Next Article in Journal
Randomized Trial of General Strength and Conditioning versus Motor Control and Manual Therapy for Chronic Low Back Pain on Physical and Self-Report Outcomes
Next Article in Special Issue
Chronic Gastritis Is Associated with a Decreased High-Density Lipid Level: Histological Features of Gastritis Based on the Updated Sydney System
Previous Article in Journal
Photobiomodulation—Underlying Mechanism and Clinical Applications
Previous Article in Special Issue
Differences in Results and Related Factors between Hospital-At-Home Modalities in Catalonia: A Cross-Sectional Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Visual Verticality Perception in Spinal Diseases: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis

by
Esteban Obrero-Gaitán
1,
Francisco Molina
1,*,
Rafael Del-Pino-Casado
2,
Alfonso Javier Ibáñez-Vera
1,
Daniel Rodríguez-Almagro
1 and
Rafael Lomas-Vega
1
1
Department of Health Sciences, University of Jaén, Paraje Las Lagunillas s/n, 23071 Jaén, Spain
2
Department of Nursing, University of Jaén, Paraje Las Lagunillas s/n, 23071 Jaén, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9(6), 1725; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9061725
Submission received: 12 May 2020 / Revised: 27 May 2020 / Accepted: 28 May 2020 / Published: 3 June 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Clinical Medicine of Healthcare and Sustainability)

Abstract

:
Patients diagnosed with traumatic or non-traumatic spinal pain and idiopathic scoliosis frequently suffer from imbalance. The evaluation of the perception of verticality by means of visual tests emerges as a quick and easy tool for clinical management of the balance disorders. Several studies have assessed the visual perception of verticality in spinal diseases obtaining controversial results. The aim of our study is to analyze the perception of visual verticality in subjects with several spinal diseases in comparison with healthy subjects. A meta-analysis was carried out. PubMed MEDLINE, Scopus, WoS, CINAHL, and SciELO databases were searched until January 2020. The standardized mean difference (SMD) was calculated to analyze differences between patients and healthy controls. Fifteen studies with a total of 2052 patients were included. In comparison with healthy subjects, a misperception of verticality was found in patients with spinal pain when the perception of the verticality was assessed with the rod and frame test (SMD = 0.339; 95% confidence interval (CI) = 0.181, 0.497; p < 0.001). It seems that the perception of visual verticality is not altered in patients with idiopathic scoliosis (p = 0.294). The present meta-analysis shows a misperception of visual verticality only in patients with spinal pain.

1. Introduction

Spinal diseases include pathologies of the neck, thorax, and the lumbosacral area of the spine [1,2]. Spine-related disorders are among the most common problems in clinical medicine [3,4,5]. The last Global Burden of Disease Study reported that pain related to spinal diseases is the main cause of disability [6,7], work absenteeism [8], and socioeconomic burden worldwide [9]. In the United States, neck and back pain are the second cause for visits to physicians and cause high levels of disability and financial burden [10]. At least 80% of adults have experienced neck or low back pain at some point in their life [11]. In the United States, the economic burden of spinal diseases was 87.6 billion dollars in 2013, one of the highest after diabetes, stroke, and ischemic heart disease [12]. For these reasons, it is of paramount importance to establish effective and accessible treatments to promote health and reduce the negative consequences of spinal diseases [13,14].
In addition to pain, ocular and head motion impairments, dizziness, and unsteadiness are frequent complaints in patients with spinal diseases such as neck pain [15]. Balance impairment has been found to be common among patients with spinal diseases [16,17]. For example, a distortion of body balance and body postural control has been described in patients with a variety of neck injuries [18] probably due to a mismatch of the proprioceptive, visual, and vestibular afferent information needed to maintain proper balance [19]. The literature suggests that such postural alterations mainly appear in the acute phase of the condition, during the early stages of spinal diseases, and that they are valid predictors of a poor prognosis [20]. Furthermore, previous studies have established a possible relationship between idiopathic scoliosis and poor postural control [21,22], due to a hypothetical vestibular dysfunction or to alterations in the integration of sensorimotor information [23].
The perception of verticality involves the estimation of own-body verticality (subjective postural vertical, or SPV) and the verticality of objects appearing in the vision field (subjective visual vertical test, SVV; rod and frame test, RFT) or perceived by touch (haptic vertical) [24]. The SVV is the main test proposed to assess the perception of verticality in neurotological research and clinical practice [25]. SVV is a test that depicts one’s own body position in space with respect to gravity, and it is related with the subject’s ability to maintain balance and posture [26]. In this test, the subject adjusts a visible luminous line, in darkness, with the perceived direction of the earth vertical (the gravity line) without other visual references [27,28,29]. Another test designed to determine the perception of visual verticality is the RFT. In it, a rod is displayed, in darkness, inside a tilted or untilted frame with respect to the earth vertical [30]. Although both tests evaluate the visual perception of verticality, the SVV mainly assesses the vestibular contribution to the creation of the sense of verticality [31]. However, when the visual background is altered in the RFT by a tilted or untilted frame, proprioceptive and visual inputs are required to estimate visual verticality [32].
Finding out which potential alterations of visual verticality may occur in different spinal diseases would certainly shed light on the physiopathological mechanisms of these substantial and widespread health problems and open new therapeutic pathways for their treatment. For this reason, in recent years, interest has grown concerning the measurement of the perception of visual verticality in spinal diseases. However, studies looking into the matter have yielded contradictory results, probably due to methodological differences in the assessment and interpretation of the perception of visual verticality through RFT or SVV [33,34]. Therefore, the aim of our review and meta-analysis was to analyze the misperception of visual verticality in patients with several spinal pain and deformity diseases in comparison with healthy subjects.

2. Methods

2.1. Protocol

This meta-analysis was conducted according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses statement (PRISMA) [35] and the recommendations of the Meta-Analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) group [36].

2.2. Data Sources and Search Strategy

Two authors (E.O.-G. and R.L.-V.) independently carried out the bibliographic search between 26 December 2019 and 30 January 2020. A bibliographic search was performed for relevant literature in PubMed, MEDLINE, Scopus, Web of Science, CINAHL, and SciELO as well as in the reference lists of the full-text articles retrieved and other reviews. The keywords used were “verticality sense”, “spinal diseases”, “subjective visual vertical”, and “rod and frame test”. Medical subject headings (MeSH) and keyword bases in main studies were searched using the combination proposed in each database with the appropriate tags. No language and publication date restrictions were used. Table 1 shows the search strategies for the different databases.

2.3. Study Selection and Inclusion Criteria

Two blinded reviewers (E.O.-G. and R.L.-V.) independently screened the titles and abstracts. Each article was examined in detail if at least one of the researchers selected it during the inclusion phase based on the title or abstract. Discrepancies that arose during full-text screening were resolved by a third researcher (F.M.).
The inclusion criteria used were as follows: (1) observational studies, including cross-sectional, case–control, and cohort studies; (2) studies that measure the ability of patients with spinal diseases to estimate static SVV in the sitting or upright position; (3) studies that use the SVV test and/or the RFT with tilted and untilted frame with respect to earth vertical; and (4) studies that included a comparison groups of healthy subjects. The exclusion criteria proposed were as follows: (1) studies that assess the perception of visual verticality after any therapeutic intervention; (2) observational studies with only one group and (3) studies not susceptible to obtain the absolute error mean and its standard deviation with validated procedures according to the “Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions” [37] and the recommendations of Hozo et al. (2005) [38].

2.4. Data Extraction

Two independent reviewers (E.O.-G. and D.R.-A.) collected the data of the exposed and non-exposed groups of each study using a standardized data-collection form. Disagreements were solved by consensus with a third reviewer (R.L.-V.). The characteristics recorded of each study selected for the review were the research design; authorship and publication date; total sample size; the number of participants in the exposed (patients diagnoses of a spinal disease) and non-exposed group (healthy individuals); age; sex (male or female); type of spinal disease such as spinal pain (neck or low back pain) or musculoskeletal spinal deformity (idiopathic scoliosis); and the phase of progression since the onset (acute or chronic). The primary outcome was the perception of visual verticality using the absolute error mean and its standard deviation (SD) measured with SVV test or RFT in the exposed and healthy subjects. Other error calculation measurements of the RFT, as the variability of the error (VE), were not included in our meta-analysis due to their limited use in the scientific literature [39]. When a study met the inclusion criteria and did not provide the SD necessary for the statistical analysis, we collected the standard error mean, ranges (minimum and maximum), interquartile ranges, and the sample size to be transformed into in SD, according to the “Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Intervention” [37]. Only values obtained from the SVV test and RFT were analyzed in the quantitative synthesis. Data from the selected studies were extracted at baseline without any therapeutic intervention on the control or exposed groups.

2.5. Quality Assessment

The Newcastle–Ottawa Scale (NOS) [40] was used to assess the methodological quality of the studies included in our meta-analysis. Three criteria are included in the NOS checklist: study selection (maximum, 4 stars), comparability (maximum, 2 stars) and assessment of outcome/exposure (maximum, 3 stars) [41]. Quality scores ranged from 0 (lowest) to 9 stars (highest) [42]. The NOS score classifies the studies as low quality (score 1–3), moderate quality (score 4–6), or high quality (score 7–9) [40,43]. Following the recommendations of Meader (2014) [44] and the Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development, and Evaluation (GRADE) system [44], inconsistency, imprecision, and risk of publication bias were also evaluated. Inconsistency was evaluated through heterogeneity of findings in individual studies [45], and imprecision was assessed through the number of included studies (large: >10 studies; moderate: 5–10 studies; and small: <5 studies) and the median sample size (high: >300 participants; intermediate: 100–300 participants; and low: <100 participants) [37,44].
Two independent reviewers (A.J.I.-V. and F.M.) assessed the quality of included studies, and a third author (R.L.-V.) resolved the possible disagreements.

2.6. Statistical Analysis

The statistical software Comprehensive Meta-Analysis (Version 3.3.070 Biostat, Englewood, NJ, USA) [46] was used to perform the meta-analysis. Two authors carried out the quantitative synthesis (R.D.-P.-C and E.O.-G.). According to the recommendations of Cooper (2009) [47], the random-effects model of DerSimonian and Laird [48] was used to estimate the pooled effect and its 95% confidence interval (CI) with the aim to improve the generalization of the findings. The pooled effect was calculated as a standardized mean difference (SMD) [37,49]. The magnitude of the SMD can be interpreted in three effect strength levels: small (SMD = 0.2); medium (SMD = 0.5); and large (SMD = 0.8) [49,50]. Forest plots were used to display our findings [51]. The potential asymmetry and publication bias were visually evaluated by funnel plot [52,53], and the adjusted pooled effect was estimated (taking into account any possible publication bias) by the trim-and-fill method [54,55]. Heterogeneity was analyzed using Cochran’s Q test [56] and I2 statistic of Higgins [45] (where <25% indicates slight heterogeneity; 25%–50%, moderate heterogeneity; and ≥50%, high heterogeneity). Spinal pain diseases and idiopathic scoliosis were analyzed independently in three meta-analyses according to the test employed to measure the subjects’ perception of verticality in comparison to healthy controls, given the specific characteristics of each.

3. Results

3.1. Study Selection

Information concerning the bibliographic search and study selection process is displayed in the PRISMA flow chart (Figure 1). Based on the search criteria, 137 references were retrieved from the different databases and 4 additional records were identified in the lists of references of other articles. After removing duplicates, 101 records were reviewed. Of these, 74 studies were deleted for not being relevant by title and abstract and 12 records were rejected for not meeting the inclusion criteria. Figure 1 shows the number of records removed and the reasons for their exclusion. Finally, 15 studies [57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69,70,71] were included in this review.

3.2. Characteristics of Studies Included in the Meta-Analysis

The main characteristics of the studies analyzed are shown in Table 2. The selected studies including 11 studies with 14 samples of patients with spinal pain [57,58,59,61,62,63,64,68,69,70,71] and 4 studies with 4 samples of idiopathic scoliosis patients [60,65,66,67]. A total of 31 independent comparisons were identified. The total number of participants included in the selected studies was 2052 (mean age: 41.73 ± 16.11 years old). The mean number of participants per study was 67.48. At first, results were grouped in three overall meta-analyses. Two meta-analyses were performed for subjects with spinal pain examined using SVV or RFT. For idiopathic scoliosis, only SVV data were available.

3.3. Quality Assessment of Studies Included in the Meta-Analysis

The methodological quality of the selected studies, assessed by the Newcastle–Ottawa Scale (NOS), was low-to-moderate (mean quality: 4). Six studies [58,62,63,66,68,71] (40% of the selected references) were deemed low quality and the remaining 9 studies [57,59,60,61,64,65,67,69,70] (60% of included studies) were considered of moderate quality. Table 3 shows the NOS rating for selection, comparability, and exposure/outcome of the studies included in this review.

3.4. Results of the Overall Meta-Analysis about Perception of Visual Verticality in Spinal Pain Patients Measured with the SVV Test

In 7 studies [57,58,61,63,68,70,71] with 8 independent comparisons, the misperception of visual verticality was measured using the SVV test. These studies included 459 patients (74% female and 26% male with a mean age of 49.75 ± 11.80 years old) with spinal pain, whether neck pain [58,61,63,68,70,71] or low back pain [57] (Table 4). The pooled effect (SMD = 0.199; 95% CI = −0.04, 0.386; p = 0.056) revealed that patients with spinal pain, as measured by the SVV test and regardless of pain locus, did not exhibit signs of misperceiving visual verticality with respect to the earth vertical (Figure 2). The funnel plot was asymmetric (Figure S1), and the adjusted pooled effect calculated with the trim-and-fill method (adjusted SMD = 0.150) suggested a risk of publication bias. There was no heterogeneity among studies (I2 = 0%), and the number of participants per study (57.37) suggests that results have a low precision level.

3.5. Results of the Overall Meta-Analysis about Perception of Visual Verticality Measured with RFT in Patients with Different Spinal Pain

When RFT was used to assess the perception of visual verticality in patients with spinal pain, 8 studies [57,59,62,63,64,69,70,71] with 19 independent comparisons were selected to perform the meta-analysis. A total of 1308 individuals (69% female and 31% male with a mean age of 44.17 ± 13.12 years old) with spinal pain were reported in these articles. The participants of 7 studies [59,62,63,64,69,70,71] had been diagnosed with neck pain, and only one study concerned subjects with low back pain [57] (Table 4). The pooled effect size (SMD = 0.339; 95% CI = 0.181, 0.497; p < 0.001) signaled a moderate misperception of visual verticality in patients with spinal pain assessed with RFT (Figure 3). The funnel plot was clearly asymmetric and a certain risk of publication bias was detected (Figure S2). This was later confirmed by the calculation of the adjusted pooled effect through the trim-and-fill method. The adjusted SMD was 0.230, which suggests that the original pooled effect could be overestimated by 33%. Heterogeneity was not present in this meta-analysis (I2 = 0%), and the number of participants per study (68.84) suggested a low precision level.

3.6. Results of the Overall Meta-Analysis about Perception of Visual Verticality in Patients with Idiopathic Scoliosis

A total of 4 studies [60,65,66,67] with 4 independent comparisons reported SVV test data from 285 participants (238 females and 37 males with a mean age of 14.48 ± 0.37 years old). Their pooled effect (SMD = 0.314; 95% CI = −0.273, 0.901; p = 0.294) was not statistically significant (Table 4). It may therefore be concluded that patients with idiopathic scoliosis subject to the SVV test do not exhibit signs of misperception of the visual verticality in comparison with healthy subjects (Figure 4). No studies employed RFT to determine perception of visual verticality. Publication bias was not considered given that the funnel plot was symmetric (Figure S3) and the adjusted pooled effect, calculated with the trim-and-fill method, did not differ from the original. Heterogeneity was very slight (I2 = 2%), and the number of participants per study was 71.25, revealing a low precision level.

4. Discussion

This is the first systematic review with meta-analysis to look into the alteration of the visual perception of verticality in patients with spinal diseases. The assessment of the perception of visual verticality in research and clinical practice is frequently carried out by practitioners [72]. There are two tools that are commonly used to measure the visual perception of verticality: The subjective visual vertical test (SVV) and the rod and frame test (RFT). Different studies looking into perception of visual verticality have revealed that it is impaired in neurological disorders such as stroke [73,74,75], Parkinson’s disease [76,77], multiple sclerosis [78], vestibular dysfunctions [79,80,81], chronic dizziness [82], and type II diabetes mellitus [83], among others.
The sense of verticality is a complex construct of the central nervous system (CNS) created from the visual, vestibular, and proprioceptive inputs of the neck and eye muscles [84,85]. A failure in one or more of these information systems could produce a misperception of verticality and result in related balance disorders [86,87]. Several studies have detected balance impairment in patients with spinal pain, especially those experiencing neck pain derived from traumatic and non-traumatic neck lesions [18,19,88]. This distorted balance has been related to alterations in somatosensory cervical inputs in the absence of other vestibular pathologies [89].
Our results confirm an impairment of the visual verticality perception in spinal pain patients when they were assessed with RFT. It is important to highlight that the RFT evaluates the contribution of visual and proprioceptive afferent information to the development of the sense of visual verticality [57], while SVV mainly analyzes the weight of vestibular cues in the perception of verticality [31,90]. For this reason, our findings could be interpreted as a specific failure in integrating visual and proprioceptive inputs in order to generate a perception of visual verticality in patients with spinal pain.
Previous neurophysiological studies have explained the relationship between visual inputs and spinal proprioceptive information for the creation of a perception of verticality [75]. Mechanoreceptors from the neck establish central and reflex connections with the vestibular, visual, and sympathetic systems [91]. Some reflexes, such as the cervico-collic reflex (CCR), the cervico-ocular reflex (COR), and the tonic neck reflex (TNR) are required to exercise precise control of head and eye movements and to maintain postural stability [92]. Along these lines, studies performed in animals or humans without vestibular dysfunctions have detected an increase in the COR after damage was inflicted in experimental conditions to the neck proprioceptive inputs [93,94]. Moreover, observational studies have described distorted oculo-motor control, disturbances in gaze stability, and alterations in head eye coordination, vergence, saccades, and ocular reflex activity in patients with traumatic neck pain or myofascial neck pain [95]. Taking this together, we suggest that the misperception of visual verticality and related balance disorders observed in patients with neck pain mainly derived from the cervico-ocular pathways alteration, but not from cervico-vestibular impairments. Following the recommendations of Grod and Diakow [71], Bagust [64], and Docherty [70], we believe that RFT should be used to assess the perception of visual verticality in patients with spinal pain in order to explore the relationship between neck pain and vertical discernment.
The present meta-analysis also analyzed the perception of visual verticality in idiopathic scoliosis. The hypothetic relationship between the function of the vestibular pathways and scoliosis development comes from animal and human studies. In animal experimental studies, the researchers induced spinal deformities after surgical damage on the vestibular apparatus [96,97]. These studies performed in Xenopus laevis larvae have described both a decrease in the number of fibers on the ipsilesional VIIIth nerve and a loss in the number of neurons in the ipsilesional tangential vestibular nucleus (TAN) after the experimental unilateral labyrinthectomy (UL). This cell death mainly affects the vestibulospinal neurons in the TAN. It is worth mentioning that TAN is the main graviceptive relay vestibular nucleus, and it is closely related to the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) [98]. The loss of neurons in this nucleus after UL could provoke a bilateral imbalance of descending activity and spinal motoneuronal drive and may cause an irreversible asymmetric tone of axial and limb muscles. In consequence, the scoliotic deformity observed in this amphibian model may be the result of the permanent muscle tone imbalance. Since the TAN has a similar function to that of the mammals Deiter’s nucleus [99], the authors of these studies suggest that a previous alteration in the vestibular apparatus could provoke a spinal deformation in scoliosis patients. Moreover, some genome-wide studies [100,101] have detected in patients with scoliosis a single nucleotide polymorphism in genome regions that code for proteins implicated in vestibular development (CHL 1) [100] and vestibular function (Cntnap2) [102].
Misperception of visual verticality in patients with idiopathic scoliosis has only been measured with the SVV test. Our results demonstrate that patients with idiopathic scoliosis do not show a misperception of visual verticality when the contribution of vestibular inputs was analyzed. We suggest that the spinal deformity observed in patients with idiopathic scoliosis could be an adaptive response to a vestibular dysfunction [97]. Our hypothesis is that a previous misperception of the visual verticality due to vestibular impairment during the infant stage would force the patients to reach an inclined-rotated spine and head position in which a correct estimation of the visual verticality may be performed.
Some limitations must be considered regarding this meta-analysis. Firstly, some studies found in our bibliographic search were not included in the quantitative synthesis for failing to report the SVV or RFT values of the control group. Secondly, the low number of published studies reporting valid data for SVV or RFT reduces the precision of our meta-analysis. Moreover, future studies should determine the error of the SVV or RFT using more sensitive calculations such as the variability of the error (VE) instead of the absolute mean error or the constant error. Yet another limitation was that, in the studies considered here, the misperception of visual verticality in idiopathic scoliosis was not estimated by means of RFT. Due to this, the visual contribution to the perception of verticality and its relationship with spinal proprioception has yet to be explored for patients with idiopathic scoliosis. Before any of our findings are generalized, we must also take into account the limitations related with publication bias, although heterogeneity was not present. Future research should use RFT to examine patients with idiopathic scoliosis in order to add to the understanding of how the visual and proprioceptive systems contribute to the development of idiopathic scoliosis.

5. Conclusions

The present meta-analysis evaluated the relationship between spinal diseases and the perception of visual verticality. Patients with spinal pain diseases showed a misperception of visual verticality measured when examined using RFT. When the contribution of the vestibular inputs to the sense of verticality was analyzed in spinal diseases using the SVV test, subjects with spinal pain and idiopathic scoliosis did not show a misperception of visual verticality. In patients with spinal diseases, misperception of visual verticality seems to be related to visual and proprioceptive disturbances rather than to vestibular impairments.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/2077-0383/9/6/1725/s1, Figure S1: Funnel plot of the meta-analysis of patients with spinal pain assessed with SVV test; Figure S2: Funnel plot of the meta-analysis of patients with spinal pain assessed with RFT; and Figure S3: Funnel plot of the meta-analysis of patients with idiopathic scoliosis assessed with SVV test.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization—R.L.-V., M.F., and E.O.-G.; methodology—R.L.-V. and E.O.-G.; software—E.O.-G. and R.D.-P.-C.; validation—D.R.-A., A.J.I.-V., and F.M.; formal analysis—E.O.-G. and R.D.-P.-C.; investigation—R.L.-V., F.M., and E.O.-G.; resources—R.L.-V., E.O.-G., and A.J.I.-V.; data curation—R.L.-V., E.O.-G., and F.M.; writing, original draft preparation—E.O.-G., F.M., R.D.-P.-C., A.J.I.-V., D.R.-A., and R.L.-V.; writing, review and editing—R.L.-V., E.O.-G., and F.M.; visualization—E.O.-G., F.M., R.D.-P.-C., A.J.I.-V., D.R.-A., and R.L.-V.; supervision—F.M. and E.O.-G.; project administration—R.L.-V. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the European Regional Development fund (FEDER) in the “PO FEDER 2014-2020” of Andalusia (Spain) with the project reference: 1263880. E.O.-G. was supported by grant number FPU17/01619 of the Ministry of Science, Innovation, and Universities, Government of Spain.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge Jeff Bagust for his contribution to the conceptual understanding of the perception of visual verticality.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Ferreira, M.L.; de Luca, K. Spinal pain and its impact on older people. Best Pract. Res. Clin. Rheumatol. 2017, 31, 192–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Connelly, L.B.; Woolf, A.; Brooks, P. Cost-Effectiveness of Interventions for Musculoskeletal Conditions; Jamison, D.T., Breman, J.G., Measham, A.R., Alleyne, G., Claeson, M., Evans, D.B., Jha, P., Mills, A., Musgrove, P., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2006; ISBN 0-8213-6179-1. [Google Scholar]
  3. Friedly, J.; Standaert, C.; Chan, L. Epidemiology of spine care: The back pain dilemma. Phys. Med. Rehabil. Clin. N. Am. 2010, 21, 659–677. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Hoy, D.G.; Protani, M.; De, R.; Buchbinder, R. The epidemiology of neck pain. Best Pract. Res. Clin. Rheumatol. 2010, 24, 783–792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Hoy, D.; Brooks, P.; Blyth, F.; Buchbinder, R. The Epidemiology of low back pain. Best Pract. Res. Clin. Rheumatol. 2010, 24, 769–781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Vos, T.; Barber, R.M.; Bell, B.; Bertozzi-Villa, A.; Biryukov, S.; Bolliger, I.; Charlson, F.; Davis, A.; Degenhardt, L.; Dicker, D.; et al. Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 301 acute and chronic diseases and injuries in 188 countries, 1990–2013: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2013. Lancet (London, England) 2015, 386, 743–800. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. March, L.; Smith, E.U.R.; Hoy, D.G.; Cross, M.J.; Sanchez-Riera, L.; Blyth, F.; Buchbinder, R.; Vos, T.; Woolf, A.D. Burden of disability due to musculoskeletal (MSK) disorders. Best Pract. Res. Clin. Rheumatol. 2014, 28, 353–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Lardon, A.; Dubois, J.-D.; Cantin, V.; Piche, M.; Descarreaux, M. Predictors of disability and absenteeism in workers with non-specific low back pain: A longitudinal 15-month study. Appl. Ergon. 2018, 68, 176–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. de Luca, K.E.; Parkinson, L.; Haldeman, S.; Byles, J.E.; Blyth, F. The Relationship Between Spinal Pain and Comorbidity: A Cross-sectional Analysis of 579 Community-Dwelling, Older Australian Women. J. Manip. Physiol. Ther. 2017, 40, 459–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Lee, C.-H.; Chung, C.K.; Kim, C.H.; Kwon, J.-W. Health Care Burden of Spinal Diseases in the Republic of Korea: Analysis of a Nationwide Database from 2012 through 2016. Neurospine 2018, 15, 66–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Patel, E.A.; Perloff, M.D. Radicular Pain Syndromes: Cervical, Lumbar, and Spinal Stenosis. Semin. Neurol. 2018, 38, 634–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Dieleman, J.L.; Baral, R.; Birger, M.; Bui, A.L.; Bulchis, A.; Chapin, A.; Hamavid, H.; Horst, C.; Johnson, E.K.; Joseph, J.; et al. US Spending on Personal Health Care and Public Health, 1996–2013. JAMA 2016, 316, 2627–2646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Green, B.N.; Johnson, C.D.; Haldeman, S.; Kane, E.J.; Clay, M.B.; Griffith, E.A.; Castellote, J.M.; Smuck, M.; Rajasekaran, S.; Hurwitz, E.L.; et al. The Global Spine Care Initiative: Public health and prevention interventions for common spine disorders in low- and middle-income communities. Eur. Spine J. Off. Publ. Eur. Spine Soc. Eur. Spinal Deform. Soc. Eur. Sect. Cerv. Spine Res. Soc. 2018, 27, 838–850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Buchbinder, R.; van Tulder, M.; Öberg, B.; Costa, L.M.; Woolf, A.; Schoene, M.; Croft, P. Low back pain: A call for action. Lancet (London, England) 2018, 391, 2384–2388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Treleaven, J. Dizziness, Unsteadiness, Visual Disturbances, and Sensorimotor Control in Traumatic Neck Pain. J. Orthop. Sports Phys. Ther. 2017, 47, 492–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Thornes, E.; Robinson, H.S.; Vollestad, N.K. Dynamic balance in patients with degenerative lumbar spinal stenosis; a cross-sectional study. BMC Musculoskelet. Disord. 2018, 19, 192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Iversen, M.D.; Kale, M.K.; Sullivan, J.T.J. Pilot case control study of postural sway and balance performance in aging adults with degenerative lumbar spinal stenosis. J. Geriatr. Phys. Ther. 2009, 32, 15–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Saadat, M.; Salehi, R.; Negahban, H.; Shaterzadeh, M.J.; Mehravar, M.; Hessam, M. Postural stability in patients with non-specific chronic neck pain: A comparative study with healthy people. Med. J. Islam. Repub. Iran 2018, 32, 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Treleaven, J. Sensorimotor disturbances in neck disorders affecting postural stability, head and eye movement control—Part 2: Case studies. Man. Ther. 2008, 13, 266–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Johanson, E.; Brumagne, S.; Janssens, L.; Pijnenburg, M.; Claeys, K.; Paasuke, M. The effect of acute back muscle fatigue on postural control strategy in people with and without recurrent low back pain. Eur. Spine J. Off. Publ. Eur. Spine Soc. Eur. Spinal Deform. Soc. Eur. Sect. Cerv. Spine Res. Soc. 2011, 20, 2152–2159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Dufvenberg, M.; Adeyemi, F.; Rajendran, I.; Oberg, B.; Abbott, A. Does postural stability differ between adolescents with idiopathic scoliosis and typically developed? A systematic literature review and meta-analysis. Scoliosis Spinal Disord. 2018, 13, 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Haddas, R.; Lieberman, I.H. The Change in Sway and Neuromuscular Activity in Adult Degenerative Scoliosis Patients Pre and Post Surgery Compared With Controls. Spine (Phila Pa 1976) 2019, 44, E899–E907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Antoniadou, N.; Hatzitaki, V.; Stavridis, S.I.; Samoladas, E. Verticality perception reveals a vestibular deficit in adolescents with idiopathic scoliosis. Exp. Brain Res. 2018, 236, 1725–1734. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Dieterich, M.; Brandt, T. Perception of Verticality and Vestibular Disorders of Balance and Falls. Front. Neurol. 2019, 10, 172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Min, K.K.; Ha, J.S.; Kim, M.J.; Cho, C.H.; Cha, H.E.; Lee, J.H. Clinical use of subjective visual horizontal and vertical in patients of unilateral vestibular neuritis. Otol. Neurotol. 2007, 28, 520–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Mazibrada, G.; Tariq, S.; Perennou, D.; Gresty, M.; Greenwood, R.; Bronstein, A.M. The peripheral nervous system and the perception of verticality. Gait Posture 2008, 27, 202–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Piscicelli, C.; Barra, J.; Davoine, P.; Chrispin, A.; Nadeau, S.; Pérennou, D. Inter- and Intra-Rater Reliability of the Visual Vertical in Subacute Stroke. Stroke 2015, 46, 1979–1983. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Piscicelli, C.; Nadeau, S.; Barra, J.; Perennou, D. Assessing the visual vertical: How many trials are required? BMC Neurol. 2015, 15, 215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Funabashi, M.; Santos-Pontelli, T.E.G.; Colafêmina, J.F.; Pavan, T.Z.; Carneiro, A.A.O.; Takayanagui, O.M. A newmethod to analyze the subjective visual vertical in patients with bilateral vestibular dysfunction. Clinics 2012, 67, 1127–1131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Bagust, J.; Docherty, S.; Haynes, W.; Telford, R.; Isableu, B. Changes in rod and frame test scores recorded in schoolchildren during development--a longitudinal study. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e65321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Perennou, D.; Piscicelli, C.; Barbieri, G.; Jaeger, M.; Marquer, A.; Barra, J. Measuring verticality perception after stroke: Why and how? Neurophysiol. Clin. 2014, 44, 25–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Humphreys, B.K. Cervical outcome measures: Testing for postural stability and balance. J. Manip. Physiol. Ther. 2008, 31, 540–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Bronstein, A.M. The interaction of otolith and proprioceptive information in the perception of verticality. The effects of labyrinthine and CNS disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1999, 871, 324–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Lubeck, A.J.A.; Bos, J.E.; Stins, J.F. Framing visual roll-motion affects postural sway and the subjective visual vertical. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2016, 78, 2612–2620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzlaff, J.; Altman, D.G. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2009, 62, 1006–1012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Stroup, D.F.; Berlin, J.A.; Morton, S.C.; Olkin, I.; Williamson, G.D.; Rennie, D.; Moher, D.; Becker, B.J.; Sipe, T.A.; Thacker, S.B. Meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology: A proposal for reporting. Meta-analysis Of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) group. JAMA 2000, 283, 2008–2012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Higgins, J.P.T.; Green, S. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Intervention Version 5.1.0; updated March 2011; The Cochrane Collaboration: London, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  38. Hozo, S.P.; Djulbegovic, B.; Hozo, I. Estimating the mean and variance from the median, range, and the size of a sample. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2005, 5, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  39. Bagust, J.; Docherty, S.; Abdul Razzak, R. Re: High variability of the subjective visual vertical test of vertical perception, in some people with neck pain—Should this be a standard measure of cervical proprioception? Man. Ther. 2015, 20, e18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Wells, G.A.; Shea, B.; O’Connell, D.; Peterson, J.; Welch, V.; Losos, M.; Tugwell, P. The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for Assessing the Quality of Nonrandomised Studies in Meta-Analyses. Available online: http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.asp (accessed on 13 May 2020).
  41. Stang, A. Critical evaluation of the Newcastle-Ottawa scale for the assessment of the quality of nonrandomized studies in meta-analyses. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 2010, 25, 603–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Lo, C.K.-L.; Mertz, D.; Loeb, M. Newcastle-Ottawa Scale: Comparing reviewers’ to authors’ assessments. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2014, 14, 45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Luchini, C.; Stubbs, B.; Solmi, M.; Veronese, N. Assessing the quality of studies in meta-analyses: Advantages and limitations of the Newcastle Ottawa Scale. World J. Meta-Anal. 2017, 5, 80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Meader, N.; King, K.; Llewellyn, A.; Norman, G.; Brown, J.; Rodgers, M.; Moe-Byrne, T.; Higgins, J.P.; Sowden, A.; Stewart, G. A checklist designed to aid consistency and reproducibility of GRADE assessments: Development and pilot validation. Syst. Rev. 2014, 3, 82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Higgins, J.P.T.; Thompson, S.G.; Deeks, J.J.; Altman, D.G. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ 2003, 327, 557–560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Borenstein, M.; Hedges, L.; Higgins, J.; Rothstein, H. Comprehensive Meta-Analysis Software Version 3; Biostat, Inc.: Englewood, NJ, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  47. Cooper, H.; Hedges, L.V.; Valentine, J.C. The Handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis; Russell Sage Foundation: New York, NY, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  48. DerSimonian, R.; Laird, N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials. Control. Clin. Trials 1986, 7, 177–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
  50. Faraone, S.V. Interpreting estimates of treatment effects: Implications for managed care. Pharm. Ther. 2008, 33, 700. [Google Scholar]
  51. Rücker, G.; Schwarzer, G. Beyond the forest plot: The drapery plot. Res. Synth. Methods 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Sterne, J.A.C.; Egger, M. Funnel plots for detecting bias in meta-analysis: Guidelines on choice of axis. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2001, 54, 1046–1055. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Peters, J.L.; Sutton, A.J.; Jones, D.R.; Abrams, K.R.; Rushton, L. Comparison of two methods to detect publication bias in meta-analysis. JAMA 2006, 295, 676–680. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Duval, S.; Tweedie, R. Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics 2000, 56, 455–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Shi, L.; Lin, L.; Omboni, S. The trim-and-fill method for publication bias: Practical guidelines and recommendations based on a large database of meta-analyses. Medicine 2019, 98, e15987. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Higgins, J.P.T.; Altman, D.G.; Gøtzsche, P.C.; Jüni, P.; Moher, D.; Oxman, A.D.; Savovic, J.; Schulz, K.F.; Weeks, L.; Sterne, J.A.C. The Cochrane Collaboration’s tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ 2011, 343, d5928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Alrwaily, M.; Sparto, P.J.; Whitney, S.L. Perception of verticality is altered in people with severe chronic low back pain compared to healthy controls: A cross-sectional study. Musculoskelet. Sci. Pract. 2020, 45, 102074. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Anastasopoulos, D.; Bhatia, K.; Bronstein, A.M.; Marsden, C.D.; Gresty, M.A. Perception of spatial orientation in spasmodic torticollis. Part 2: The visual vertical. Mov. Disord. 1997, 12, 709–714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Hiengkaew, V.; Panichaporn, W.; Thanungkul, S. Postural balance, visual verticality perception, and its association in individuals with and without neck pain. J. Med. Assoc. Thail. 2014, 97, S70–S74. [Google Scholar]
  60. Le Berre, M.; Pradeau, C.; Brouillard, A.; Coget, M.; Massot, C.; Catanzariti, J.-F. Do Adolescents With Idiopathic Scoliosis Have an Erroneous Perception of the Gravitational Vertical? Spine Deform. 2019, 7, 71–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Platho-Elwischger, K.; Kranz, G.; Sycha, T.; Dunkler, D.; Rommer, P.; Mueller, C.; Auff, E.; Wiest, G. Plasticity of static graviceptive function in patients with cervical dystonia. J. Neurol. Sci. 2017, 373, 230–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Treleaven, J.; Takasaki, H. High variability of the subjective visual vertical test of vertical perception, in some people with neck pain—Should this be a standard measure of cervical proprioception? Man. Ther. 2015, 20, 183–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Uthaikhup, S.; Jull, G.; Sungkarat, S.; Treleaven, J. The influence of neck pain on sensorimotor function in the elderly. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 2012, 55, 667–672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Bagust, J.; Rix, G.D.; Hurst, H.C. Use of a Computer Rod and Frame (CRAF) Test to assess errors in the perception of visual vertical in a clinical setting—A pilot study. Clin. Chiropr. 2005, 8, 134–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Cakrt, O.; Slabý, K.; Viktorinová, L.; Kolář, P.; Jeřábek, J. Subjective visual vertical in patients with idiopatic scoliosis. J. Vestib. Res. 2011, 21, 161–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Catanzariti, J.-F.; Le Berre, M.; Coget, M.; Guyot, M.; Agnani, O.; Donzé, C. Subjective Visual Vertical and Adolescent Idiopathic Scoliosis (AIS). Ann. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 2015, 58, e111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Cheung, J.; Sluiter, W.J.; Veldhuizen, A.G.; Cool, J.C.; Van Horn, J.R. Perception of vertical and horizontal orientation in children with scoliosis. J. Orthop. Res. 2002, 20, 416–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. De Pauw, J.; De Hertogh, W.; Mercelis, R.; Saeys, W.; Hallemans, A.; Michiels, S.; Truijen, S.; Cras, P. Is perception of visual verticality intact in patients with idiopathic cervical dystonia? Acta Neurol. Belg. 2018, 118, 77–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. de Zoete, R.M.J.; Osmotherly, P.G.; Rivett, D.A.; Snodgrass, S.J. No Differences Between Individuals With Chronic Idiopathic Neck Pain and Asymptomatic Individuals on 7 Cervical Sensorimotor Control Tests: A Cross-sectional Study. J. Orthop. Sports Phys. Ther. 2020, 50, 33–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Docherty, S.; Schärer, R.; Bagust, J.; Humphreys, B.K. Perception of subjective visual vertical and horizontal in patients with chronic neck pain: A cross-sectional observational study. Man. Ther. 2012, 17, 133–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Grod, J.P.; Diakow, P.R. Effect of neck pain on verticality perception: A cohort study. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2002, 83, 412–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Piscicelli, C.; Perennou, D. Visual verticality perception after stroke: A systematic review of methodological approaches and suggestions for standardization. Ann. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 2017, 60, 208–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Molina, F.; Lomas-Vega, R.; Obrero-Gaitán, E.; Rus, A.; Almagro, D.R.; Del-Pino-Casado, R. Misperception of the subjective visual vertical in neurological patients with or without stroke: A meta-analysis. NeuroRehabilitation 2019, 44, 379–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Mansfield, A.; Fraser, L.; Rajachandrakumar, R.; Danells, C.J.; Knorr, S.; Campos, J. Is perception of vertical impaired in individuals with chronic stroke with a history of “pushing”? Neurosci. Lett. 2015, 590, 172–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Saeys, W.; Herssens, N.; Verwulgen, S.; Truijen, S. Sensory information and the perception of verticality in post-stroke patients. Another point of view in sensory reweighting strategies. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0199098. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Pereira, C.B.; Kanashiro, A.K.; Maia, F.M.; Barbosa, E.R. Correlation of impaired subjective visual vertical and postural instability in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Sci. 2014, 346, 60–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Schindlbeck, K.A.; Naumann, W.; Maier, A.; Ehlen, F.; Marzinzik, F.; Klostermann, F. Disturbance of verticality perception and postural dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease. Acta Neurol. Scand. 2018, 137, 212–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Klatt, B.N.; Sparto, P.J.; Terhorst, L.; Winser, S.; Heyman, R.; Whitney, S.L. Relationship between subjective visual vertical and balance in individuals with multiple sclerosis. Physiother. Res. Int. 2019, 24, e1757. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  79. Ogawa, Y.; Otsuka, K.; Shimizu, S.; Inagaki, T.; Kondo, T.; Suzuki, M. Subjective visual vertical perception in patients with vestibular neuritis and sudden sensorineural hearing loss. J. Vestib. Res. 2012, 22, 205–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Nair, M.A.; Mulavara, A.P.; Bloomberg, J.J.; Sangi-Haghpeykar, H.; Cohen, H.S. Visual dependence and spatial orientation in benign paroxysmal positional vertigo. J. Vestib. Res. 2018, 27, 279–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Goto, F.; Kobayashi, H.; Saito, A.; Hayashi, Y.; Higashino, K.; Kunihiro, T.; Kanzaki, J. Compensatory changes in static and dynamic subjective visual vertical in patients following vestibular schwanoma surgery. Auris Nasus Larynx 2003, 30, 29–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Pinar, H.S.; Ardic, F.N.; Topuz, B.; Kara, C.O. Subjective visual vertical and subjective visual horizontal measures in patients with chronic dizziness. J. Otolaryngol. 2005, 34, 121–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Razzak, R.A.; Bagust, J.; Docherty, S.; Hussein, W.; Al-Otaibi, A. Augmented asymmetrical visual field dependence in asymptomatic diabetics: Evidence of subclinical asymmetrical bilateral vestibular dysfunction. J. Diabetes Complicat. 2015, 29, 68–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Mittelstaedt, H. The role of the otoliths in perception of the vertical and in path integration. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1999, 871, 334–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Barra, J.; Perennou, D. Is the sense of verticality vestibular? Neurophysiol. Clin. 2013, 43, 197–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Mori, L.; Putzolu, M.; Bonassi, G.; Galeoto, G.; Mezzarobba, S.; Trompetto, C.; Avanzino, L.; Marchese, R.; Abbruzzese, G.; Pelosin, E. Haptic perception of verticality correlates with postural and balance deficits in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 2019, 66, 45–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Panic, H.; Panic, A.S.; DiZio, P.; Lackner, J.R. Direction of balance and perception of the upright are perceptually dissociable. J. Neurophysiol. 2015, 113, 3600–3609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  88. Vacherot, F.; Vaugoyeau, M.; Mallau, S.; Soulayrol, S.; Assaiante, C.; Azulay, J.P. Postural control and sensory integration in cervical dystonia. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2007, 118, 1019–1027. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Karlberg, M.; Johansson, R.; Magnusson, M.; Fransson, P.A. Dizziness of suspected cervical origin distinguished by posturographic assessment of human postural dynamics. J. Vestib. Res. 1996, 6, 37–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Chetana, N.; Jayesh, R. Subjective Visual Vertical in Various Vestibular Disorders by Using a Simple Bucket Test. Indian J. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 2015, 67, 180–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  91. Kristjansson, E.; Treleaven, J. Sensorimotor function and dizziness in neck pain: Implications for assessment and management. J. Orthop. Sports Phys. Ther. 2009, 39, 364–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  92. Della Casa, E.; Affolter Helbling, J.; Meichtry, A.; Luomajoki, H.; Kool, J. Head-eye movement control tests in patients with chronic neck pain; inter-observer reliability and discriminative validity. BMC Musculoskelet. Disord. 2014, 15, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  93. Bove, M.; Courtine, G.; Schieppati, M. Neck muscle vibration and spatial orientation during stepping in place in humans. J. Neurophysiol. 2002, 88, 2232–2241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Gosselin, G.; Rassoulian, H.; Brown, I. Effects of neck extensor muscles fatigue on balance. Clin. Biomech. (Bristol, Avon) 2004, 19, 473–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Sanchez-Gonzalez, M.C.; Perez-Cabezas, V.; Lopez-Izquierdo, I.; Gutierrez-Sanchez, E.; Ruiz-Molinero, C.; Rebollo-Salas, M.; Jimenez-Rejano, J.J. Is it possible to relate accommodative visual dysfunctions to neck pain? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2018, 1421, 62–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Lambert, F.M.; Malinvaud, D.; Glaunes, J.; Bergot, C.; Straka, H.; Vidal, P.-P. Vestibular asymmetry as the cause of idiopathic scoliosis: A possible answer from Xenopus. J. Neurosci. 2009, 29, 12477–12483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  97. Lambert, F.M.; Malinvaud, D.; Gratacap, M.; Straka, H.; Vidal, P.-P. Restricted neural plasticity in vestibulospinal pathways after unilateral labyrinthectomy as the origin for scoliotic deformations. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 6845–6856. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  98. Suwa, H.; Gilland, E.; Baker, R. Otolith ocular reflex function of the tangential nucleus in teleost fish. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1999, 871, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  99. Shao, M.; Hirsch, J.C.; Peusner, K.D. Maturation of firing pattern in chick vestibular nucleus neurons. Neuroscience 2006, 141, 711–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  100. Sharma, S.; Gao, X.; Londono, D.; Devroy, S.E.; Mauldin, K.N.; Frankel, J.T.; Brandon, J.M.; Zhang, D.; Li, Q.-Z.; Dobbs, M.B.; et al. Genome-wide association studies of adolescent idiopathic scoliosis suggest candidate susceptibility genes. Hum. Mol. Genet. 2011, 20, 1456–1466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Zhou, S.; Zhu, Z.; Qiu, X.; Wu, W.; Wang, W.; Liu, Z.; Lv, F.; Qiu, Y. Association study of IL-17RC, CHL1, DSCAM and CNTNAP2 genes polymorphisms with adolescent idiopathic scoliosis susceptibility in a Chinese Han population. Stud. Health Technol. Inform. 2012, 176, 47–51. [Google Scholar]
  102. Brunner, D.; Kabitzke, P.; He, D.; Cox, K.; Thiede, L.; Hanania, T.; Sabath, E.; Alexandrov, V.; Saxe, M.; Peles, E.; et al. Comprehensive Analysis of the 16p11.2 Deletion and Null Cntnap2 Mouse Models of Autism Spectrum Disorder. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0134572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) flow chart.
Figure 1. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) flow chart.
Jcm 09 01725 g001
Figure 2. Forest plot for the perception of the visual verticality assessed with the SVV test in patients with spinal pain (effect sizes are expressed as standardized mean difference).
Figure 2. Forest plot for the perception of the visual verticality assessed with the SVV test in patients with spinal pain (effect sizes are expressed as standardized mean difference).
Jcm 09 01725 g002
Figure 3. Forest plot for the perception of the visual verticality assessed with the RFT in patients with spinal pain (effect sizes are expressed as standardized mean difference).
Figure 3. Forest plot for the perception of the visual verticality assessed with the RFT in patients with spinal pain (effect sizes are expressed as standardized mean difference).
Jcm 09 01725 g003
Figure 4. Forest plot for the perception of the visual verticality assessed with the SVV in patients with idiopathic scoliosis (effect sizes are expressed as standardized mean difference).
Figure 4. Forest plot for the perception of the visual verticality assessed with the SVV in patients with idiopathic scoliosis (effect sizes are expressed as standardized mean difference).
Jcm 09 01725 g004
Table 1. Bibliographic search strategy.
Table 1. Bibliographic search strategy.
DatabasesBibliographic Search Strategy
MEDLINE PubMed(sensory integration (tiab) OR vertical perception (tiab) OR vertical sense (tiab) OR subjective visual vertical (tiab) OR subjective visual vertical test (tiab) OR subjective visual vertical perception (tiab) OR “rod and frame” (tiab)) and (spine (mh) OR spine (tiab) OR musculoskeletal disease (mh) OR musculoskeletal diseases (tiab) OR spinal diseases (mh) OR spinal diseases (tiab) OR spinal curvatures (mh) OR spinal curvatures (tiab) OR neck pain (mh) OR neck pain (tiab) OR neck injuries (mh) OR neck injuries (tiab) OR scoliosis (mh) OR scoliosis (tiab) OR adolescent idiopathic scoliosis (tiab) OR low back pain (mh) OR low back pain (tiab)
Scopus(TITLE-ABS-KEY ((“subjective visual vertical” OR “perception of verticality” OR “visual verticality” OR “verticality sense” OR “rod and frame”)) AND TITLE-ABS-KEY ((“spinal pain” OR “spinal diseases” OR “musculoskeletal disorders” OR “neck pain” OR “neck injuries” OR “idiopathic scoliosis” OR “low back pain”)))
Web of ScienceTOPIC:((*subjective visual vertical* OR *perception of verticality* OR *visual verticality* OR *rod and frame*)) AND TOPIC: ((*spinal pain* OR *spinal diseases* OR *musculoskeletal disorders* OR *neck pain* OR *neck injuries* OR *idiopathic scoliosis* OR *low back pain*))
SciELO(subjective visual vertical or verticality perception or verticality or verticality sense)
CINAHL(AB perception of verticality OR AB verticality sense OR AB subjective visual vertical OR AB visual vertical OR AB “rod and frame”) AND (AB musculoskeletal diseases OR AB neck pain OR AB idiopathic scoliosis OR AB low back pain)
Table 2. Main characteristics of the studies included in the meta-analysis.
Table 2. Main characteristics of the studies included in the meta-analysis.
Study____________________Exposed GroupControl Group
Author and YearDesignVV
Measure
NNexpAge (mean)Gender
(M/F)
PathologyOnsetNcAge (mean)Gender (M/F)
Alrwaily et al. 2020 [57]CCSVV/
RFT
643942.414/27LBPC25348/17
Anastasopoulos et al. 1997 [58]CCSVV582950.2NRNPC2945.5NR
Bagust et al.
2005 [64]
CCRFT887138.9724/47NPC/A1733.67/10
Cakrt et al.
2011 [65]
CCSVV462314.55/18ISC23145/18
Catanzariti et al.
2015 [66]
CCSVV403514.24/31NPC514.95/0
Cheung et al.
2002 [67]
CHSVV129891515/74ISC40128/32
De Pauw et al. 2017 [68]CCSVV542459.24/20NPC3059.412/18
De Zoete et al. 2020 [69]CCRFT1005035.520/30NPC50NR23/27
Docherty et al. 2012 [70]CCSVV/
RFT
1005048.110/40NPC5047.910/40
Grod et al.
2002 [71]
CHSVV/
RFT
361938.58/11NPA1738.610/7
Hiengkaew et al. 2014 [59]CCRFT281444,43/11NPC1443,67/7
Le Berre et al.
2019 [60]
CCSVV603014.230/0ISC3013.930/0
Platho-Elwischger et al. 2017 [61]CCSVV43305911/19NPC1352,84/9
Treleaven et al. 2015 [62]CCRFT1267833,531/47NPC4829,413/35
Uthaikhup et al. 2012 [63]CCRFT402073,26/14NPC2069,58/12
VV measure: Visual Vertical measure; CC: case–control studies; CH: cohort studies; SVV: subjective visual vertical test; RFT: rod and frame test; N: sample size; Nexp: cases sample size; M: male; F: female; LBP: low back pain; NP: neck pain; IS: idiopathic scoliosis; C: chronic; A: acute; Nc: controls sample size; NR: not reported data.
Table 3. Newcastle–Ottawa Scale (NOS) score for methodological quality assessment of included studies.
Table 3. Newcastle–Ottawa Scale (NOS) score for methodological quality assessment of included studies.
StudyS1S2S3S4C1E1E2E3Total
Alrwaily et al. 2020 [57]-*****-*-6
Anastasopoulos et al. 1997 [58]----*---1
Bagust et al. 2005 [64]--****-**6
Cakrt et al. 2011 [65]*-*****--6
Catanzariti et al. 2015 [66]-------*1
Cheung et al. 2002 [67]-****---4
De Pauw et al. 2017 [68]---***--3
De Zoete et al. 2020 [69]--***-*-4
Docherty et al. 2012 [70]--****--*5
Grod et al. 2002 [71]----*---1
Hiengkaew et al. 2014 [59]--****--*5
Le Berre et al. 2019 [60]-*-**-**5
Platho-Elwischger et al. 2017 [61]-*****--*6
Treleaven et al. 2015 [62]---**--*3
Uthaikhup et al. 2012 [63]--**--*-3
Each study can be awarded a maximum of one star for each numbered item within the Selection (S) and Exposure (E) categories. A maximum of two stars can be given for Comparability (C). S1: adequate case definition; S2: representativeness of the cases; S3: selection of controls; S4: definition of controls; C1: comparability of cases and controls; E1: ascertainment of exposure; E2: same method of ascertainment for cases and controls; E3: non-response rate.
Table 4. Summary of findings in the meta-analyses.
Table 4. Summary of findings in the meta-analyses.
Effect SizePublication BiasHeterogeneity
GroupKNNsSMD95% CIp-ValueFunnel PlotTrim and Fillq-ValueI2
Adjusted
SMD
% of Change
SVV in spinal pain patients845957.370.191−0.040;
0.386
0.054Asym.0.15022%3.8570%
RFT in spinal pain patients19130868.840.3390.181;
0.497
0.000Asym.0.24828%16.1240%
SVV in
IS patients
432581.250.314−0.273;
0.901
0.294Sym.0.3140%3.0612%
SVV: subjective visual vertical test; RFT: rod and frame test; IS: idiopathic scoliosis; K: number of studies; N: number of participants in each meta-analysis; Ns: number of participants per study; SMD: standardized mean difference; 95% CI: confidence interval 95%; Asym: asymmetric funnel plot; Sym: symmetric funnel plot; I2: degree of inconsistency of Higgins.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Obrero-Gaitán, E.; Molina, F.; Del-Pino-Casado, R.; Ibáñez-Vera, A.J.; Rodríguez-Almagro, D.; Lomas-Vega, R. Visual Verticality Perception in Spinal Diseases: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 1725. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9061725

AMA Style

Obrero-Gaitán E, Molina F, Del-Pino-Casado R, Ibáñez-Vera AJ, Rodríguez-Almagro D, Lomas-Vega R. Visual Verticality Perception in Spinal Diseases: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2020; 9(6):1725. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9061725

Chicago/Turabian Style

Obrero-Gaitán, Esteban, Francisco Molina, Rafael Del-Pino-Casado, Alfonso Javier Ibáñez-Vera, Daniel Rodríguez-Almagro, and Rafael Lomas-Vega. 2020. "Visual Verticality Perception in Spinal Diseases: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis" Journal of Clinical Medicine 9, no. 6: 1725. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9061725

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop