
Monitoring of these animals has resulted in several studies of their ecology. Farmer et al. (2006) studied the behaviour in the 14 months post-release of 16 chimpanzees released between 1996 and 1999 [12] and found that the released chimpanzees had activity budgets within the range of wild chimpanzees, although grooming time was low compared with wild populations in similar habitats. The same animals ate more than 239 plant parts post-release, including at least 122 species, of which 62 were identified [12].
Here we extend the analyses of feeding ecology of these animals using post-release monitoring data from 1997–1999 and 2001–2005 and investigate possible adaptations in feeding ecology and activity patterns in released chimpanzees over a post-release time period of up to eight years. In particular, we address the following questions: (1) Are there changes in the feeding ecology in released chimpanzees over the investigated time period? (2) Are there changes in activity patterns in released chimpanzees over the investigated time period?

2. Methods and Study Subjects

2.1. Study Site

The study site, the “Triangle”, is located in the North of the Conkouati lagoon in the Conkouati-Douli National Park (3°33'–4°02'S; 11°10'–11°40'E), Republic of Congo (Figure 1). The “Triangle” has an area of 22 km², and was chosen in 1996 by HELP Congo to be the release site of wild-born orphan chimpanzees due to its abundance of food, especially fruits, its low population density of wild chimpanzees (0.17–0.33 individuals/km2), and absence of people [12,14,20]. The rainy season is concentrated from October to May and there is a dry season from June to September [21].
Six hundred species of plants have been identified, including around 100 species known to be eaten by wild chimpanzees [20]. The “Triangle” is surrounded by two rivers, but is not fully isolated as natural bridges occur, which allow the released chimpanzees to travel freely and to have access to two other areas of the Conkouati-Douli National Park: the Man Fai Tai forest and the Reserve (Figure 1).
Figure 1. Map of the release site Triangle within the sanctuary of HELP Congo (the release site so called the “Triangle” of 22 km² area, is composed of three sites, the Triangle forest, the Man Faï Taï forest and the Reserve. Chimpanzees are free ranging, using natural bridges to cross the rivers).
Figure 1. Map of the release site Triangle within the sanctuary of HELP Congo (the release site so called the “Triangle” of 22 km² area, is composed of three sites, the Triangle forest, the Man Faï Taï forest and the Reserve. Chimpanzees are free ranging, using natural bridges to cross the rivers).
Animals 03 00532 g001

2.2. Study Animals, Data Collection and Analyses

We investigated the feeding ecology and activity patterns in eight wild-born orphaned chimpanzees. All were separated from their mothers, who were presumably killed for the bush meat market, and arrived as infants in captivity, aged from approximately six months to three years old (Table 1). They were rescued by the Congolese organization HELP Congo and were transferred to three small islands, of 5–10 km², in the middle of the lagoon of Conkouati (Figure 1) in 1991. They did not have contact with wild chimpanzees on these islands. The chimpanzees were released between 1996 and 1999 at the “Triangle” (details given in [12,14]) (Table 1). Chimpanzees selected for the release were between 6 and 11 years (age at release: 6.3–10.9 years, mean 8.3 years, s.d. 1.6 years). Young animals without dependant infants were chosen as it was thought that they would have more chance to integrate with wild communities at this stage of sexual maturity [14] To minimise possible risk of disease transmission, the released animals chosen had all been living on the isolated islands since 1991 and were healthy; health checks were also carried out prior to release [14,20,22]. Table 1 gives information on the characteristics of released studied chimpanzees.
Table 1. Characteristics of released focal chimpanzees.
Table 1. Characteristics of released focal chimpanzees.
IDSexArrival age (years)/ Estimated date of birth/Date arrived sanctuary)Release age (years)/ Date releaseNumber data blocks (a) (dates data collection)Date death (cause)Time since release to 2009 or death (years)Offspring born in Triangle (b)No. infants known up to 2009
BounieF5.0
Jan 1987–Jan 1992
9.8
Nov 1996
4
(1997–1998)
13Yes1
ChoupetteF1.8
Nov 1989–Aug 1991
6.9
Nov 1996
10
(1997–1999, 2003–2004)
13Yes3
JeanetteF3.2
Jun 1988–Aug 1991
8.3
Nov 1996
10
(1997–1999, 2003–2004)
13Yes1
MekoutouM2.0
Jun 1990–Jun 1992
6.3
Nov 1996
11
(1997–1999, 2003–2005)
Apr 2006 (unknown)10Yes1
RosetteF1.5
Feb 1990–Aug 1991
6.9
Feb 1997
6
(1997–1999)
12
KoutouM2.2
Jun 1989–Aug 1991
9.6
Feb 1999
12
(1999, 2001–2005)
Mar 2009 (Wild attack)10
ChinoisM2.2
Jun 1989–Aug 1991
11.0
Jun 2000
10
(2001–2005)
9
BilingaM0.4
Jun 1993–Nov 1993
8.0
Jun 2001
4
(2002–2004)
Oct 2004 (Oesopha-gostomum infection)3
(a) Data were means for 3-month blocks (see text for details); (b) Genetic tests were confirmed for males’ offspring [23].
A protocol of post release monitoring was established to gather information on the process of release [12]. All released chimpanzees were fitted with radio-collars (Telonics Inc., USA). Following their release, the animals were followed from nest to nest, seven days a week, by a trained team of several Congolese field assistants and international volunteers. Data collected included diet, activity budget, nesting, ranging behaviour, female reproductive status, health status and interactions with wild conspecifics [14]. Whenever possible, observers maintained a minimum distance of 7 m between themselves and the chimpanzees to reduce disease transmission [2]. Gradually, as the chimpanzees grew independent, the staff increased the security distance between themselves and the chimpanzees [22] to 7 meters minimum.
Data analysed here were collected on feeding ecology and activity patterns from 1997–1999 and 2001–2005. Observers recorded behaviour using focal sampling carried out with instantaneous observations every 10 minutes [24], recording information onto check sheets using standard activity and dietary definitions (Table 2). For feeding behaviour, plant taxa and food parts eaten were also noted (Table 2). Observations were made mostly from dawn to dusk (06:00 to 18:00), but sometimes for shorter periods, depending on weather conditions and the available number of field assistants. Once integrated into wild communities, females were not the subject of focal follows and were only observed interacting with reintroduced males.
For the statistical analyses of the long-term activity and feeding pattern in the released chimpanzees, only data fulfilling the following criteria were included: (1) a minimum of continual data collection for 10 days/month for at least 24 months post-release (range 25–67 months), and (2) overall data availability over a period spanning at least three years post-release (range 3–13 years). Using these criteria we extracted data from the long-term records for four females and four males aged from 6 to 11 years at release (Table 1). Data for each animal were analysed for 10 days per month for two three-month periods in each year (March–May and September–November) (Table 1). As data collection quality and quantity varied, these months were chosen to maximize good quality data, rather than ensure equal coverage of wet and dry seasons. Data were available for 61 full three month blocks, and an additional 6 blocks, where data for only two of the three months were available, were also included.
Table 2. Activity and dietary definitions.
Table 2. Activity and dietary definitions.
Activity budgetDefinition
TravelMoving (walking, running, jumping) on the ground or in the trees, climbing or descending
AllogroomingGrooming another chimpanzee
RestLying, sleeping, sitting, not moving
FeedRemoving food items from a substrate, eating, chewing, swallowing
OtherAll the activities that do not correspond to those listed above, e.g., aggression, play, copulation, veterinary interventions, displays, hunting, nest building and coprophagy
Dietary itemsItems included
PlantFruit (Fr.), leaf (L.), flower (Fl.), seed (Se.), and stem (St.)
InsectAnts, larvae, caterpillars, termites and grasshoppers
MeatBirds, pangolins, and squirrels
Other food itemsSap (exudates eaten from tree trunks/branches), bark, soil, clay and honey
Plant species and plant parts eaten were recorded on the observation sheets. Each of these parts are analysed as separate food items in the study. When a plant taxon was not known it was listed as Unknown. A food list was made from this information (Table 3), including the vernacular name (when possible) and the scientific name of the plants eaten by the chimpanzees [25]. Plants eaten were classified to genus level in most cases and to species level where possible (Table 3).
The means for 10 days of each month per animal were calculated to give monthly percentages for each of the behaviours. These were then averaged again for each three-month period. This gave 67 period means for the eight animals (range 4–12 per animal). This method thus allows changes of behaviour to be tracked over time (i.e., at six-monthly intervals).
The ages and time since release of each animal were recorded for the mid-month for each three-month period, i.e., in April and October.
Table 3. Plant items eaten, listed in order of importance in diet: Fruit (Fr.), Leaf (L.), Flower (Fl.), Seed (Se.), Stem (St.). Herb: herbaceous plant up to 5 m height [25,26,27]. Shrub: 1–2 m height; 2 m height & 10 cm in diameter; 10–20 cm in diameter [25]. Tree: 20–50 cm in diameter, 50–100 cm in diameter, more than 100 cm in diameter [25]. Liana: supple stems with contortion, tendrils, hooks of great heights. Woody liana & herbaceous liana differ with the woodiness of the stem [25].
Table 3. Plant items eaten, listed in order of importance in diet: Fruit (Fr.), Leaf (L.), Flower (Fl.), Seed (Se.), Stem (St.). Herb: herbaceous plant up to 5 m height [25,26,27]. Shrub: 1–2 m height; 2 m height & 10 cm in diameter; 10–20 cm in diameter [25]. Tree: 20–50 cm in diameter, 50–100 cm in diameter, more than 100 cm in diameter [25]. Liana: supple stems with contortion, tendrils, hooks of great heights. Woody liana & herbaceous liana differ with the woodiness of the stem [25].
Genus/SpeciesFamily% dietLocal namePart consumedPlant type
Fl.Fr.L.Se.St.
Irvingia gabonensisIrvingiaceae15 x x Tree
VitexVerbenaceae14 xxx xTree
StaudtiaMyristicaceae10Niove x x Tree
NaucleaRubiaceae9Bilinga x Tree
Elaeis guineensisPalmae8 xx Tree
MillettiaPapilionaceae6 x x Tree
AframomumZingiberaceae6Ntoundouxx xxHerb
DialiumCaesalpiniaceae5Dakar xx Tree
GrewiaTiliaceae5 x x Tree
LandolphiaApocynaceae5Malombo x x Liana
ScytopetalumScytopetalaceae4 x Shrub
SantiriaBurseraceae3 x Tree
HexalobusAnnonaceae2Vadou x Tree
IpomoeaConvolvulaceae2 xx Herb
WarneckeaMelastomataceae2 x xxHerb
DiospyrosEbenaceae1 xxx Tree
FicusMoraceae1 xx Shrub
Saccoglottis gabonensisHumiriaceae1Ozouga x Tree
UreraUrticaceae1 x Woody liana
AfzeliaCaesalpiniaceae<1 x Tree
AgelaeaConnaraceae<1 x x Woody liana
AlchorneaEuphorbiaceae<1Mukukusa x x Shrub
AllanblackiaClusiaceae<1 x Tree
AncistrophyllumPalmae<1Mukawa xLiana
AnnonaAnnonaceae<1 x Shrub
AspleniumAspleniaceae<1 x Herb
BerliniaCaesalpiniaceae<1Ebiarax x Tree
BrideliaEuphorbiaceae<1 x Tree
CanthiumRubiaceae<1 x Shrub
ChytranthusSapindaceae<1 x Tree
CissusVitaceae<1 x Liana
CitrusRutaceae<1 x Tree
Cleistopholis patensAnnonaceae<1 x Tree
ColaSterculiaceae<1 xxx Tree
CostusZingiberaceae<1Mussangavulu xx xHerb
DacryodesBurseraceae<1Safou x Tree
DesplatsiaTiliaceae<1 xx Tree
DiogoaOlacaceae<1 x Tree
EremospathaPalmae<1Mbamba x xLiana
EnantiaAnnonaceae<1 x Tree
Gnetum africanumGnetaceae<1Mfumbu x Tree
GuibourtiaCaesalpiniaceae<1 x x Tree
HaumaniaMarantaceae<1 x Herb
HibiscusMalvaceae<1 x Herb
HypselodelphisMarantaceae<1Mangongolo x Herb
KlainedoxaIrvingiaceae<1 x Tree
MacarangaEuphorbiaceae<1Msamsa x Tree
ManihotEuphorbiaceae<1Manioc x Tree
MapaniaCyperaceae<1 x xHerb
MarantochloaMarantaceae<1 x Herb
Milicia excelsaMoraceae<1Iroko x Tree
MimosaMimosaceae<1 xx Liana
MusangaMoraceae<1Musenga x Tree
MyrianthusMoraceae<1 x Tree
PalisotaCommelinaceae<1 x xHerb
ParkiaMimosaceae<1 xxx Tree
PentaclethraMimosaceae<1Tibossi xx Tree
Pentadesma butyraceaClusiaceae<1 x Tree
PolyalthiaAnnonaceae<1 x Tree
PseudospondiasAnacardiaceae<1Ngarila xx Tree
SalaciaCelastraceae<1 xx Tree
StrychnosLoganiaceae<1 x Woody liana
Symphonia globuliferaClusiaceae<1 x Tree
TrachyphryniumMarantaceae<1 xHerb
TrichoscyphaAnacardiaceae<1Mvuta x Tree
UapacaEuphorbiaceae<1 x Tree
UvariaAnnonaceae<1 x Tree
Uvariastrum pierreanumAnnonaceae<1 xx Tree
XylopiaAnnonaceae<1Moukana x Tree
TreculiaMoraceae<1 x Tree
PycnanthusMyristicaceae<1Mulomba x Tree
Ongokea goreOlacaceae<1 x Tree
PodococcusPalmae<1 x Tree
RaphiaPalmae<1Mahouhou xx xTree
Panda oleosaPandaceae<1 x Tree
LeptoderrisPapilionaceae<1 xx Shrub
PterocarpusPapilionaceae<1 x Tree
PassifloraPassifloraceae<1 x Herb
Oxytenanthera abyssinicaPoaceae<1Bambou x Herb
PorterandiaRubiaceae<1 x Tree
SherbourniaRubiaceae<1 x Tree
PancoviaSapindaceae<1 x Tree
BaillonellaSapotaceae<1Moabi x Tree
SterculiaSterculiaceae<1Ikirulangaxxxx Tree
Glyphaea brevisTiliaceae<1 x Tree

2.3. Statistics

Age data were normally distributed but time since release were not (Kolmogorov-Smirnov test p < 0.05). Most activity budget and diet data were normally distributed, except for percentage of flowers, seed, meat and insects which are non-parametric (Kolmogorov-Smirnov test p < 0.05), Correlation tests were conducted to investigate relationships between behaviour, diet (consumption of each food type) and time since release (months) and age (months) of the subjects (Pearson’s Correlation Coefficient for parametric data and Spearman’s Correlation Coefficient for non-parametric data).

3. Results and Discussion

3.1. Results

3.1.1. Feeding Patterns

We identified a total of 86 plant taxa (Table 3) from at least 42 plant families that were eaten by the eight chimpanzees. The chimpanzees were observed to feed on all plant parts, the preferred plant parts being fruits (58%), followed by leaves (18%), stem (16%) and seeds (3%). Insects, soil, and meat only represented a small part (less than 3% in total) of the diet.
When comparing the feeding ecology in the released chimpanzees over time, we found a negative correlation between time since release and fruit consumption (Figure 2, rs = −0.311, p = 0.010, n = 67) but not between fruit consumption and age (r = −0.198, p = 0.108, n = 67). Time since release and insect consumption were also negatively correlated (Figure 2, rs = −0.350, p < 0.004, n = 67) as was age and insect consumption (Figure 3, rs = 0.429, p < 0.001, n = 67). Conversely we found a positive correlation between seed consumption and time since release (Figure 2, rs = 0.567, p < 0.001, n = 67) and with seed consumption and age (Figure 3, rs = 0.544, p < 0.001, n = 67). There were no correlations with leaf consumption and either age (r = −0.139, p = 0.261) or time since release (rs = −0.165, p = 0.182, n = 67), nor with flower consumption and age (r = 0.080, p = 0.519, n = 67) ortime since release (rs = 0.039, p = 0.752, n = 67), nor with meat consumption and age (r = −0.017, p = 0.890, n = 67) or time since release (rs = 0.040, p = 0.749, n = 67).
Figure 2. Significant relationships with time since release and (a) % insect eating; (b)% fruit eating; (c) % seed eating; (d) % time travelling. Each individual is represented as a separate symbol. Females are shown as triangles and males as squares.
Figure 2. Significant relationships with time since release and (a) % insect eating; (b)% fruit eating; (c) % seed eating; (d) % time travelling. Each individual is represented as a separate symbol. Females are shown as triangles and males as squares.
Animals 03 00532 g002
Figure 3. Significant relationships with age and (a) % insect eating; (b) % seed eating; (c) % time grooming. Each individual is represented as a separate symbol. Females are shown as triangles and males as squares.
Figure 3. Significant relationships with age and (a) % insect eating; (b) % seed eating; (c) % time grooming. Each individual is represented as a separate symbol. Females are shown as triangles and males as squares.
Animals 03 00532 g003
When each of the feeding measures is considered for individual animals the same trends were seen but none of the correlations were statistically significant, although the directionalities of some effects were significant. One-tailed T-tests were used to investigate whether correlation coefficients for individuals were significantly different from zero; age and insect eating have a negative correlation that is significantly different from zero (p = 0.024, n = 7), age and seed a positive correlation (p > 0.001, n = 7), and time since release and fruit consumption a trend towards a negative correlation (p = 0.09, n = 7). It should be noted that correlation coefficients for either age or time since release and any behaviour will be exactly the same within individuals as the age and time since release are completely correlated within individuals.

3.1.2. Activity Patterns

The chimpanzees spent most of their time feeding (51.51%), followed by resting (26.46%), travelling (14.90%), and grooming (2.49%). Over time, we found a marginally insignificant negative correlation between the time since release and time spent on travelling (Figure 2, rs = −0.235, p = 0.056, n = 67), which became significant when one extreme outlier was removed (rs = −0.292, p = 0.017, n = 66). There was no significant correlation between age and time spent travelling (r = −0.081, p = 0.514, n = 67). There was a positive correlation between the age of animals and time spent on grooming (Figure 3, r = 0.439, p < 0.001, n = 67) but the similar positive relationship was not significant for time since release and time spent on grooming (rs = 0.161, p = 0.193, n = 67). No significant correlations were found between feeding and age (r = −0.045, p = 0.720, n = 67) or time since release (rs = −0.004, p = 0.972, n = 67), nor with resting and age (r = −0.023, p = 0.851, n = 67) or time since release (rs = 0.163, p = 0.187, n = 67).
When each of the behaviours is considered for individual animals, the same trends were seen but none of the correlations were statistically significant, although the directionalities of some effects were significant. One-tailed T-tests were used to investigate whether correlation coefficients for individuals were significantly different from zero; age and travel has a negative correlation that is significantly different from zero (p = 0.050, n = 7), although age and grooming shows a trend towards a positive correlation this is not significant (p = 0.194, n = 7).

3.2. Discussion

Our study reveals interesting changes in feeding ecology and activity patterns in released chimpanzees over the investigated time period of eight years. We observed similarities with the food items consumed by wild chimpanzees. The high levels of fruit in the diet (58%) followed by leaf (18%) and then stem (16%) is similar to that found in wild studies where fruit is also the main dietary item [16,28,29] and to the diet of the island released population at Ipassa in Gabon [15].
The percentage of the main food types, fruit and leaf, in the diet of the released chimpanzees is within the range found in wild chimpanzees. Fruit consumption of the released animals (58%) is within the range of wild chimpanzee e.g., at Lopé, Gabon (66%), Assirik, Senegal (58%), Bossou, Guinea (52%), Gombe, Tanzania (43%), Mahale, Tanzania (32%) and Semliki, Uganda (39%) [30,31,32,33] and the released chimpanzees of Ipassa, Gabon (67%) [15]. Leaf consumption (18%) is also within the wild chimpanzee range, e.g., Assirik (10%), Lopé (12%), Bossou (18%), (27%), Semliki (30%), Mahale (37%) and similar to the released at Ipassa (10%) [15,30,31,32,33].
Wild populations of chimpanzees are reported to consume variable numbers of plants, from only 45 plant species being recorded in Semliki, Uganda [33] to 198 plant species recorded in Mahale, Tanzania [32] and more than 200 plant species recorded in Bossou population (Guinea) [28]. Thus, it appears that the diversity of diet in the released chimpanzees of Conkouati-Douli (86 plant taxa, 42 plant families) is within the range found in wild animals.
Previous studies at Conkouati-Douli showed that the released chimpanzees added several feeding plants to their diet, presumably by a trial and error learning, during the first years of their release [20]. Farmer (2006) reported that, three years after their release, the chimpanzees fed on different parts of more than 122 plant species, with the main diet being made up of 44% fruits and 26% leaves. Over eight years, our study showed fruit and leaves are similarly important but overall, fruits make up the greater part of the diet, and leaves a lesser part. These feeding differences might be explained by the time scale over which data were collected or by variation in annual food availability [34,35,36]. In 2006, Farmer et al. [12] collected data equally over both wet and dry seasons but we have more data on the wet months; this may partly explain the higher fruit and lower leaf consumption in our study, as the rainy season (October to May) corresponds to fruit abundance and the dry season (June to September) corresponds to fruit scarcity [22]. Unfortunately, we do not have data on food availability over time, so we cannot be sure whether inter-annual variation in food availability is also important.
Our study does show that, when the same months are compared across years, fruit consumption decreased over time. One reason for the change in diet over time may be that during the captive period and early pre-release state (spent in semi-liberty on islands), the animals were mainly being fed with fruits (e.g., banana, papaya, figs) and therefore may have preferentially chosen to feed on fruit soon after release [12,20]. The decline in fruit eating is most probably because, in later years, the dietary repertoire expanded to include a greater range of food types, in particular seeds, as is found in wild chimpanzees [33,34,35,36,37].
The decline in insect consumption over time is surprising as it might be assumed that finding and capturing insects would become easier, and thus, performed more frequently, for the animals as they matured and spent more time in the wild. However, the increase in seed eating (which may be explained by the chimpanzees learning to find and process such foods) may partly explain this as both seeds and insects have a high protein content; thus an increase in seed eating may be balanced by less insect feeding. An alternative explanation is that the chimpanzees did not decrease the total amount of insects they ate but instead managed to eat them more efficiently and thus, spent less time processing these foods (this may be particularly true for feeding on social insects using tools). Investigating this would require collection of detailed data on foraging efficiency.
Surprisingly, there is a smaller number of consumed fruit taxa in our study as compared with an earlier study [12]. The lower number of taxa may be partly due to our study not including all months of the year, and to differences in the accuracy of data recording. However, the large difference (86 vs. 122 fruits species), and the overall decline in time spent feeding on fruits over the years, suggests that the difference may be a real one. This might be explained by the animals first sampling a wide range of fruits but then excluding some as they became more adept at finding preferred foods. Optimal foraging theory predicts that animals should forage in such a way as to maximize their net energy intake per unit time [38,39]. That means the chimpanzees should behave in such a way as to search and find, handle and consume fruits with the most net energy input (energy content/digestive turnover) while spending the least amount of energy and time in doing so. This could be an interesting line of new research.
Another explanation for the decline in number of fruit taxa consumed might be the learned avoidance of specific fruits due to a high content of secondary plant chemicals. It is known that primates sometimes avoid fruits with high contents of secondary plant chemicals such as alkaloids and tannins and the chimpanzees may have learnt this over time [40]. Although chimpanzees may be able to tolerate higher tannin levels by eating soil as a detoxification strategy [41], using such behaviour to allow feeding on less palatable foods may need to be learnt. We have no evidence for this hypothesis at present and testing it would require further research, both on feeding behaviour and secondary compounds in dietary items.
The observed changes in activity patterns, with a decreased time spent on travelling and an increase in time spent on social activities (grooming) may also indicate adaptations to the new environment. We suggest that after the first years of being released, the chimpanzees had to travel more intensively due to the necessity of exploring their new habitat, finding new food patches, and defining their home range within an area inhabited by wild chimpanzees. Once the home range was defined and food patches identified, travelling time for food searching and feeding time decreased. While travelling decreased, the amount of time spent on allogrooming increased as the animals matured, which is of high importance for social stabilization [18].

4. Conclusions

In conclusion, the changes in activity pattern as well as the changes in feeding ecology in the released chimpanzees seem to reflect important long-term behavioural and ecological adaptations to their environment. It is widely accepted that primates can adjust their behaviour to different ecological and dietary niches [36]. Here we demonstrate that orphan chimpanzees, which were separated from their natal group at an early age, lacking maternal contact and natal social culture, have succeeded in adapting to a new environment over time. This study suggests that reintroduction and/or reinforcement of existing populations may be possible and have benefits both to animal welfare and to conservation efforts.
Further studies should focus on the nutritive components of the items eaten (and not eaten) by the released chimpanzees as well as the learning processes in the food choice of the new released chimpanzees, as this information was not included in the data sheet of the studied subjects. Furthermore, it would be interesting to study the energy expenditure and feeding efficiency in released chimpanzees to investigate changes towards a more optimized energy balance over years.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the Conservator of the Conkouati-Douli National Park, Grégoire Bonassidi for his support and for permission to carry out this work, as well as the Congolese government for allowing this research. We would like to thank all the staff and lecturers at Roehampton University and people in the Republic of Congo who supported us during the study: Rebecca Atencia, Charles Doumenge, Kay H. Farmer, Myriam Vacher Vallas, Yann Le Hellaye; and all HELP Congo’s field assistants that helped us while in the field. Finally, we are grateful to Rebecca Moore for the English correction.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. 2013. Available online: www.iucnredlist.org (accessed on 4 June 2013).
  2. Beck, B.; Walkup, K.; Rodrigues, M.; Unwin, S.; Travis, D.; Stoinski, T. Best Practice Guidelines for the Re-Introduction of Great Apes; Occasional Paper of the IUCN Species Survival Commission No. 35; UICN/SSC Primate Specialist Group: Gland, Switzerland, 2007; pp. 7–8. [Google Scholar]
  3. Borner, M. The rehabilitated chimpanzees of Rubondo Island. Oryx 1985, 19, 151–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Hannah, A.; McGrew, W. Rehabilitation of captive chimpanzees. In Primate Responses to Environmental Change; Box, H.O., Ed.; Chapman & Hall: London, UK, 1991; pp. 167–186. [Google Scholar]
  5. Yaeger, C.P. Orangutan rehabilitation in Tanjung Putting National Park, Indonesia. Conserv. Biol. 1997, 11, 802–805. [Google Scholar]
  6. Humle, T.; Colin, C.; Laurans, M.; Raballand, E. Group Release of Sanctuary Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in the Haut Niger National Park, Guinea, West Africa: Ranging Patterns and Lessons So Far. Int. J. Primatol. 2011, 32, 456–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Custance, D.M.; Whiten, A.; Fredman, T. Social learning and primate reintroduction. Int. J. Primatol. 2002, 23, 479–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Seddon, P.J. Persistence without intervention: Assessing success in wildlife reintroductions. Trends Ecol. Evol. 1999, 14, 503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Paredes, J. The Adaptation Process of Seven Released Chimpanzees (Pan t. troglodytes) in the Reserve of Conkouati, Congo; Unpublished Report; H.E.L.P., Congo: Pointe Noire, Congo, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  10. Brewer, S. The Chimpanzees of Mt Assirik; A. Knopf: New York, NY, USA, 1978. [Google Scholar]
  11. Marsden, S.B.; Marsden, D.; Thompson, M.E. Demographic and female life history parameters of free-ranging chimpanzees at the chimpanzee rehabilitation project, river Gambia National Park. Int. J. Primatol. 2006, 27, 391–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Farmer, K.H.; Buchanan-Smith, H.M.; Jamart, A. Behavioural adaptation of Pan troglodytes troglodytes. Int. J. Primatol. 2006, 27, 747–767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Moscovice, L.R.; Issa, M.H.; Petrzelkova, K.J.; Keuler, N.S.; Snowdon, C.T.; Huffman, M.A. Fruit availability, chimpanzee diet, and grouping patterns on Rubondo Island, Tanzania. Am. J. Primatol. 2007, 69, 487–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Goossens, B.; Setchell, J.M.; Tchidongo, E.; Dilambaka, E.; Vidal, C.; Ancrenaz, M.; Jamart, A. Survival, interactions with conspecifics and reproduction in 37 chimpanzees released into the wild. Biol. Conserv. 2005, 123, 461–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Hladik, C.M. Alimentation et activité d’un groupe de chimpanzés réintroduits en forêt Gabonaise. La Terre et la Vie 1973, 27, 343–413. [Google Scholar]
  16. Hladik, C.M. Chimpanzees of Gabon and chimpanzees of Gombe: Some comparative data on the diet. In Primate Ecology: Studies of Feeding and Ranging in Lemurs, Monkeys, and Apes; Clutton-Brock, T.H., Ed.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1977; pp. 480–501. [Google Scholar]
  17. Goossens, B.; Funk, S.M.; Vidal, C.; Latour, S.; Jamart, A.; Ancrenaz, M.; Wickings, E.J.; Tutin, C.E.G.; Bruford, M.W. Measuring genetic diversity in translocation programs: Principles and application to a chimpanzee release project. Anim. Conserv. 2002, 5, 225–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Le Hellaye, Y.; Goossens, B.; Jamart, A.; Curtis, D.J. Acquisition of fission-fusion social organization in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes troglodytes) community released into the wild. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2010, 64, 349–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Moscovice, L.R.; Mbago, F.; Snowdon, C.T.; Huffman, M.A. Ecological features and ranging patterns at a chimpanzee release site on Rubondo Island, Tanzania. Biol. Conserv. 2010, 143, 2711–2721. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Tutin, C.E.G.; Ancrenaz, M.; Paredes, J.; Vacher-Vallas, M.; Vidal, C.; Goossens, B.; Bruford, M.W.; Jamart, A. Conservation biology framework for the release of wild-born orphaned chimpanzees into the Conkouati Reserve, Congo. Conserv. Biol. 2001, 15, 1247–1257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Farmer, K.H. Pan-African Sanctuary Alliance: Status and Range of Activities for Great Ape Conservation. Am. J. Primat. 2002, 58, 117–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Tutin, C.E.G. Expertise sur les Possibilités de Réintroduction de Chimpanzés en Forêt Naturelle dans la Réserve de Conkouati, Congo; Unpublished Report; H.E.L.P., Congo: Pointe Noire, Congo, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  23. Goossens, B.; Setchell, J.M.; Vidal, C.; Dilambaka, E.; Jamart, A. Successful reproduction in wild-released orphan chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes troglodytes). Primates 2003, 44, 67–69. [Google Scholar]
  24. Altmann, J. Observational Study of Behaviour: Sampling Methods. Behaviour 1973, 48, 1–41. [Google Scholar]
  25. Letouzey, R. Manual of Forest Botany: Tropical Africa; Centre Technique Forestier Tropical, Nogent-sur-Marne, France; Ministère de l'Enseignement Supérieur et de la Recherche Scientifique: Yaounde, Cameroon, 1986; Volumes 1, 2A, 2B. [Google Scholar]
  26. Head, J.; Boesch, C.; Makaga, L.; Robbins, M.M. Sympatric Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes troglodytes) and Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) in Loango National Park, Gabon: Dietary Composition, Seasonality, and Intersite Comparisons. Int. J. Primates 2011, 32, 755–775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Nkurunungi, J.B.; Ganas, J.; Robbins, M.M.; Stanford, C.B. A comparison of two mountain gorilla habitats in Bwindi Impenetrable National Park, Uganda. Afr. J. Ecol. 2004, 42, 289–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Sugiyama, Y.; Koman, J. The flora of Bossou: Its utilisation by chimpanzees and humans. Afr. Study Monogr. 1992, 13, 127–169. [Google Scholar]
  29. Tutin, C.E.G.; Fernandez, M. Composition of the diet of chimpanzees and comparisons with that of sympatric lowland gorillas in the Lope Reserve, Gabon. Am. J. Primatol. 1993, 30, 195–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Wrangham, R.W. Feeding behaviour of chimpanzees in Gombe National Park, Tanzania. In Primate Ecology: Studies of Feeding and Ranging on Lemurs, Monkeys and Apes; Clutton-Brock, T., Ed.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1977; pp. 503–538. [Google Scholar]
  31. McGrew, W.; Baldwin, P.J.; Tutin, C.E. Diet of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) at Mt Assirik, Senegal: I. Composition. Am. J. Primatol. 1988, 16, 213–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Nishida, T.; Uehara, S. Natural diet of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii): Longterm record from the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. Afr. Study Monogr. 1983, 3, 109–130. [Google Scholar]
  33. Hunt, K.D.; McGrew, W. Chimpanzees in the dry habitats of Assirik, Senegal and Semliki Wildlife Reserve, Uganda. In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos; Boesch, C., Hohmann, G., Marchant, L., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2002; pp. 35–51. [Google Scholar]
  34. Boesch, C.; Boesch-Achermann, H. The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest—Behavioural Ecology and Evolution; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  35. Boesch, C.; Head, J.; Robbins, M.M. Complex tool sets for honey extraction among chimpanzees in Loango National Park, Gabon. J. Hum. Evol. 2009, 56, 560–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Strier, K.B. Primate Behavioral Ecology; University of Wisconsin-Madison: Madison, WI, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  37. Nishida, T. The Social Group of Wild Chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains. Primates 1968, 9, 167–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. MacArthur, R.H.; Pianka, E.R. On optimal use of a patchy environment. Am. Nat. 1966, 100, 603–609. [Google Scholar]
  39. Stephens, D.W.; Krebs, J.R. Foraging Theory; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
  40. Ganzhorn, J.U. Nested patterns of species composition and its implications for lemur biogeography in Madagascar. Folia Primatol. 1998, 69, 332–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Reynolds, V.; Plumptre, A.J.; Greenham, J.; Harborne, J. Condensed tannins and sugars in the diet of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Budongo Forest, Uganda. Oecologica 1998, 115, 331–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Renaud, A.; Jamart, A.; Goossens, B.; Ross, C. A Longitudinal Study on Feeding Behaviour and Activity Patterns of Released Chimpanzees in Conkouati-Douli National Park, Republic of Congo. Animals 2013, 3, 532-550. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani3020532

AMA Style

Renaud A, Jamart A, Goossens B, Ross C. A Longitudinal Study on Feeding Behaviour and Activity Patterns of Released Chimpanzees in Conkouati-Douli National Park, Republic of Congo. Animals. 2013; 3(2):532-550. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani3020532

Chicago/Turabian Style

Renaud, Amandine, Aliette Jamart, Benoit Goossens, and Caroline Ross. 2013. "A Longitudinal Study on Feeding Behaviour and Activity Patterns of Released Chimpanzees in Conkouati-Douli National Park, Republic of Congo" Animals 3, no. 2: 532-550. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani3020532

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop