1. Introduction
Social media have a strong impact on the way users interact and share information. The process through which users create and share opinions on brands, products, and services, i.e., the electronic word-of-mouth (eWOM) is gaining increasing attention. In the online context, the eWOM has been transformed from a communication act that takes place in a private one-to-one context to a one-to-many complex interaction. This represents the most powerful aspect of the eWOM. The reach of information sharing through eWOM can be both broad and fast. Companies know that controlling the dynamics of information sharing is very difficult. This need for improving control is one of the reasons why there is a growing interest in understanding how the structure of a social network can affect the dynamics of user interaction and information sharing.
Several previous studies have focused on the role of influencers,
i.
e., nodes with a central position in the network. In particular, microblogging platforms such as Twitter are the focus of a wide range of studies that aim at understanding how messages spread inside the social network and how the role of the message author impacts on message reach. Microblogging networks are more and more used by companies as a communication medium for the promotion and engagement of customers. An emerging paradigm for the study of social networks as a communication medium is the attention economy [
1]. This paradigm starts from the observation that brands are involved in a competition for gaining the attention of possible customers. While on traditional media attention can focus not only on content, but also on the way a message is conveyed, on social media content plays a more central role [
2]. Content is even more central with microblogging, as the shortness of messages compels users to focus on the core of the information that they want to share. On Twitter, the standard size of a message limited to 140 characters is roughly the typical size of headlines and encourages users to produce content that are easy to consume.
Our claim is that, while the information shared by influencers has a broader reach, the content of messages plays a critical role and can be a determinant of the social influence of the message irrespective of the centrality of the message’s author. We make a distinction between influence and influencers. While nodes are influencers depending on their centrality in the social network, influence is the actual impact of messages, which depends not only on the structure of the network, but also on the ability of message content to raise attention. Studying how content spread within social networks can be useful for explaining why some trends are followed more quickly and successfully than others, thus providing an invaluable input to business intelligence. The concept of influence can provide more accurate insights on how companies can leverage social media to strengthen their brand’s reputation.
In this paper, we put forward a set of hypotheses supporting the claim that the content of messages plays a critical role and can be a determinant of the social influence of the message irrespective of the centrality of the message’s author. In this paper, we focus on multimedia content as an important characteristic of online communication patterns. We put forward the following three main hypotheses: (1) posts with a link to multimedia content (photo or video) are more retweeted than posts without a link; (2) posts linking a photo are more retweeted than posts linking a video, and 3) posts linking a video raise more sentiment than posts linking a photo. Hypotheses are tested on a sample of roughly two million tweets posted in July 2011 including comments on Berlin, London, Madrid, and Milan relevant from a tourism perspective.
The remainder of this paper is structured as follows.
Section 2 presents related research and highlights the innovative aspects of this work.
Section 3 discusses our research hypotheses, while
Section 4 reports testing results. Finally,
Section 5 draws some conclusions and presents future research directions.
3. Research Hypotheses
In this paper, we still focus on Twitter, but we address multimedia content as an important characteristic of tweets. Regarding multimedia content, a qualitative analysis conducted by Crowd Science [
20] on Facebook in 2011 shows how posts with multimedia content, especially pictures, receive a significantly higher number of
likes (see
Figure 1). To the best of our knowledge, the literature does not provide similar studies focusing on Twitter. To fill this literature gap, we put forward the following research hypothesis aiming at verifying whether this finding is valid with Twitter:
H1: Tweets with a link to multimedia content (photo or video) are more retweeted than posts without a link.
Figure 1.
Multimedia and Facebook likes [
20].
In our second hypothesis, we make a distinction between videos and photos. As a general observation, we posit that photos convey a more immediate message, as they do not involve a sustained attention to be understood. The literature provides evidence highlighting the virality of shorter videos, irrespective of technical issues related to bandwidth and devices [
21]. In general, shorter videos are recognized to be more viral [
22]. Conversely, longer videos require more motivation to be viewed and, from a technical standpoint, are more demanding in terms of bandwidth and more restrictive on the set of enabled mobile devices. Photos are more accessible, require limited time and resources to be downloaded, can be viewed on any mobile device, and have been proved to have a longer persistence in people’s memory [
23]. These considerations lead us to the following hypothesis:
H2: Tweets linking a photo are more retweeted than posts linking a video.
On the other hand, there is general agreement in crediting a greater emotional impact to videos [
21,
22,
23,
24,
25,
26]. Videos can serve as a powerful communication vehicle that can be made more impactful by designing the right combination of color, content, lighting, and movement of an image. These characteristics are used extensively by professionals on web sites and advertisements to draw attention and leave a lasting emotional impression. Compared to videos, photos provide more limited design options, with a generally lower emotional impact. We focused on the sentiment of posts to characterize the emotional impact of content. This leads to our third hypothesis.
H3: Tweets linking a video raise more sentiment than tweets linking a photo.
Note that hypotheses H1–H3 are expressed in terms of volumes of posts. Similar considerations can be made to reformulate hypotheses H1–H3 in terms of speed of retweeting. Hypothesis H1 posits that volumes of retweeting are greater for tweets linking multimedia content, as this type of content is broadly recognized to be viral. However, virality has both a volume and a time speed dimension. For example, Wallsten notes how the speed with which viral videos cross the boundaries of one’s circle of direct friends is far reaching in a very short time frame, compared to more traditional forms of communication based on broadcasting [
27].
H4: Tweets with a link to multimedia content (photo or video) receive more retweets per time unit than posts without a link.
Following a short URL linking to a photo is clearly faster than following a link to a video. Previous studies from cognitive sciences indicate that the emotional status of individuals changes rapidly with a sequence of images [
28]. It has been demonstrated that images embedded in a video create an unconscious permanent reaction when they are not consistent with the storyline underlying the video stream. This technique is adopted in psychology to recall traumatic events [
29] or in education to stimulate reactions and reduce feedback time [
28]. Overall, previous studies indicate that the time required to obtain a reaction is shorter for images compared to other forms of communication and, in particular, videos [
30].These considerations lead us to the following hypothesis.
H5: Tweets linking a photo are retweeted more quickly than posts linking a video.
In H6, we mitigate H5 by positing that videos show faster dynamics compared to photos when they demonstrate capable of raising sentiment. As noted before, videos can have a stronger emotional impact. When they do, they raise sentiment. We now focus on videos that have raised sentiment, as expressed by the comments attached to the posting or reposting of their URLs. This subset of videos may have the capability of raising more interest than pictures and, thus, receive more attention in terms of retweets per time unit. Photos have been found to be more accessible and more likely to be remembered, but also less viral [
31]. Our next hypothesis states that viral videos have faster dynamics compared to photos.
H6: Among tweets with sentiment, tweets linking a video receive more retweets per time unit than tweets linking a photo.
4. Empirical Testing
Hypotheses are tested on a sample of approximately two million tweets posted in July 2011 including comments on Berlin, London, Madrid, and Milan relevant from a tourism perspective. Analyses have been limited to tweets written in the English language. Retweets are roughly 270,000 out of our total sample. We have clustered our sample by grouping all retweets of a post with the original post. In this way, we have obtained 110,000 clusters. The number of retweets per cluster ranges from 8515 to one, with mean value equal to two.
We have divided clusters into two sets, the first including clusters where the original tweet has a link to multimedia information, the second including tweets without a link or linking other types of content. Then, we have made a distinction between photos and videos by automatically detecting links to the most common sources of images, including Flickr, Instagram, TwitPic and Yfrog, and videos, including TwitVid, TwitCam, YouTube, and Vimeo. This process resulted in 1627 clusters of tweets linking photos and 586 clusters of tweets linking videos.
A descriptive analysis of the samples highlights the presence of many zeros,
i.
e., the distributions of our samples are right-skewed. Following a common rule of thumb, we preliminary tested the medians of the distributions. We adopted the Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney (WMW) test because of its recognized effectiveness [
32].
We have run a WMW test of the distributions associated with tweets containing a link to multimedia content and tweets without a link. The descriptive statistics of the samples are reported in
Table 1. The WMW test (
z = 5,765,
p = 0.000) shows that there are significant differences between the two distributions. Often this statistic is used to compare a hypothesis regarding equality of medians. Since the
U statistic (and the normalized version,
z) tests whether two samples are drawn from identical populations, equality of medians follows as a consequence.
Then, we have run a
t-
test on the mean value of retweets in our sample sets. Results are reported in
Table 2.
Table 1.
Descriptive statistics of our sample.
Cluster | N | Mean | Standard Deviation | Standard Error Mean |
---|
No Link | 74,294 | 1.550 | 34.937 | 0.128 |
Photos and Videos | 2,213 | 2.770 | 26.390 | 0.561 |
Table 2 shows how the
t-
test indicates that the difference of the mean value of retweets with and without links to multimedia information is statistically significant (
p = 0.033). We can conclude that there is a statistically significant difference between the mean number of retweets for posts that link multimedia information and posts that do not link multimedia information. This means that the hypothesis 1 is supported by our data.
Table 2.
T-test for equality of mean values (H1).
t | df | Significant (2-tailed) | Mean Difference | Standard Error Difference | 95% Confidence Interval of the Difference |
---|
Lower | Upper |
---|
−2.137 | 2,448.788 | 0.033 | −1.230 | 0.575 | −2.358 | −0.101 |
Table 3 shows the mean value and standard deviation of the number of retweets of posts linking photos compared to posts linking videos. Clearly, posts linking photos are retweeted five times more than posts linking videos.
Table 3.
Descriptive statistics of our sample.
Cluster | N | Mean | Standard Deviation | Standard Error Mean |
---|
Photos | 1,626 | 3.520 | 30.707 | 0.762 |
Videos | 586 | 0.730 | 2.889 | 0.119 |
A WMW test (
z = −3,628,
p = 0.000) shows that there are significant differences between the two distributions. The results of
t-
test reported in
Table 4 support hypothesis 2.
Table 4.
T-test on retweeting of posts linking photos vs. videos (H2).
T | df | Significant (2-tailed) | Mean Difference | Standard Error Difference | 95% Confidence Interval of the Difference |
---|
Lower | Upper |
---|
3.620 | 1,702.950 | 0.000 | 2.790 | 0.771 | 1.278 | 4.302 |
In order to test the third hypothesis, we used a semantic analysis tool [
33] to select tweets that express opinions on Milan city,
i.
e., tweets carrying sentiment (either positive or negative). Opinions have been classified according to the Anholt model [
34], providing a set of city brand drivers relevant from a tourism perspective, e.g., arts and culture, services and transports, food and drinks,
etc.
Table 5 shows the mean value and standard deviation of the number of tweets with sentiment (positive or negative) linking photos compared to posts linking videos. Clearly, posts linking videos raise more sentiment than posts linking photos.
Table 5.
Descriptive statistics of our sample.
Cluster | N | Mean | Standard Deviation | Standard Error Mean |
---|
Photos | 159 | 3.383 | 9.212 | 0.730 |
Videos | 83 | 4.060 | 4.575 | 0.502 |
Because of the small sample available for validating the third hypothesis, we adopted the Kolmogorov–Smirnov (KS) test instead of the Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney. Indeed, for very small samples the KS test is preferable to the Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney test, while the latter is preferred for large samples [
35]. The KS test (
D = 1,372,
p = 0.040) shows that there are significant differences between the two distributions. The results of t-test reported in
Table 6 support hypothesis 3.
Table 6.
T-test on sentiment of posts linking photos vs. videos (H3).
t | df | Significant (2-tailed) | Mean Difference | Standard Error Difference | 95% Confidence Interval of the Difference |
---|
Lower | Upper |
---|
−2.063 | 1,405.219 | 0.039 | −0.245 | 0.119 | −0.479 | −0.012 |
The same dataset has been used to validate the hypotheses about the speed of retweeting. We have calculated the time (in seconds) elapsed between each original tweet and its retweets. A Kendall’s Tau correlation test [
36] was run to determine the relationship between the volumes and the speed of retweeting. A small, positive correlation was found between the volumes and the speed of retweeting, with high statistical significance (
τ = 0.231,
p = 0.002). This small correlation supports the need for testing hypotheses 4–6.
Table 7 shows the mean value and standard deviation of the retweeting times of tweets linking multimedia content compared to posts without a link. Cleary, the mean values of the two sets of tweets are considerably different.
Table 7.
Descriptive statistics: multimedia vs. no link (speed of retweeting).
Cluster | N | Mean | Standard Deviation | Standard Error Mean |
---|
No Link | 114,809 | 412.580 | 206.274 | 0.609 |
Photos and Videos | 6,141 | 283.180 | 169.649 | 2.165 |
The results of
t-
test reported in
Table 8 support hypothesis 4.
Table 8.
T-test on speed of retweeting of post linking multimedia vs. no link (H4).
t | df | Significant (2-tailed) | Mean Difference | Standard Error Difference | 95% Confidence Interval of the Difference |
---|
Lower | Upper |
---|
57.539 | 7,147.067 | 0.000 | 129.395 | 2.249 | 124.986 | 133.803 |
Table 9 shows the mean value and standard deviation of the retweeting times of tweets linking photos compared to tweets linking videos. Data show that posts linking photos are retweeted more quickly than posts linking videos.
Table 9.
Descriptive statistics: photos vs. videos (speed of retweeting).
Cluster | N | Mean | Standard Deviation | Standard Error Mean |
---|
Photos | 5,716 | 274.590 | 166.682 | 2.205 |
Videos | 425 | 398.780 | 167.058 | 8.104 |
The results of
t-
test reported in
Table 10 support hypothesis 5.
Table 10.
T-test on speed of retweeting of post linking photos vs. videos (H5).
t | df | Significant (2-tailed) | Mean Difference | Standard Error Difference | 95% Confidence Interval of the Difference |
---|
Lower | Upper |
---|
−14.788 | 488.892 | 0.000 | −124.194 | 8.398 | −140.695 | −107.693 |
Finally,
Table 11 shows the mean value and standard deviation of the retweeting times of tweets with sentiment (positive or negative) linking photos compared to posts linking videos. As in the previous case, posts linking photos are retweeted more quickly than posts linking videos.
Table 11.
Descriptive statistics: tweets with sentiment linking photos vs. videos (speed of retweeting).
Cluster | N | Mean | Standard Deviation | Standard Error Mean |
---|
Photos | 234 | 332.190 | 212.080 | 13.864 |
Videos | 36 | 429.890 | 197.001 | 32.834 |
The results of
t-
test reported in
Table 12 support hypothesis 6.
Table 12.
T-test on speed of retweeting of post with sentiment linking photos vs. videos (H6).
t | df | Significant (2-tailed) | Mean Difference | Standard Error Difference | 95% Confidence Interval of the Difference |
---|
Lower | Upper |
---|
−2.741 | 48.363 | 0.009 | −97.697 | 35.641 | −169.343 | −26.050 |