Next Article in Journal
Cadmium Sources and Toxicity
Next Article in Special Issue
Mechanisms of Neurotoxicity Associated with Exposure to the Herbicide Atrazine
Previous Article in Journal
Comparison of the Respiratory Toxicity and Total Cholinesterase Activities in Dimethyl Versus Diethyl Paraoxon-Poisoned Rats
Previous Article in Special Issue
Chlorpyrifos- and Dichlorvos-Induced Oxidative and Neurogenic Damage Elicits Neuro-Cognitive Deficits and Increases Anxiety-Like Behavior in Wild-Type Rats
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

A Neurodevelopmental Model of Combined Pyrethroid and Chronic Stress Exposure

1
Department of Environmental Health Sciences, Emory University Rollins School of Public Health, Atlanta, GA 30329, USA
2
Center for Neurodegenerative Disease, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Toxics 2019, 7(2), 24; https://doi.org/10.3390/toxics7020024
Submission received: 1 March 2019 / Revised: 26 April 2019 / Accepted: 30 April 2019 / Published: 2 May 2019
(This article belongs to the Collection Xenobiotics in Developmental Neurotoxicity)

Abstract

:
Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most common neurodevelopmental disorders of childhood and previous studies indicate the dopamine system plays a major role in ADHD pathogenesis. Two environmental exposures independently associated with dopaminergic dysfunction and ADHD risk include exposure to deltamethrin, a pyrethroid insecticide, and chronic stress. We hypothesized that combined neurodevelopmental exposure to both deltamethrin and corticosterone (CORT), the major stress hormone in rodents, would result in additive changes within the dopamine system. To study this, we developed a novel dual exposure paradigm and exposed pregnant C57BL/6 dams to 3 mg/kg deltamethrin through gestation and weaning, and their offspring to 25 μg/mL CORT dissolved in the drinking water through adulthood. Midbrain RNA expression as well as striatal and cortical protein expression of key dopaminergic components were investigated, in addition to ADHD-like behavioral tasks and electrochemical dopamine dynamics via fast-scan cyclic voltammetry. Given the well-described sexual dimorphism of ADHD, males and females were assessed separately. Males exposed to deltamethrin had significantly decreased midbrain Pitx3 expression, decreased cortical tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) expression, increased activity in the Y maze, and increased dopamine uptake rate in the dorsal striatum. These effects did not occur in males exposed to CORT only, or in males exposed to both deltamethrin and CORT, suggesting that CORT may attenuate these effects. Additionally, deltamethrin- and CORT-exposed females did not display these dopaminergic features, which indicates these changes are sex-specific. Our results show dopaminergic changes from the RNA through the functional level. Moreover, these data illustrate the importance of testing multiple environmental exposures together to better understand how combined exposures that occur in certain vulnerable populations could affect similar neurodevelopmental systems, as well as the importance of studying sex differences of these alterations.

1. Introduction

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most common neurodevelopmental disorders of childhood [1]. Characteristics include symptoms within three domains: inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity, that manifest by age 12 and can persist into adulthood [2,3]. To date, no singular pathogenic mechanism of ADHD is known, and ADHD is likely to be multifactorial, involving genetic, epigenetic, and environmental components. Several monoaminergic neurotransmitter circuits have been implicated in ADHD. Notably, genetic studies reveal variants in several genes related to the dopamine system are associated with ADHD, including dopaminergic receptors, enzymes, and transporters [4,5,6,7]. Variants in the dopamine receptor 5 gene (DRD5) modulate age of ADHD onset, while variants in the dopamine transporter (DAT1) gene predict severity of hyperactivity and impulsivity symptoms [8,9]. Lower DNA methylation of DRD4 is also associated with an increase in ADHD symptoms in children at age 6 [10]. Cohort studies associate altered DAT levels with ADHD [11,12,13] and functional MRI studies indicate that dopaminergic brain regions are important in children with ADHD and ADHD-associated SNPs (single nucleotide polymorphisms) in the DAT1 gene [14,15]. Additionally, a Dat1 knockout mouse displays hyperactivity, while a Dat1 overexpressing mouse model shows impulsivity behaviors [16,17,18,19,20]. Finally, methylphenidate targets the dopamine and norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors and a differential drug response is associated with Dat1 genotype [21]. Based on these genetic, mechanistic, and pharmacologic data, there is strong evidence for a role of dopaminergic signaling in ADHD pathogenesis.
Dopamine is a catecholamine neurotransmitter synthesized by tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) and metabolized by catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) and monoamine oxidase (MAO) enzymes [22], and subsequently transported into dopaminergic vesicles by the vesicular monoamine transporter 2 (VMAT2) [23]. Upon stimulation, vesicles release dopamine across the synapse [24]. Dopamine interacts with postsynaptic dopamine receptors (DRD1–5) and is then recycled via DAT or metabolized after neurotransmission [24]. Dopamine is primarily produced by dopaminergic cell bodies in the midbrain, specifically in the substantia nigra pars compacta (SNpc) and ventral tegmental area (VTA) [25]. The SNpc sends projections to the dorsal striatum, where dopaminergic signaling plays a key role in learning and motor function [26]. The VTA sends projections to the nucleus accumbens, olfactory bulb, amygdala, hippocampus, prefrontal cortex, and cingulate cortex. In these areas, dopamine modulates motivation, reward-related behavior, attention, and memory [26]. Many of these dopaminergic networks are implicated in ADHD [27].
While extensive genetic studies suggest that ADHD is familial, this does not preclude exogenous risk factors that further modulate risk of the disease [28]. Studies of various perinatal and postnatal environmental exposures including maternal smoking [29], early childhood adversity [30,31,32,33], and exposure to environmental toxicants have been linked with ADHD [28,34,35]. The neurodevelopmental process is especially vulnerable to environmental disruption because environmental contaminants may cross the placental barrier, pass through the immature blood-brain barrier, or transfer into breast milk where additional exposure can occur [36,37,38,39]. ADHD prevalence is higher in low-income children [40], suggesting environmental risk factors affect certain socioeconomic groups differentially and contribute to this health disparity. Two environmental risk factors linked to ADHD and low socioeconomic status are exposure to pyrethroid insecticides and chronic psychosocial stress [30,41,42,43,44].
Pyrethroid insecticides are synthetic analogues of pyrethrins, compounds derived from Chrysanthemum cinerariaefolium, that bind the α subunit of voltage-gated Na+ channels and hold them open to cause neuronal hyperexcitability [45]. Type II pyrethroids, such as deltamethrin, can also inhibit GABAA receptors and voltage-gated chloride channels to potentiate neuronal excitability [46]. Although pyrethroids are known to target neuronal channels present in most neurons, work from our group and others has shown that the dopamine system is uniquely vulnerable to pyrethroid exposure. Treatment of adult mice with pyrethroids increases maximal dopamine uptake and increases striatal DAT levels [47,48]. This increase in dopamine uptake is mediated by DAT [49], and alterations in dopamine release were replicated in experiments directly injecting pyrethroids into the rat striatum [50]. More recently, we discovered that exposure to deltamethrin during critical periods of neurodevelopment results in similar alterations in the dopamine system. Specifically, we observed increased DAT and dopamine receptor 1 (DRD1) expression that was associated with hyperactivity and impulsivity, as well as deficient memory and attention behaviors that reflect clinical symptoms of ADHD [41]. In addition to animal studies, elevated levels of pyrethroids are associated with ADHD in humans. Notably, children in the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) that have detectable pyrethroid metabolites in urine have an increased risk of ADHD [41]. This association appears to be higher in boys, and in children with hyperactive-impulsive symptoms [51]. Together, these results indicate that pyrethroids alter dopamine activity, and pyrethroid exposure is an important ADHD risk factor to evaluate.
Another environmental risk associated with ADHD is socioeconomic status [30,52]. At least part of this risk has been attributed to the higher levels of psychosocial stress experienced by individuals in these communities due to factors within their homes and communities [53,54,55]. This psychosocial stress activates the hypothalamic–pituitary adrenal axis to release the major stress hormone, cortisol in humans and corticosterone in rodents [56]. Studies measuring children’s hair cortisol levels show an inverse relationship between socioeconomic status and cortisol [57,58], suggesting effects of disproportionate psychosocial stress start early in life. In rodent models, corticosterone has been shown to interact with the dopamine system: acute stressors such as a tail pinch induce dopamine release in the medial prefrontal cortex, and this is regulated by glucocorticoid action on dopamine neurons in the VTA [59]. Rodent pups separated from their mothers, a model of early life stress, exhibit hyperactivity and decreased attention that improves with methylphenidate [44]. Additionally, rats subjected to a visible burrow system, a chronic stress paradigm, exhibit decreased DAT and DRD2 expression in the striatum [60]. These results indicate that chronic stress influences dopamine signaling and neurologic alterations that may underlie ADHD. Interestingly, glucocorticoid receptors, which potentiate the signaling of the stress hormone cortisol, co-localize with dopamine neurons in the SNpc and VTA [61], suggesting that stress may impact development and function of the dopamine system. In addition to experiencing greater amounts of psychosocial stress, low socioeconomic status groups are more likely to be exposed to environmental contaminants [62], including pyrethroid insecticides. Elevated levels of pyrethroids have been observed in low-income, urban populations, likely due to poor housing conditions and increased need for pest control [63,64,65]. Certain populations may therefore be exposed to high amounts of both cortisol and pyrethroid insecticides that could modulate risk of dopaminergic dysfunction and development of ADHD.
We hypothesized that combined pyrethroid insecticide and stress hormone exposure would lead to additive changes in RNA expression, protein expression, and function of key components of the dopamine system, thereby potentially increasing the risk of developing ADHD in certain populations. Previous studies have investigated the effects of singular environmental exposures on the development of the dopamine system. However, we developed a novel neurodevelopmental mouse model of combined exposure to deltamethrin, a pyrethroid insecticide, and corticosterone (CORT), the major stress hormone in rodents, from gestation through adulthood. Our exposure approach is the first to describe dopaminergic RNA expression, protein expression, and functional effects in response to these two exposures together, and is also the first to describe sex-specific dopaminergic effects of isolated deltamethrin and oral CORT exposure in greater detail. We illustrate that there are effects of deltamethrin on dopaminergic gene expression, protein expression, behavior, and release kinetics. Interestingly, our results show that these effects are not additive as we initially hypothesized, though our results are sex-specific and contribute to the current understanding of the well-described sexual dimorphism of ADHD.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Animals

Eight-week-old C57BL/6NCrl wild-type mice (Charles River Labs, Wilmington, MA, USA) received food and water ad libitum and were maintained on a 12:12 dark/light cycle. Females were dosed with 3 mg/kg deltamethrin (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA) every three days for two weeks prior to breeding in triplicate. Deltamethrin dosing continued during breeding and gestation. One male and one female mouse per litter were utilized for each experiment to reduce confounding due to litter effects. Throughout, an independent litter represents one sample (n = 1). All experiments were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at Emory University (Protocol DAR-4000074-ENTRPR-N, approved at 14 September 2017) and were conducted in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide of Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.

2.2. Exposure Paradigm

We utilized a deltamethrin dose of 3 mg/kg based on our previous study showing dopaminergic effects upon neurodevelopmental exposure to 3 mg/kg deltamethrin in male offspring. Additionally, this dose models a realistic exposure in the human population [66,67,68,69,70,71,72,73,74] and is lower than the developmental no observable adverse effect limit (NOAEL). Adult C57BL/6NCrl females were randomized to the deltamethrin or vehicle treatment group and were dosed every 3 days during gestation, lactation, and weaning at postnatal day (PND) 21. Deltamethrin was administered via corn oil dissolved in peanut butter to minimize trauma to pregnant mice. Upon weaning, litters were randomized to receive either oral corticosterone (CORT; Steraloids Inc., Newport, RI, USA) dissolved in drinking water or a drinking water vehicle. CORT was dissolved in the drinking water to minimize handling stress and reduce variation in CORT levels seen in behavioral chronic stress paradigms [75,76]. CORT doses were prepared as previously described [77]. Offspring were continuously exposed to CORT or drinking water vehicle from adolescence through adulthood (PND21–60) (Figure 1). We chose our CORT exposure paradigm because we wanted to model a more persistent state of chronic stress than oft-used behavioral chronic mild stress paradigms [77]. Behavioral chronic mild stress paradigms involve randomization of stressful conditions such as damp bedding, a reversed dark/light cycle, and cage tilting; they are often highly variable in administration and outcomes [76]. Water bottles were weighed daily to assess intake and mice were weighed at weaning and at least once a week to assess proper weight gain. At PND60, offspring either underwent behavioral testing or were sacrificed for fast-scan cyclic voltammetry. All remaining offspring were sacrificed for molecular and pathologic studies.

2.3. Behavioral Analyses

One male and one female offspring from each litter were tested at 8–10 weeks of age. Mice were habituated to the testing room for at least one hour before testing. Behavioral tests were conducted during the same time window for each test by the same set of researchers to reduce confounding due to circadian and olfactory cues. For all animals, the order of behavioral testing was: (1) locomotor activity, (2) Y maze, and (3) marble burying.

2.3.1. Locomotor Activity

To assess locomotor activity, mice were placed in sound-attenuated chambers equipped with photo beams (Med Associates, St. Albans, VT, USA). Mice were habituated for 30 min and then observed for an additional 90 min. Number of ambulations, measured by the number of beam breaks, was quantified in 5-min intervals after the habituation period.

2.3.2. Y Maze

Mice moved freely in the three-arm maze for eight min total and were recorded with a digital video camera. Noldus EthoVision XT Version 13 software (Wageningen, The Netherlands) tracked alternation behavior and total distance traveled in the maze. One arm entry was scored whenever a mouse entered all four paws into a maze arm. An error consisted of an entry into a maze arm that was the same as one of the previous two arm entries. Percent error was calculated by dividing the number of errors by the total entries – 1.

2.3.3. Marble Burying

Twenty marbles were placed in a 4 × 5 pattern on top of 2 inches of fresh bedding in a clean cage. Mice were placed at the lower right corner of the cage and allowed to explore freely for 30 min. After 30 min of exploration, mice were removed, and two independent observers scored the total number of marbles buried at least 50%. Any discrepancies greater than 2 marbles were discussed and scores were averaged for each animal.

2.4. Serum Corticosterone Enzyme-Linked Immunsorbent Assay (ELISA)

Following decapitation, trunk blood was collected from mice between 8–10 weeks of age and placed into 1.5 mL centrifuge tubes and allowed to clot at room temperature for one hour. Samples were centrifuged at 10,000 rpm for 10 min at 4 °C. Serum was carefully pipetted off, placed into a clean 1.5 mL centrifuge tube, and stored at −80 °C until ELISA analysis. ELISAs were performed per manufacturer guidelines (Enzo Life Sciences, Farmingdale, NY, USA) and samples were run in triplicate and averaged for each animal.

2.5. mRNA Expression Analysis

Mice underwent rapid decapitation at 8–10 weeks of age and the midbrain was isolated and immediately flash frozen. Total RNA and DNA were extracted with a Qiagen Allprep DNA/RNA Mini Kit. DNA was saved and RNA was extracted per manufacturer recommendations (Qiagen, Germantown, MD, USA). 10 ng total RNA was converted to cDNA using SuperScript IV Reverse Transcriptase (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA) and the PCR cycle conditions recommended by the manufacturer: 94 °C – 2:00 m; 35 cycles: 94 °C – 0:15 m, 55 °C – 0:30 m, 68 °C – 1:00 m; 4 °C – hold. Taqman gene expression assays were utilized for the genes of interest and are listed in Supplemental Table S1. Genes of interest included genes critical for dopamine transport (Dat1, Vmat2), dopamine synthesis and metabolism (Comt, Th), transcriptional regulation of dopamine genes (Nurr1, Pitx3), and the glucocorticoid receptor (Nr3c1). Each assay plate contained a non-template control, positive control derived from pooled adult mouse brain tissue, and a beta actin internal standard. ΔΔCt values were calculated for each animal at every gene tested, and results are expressed relative to gene expression of the vehicle/vehicle control group.

2.6. Immunoblotting

Striatal and cortical samples were dissected upon rapid decapitation of animals at 8–10 weeks of age and immunoblotting was performed as previously described [78]. Striata were homogenized, subjected to PAGE and electrophoretically transferred to polyvinylidene difluoride (PVDF) membranes. Membranes were blocked in 7.5% nonfat dry milk in tris-buffered saline solution to reduce nonspecific binding and incubated overnight in primary antibody. We utilized horseradish peroxidase secondary antibodies (Jackson ImmunoResearch, West Grove, PA, USA) and enhanced chemiluminescence using SuperSignal West Dura Extended Duration Substrate (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA). Luminescence and subsequent densitometric analysis were performed using the ImageLab Version 3.0 software (Bio-Rad, Hercules, CA, USA), and all expression values were normalized to β-actin. Samples across multiple gels were randomized on exposure group and sex to avoid batch effects. Primary antibodies were utilized at the following dilutions: rat anti-DAT (1:1000, Millipore, Burlington, MA, USA), polyclonal rabbit anti-VMAT2 serum (1:5000, Covance Custom Immunology Services, Denver, PA, USA), rabbit anti-TH (1:1000, Millipore, Burlington, MA, USA), mouse anti-COMT (1:1000, Novus Biologicals, Centennial, CO, USA), and mouse anti-β-actin (1:5000, Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA); all are monoclonal unless otherwise noted. The corresponding secondary HRP-linked antibodies were used (1:7500, Jackson ImmunoResearch, West Grove, PA, USA). The rabbit polyclonal VMAT2 antibody was raised against the C-terminal region of mouse VMAT2 as previously described by our group [79].

2.7. Fast-Scan Cyclic Voltammetry

Voltammetry was performed as previously described [80,81]. Briefly, mice underwent rapid decapitation at 8–10 weeks of age and the right hemisphere was isolated. The right hemisphere was immersed in oxygenated sucrose aCSF solution (193 mM sucrose, 11 mM d-glucose, 1.2 mM CaCl2·2H2O, 4.5 mM KCl, 25 mM NaHCO3, 20.5 mM NaCl, 1.2 mM NaH2PO4, 2.6 mM MgCl2) for 30 s prior to sectioning at 300 µm using a Leica vibratome. Slices were incubated in oxygenated HEPES (4-(2-hydroxyethyl)-1-piperazineethanesulfonic acid) aCSF solution (19.7 mM HEPES, 11 mM d-glucose, 2.4 mM CaCl2·2H2O, 25 mM NaHCO3, 126.4 mM NaCl, 2.5 mM KCl, 1.2 mM NaH2PO4 monobasic, 2.6 mM MgCl2) for at least 30 min prior to testing. Five recordings were taken from five different sites along a single unilateral dorsal striatal slice for each animal. There was a 5-min interval between each 2.31 V stimulation. TarHeel CV Version 4.41 software (University of North Carolina, Durham, NC, USA) and a custom potentiostat (UEI, UNC Electronics Shop, Durham, NC, USA) were utilized for application of waveforms, stimulus, and current monitoring. To detect dopamine, a waveform of a −0.4 V holding potential versus an Ag/Ag Cl (In Vivo Metric, Healdsburg, CA, USA) reference electrode was used, with an applied voltage ramp from −0.4 V to 1.0 V and back at a rate of 600 V/s at 60 Hz. The maximal release was averaged for each striatal slice and carbon-fiber recording microelectrodes were calibrated with dopamine standards. Dopamine uptake was parameterized via tau, a time constant representing the amount of time necessary to return to 2/3 of baseline current after stimulation. Tau is derived from an exponential curve fitted to the dopamine current trace via a least squares constrained exponential fit algorithm that has previously been described and determined to be most accurate for measuring dopamine uptake compared to other kinetic parameters measured via fast-scan cyclic voltammetry [82]. Demon Voltammetry Version 110713 software (University of North Carolina, Durham, NC, USA) [82] was used to calculate kinetic constants describing release and uptake of dopamine, via nonlinear logistic regression.

2.8. Statistical Analyses

Statistical analyses were conducted in GraphPad Prism Version 8 (San Diego, CA, USA) unless otherwise indicated. A t-test was used to compare the serum corticosterone levels of female dams exposed to either deltamethrin or the vehicle. All other analyses investigated continuous outcomes in the offspring and utilized a one-way ANOVA because treatment had four levels: vehicle/vehicle, deltamethrin/vehicle, CORT/vehicle, and deltamethrin/CORT. We stratified by sex instead of using a two-way ANOVA because one male and one female offspring were utilized from each litter. These samples would thus violate the independent sample assumption because the litter was considered the smallest independent statistical unit. Additionally, one male and one female offspring were utilized from each litter for each experiment. Not all litters contained at least four males and four females and therefore there were differences in the number of subjects in some groups. However, the exposures themselves did not significantly affect body weight gain and survival (data not shown). A significance level of 0.05 was used throughout.

3. Results

3.1. CORT Exposure Decreases Serum CORT Independent of Deltamethrin Exposure

To test whether deltamethrin exposure itself would affect circulating CORT levels in pregnant dams and potentially affect results observed in deltamethrin and CORT-exposed offspring, we assessed serum CORT levels via ELISA. We did not observe statistically significant differences in serum CORT of control and deltamethrin-exposed dams (125.90 ng/mL ± 57.38 vs. 68.02 ng/mL ± 17.87, p = 0.33) (Figure 2). We also did not observe significant alterations in serum CORT of deltamethrin-only exposed male and female offspring compared to the vehicle/vehicle group. However, we did observe significantly decreased serum CORT levels in the CORT-only and deltamethrin/CORT groups in females offspring compared to the vehicle/vehicle group (p = 0.01 and p = 0.01, respectively), indicating that the CORT exposure itself likely influenced circulating CORT levels independent of deltamethrin exposure in females. In addition, there was a significant overall effect of sex on serum CORT levels (p < 0.0001), illustrating sex-specific differences in circulating CORT levels.

3.2. Sex-Specific Alterations in the Expression of Key Dopaminergic Genes

While we have previously studied alterations in the expression of key dopaminergic genes in the midbrain in response to dieldrin and heptachlor exposure, we had not yet investigated changes that occur in response to pyrethroid insecticide exposure [83,84]. Here, we sought to assess whether the expression of Pitx3 and Nurr1 transcription factors involved in development of the dopaminergic phenotype, as well as downstream dopaminergic targets Th, Comt, Dat1, and Vmat2, were altered in the midbrains of offspring exposed to deltamethrin, CORT, or both during neurodevelopment. We also assessed whether the expression of Nr3c1, the glucocorticoid receptor, would change in response to the aforementioned exposures.
Pitx3 gene expression was significantly decreased in deltamethrin-only exposed males when compared to vehicle/vehicle exposed males (1.64 ± 0.34 vs. 0.90 ± 0.07, p = 0.04), but not deltamethrin-only exposed females (0.87 ± 0.13 vs. 1.6 ± 0.51, p = 0.29) (Figure 3B). There was also no significant difference in Pitx3 expression in CORT-only and deltamethrin/CORT exposed males, suggesting that CORT exposure may mediate Pitx3 expression when combined with deltamethrin exposure. Similar expression effects were observed for Nurr1 transcription factor expression (Figure 3A), as well as for Dat1 and Th (Figure 3D,E), though none of these effects reached statistical significance at α = 0.05. Interestingly, gene expression of Pitx3, Nurr1, Dat1, and Th trended upwards for females exposed to deltamethrin, opposite the trends observed in males, with an intermediate effect in the deltamethrin/CORT-exposed females (Figure 3). We did not see significant effects of deltamethrin and CORT exposure on mRNA expression of Vmat2, Nr3c1, or Comt in the midbrain (Figure 3).

3.3. TH and VMAT2 Expression Were Significantly Decreased in Striatum of Females Exposed to Deltamethrin/CORT, and TH Expression Was Significantly Decreased in the Frontal Cortex of Males Exposed to Deltamethrin

Previous studies have observed alterations in key dopaminergic proteins in response to various insecticide and chronic stress exposures in the striatum, a highly dopaminergic brain region [41,49,60,83,84,85,86,87,88]. We evaluated whether combined deltamethrin/CORT exposure would lead to additive effects on dopaminergic protein expression in the striatum via immunoblotting. In striata of females exposed to both deltamethrin and CORT, TH expression was significantly decreased compared to vehicle/vehicle control females (2.23 ± 0.24 vs. 1.15 ± 0.20, p = 0.01) (Figure 4C). There was a significant difference in TH expression between CORT-only exposed females and deltamethrin/CORT-exposed females, suggesting that dual exposure with deltamethrin may mediate the effect in females (2.47 ± 0.47 vs. 1.15 ± 0.45, p = 0.02) (Figure 4C). In addition, there was also a significant decrease in VMAT2 expression in the striata of deltamethrin/CORT-exposed females (1.92 ± 0.28 vs. 0.89 ± 0.13, p = 0.045) (Figure 4D). Furthermore, there was also a significant difference in VMAT2 expression between deltamethrin-only exposed females and deltamethrin-CORT-exposed females, suggesting that dual exposure with CORT mediates the effect on VMAT2 expression (2.14 ± 0.55 vs. 0.89 ± 0.13, p = 0.02) (Figure 4D). The effects on TH and VMAT2 expression were not observed in striata of males exposed to deltamethrin/CORT. There were no significant alterations in COMT and DAT expression in the striatum of males and females exposed to deltamethrin-only, CORT-only, and deltamethrin/CORT (Figure 4A,B). Additionally, we did not observe any changes in expression of NR3C1, which encodes the glucocorticoid receptor, in any of the exposure groups in males or females (data not shown).
Next, we measured whether expression of key dopaminergic proteins change in response to deltamethrin and CORT exposure in the frontal cortex, another brain region that receives dopaminergic projections from the midbrain and has been associated with ADHD pathogenesis. There was a significant decrease in TH expression in the frontal cortex of deltamethrin-only exposed males compared to vehicle/vehicle controls (4.350 ± 1.098 vs. 2.380 ± 0.6870, p = 0.0340) (Figure 5C), but no significant difference in TH expression in dually-exposed males, suggesting that CORT may mediate the effect of deltamethrin on TH-expression. There is no significant change in expression of VMAT2, DAT, and TH of males and females exposed to deltamethrin-only, CORT-only, and deltamethrin/CORT when compared to the vehicle/vehicle control groups (Figure 5A,B).

3.4. Deltamethrin Exposure Significantly Slowed Striatal Dopamine Uptake Rate in Males

To investigate dopamine release and uptake dynamics ex vivo we employed fast-scan cyclic voltammetry as previously described [80,81,89]. This technique harnesses dopamine’s electrochemical properties and provides data on peak dopamine release in the striatum as well as dopamine uptake by DAT. Functionally, we also do not observe significant changes in peak dopamine release in any of the four exposure groups via voltammetry, in neither males nor females (Figure 6A). In contrast, we do observe a significant increase in the length of time necessary for dopamine uptake in striata of deltamethrin-only exposed males (Figure 6B). Dopamine uptake rate and, subsequently tau, were affected by changes to dopamine transporter function [82], suggesting that there may be more subtle changes in functionality but not overall DAT expression levels. There were no alterations in tau in dually-exposed deltamethrin/CORT males, suggesting that CORT may mediate the effect of deltamethrin on dopamine uptake dynamics (Figure 6B). These effects also appear to be sex-specific as there was neither a significant difference in dopamine release nor in tau in females in any of the exposure groups (Figure 6).

3.5. Males Exposed to Deltamethrin Display Increased Activity in Y Maze but Not Open Field

Given the alterations in mRNA expression, protein expression, and dopamine function, we next investigated whether these changes would result in ADHD-like behaviors. Hyperactivity is a cardinal feature of ADHD symptomology [2]. Genetic models of ADHD, as well as animal exposure models of environmental ADHD susceptibility, have tested locomotor activity through various methods [41,44,90,91,92,93]. We assessed locomotor activity over a one-hour period after mice were allowed to habituate to an open field box. We did not observe significant differences in the number of beam breaks in males or females in any of the exposure groups (p = 0.6199 for males, p = 0.5471 for females) (Figure 7A). There was also no significant difference in locomotor behavior during the habituation period (data not shown).
There was no significant difference between exposure groups or sexes in alternation behavior or in the percentage of errors, suggesting that we did not detect alterations in working memory or attention in this assay (Figure 7B,C). However, males exposed to deltamethrin traveled a significantly longer distance in the Y-maze in 8 min compared to vehicle/vehicle control males (2452.9 cm ± 245.4 vs. 2690.8 cm ± 134.7, p = 0.0368), as well as when compared to males exposed to CORT (2452.9 cm ± 245.4 vs. 1915.0 cm ± 113.2, p = 0.0050) (Figure 7D). Thus, while mice did not display hyperactivity in the locomotor activity assay, they do display increased activity in the Y maze assay. This effect appears to be sex-specific, as there was no significant difference in distance traveled among the female exposure groups. Males dually exposed to deltamethrin and CORT did not travel an increased distance compared to the vehicle/vehicle control group (1915.0 cm ± 113.2 vs. 2284.3 cm ± 179.9 cm vs., p = 0.5109), suggesting that CORT exposure may mediate the hyperactivity effects of deltamethrin in the Y maze test.

3.6. No Difference in Impulsivity as Measured Via Marble Burying in Animals Exposed to Deltamethrin and CORT

Marble burying behavior has been used previously to test impulsivity in genetic models of ADHD predisposition [80]. While vehicle/vehicle control mice display expected burying behaviors, we did not observe significant differences in the absolute number of marbles buried at least 50% after 30 min in male or females in any of the exposure groups (p = 0.9754 for males, p = 0.6903 for females) (Figure 8).

4. Discussion

The etiology and pathogenesis of ADHD are still unclear, but ADHD continues to present a substantial public health problem and there is a need for additional research probing ADHD-associated neurologic pathways and sources of pathology. The existing data on ADHD suggest that alterations in dopaminergic signaling due to pyrethroid insecticide exposure influences ADHD pathogenesis and are important for studying ADHD risk [41,51]. Chronic stress exposure also alters the dopamine system and could further potentiate the risk of ADHD [60,87,94,95,96]. Given the independent effects of pyrethroid insecticides and chronic stress on the dopamine system, we hypothesized that combined pyrethroid insecticide and stress hormone exposure during critical periods of neurodevelopment would additively alter protein expression and the function of key components of the dopamine system, thereby further increasing the risk of developing ADHD.

4.1. Summary of Findings

In our study, we exposed pregnant dams to deltamethrin and their offspring to CORT dissolved in drinking water until adulthood and then employed a suite of behavioral and molecular approaches to gain a more comprehensive understanding of alterations to the dopamine system in response to dual exposures. Although deltamethrin did not affect serum CORT levels of male and female offspring, exposure to CORT in drinking water led to a decrease in serum CORT in male and female offspring exposed and unexposed to deltamethrin. Additionally, male offspring exposed to deltamethrin demonstrated a reduction in the expression of the midbrain transcription factor, Pitx3. Although changes in mRNA expression for specific dopaminergic phenotypic markers were not observed, a gender- and region-specific effect of TH and VMAT2 protein expression was detected. The functional consequences of these alterations manifested in a reduction in dopamine uptake in the striatum of male offspring exposed to deltamethrin, which corresponded with increased locomotor activity in the Y maze for these animals.

4.2. Glucocorticoid Signaling and DM/CORT Exposure

No studies have previously examined the effects of developmental pyrethroid insecticide exposure on endogenous CORT production. Thus, our findings provide some novel insight into the effects of developmental exposure to deltamethrin on the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis and CORT levels in offspring. While deltamethrin did not impact CORT levels at the time points we evaluated, we cannot rule out the possibility that assessment at earlier ages would have shown an alteration in CORT that did not persist as they aged and deltamethrin exposure was ceased. In contrast, a few studies have investigated the effects of oral CORT administration on serum CORT production. One study previously reported that adult male mice exposed to 25 μg/mL CORT in drinking water had significantly decreased serum CORT two weeks after exposure [97], but did not have significantly altered endogenous CORT production one month after exposure [77]. Additionally, females exposed to 35 μg/mL CORT for four weeks during adolescence show a significant decrease in plasma CORT levels after forced swim testing, suggesting a dampening effect on the normal stress response following exogenous CORT administration [98].
While these results support the reductions in CORT levels found in our study, these results run counterintuitive to our original hypothesis. Nonetheless, stimulation of the endogenous stress response generated by CORT exposure may provide some insight into the feedback mechanisms that mediate serum CORT levels in our model. In general, the stress response is mediated via actions of the HPA axis. Ordinarily, an acute stressor causes release of corticotropin-release factor (CRF) from the amygdala, subsequent adrenocorticotrophic hormone (ACTH) release from the pituitary, and glucocorticoid release from the adrenal glands. Glucocorticoids then inhibit the amygdala and pituitary gland via a negative feedback loop, leading to an overall reduction in CORT release [99,100]. Under circumstances of early life and chronic stress, HPA axis hyperactivity and impaired negative feedback are typically observed [99,100], though the type of stressor, sex, genetic predisposition, and environmental factors can all modulate the influence chronic stress has on maturation and function of the HPA axis and downstream CORT levels [101]. Thus, various inputs can impact the function of the HPA axis through alteration to the limbic nuclei as well as adrenal gland. Our results and those of others suggest that chronic oral CORT administration may impact the HPA axis centrally. A previous study showed that oral CORT given to rats during adolescence increases neuronal activity in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus and causes a blunted CORT response to restraint stress [102,103]. Additionally, chronic oral CORT was found to result in a reduction in plasma CORT, independent of damage or removal of the adrenal glands [104]. These findings support the central-acting effects of exogenous CORT in mediating serum CORT production and levels. Although deltamethrin did not result in a measurable change in CORT levels at the time points we measured, the impact of chronic CORT exposure suggests a more complex regulation of serum CORT levels, which could significantly affect the development of the dopamine system.

4.3. Altered Midbrain RNA Expression

Through our novel neurodevelopmental combined exposure paradigm, we determined that midbrain expression of Pitx3 was significantly decreased in adult males exposed to deltamethrin, with a similar trend in Nurr1 expression. Pitx3 and Nurr1 are transcription factors with essential roles in the phenotypic development and maintenance of dopaminergic neurons in the midbrain [105,106,107]. Pitx3 and Nurr1 expression are normally induced by embryonic day (E) 11.5 in mice and are predominantly localized to the SNpc and VTA in the midbrain, where the densest population of dopamine neurons resides. Highlighting their function in dopamine neuron survival, genetic reduction of Pitx3 or Nurr1 in mice results in aberrant differentiation or loss of dopamine neurons in the midbrain and a concomitant loss of dopaminergic projections to the dorsal striatum [108,109,110,111,112,113,114]. While both transcription factors are critical in development of the dopamine system, Nurr1 helps determine and maintain a neuron’s dopaminergic phenotype. Conversely, Pitx3 does so only for a laterally-located subset of midbrain dopaminergic neurons in the SNpc [115]. Lateral subpopulations in the SNpc that express Pitx3 are also more susceptible to the dopaminergic neurotoxicant, MPTP [116]. Perhaps, we detected Pitx3 but not Nurr1 alterations because Pitx3-expressing subpopulations are more sensitive to neurotoxic dopamine perturbations by deltamethrin than other subtypes of dopaminergic neurons found in this region. As far as we are aware, these are the first data evaluating the impact of developmental deltamethrin and CORT exposure on transcription factors associated with development of the mesencephalic dopamine region.
Interestingly, although we observed changes in transcription factors that modulate Dat1, Vmat2, and Th expression, we did not find observable changes in overall expression of these genes in the adult midbrain. While we were able to measure alterations to Pitx3, it may be that these changes were not robust enough to elicit a significant change in mRNA expression of downstream genes. Additionally, alterations in gene expression of these dopaminergic components may be transient and do not persist into adulthood after the exposure to deltamethrin and CORT, making it difficult to capture these changes in our exposure paradigm.

4.4. Altered Striatal and Cortical Protein Expression

To our knowledge no one has assessed striatal COMT, DAT, TH, and VMAT2, nor has anyone assessed cortical DAT, TH, and VMAT2 after combined exposure to deltamethrin and CORT during neurodevelopment. Utilizing this paradigm, we found no explicit changes in these proteins with CORT exposure alone but did observe changes in striatal TH and VMAT2 following exposure to combined deltamethrin and CORT, while cortical TH was reduced after deltamethrin alone. The striatal expression of TH and VMAT2 in female offspring exposed to a combination of deltamethrin and CORT was significantly decreased, while expression of these proteins remained unchanged in male mice. Previous studies have observed increases in striatal DAT expression at 6 weeks of age following a neurodevelopmental exposure to 3 mg/kg deltamethrin [41]. We measured striatal DAT expression at 8–10 weeks and did not observe this increase, possibly because the effect of deltamethrin on striatal DAT expression does not persist into adulthood using this exposure paradigm.
Previous studies that have investigated the dopaminergic effects of psychosocial stress provide a few possible explanations for the consequences of CORT in dopaminergic neurodevelopment in our study. Rodents exposed to various forms of prolonged behavioral psychosocial stress display decreased density and binding of DAT in the dorsal striatum [87], dorsolateral caudate putamen [60], and nucleus accumbens [60]. However, these results were observed in male tree shrews [87] and male rats [60] exposed to psychosocial stress in adulthood. Since we employed a neurodevelopmental exposure paradigm and male and female mice received oral CORT from weaning through adolescence, this differential stress exposure paradigm might explain why we did not observe any changes in DAT expression in the striatum and frontal cortex. Potentially, deltamethrin exposure from gestation through weaning increased the susceptibility of dopaminergic neurons in the striatum, and subsequent exposure to oral CORT from adolescence to adulthood then had additional neurotoxic effects that led to a measurable decrease in TH and VMAT2. This could help explain why we did not observe decreased striatal TH and VMAT2 in the deltamethrin-only and CORT-only groups.
In contrast to our findings in the striatum, adult males exposed to deltamethrin-only during neurodevelopment showed significantly decreased TH expression in the cortex. This change in males is not present in the dually-exposed deltamethrin/CORT group. These findings suggest a differential impact of deltamethrin and CORT on the mesolimbic circuitry compared with the nigrostriatal circuitry.

4.5. Functional Consequences of Deltamethrin and CORT Exposure

Next, we investigated whether the molecular changes we observed would incur functional consequences via ex vivo fast-scan cyclic voltammetry and a battery of behavioral assays. Via electrochemical analysis of dopamine release dynamics, we observed that males exposed to deltamethrin did not exhibit differences in peak dopamine release in the striatum, but males exposed to deltamethrin did show a significant impairment in dopamine uptake. In our electrochemical voltammetry studies, dopamine uptake was parameterized as the time constant tau. Tau describes the amount of time to return to 2/3 of baseline current [82,117] and is increased by administration of known pharmacologic dopamine uptake inhibitors, such as amantadine [118,119], as well as cocaine [119]. Our results were surprising, given the lack of effect of deltamethrin exposure on DAT expression in the striatum. These findings suggest that developmental exposure to deltamethrin can elicit a significant impairment in DAT function, that is independent of concomitant changes in DAT expression.
DAT is an integral membrane protein that sequesters cytosolic dopamine into vesicles, thereby controlling dopaminergic signaling pre- and post-synaptically [120]. It acts as a symporter to bind two sodium ions, one chloride ion, and the dopamine substrate, moving between inward and outward facing states relative to the dopamine vesicle [121]. Genetic and pharmacologic manipulation studies of DAT illustrate that DAT function could be affected in many ways [122]. Previous studies show that the DAT transport function can be impacted by binding in different conformational states, for example. Cocaine and methylphenidate, a first-line ADHD medication, stabilize DAT’s outward-facing conformation [123], whereas other neuropsychiatric medications, such as bupropion and modafinil, stabilize the inward-facing conformation [124]. Conformation changes in DAT are critical for its proper function as a symporter, as it must move between these conformational states to properly transport dopamine. Perhaps, deltamethrin and CORT exposure abnormally stabilize or impair one of these formations and this leads to a slowed mechanism of dopamine uptake that is independent of changes in DAT expression or dopamine release. There are a few examples of functional DAT1 mutations that confer changes in dopamine uptake, but not necessarily dopamine release or expression. One group transfected a human DAT1 mutation associated with autism spectrum disorder, T365M, into cells and observed significantly lower maximal velocity of dopamine influx. However, neither the affinity of dopamine nor overall expression of DAT was affected [125]. In another in vitro study of two DAT1 mutations found in an adult patient with comorbid Parkinson’s disease and ADHD, the DAT1 I312D mutation was associated with significantly lower maximal velocity of dopamine influx but no difference in overall DAT protein expression as well [126]. Thus, in alignment with the aforementioned studies, we did not observe significantly altered DAT expression or release in the striatum of males exposed to deltamethrin/CORT, but dopamine influx velocity was significantly decreased. Dual deltamethrin/CORT exposure seems to attenuate the effect of deltamethrin on dopamine uptake in the striata of adult males, suggesting that CORT exposure could still play a role in determining the rate of dopamine uptake.
Given the observed changes in mRNA expression, protein expression, and DAT functionality, we then tested ADHD-like behaviors. Interestingly, males exposed to deltamethrin demonstrate hyperactive behavior in the Y maze, though no differences in attention or impulsivity were measured. It is possible that our behavioral endpoints were not sensitive enough to detect changes at this level, especially given the lack of alterations in DAT expression and peak dopamine release ex vivo.

5. Conclusions

In summary, we established a neurodevelopmental exposure paradigm of joint environmental factors that resulted in novel CORT reduction, RNA expression, striatal and cortical protein expression, and dopamine uptake rate findings. These findings contribute to our understanding of potential mechanisms of action of deltamethrin and oral CORT, both alone and in combination. Due to the dopaminergic effects previously described by our group and others after exposure to either deltamethrin alone or psychosocial stress or CORT alone, we hypothesized there would be synergistic effects in the dopamine system after developmental exposure to these in combination. Interestingly, we did not observe significant synergistic effects of the combined deltamethrin/CORT exposure on mRNA and protein expression of several key components within the dopamine system. Instead our data suggest that dual exposure to the major stress hormone CORT may actually dampen the effects of deltamethrin on the development of the dopamine system in males. Our study demonstrates a broad effect on the development and function of the dopamine circuit, which may be impacted by temporal aspects of deltamethrin and CORT exposure during neurodevelopment. The rationale for following up on these results is based on our understanding of the “multiple hit” hypothesis, which suggests environmental exposures render a biological system more vulnerable to subsequent environmental insults [127]. In the context of this study, while the initial exposures to deltamethrin or CORT may not manifest in an overt pathological effect on the dopamine circuit, future exposures to stress, deltamethrin, or other environmental factors could unmask more profound neurotoxicological endpoints. Aligned with this, our study highlights the importance of testing multiple environmental exposures in conjunction to better understand how combined exposures present in real-life can affect similar neurodevelopmental systems and could differentially affect particularly vulnerable pediatric populations. First-line treatment with methylphenidate, a stimulant, resolves symptoms of the three domains for the majority of children with ADHD, but treatment response is variable and use is associated with stunted growth [128], neural plasticity effects [129], and substance abuse [130]. Studies also indicate that children with ADHD report deficits in psychosocial well-being and family life [131]. Thus, while significant clinical and research advancements have been realized, ADHD continues to present a major public health burden. Further study of predisposing mechanisms and pathophysiology is therefore merited.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/2305-6304/7/2/24/s1, Table S1: Taqman Gene Expression Assays.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.I.V., W.M.C., and C.J.M.; methodology, A.I.V., W.M.C., and C.J.M.; validation, A.I.V. and W.M.C.; formal analysis, A.I.V. and M.C.; investigation, A.I.V. and M.C.; resources, W.C.M. and C.J.M.; data curation, A.I.V., M.C., and W.C.M.; writing—original draft preparation, A.I.V.; writing—review and editing, A.I.V., W.M.C., C.J.M., and M.C.; visualization, A.I.V.; supervision, W.M.C. and C.J.M.; project administration, A.I.V. and W.M.C.; funding acquisition, W.M.C. and C.J.M.

Funding

This research was funded by the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences of the National Institutes of Health, grant numbers F30ES029018 (A.I.V.), T32ES012870 (A.I.V.), 1R01ES029212 (C.J.M., W.M.C.), and 1R01ES023829 (G.W.M.). Locomotor activity and Y maze experiments were conducted by the Rodent Behavioral Core, which is subsidized by the Emory University School of Medicine and is one of the Emory Integrated Core Facilities. Additional core support was provided by the Emory Neuroscience NINDS Core Facilities (P30NS055077). Further core support was provided by the Georgia Clinical & Translational Science Alliance of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number UL1TR002378 and by the HERCULES Exposome Research Center (P30ES019776).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Gary W. Miller and the rest of his lab group for the use of their fast-scan cyclic voltammetry as well as thoughtful feedback on the experimental design and manuscript preparation.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

References

  1. Thomas, R.; Sanders, S.; Doust, J.; Beller, E.; Glasziou, P. Prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics 2015, 135, e994–e1001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Pub.: Arlington, VA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  3. Fayyad, J.; Sampson, N.A.; Hwang, I.; Adamowski, T.; Aguilar-Gaxiola, S.; Al-Hamzawi, A.; Andrade, L.H.; Borges, G.; de Girolamo, G.; Florescu, S.; et al. The descriptive epidemiology of DSM-IV Adult ADHD in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. Atten. Deficit Hyperact. Disord. 2017, 9, 47–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Gizer, I.R.; Ficks, C.; Waldman, I.D. Candidate gene studies of ADHD: A meta-analytic review. Hum. Genet. 2009, 126, 51–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Mortimer, N.; Ganster, T.; O’Leary, A.; Popp, S.; Freudenberg, F.; Reif, A.; Soler Artigas, M.; Ribases, M.; Ramos-Quiroga, J.A.; Lesch, K.P.; et al. Dissociation of impulsivity and aggression in mice deficient for the ADHD risk gene Adgrl3: Evidence for dopamine transporter dysregulation. Neuropharmacology 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Grunblatt, E.; Werling, A.M.; Roth, A.; Romanos, M.; Walitza, S. Association study and a systematic meta-analysis of the VNTR polymorphism in the 3‘-UTR of dopamine transporter gene and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Neural Transm. 2019, 126, 517–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Fraporti, T.T.; Contini, V.; Tovo-Rodrigues, L.; Recamonde-Mendoza, M.; Rovaris, D.L.; Rohde, L.A.; Hutz, M.H.; Salatino-Oliveira, A.; Genro, J.P. Synergistic effects between ADORA2A and DRD2 genes on anxiety disorders in children with ADHD. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Akutagava-Martins, G.C.; Salatino-Oliveira, A.; Kieling, C.; Genro, J.P.; Polanczyk, G.V.; Anselmi, L.; Menezes, A.M.; Goncalves, H.; Wehrmeister, F.C.; Barros, F.C.; et al. COMT and DAT1 genes are associated with hyperactivity and inattention traits in the 1993 Pelotas Birth Cohort: Evidence of sex-specific combined effect. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. Jpn. 2016, 41, 150270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Maitra, S.; Sarkar, K.; Sinha, S.; Mukhopadhyay, K. The Dopamine Receptor D5 May Influence Age of Onset: An Exploratory Study on Indo-Caucasoid ADHD Subjects. J. Child Neurol. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Van Mil, N.H.; Steegers-Theunissen, R.P.; Bouwland-Both, M.I.; Verbiest, M.M.; Rijlaarsdam, J.; Hofman, A.; Steegers, E.A.; Heijmans, B.T.; Jaddoe, V.W.; Verhulst, F.C.; et al. DNA methylation profiles at birth and child ADHD symptoms. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2014, 49, 51–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  11. Volkow, N.D.; Fowler, J.S.; Wang, G.; Ding, Y.; Gatley, S.J. Mechanism of action of methylphenidate: Insights from PET imaging studies. J. Atten. Disord. 2002, 6 (Suppl. 1), S31–S43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Madras, B.K.; Miller, G.M.; Fischman, A.J. The dopamine transporter and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 2005, 57, 1397–1409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Sakrikar, D.; Mazei-Robison, M.S.; Mergy, M.A.; Richtand, N.W.; Han, Q.; Hamilton, P.J.; Bowton, E.; Galli, A.; Veenstra-Vanderweele, J.; Gill, M.; et al. Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder-derived coding variation in the dopamine transporter disrupts microdomain targeting and trafficking regulation. J. Neurosci. 2012, 32, 5385–5397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Khadka, S.; Pearlson, G.D.; Calhoun, V.D.; Liu, J.; Gelernter, J.; Bessette, K.L.; Stevens, M.C. Multivariate Imaging Genetics Study of MRI Gray Matter Volume and SNPs Reveals Biological Pathways Correlated with Brain Structural Differences in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Front. Psychiatry 2016, 7, 128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Fernandez-Jaen, A.; Albert, J.; Fernandez-Mayoralas, D.M.; Lopez-Martin, S.; Fernandez-Perrone, A.L.; Jimenez de la Pena, M.; Calleja-Perez, B.; Recio Rodriguez, M.; Lopez Arribas, S. Cingulate Cortical Thickness and Dopamine Transporter (DAT1) Genotype in Children and Adolescents With ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Gainetdinov, R.R.; Jones, S.R.; Caron, M.G. Functional hyperdopaminergia in dopamine transporter knock-out mice. Biol. Psychiatry 1999, 46, 303–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Salahpour, A.; Ramsey, A.J.; Medvedev, I.O.; Kile, B.; Sotnikova, T.D.; Holmstrand, E.; Ghisi, V.; Nicholls, P.J.; Wong, L.; Murphy, K.; et al. Increased amphetamine-induced hyperactivity and reward in mice overexpressing the dopamine transporter. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2008, 105, 4405–4410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Leo, D.; Gainetdinov, R.R. Transgenic mouse models for ADHD. Cell Tissue Res. 2013, 354, 259–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. Gainetdinov, R.R.; Caron, M.G. An animal model of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Mol. Med. Today 2000, 6, 43–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Efimova, E.V.; Gainetdinov, R.R.; Budygin, E.A.; Sotnikova, T.D. Dopamine transporter mutant animals: A translational perspective. J. Neurogenet. 2016, 30, 5–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Kasparbauer, A.M.; Rujescu, D.; Riedel, M.; Pogarell, O.; Costa, A.; Meindl, T.; la Fougere, C.; Ettinger, U. Methylphenidate effects on brain activity as a function of SLC6A3 genotype and striatal dopamine transporter availability. Neuropsychopharmacology 2015, 40, 736–745. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Axelrod, J.; Weinshilboum, R. Catecholamines. N. Engl. J. Med. 1972, 287, 237–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Mulvihill, K.G. Presynaptic regulation of dopamine release: Role of the DAT and VMAT2 transporters. Neurochem. Int. 2019, 122, 94–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Klein, M.O.; Battagello, D.S.; Cardoso, A.R.; Hauser, D.N.; Bittencourt, J.C.; Correa, R.G. Dopamine: Functions, Signaling, and Association with Neurological Diseases. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 2019, 39, 31–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Purves, D. Neuroscience; Sinauer Associates: Sunderland, MA, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  26. Nestler, E.J.; Hyman, S.E.; Malenka, R.C. Molecular Neuropharmacology: A Foundation for Clinical Neuroscience, 2nd ed.; McGraw-Hill Education: New York, NY, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  27. Purper-Ouakil, D.; Ramoz, N.; Lepagnol-Bestel, A.-M.; Gorwood, P.; Simonneau, M. Neurobiology of Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Pediatr. Res. 2011, 69, 69R–76R. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Thapar, A.; Cooper, M.; Eyre, O.; Langley, K. What have we learnt about the causes of ADHD? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2013, 54, 3–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Neuman, R.J.; Lobos, E.; Reich, W.; Henderson, C.A.; Sun, L.W.; Todd, R.D. Prenatal smoking exposure and dopaminergic genotypes interact to cause a severe ADHD subtype. Biol. Psychiatry 2007, 61, 1320–1328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Russell, A.E.; Ford, T.; Williams, R.; Russell, G. The Association Between Socioeconomic Disadvantage and Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): A Systematic Review. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2016, 47, 440–458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Russell, A.E.; Ford, T.; Russell, G. Socioeconomic Associations with ADHD: Findings from a Mediation Analysis. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0128248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Van der Meer, D.; Hoekstra, P.J.; van Donkelaar, M.; Bralten, J.; Oosterlaan, J.; Heslenfeld, D.; Faraone, S.V.; Franke, B.; Buitelaar, J.K.; Hartman, C.A. Predicting attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder severity from psychosocial stress and stress-response genes: A random forest regression approach. Transl. Psychiatry 2017, 7, e1145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Class, Q.A.; Abel, K.M.; Khashan, A.S.; Rickert, M.E.; Dalman, C.; Larsson, H.; Hultman, C.M.; Langstrom, N.; Lichtenstein, P.; D’Onofrio, B.M. Offspring psychopathology following preconception, prenatal and postnatal maternal bereavement stress. Psychol. Med. 2014, 44, 71–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Pessah, I.N.; Lein, P.J.; Seegal, R.F.; Sagiv, S.K. Neurotoxicity of polychlorinated biphenyls and related organohalogens. Acta Neuropathol. 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Chang, C.H.; Yu, C.J.; Du, J.C.; Chiou, H.C.; Chen, H.C.; Yang, W.; Chung, M.Y.; Chen, Y.S.; Hwang, B.; Mao, I.F.; et al. The interactions among organophosphate pesticide exposure, oxidative stress, and genetic polymorphisms of dopamine receptor D4 increase the risk of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children. Environ. Res. 2018, 160, 339–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Shen, H.; Main, K.M.; Virtanen, H.E.; Damggard, I.N.; Haavisto, A.M.; Kaleva, M.; Boisen, K.A.; Schmidt, I.M.; Chellakooty, M.; Skakkebaek, N.E.; et al. From mother to child: Investigation of prenatal and postnatal exposure to persistent bioaccumulating toxicants using breast milk and placenta biomonitoring. Chemosphere 2007, 67, S256–S262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Landrigan, P.J.; Rauh, V.A.; Galvez, M.P. Environmental justice and the health of children. Mt. Sinai J. Med. N. Y. 2010, 77, 178–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Landrigan, P.J.; Goldman, L.R. Children’s vulnerability to toxic chemicals: A challenge and opportunity to strengthen health and environmental policy. Health Aff. (Proj. Hope) 2011, 30, 842–850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Grandjean, P.; Landrigan, P.J. Neurobehavioural effects of developmental toxicity. Lancet. Neurol. 2014, 13, 330–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Pastor, P.N.; Reuben, C.A.; Duran, C.; Howie, L.D. Association between Diagnosed ADHD and Selected Characteristics among Children Aged 4–17 Years: United States, 2011–2013; NCHS Data Brief, No 201; National Center for Health Statistics: Hyattsville, MD, USA, 2015.
  41. Richardson, J.R.; Taylor, M.M.; Shalat, S.L.; Guillot, T.S., 3rd; Caudle, W.M.; Hossain, M.M.; Mathews, T.A.; Jones, S.R.; Cory-Slechta, D.A.; Miller, G.W. Developmental pesticide exposure reproduces features of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. FASEB J. 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Quiros-Alcala, L.; Mehta, S.; Eskenazi, B. Pyrethroid pesticide exposure and parental report of learning disability and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in U.S. children: NHANES 1999–2002. Environ. Health Perspect. 2014, 122, 1336–1342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Larsson, H.; Sariaslan, A.; Langstrom, N.; D’Onofrio, B.; Lichtenstein, P. Family income in early childhood and subsequent attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A quasi-experimental study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2014, 55, 428–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Bock, J.; Breuer, S.; Poeggel, G.; Braun, K. Early life stress induces attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)-like behavioral and brain metabolic dysfunctions: Functional imaging of methylphenidate treatment in a novel rodent model. Brain Struct. Funct. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Klaassen, C.D. Casarett and Doull’s Toxicology: The Basic Science of Poisons; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 2013; Volume 1236. [Google Scholar]
  46. Burr, S.A.; Ray, D.E. Structure-activity and interaction effects of 14 different pyrethroids on voltage-gated chloride ion channels. Toxicol. Sci. 2004, 77, 341–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Bloomquist, J.R.; Barlow, R.L.; Gillette, J.S.; Li, W.; Kirby, M.L. Selective effects of insecticides on nigrostriatal dopaminergic nerve pathways. Neurotoxicology 2002, 23, 537–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Gillette, J.S.; Bloomquist, J.R. Differential up-regulation of striatal dopamine transporter and alpha-synuclein by the pyrethroid insecticide permethrin. Toxicol. Appl. Pharm. 2003, 192, 287–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Elwan, M.A.; Richardson, J.R.; Guillot, T.S.; Caudle, W.M.; Miller, G.W. Pyrethroid pesticide-induced alterations in dopamine transporter function. Toxicol. Appl. Pharm. 2006, 211, 188–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Mubarak Hossain, M.; Suzuki, T.; Sato, N.; Sato, I.; Takewaki, T.; Suzuki, K.; Tachikawa, E.; Kobayashi, H. Differential effects of pyrethroid insecticides on extracellular dopamine in the striatum of freely moving rats. Toxicol. Appl. Pharm. 2006, 217, 25–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Wagner-Schuman, M.; Richardson, J.R.; Auinger, P.; Braun, J.M.; Lanphear, B.P.; Epstein, J.N.; Yolton, K.; Froehlich, T.E. Association of pyrethroid pesticide exposure with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a nationally representative sample of U.S. children. Environ. Health Glob. Access Sci. Source 2015, 14, 44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Russell, G.; Ford, T.; Rosenberg, R.; Kelly, S. The association of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with socioeconomic disadvantage: Alternative explanations and evidence. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2014, 55, 436–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Steptoe, A.; Feldman, P.J. Neighborhood problems as sources of chronic stress: Development of a measure of neighborhood problems, and associations with socioeconomic status and health. Ann. Behav. Med. Publ. Soc. Behav. Med. 2001, 23, 177–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Baum, A.; Garofalo, J.P.; Yali, A.M. Socioeconomic status and chronic stress. Does stress account for SES effects on health? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1999, 896, 131–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. McEwen, B.S.; Gianaros, P.J. Central role of the brain in stress and adaptation: Links to socioeconomic status, health, and disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2010, 1186, 190–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. McEwen, B.S.; Tucker, P. Critical biological pathways for chronic psychosocial stress and research opportunities to advance the consideration of stress in chemical risk assessment. Am. J. Public Health 2011, 101 (Suppl. 1), S131–S139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Vaghri, Z.; Guhn, M.; Weinberg, J.; Grunau, R.E.; Yu, W.; Hertzman, C. Hair cortisol reflects socio-economic factors and hair zinc in preschoolers. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2013, 38, 331–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Vliegenthart, J.; Noppe, G.; van Rossum, E.F.; Koper, J.W.; Raat, H.; van den Akker, E.L. Socioeconomic status in children is associated with hair cortisol levels as a biological measure of chronic stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2016, 65, 9–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Butts, K.A.; Phillips, A.G. Glucocorticoid receptors in the prefrontal cortex regulate dopamine efflux to stress via descending glutamatergic feedback to the ventral tegmental area. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2013, 16, 1799–1807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Lucas, L.R.; Celen, Z.; Tamashiro, K.L.; Blanchard, R.J.; Blanchard, D.C.; Markham, C.; Sakai, R.R.; McEwen, B.S. Repeated exposure to social stress has long-term effects on indirect markers of dopaminergic activity in brain regions associated with motivated behavior. Neuroscience 2004, 124, 449–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Hensleigh, E.; Pritchard, L.M. Glucocorticoid receptor expression and sub-cellular localization in dopamine neurons of the rat midbrain. Neurosci. Lett. 2013, 556, 191–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Brown, P. Race, Class, and Environmental Health: A Review and Systematization of the Literature. Environ. Res. 1995, 69, 15–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Lu, C.; Adamkiewicz, G.; Attfield, K.R.; Kapp, M.; Spengler, J.D.; Tao, L.; Xie, S.H. Household pesticide contamination from indoor pest control applications in urban low-income public housing dwellings: A community-based participatory research. Environ. Sci. Technol. 2013, 47, 2018–2025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Julien, R.; Adamkiewicz, G.; Levy, J.I.; Bennett, D.; Nishioka, M.; Spengler, J.D. Pesticide loadings of select organophosphate and pyrethroid pesticides in urban public housing. J. Expo. Sci. Environ. Epidemiol. 2008, 18, 167–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Wason, S.C.; Julien, R.; Perry, M.J.; Smith, T.J.; Levy, J.I. Modeling exposures to organophosphates and pyrethroids for children living in an urban low-income environment. Environ. Res. 2013, 124, 13–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Castorina, R.; Bradman, A.; Fenster, L.; Barr, D.B.; Bravo, R.; Vedar, M.G.; Harnly, M.E.; McKone, T.E.; Eisen, E.A.; Eskenazi, B. Comparison of current-use pesticide and other toxicant urinary metabolite levels among pregnant women in the CHAMACOS cohort and NHANES. Environ. Health Perspect. 2010, 118, 856–863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Berkowitz, G.S.; Obel, J.; Deych, E.; Lapinski, R.; Godbold, J.; Liu, Z.; Landrigan, P.J.; Wolff, M.S. Exposure to indoor pesticides during pregnancy in a multiethnic, urban cohort. Environ. Health Perspect. 2003, 111, 79–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Barr, D.B.; Olsson, A.O.; Wong, L.Y.; Udunka, S.; Baker, S.E.; Whitehead, R.D.; Magsumbol, M.S.; Williams, B.L.; Needham, L.L. Urinary concentrations of metabolites of pyrethroid insecticides in the general U.S. population: National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 1999–2002. Environ. Health Perspect. 2010, 118, 742–748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Panuwet, P.; Prapamontol, T.; Chantara, S.; Barr, D.B. Urinary pesticide metabolites in school students from northern Thailand. Int. J. Hyg. Environ. Health 2009, 212, 288–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Lu, C.; Barr, D.B.; Pearson, M.A.; Walker, L.A.; Bravo, R. The attribution of urban and suburban children’s exposure to synthetic pyrethroid insecticides: A longitudinal assessment. J. Expo. Sci. Environ. Epidemiol. 2009, 19, 69–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Perez, J.J.; Williams, M.K.; Weerasekera, G.; Smith, K.; Whyatt, R.M.; Needham, L.L.; Barr, D.B. Measurement of pyrethroid, organophosphorus, and carbamate insecticides in human plasma using isotope dilution gas chromatography-high resolution mass spectrometry. J. Chromatogr. BAnal. Technol. Biomed. Life Sci. 2010, 878, 2554–2562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Lu, D.; Wang, D.; Feng, C.; Jin, Y.; Zhou, Z.; Wu, C.; Lin, Y.; Wang, G. Urinary concentrations of metabolites of pyrethroid insecticides in textile workers, Eastern China. Environ. Int. 2013, 60, 137–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Schettgen, T.; Heudorf, U.; Drexler, H.; Angerer, J. Pyrethroid exposure of the general population-is this due to diet. Toxicol. Lett. 2002, 134, 141–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Morgan, M.K. Children’s exposures to pyrethroid insecticides at home: A review of data collected in published exposure measurement studies conducted in the United States. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2012, 9, 2964–2985. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Gong, S.; Miao, Y.-L.; Jiao, G.-Z.; Sun, M.-J.; Li, H.; Lin, J.; Luo, M.-J.; Tan, J.-H. Dynamics and Correlation of Serum Cortisol and Corticosterone under Different Physiological or Stressful Conditions in Mice. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0117503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Yin, X.; Guven, N.; Dietis, N. Stress-based animal models of depression: Do we actually know what we are doing? Brain Res. 2016, 1652, 30–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Gourley, S.L.; Taylor, J.R. Recapitulation and reversal of a persistent depression-like syndrome in rodents. Curr. Protoc. Neurosci. 2009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Caudle, W.M.; Richardson, J.R.; Wang, M.Z.; Taylor, T.N.; Guillot, T.S.; McCormack, A.L.; Colebrooke, R.E.; Di Monte, D.A.; Emson, P.C.; Miller, G.W. Reduced vesicular storage of dopamine causes progressive nigrostriatal neurodegeneration. J. Neurosci. 2007, 27, 8138–8148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Cliburn, R.A.; Dunn, A.R.; Stout, K.A.; Hoffman, C.A.; Lohr, K.M.; Bernstein, A.I.; Winokur, E.J.; Burkett, J.; Schmitz, Y.; Caudle, W.M.; et al. Immunochemical localization of vesicular monoamine transporter 2 (VMAT2) in mouse brain. J. Chem. Neuroanat. 2017, 83–84, 82–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Dunn, A.R.; Stout, K.A.; Ozawa, M.; Lohr, K.M.; Hoffman, C.A.; Bernstein, A.I.; Li, Y.; Wang, M.; Sgobio, C.; Sastry, N.; et al. Synaptic vesicle glycoprotein 2C (SV2C) modulates dopamine release and is disrupted in Parkinson disease. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2017, 114, E2253–E2262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Lohr, K.M.; Bernstein, A.I.; Stout, K.A.; Dunn, A.R.; Lazo, C.R.; Alter, S.P.; Wang, M.; Li, Y.; Fan, X.; Hess, E.J.; et al. Increased vesicular monoamine transporter enhances dopamine release and opposes Parkinson disease-related neurodegeneration in vivo. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2014, 111, 9977–9982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  82. Yorgason, J.T.; Espana, R.A.; Jones, S.R. Demon voltammetry and analysis software: Analysis of cocaine-induced alterations in dopamine signaling using multiple kinetic measures. J. Neurosci. Methods 2011, 202, 158–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  83. Caudle, W.M.; Richardson, J.R.; Wang, M.; Miller, G.W. Perinatal heptachlor exposure increases expression of presynaptic dopaminergic markers in mouse striatum. Neurotoxicology 2005, 26, 721–728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Richardson, J.R.; Caudle, W.M.; Wang, M.; Dean, E.D.; Pennell, K.D.; Miller, G.W. Developmental exposure to the pesticide dieldrin alters the dopamine system and increases neurotoxicity in an animal model of Parkinson’s disease. FASEB J. 2006, 20, 1695–1697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Wilson, W.W.; Shapiro, L.P.; Bradner, J.M.; Caudle, W.M. Developmental exposure to the organochlorine insecticide endosulfan damages the nigrostriatal dopamine system in male offspring. Neurotoxicology 2014, 44, 279–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  86. Richardson, J.R.; Caudle, W.M.; Wang, M.Z.; Dean, E.D.; Pennell, K.D.; Miller, G.W. Developmental heptachlor exposure increases susceptibility of dopamine neurons to N-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine (MPTP)in a gender-specific manner. Neurotoxicology 2008, 29, 855–863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Isovich, E.; Mijnster, M.J.; Flugge, G.; Fuchs, E. Chronic psychosocial stress reduces the density of dopamine transporters. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2000, 12, 1071–1078. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Graf, E.N.; Wheeler, R.A.; Baker, D.A.; Ebben, A.L.; Hill, J.E.; McReynolds, J.R.; Robble, M.A.; Vranjkovic, O.; Wheeler, D.S.; Mantsch, J.R.; et al. Corticosterone acts in the nucleus accumbens to enhance dopamine signaling and potentiate reinstatement of cocaine seeking. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 11800–11810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Stout, K.A.; Dunn, A.R.; Lohr, K.M.; Alter, S.P.; Cliburn, R.A.; Guillot, T.S.; Miller, G.W. Selective Enhancement of Dopamine Release in the Ventral Pallidum of Methamphetamine-Sensitized Mice. ACS Chem. Neurosci. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Wallis, D.; Hill, D.S.; Mendez, I.A.; Abbott, L.C.; Finnell, R.H.; Wellman, P.J.; Setlow, B. Initial characterization of mice null for Lphn3, a gene implicated in ADHD and addiction. Brain Res. 2012, 1463, 85–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Kim, P.; Choi, C.S.; Park, J.H.; Joo, S.H.; Kim, S.Y.; Ko, H.M.; Kim, K.C.; Jeon, S.J.; Park, S.H.; Han, S.H.; et al. Chronic exposure to ethanol of male mice before mating produces attention deficit hyperactivity disorder-like phenotype along with epigenetic dysregulation of dopamine transporter expression in mouse offspring. J. Neurosci. Res. 2014, 92, 658–670. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  92. Majdak, P.; Ossyra, J.R.; Ossyra, J.M.; Cobert, A.J.; Hofmann, G.C.; Tse, S.; Panozzo, B.; Grogan, E.L.; Sorokina, A.; Rhodes, J.S. A new mouse model of ADHD for medication development. Sci. Rep. 2016, 6, 39472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  93. Itohara, S.; Kobayashi, Y.; Nakashiba, T. Genetic factors underlying attention and impulsivity: Mouse models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2015, 2, 46–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Barros, V.G.; Boado, L.A.; Adamo, A.M.; Caviedes, R.; Caviedes, P.; Antonelli, M.C. Corticosterone down-regulates dopamine D4 receptor in a mouse cerebral cortex neuronal cell line. Neurotox. Res. 2003, 5, 369–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Hu, L.; Yang, J.; Song, T.; Hou, N.; Liu, Y.; Zhao, X.; Zhang, D.; Wang, L.; Wang, T.; Huang, C. A new stress model, a scream sound, alters learning and monoamine levels in rat brain. Physiol. Behav. 2014, 123, 105–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Mora, F.; Segovia, G.; Del Arco, A.; de Blas, M.; Garrido, P. Stress, neurotransmitters, corticosterone and body-brain integration. Brain Res. 2012, 1476, 71–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Gourley, S.L.; Kedves, A.T.; Olausson, P.; Taylor, J.R. A history of corticosterone exposure regulates fear extinction and cortical NR2B, GluR2/3, and BDNF. Neuropsychopharmacology 2009, 34, 707–716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Mekiri, M.; Gardier, A.M.; David, D.J.; Guilloux, J.P. Chronic corticosterone administration effects on behavioral emotionality in female c57bl6 mice. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2017, 25, 94–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  99. Van Bodegom, M.; Homberg, J.R.; Henckens, M. Modulation of the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis by Early Life Stress Exposure. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 2017, 11, 87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  100. Zhu, L.J.; Liu, M.Y.; Li, H.; Liu, X.; Chen, C.; Han, Z.; Wu, H.Y.; Jing, X.; Zhou, H.H.; Suh, H.; et al. The different roles of glucocorticoids in the hippocampus and hypothalamus in chronic stress-induced HPA axis hyperactivity. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e97689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Herman, J.P.; McKlveen, J.M.; Ghosal, S.; Kopp, B.; Wulsin, A.; Makinson, R.; Scheimann, J.; Myers, B. Regulation of the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenocortical Stress Response. Compr. Physiol. 2016, 6, 603–621. [Google Scholar]
  102. Shahanoor, Z.; Sultana, R.; Baker, M.R.; Romeo, R.D. Neuroendocrine stress reactivity of male C57BL/6N mice following chronic oral corticosterone exposure during adulthood or adolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2017, 86, 218–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Ulrich-Lai, Y.M.; Herman, J.P. Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress responses. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009, 10, 397–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Robinson, S.A.; Brookshire, B.R.; Lucki, I. Corticosterone exposure augments sensitivity to the behavioral and neuroplastic effects of fluoxetine in C57BL/6 mice. Neurobiol. Stress 2016, 3, 34–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  105. Perlmann, T.; Wallen-Mackenzie, A. Nurr1, an orphan nuclear receptor with essential functions in developing dopamine cells. Cell Tissue Res. 2004, 318, 45–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Smidt, M.P.; Smits, S.M.; Burbach, J.P. Homeobox gene Pitx3 and its role in the development of dopamine neurons of the substantia nigra. Cell Tissue Res. 2004, 318, 35–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Kadkhodaei, B.; Ito, T.; Joodmardi, E.; Mattsson, B.; Rouillard, C.; Carta, M.; Muramatsu, S.; Sumi-Ichinose, C.; Nomura, T.; Metzger, D.; et al. Nurr1 is required for maintenance of maturing and adult midbrain dopamine neurons. J. Neurosci. 2009, 29, 15923–15932. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Smidt, M.P.; Smits, S.M.; Bouwmeester, H.; Hamers, F.P.; van der Linden, A.J.; Hellemons, A.J.; Graw, J.; Burbach, J.P. Early developmental failure of substantia nigra dopamine neurons in mice lacking the homeodomain gene Pitx3. Development 2004, 131, 1145–1155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  109. Smidt, M.P.; van Schaick, H.S.; Lanctôt, C.; Tremblay, J.J.; Cox, J.J.; van der Kleij, A.A.; Wolterink, G.; Drouin, J.; Burbach, J.P. A homeodomain gene Ptx3 has highly restricted brain expression in mesencephalic dopaminergic neurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1997, 94, 13305–13310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  110. Smits, S.M.; Smidt, M.P. The role of Pitx3 in survival of midbrain dopaminergic neurons. J. Neural Transm. Suppl. 2006, 70, 57–60. [Google Scholar]
  111. Bissonette, G.B.; Roesch, M.R. Development and function of the midbrain dopamine system: What we know and what we need to. Genes Brain Behav. 2016, 15, 62–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Kas, M.J.; van der Linden, A.J.; Oppelaar, H.; von Oerthel, L.; Ramakers, G.M.; Smidt, M.P. Phenotypic segregation of aphakia and Pitx3-null mutants reveals that Pitx3 deficiency increases consolidation of specific movement components. Behav. Brain Res. 2008, 186, 208–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  113. Jacobs, F.M.; van Erp, S.; van der Linden, A.J.; von Oerthel, L.; Burbach, J.P.; Smidt, M.P. Pitx3 potentiates Nurr1 in dopamine neuron terminal differentiation through release of SMRT-mediated repression. Development 2009, 136, 531–540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  114. Volpicelli, F.; De Gregorio, R.; Pulcrano, S.; Perrone-Capano, C.; di Porzio, U.; Bellenchi, G.C. Direct regulation of Pitx3 expression by Nurr1 in culture and in developing mouse midbrain. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e30661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Smidt, M.P.; Burbach, J.P. Terminal differentiation of mesodiencephalic dopaminergic neurons: The role of Nurr1 and Pitx3. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 2009, 651, 47–57. [Google Scholar]
  116. Luk, K.C.; Rymar, V.V.; van den Munckhof, P.; Nicolau, S.; Steriade, C.; Bifsha, P.; Drouin, J.; Sadikot, A.F. The transcription factor Pitx3 is expressed selectively in midbrain dopaminergic neurons susceptible to neurodegenerative stress. J. Neurochem. 2013, 125, 932–943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. SR, J.C.J. Fast Scan Cyclic Voltammetry of Dopamine and Serotonin in Mouse Brain Slices. In Electrochemical Methods for Neuroscience; Michael, A.C., Borland, L.M., Eds.; CRC Press/Taylor & Francis: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  118. Huang, E.Y.; Tsui, P.F.; Kuo, T.T.; Tsai, J.J.; Chou, Y.C.; Ma, H.I.; Chiang, Y.H.; Chen, Y.H. Amantadine ameliorates dopamine-releasing deficits and behavioral deficits in rats after fluid percussion injury. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e86354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  119. Brodnik, Z.D.; Ferris, M.J.; Jones, S.R.; Espana, R.A. Reinforcing Doses of Intravenous Cocaine Produce Only Modest Dopamine Uptake Inhibition. ACS Chem. Neurosci. 2017, 8, 281–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Giros, B.; Jaber, M.; Jones, S.R.; Wightman, R.M.; Caron, M.G. Hyperlocomotion and indifference to cocaine and amphetamine in mice lacking the dopamine transporter. Nature 1996, 379, 606–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  121. Rastedt, D.E.; Vaughan, R.A.; Foster, J.D. Palmitoylation mechanisms in dopamine transporter regulation. J. Chem. Neuroanat. 2017, 83–84, 3–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Lohr, K.M.; Masoud, S.T.; Salahpour, A.; Miller, G.W. Membrane transporters as mediators of synaptic dopamine dynamics: Implications for disease. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2017, 45, 20–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Loland, C.J.; Desai, R.I.; Zou, M.F.; Cao, J.; Grundt, P.; Gerstbrein, K.; Sitte, H.H.; Newman, A.H.; Katz, J.L.; Gether, U. Relationship between conformational changes in the dopamine transporter and cocaine-like subjective effects of uptake inhibitors. Mol Pharm. 2008, 73, 813–823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  124. Schmitt, K.C.; Reith, M.E. The atypical stimulant and nootropic modafinil interacts with the dopamine transporter in a different manner than classical cocaine-like inhibitors. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e25790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Hamilton, P.J.; Campbell, N.G.; Sharma, S.; Erreger, K.; Herborg Hansen, F.; Saunders, C.; Belovich, A.N.; Sahai, M.A.; Cook, E.H.; Gether, U.; et al. De novo mutation in the dopamine transporter gene associates dopamine dysfunction with autism spectrum disorder. Mol. Psychiatry 2013, 18, 1315–1323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  126. Hansen, F.H.; Skjorringe, T.; Yasmeen, S.; Arends, N.V.; Sahai, M.A.; Erreger, K.; Andreassen, T.F.; Holy, M.; Hamilton, P.J.; Neergheen, V.; et al. Missense dopamine transporter mutations associate with adult parkinsonism and ADHD. J. Clin. Investig. 2014, 124, 3107–3120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  127. Kraft, A.D.; Aschner, M.; Cory-Slechta, D.A.; Bilbo, S.D.; Caudle, W.M.; Makris, S.L. Unmasking silent neurotoxicity following developmental exposure to environmental toxicants. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2016, 55, 38–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  128. Faraone, S.V.; Biederman, J.; Morley, C.P.; Spencer, T.J. Effect of stimulants on height and weight: A review of the literature. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2008, 47, 994–1009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  129. Urban, K.R.; Gao, W.J. Methylphenidate and the juvenile brain: Enhancement of attention at the expense of cortical plasticity? Med. Hypotheses 2013, 81, 988–994. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  130. Mannuzza, S.; Klein, R.G.; Truong, N.L.; Moulton, J.L., 3rd; Roizen, E.R.; Howell, K.H.; Castellanos, F.X. Age of methylphenidate treatment initiation in children with ADHD and later substance abuse: Prospective follow-up into adulthood. Am. J. Psychiatry 2008, 165, 604–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  131. Klassen, A.F. Quality of life of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Expert Rev. Pharm. Outcomes Res. 2005, 5, 95–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Exposure paradigm timeline. Adult C57BL/6NCrl females were exposed to deltamethrin or vehicle every 3 days during gestation, lactation, and weaning of offspring at postnatal day (PND) 21. Deltamethrin was administered via corn oil dissolved in peanut butter to minimize trauma to pregnant dams. Corticosterone was dissolved in the drinking water to minimize handling stress and reduce variation in CORT levels seen in behavioral chronic stress paradigms.
Figure 1. Exposure paradigm timeline. Adult C57BL/6NCrl females were exposed to deltamethrin or vehicle every 3 days during gestation, lactation, and weaning of offspring at postnatal day (PND) 21. Deltamethrin was administered via corn oil dissolved in peanut butter to minimize trauma to pregnant dams. Corticosterone was dissolved in the drinking water to minimize handling stress and reduce variation in CORT levels seen in behavioral chronic stress paradigms.
Toxics 07 00024 g001
Figure 2. Exposure to oral CORT decreased endogenous CORT production independent of deltamethrin exposure in male and female offspring, and there was no significant difference in endogenous CORT production of female dams exposed to deltamethrin during gestation. We tested endogenous CORT levels via an ELISA assay to assess (A) whether deltamethrin exposure affects circulating corticosterone in the pregnant dams, which could subsequently affect in utero CORT levels observed by their offspring; and (B) whether CORT exposure affects circulating CORT in male and female offspring exposed to deltamethrin, oral CORT, or both. Exposure group differences were assessed via a two-tailed t-test for the exposed versus unexposed female dams (A) and via one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc test, stratified by sex, for the male and female offspring (B). N = 7–8 in (A), N = 5–7 in (B), and error bars represent SEM. * p < 0.05.
Figure 2. Exposure to oral CORT decreased endogenous CORT production independent of deltamethrin exposure in male and female offspring, and there was no significant difference in endogenous CORT production of female dams exposed to deltamethrin during gestation. We tested endogenous CORT levels via an ELISA assay to assess (A) whether deltamethrin exposure affects circulating corticosterone in the pregnant dams, which could subsequently affect in utero CORT levels observed by their offspring; and (B) whether CORT exposure affects circulating CORT in male and female offspring exposed to deltamethrin, oral CORT, or both. Exposure group differences were assessed via a two-tailed t-test for the exposed versus unexposed female dams (A) and via one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc test, stratified by sex, for the male and female offspring (B). N = 7–8 in (A), N = 5–7 in (B), and error bars represent SEM. * p < 0.05.
Toxics 07 00024 g002
Figure 3. Midbrain Pitx3 expression was significantly decreased in males exposed to deltamethrin/vehicle. Gene expression was assessed in the midbrain of male and female offspring exposed to one of four treatment groups: vehicle/vehicle, deltamethrin (DM)/vehicle, corticosterone (CORT)/vehicle, and DM/CORT. Midbrains were collected at 8–10 weeks of age via rapid decapitation and RNA was extracted. Gene expression was measured via Taqman assays and data was expressed as 2ΔΔCt analysis of qPCR data and compared to expression in the vehicle/vehicle group. Exposure group differences were assessed via one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc test and stratified by sex. * p < 0.05, N = 5–8, and error bars represent SEM.
Figure 3. Midbrain Pitx3 expression was significantly decreased in males exposed to deltamethrin/vehicle. Gene expression was assessed in the midbrain of male and female offspring exposed to one of four treatment groups: vehicle/vehicle, deltamethrin (DM)/vehicle, corticosterone (CORT)/vehicle, and DM/CORT. Midbrains were collected at 8–10 weeks of age via rapid decapitation and RNA was extracted. Gene expression was measured via Taqman assays and data was expressed as 2ΔΔCt analysis of qPCR data and compared to expression in the vehicle/vehicle group. Exposure group differences were assessed via one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc test and stratified by sex. * p < 0.05, N = 5–8, and error bars represent SEM.
Toxics 07 00024 g003
Figure 4. Striatal TH and VMAT2 expression was significantly decreased in female offspring exposed to deltamethrin (DM) and corticosterone (CORT). COMT (A), DAT (B), TH (C), and VMAT2 (D) protein expression in the striatum was assessed in male and female offspring exposed to one of four treatment groups. Striata were collected at 8–10 weeks of age. Protein expression is expressed as adjusted units (AU) assessed via densitometry analysis and normalized to actin. Exposure group differences were evaluated via one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc test and stratified by sex. * p < 0.05, N = 5–7, and error bars represent SEM.
Figure 4. Striatal TH and VMAT2 expression was significantly decreased in female offspring exposed to deltamethrin (DM) and corticosterone (CORT). COMT (A), DAT (B), TH (C), and VMAT2 (D) protein expression in the striatum was assessed in male and female offspring exposed to one of four treatment groups. Striata were collected at 8–10 weeks of age. Protein expression is expressed as adjusted units (AU) assessed via densitometry analysis and normalized to actin. Exposure group differences were evaluated via one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc test and stratified by sex. * p < 0.05, N = 5–7, and error bars represent SEM.
Toxics 07 00024 g004
Figure 5. Cortical TH expression was significantly decreased in male offspring exposed to deltamethrin (DM) only. DAT (A), TH (B), and VMAT2 (C) protein expression in the frontal cortex of male and female offspring exposed to one of four treatment groups was assessed. Cortices were collected at 8-10 weeks of age. Protein expression is expressed as adjusted units (AU) assessed via densitometry analysis and normalized to actin. Exposure group differences were evaluated via one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc analysis, stratified by sex. * p < 0.05, N = 5–7, and error bars represent SEM.
Figure 5. Cortical TH expression was significantly decreased in male offspring exposed to deltamethrin (DM) only. DAT (A), TH (B), and VMAT2 (C) protein expression in the frontal cortex of male and female offspring exposed to one of four treatment groups was assessed. Cortices were collected at 8-10 weeks of age. Protein expression is expressed as adjusted units (AU) assessed via densitometry analysis and normalized to actin. Exposure group differences were evaluated via one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc analysis, stratified by sex. * p < 0.05, N = 5–7, and error bars represent SEM.
Toxics 07 00024 g005
Figure 6. Significant impairment of dopamine uptake occurred in male offspring to deltamethrin (DM) only, with no significant change in peak dopamine release. Ex vivo electrochemical measurement of dopamine release and uptake via fast-scan cyclic voltammetry was measured in male and female offspring exposed to one of four treatment groups. In (A), peak dopamine release was measured in the dorsal striatum and averaged over all stimulation sites per animal. DM, corticosterone (CORT), or DM/CORT exposure did not significantly change peak dopamine release in male or female offspring. In (B), tau, a kinetic time constant, was used to characterize dopamine uptake into the synapse and tau values were averaged over all stimulation sites per animal. Deltamethrin exposure in males significantly increased tau, which represents a slowed and impaired dopamine uptake into the synapse. Exposure group differences were evaluated via one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc analyses, stratified by sex. * p < 0.05, N = 5–7, and error bars represent SEM.
Figure 6. Significant impairment of dopamine uptake occurred in male offspring to deltamethrin (DM) only, with no significant change in peak dopamine release. Ex vivo electrochemical measurement of dopamine release and uptake via fast-scan cyclic voltammetry was measured in male and female offspring exposed to one of four treatment groups. In (A), peak dopamine release was measured in the dorsal striatum and averaged over all stimulation sites per animal. DM, corticosterone (CORT), or DM/CORT exposure did not significantly change peak dopamine release in male or female offspring. In (B), tau, a kinetic time constant, was used to characterize dopamine uptake into the synapse and tau values were averaged over all stimulation sites per animal. Deltamethrin exposure in males significantly increased tau, which represents a slowed and impaired dopamine uptake into the synapse. Exposure group differences were evaluated via one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc analyses, stratified by sex. * p < 0.05, N = 5–7, and error bars represent SEM.
Toxics 07 00024 g006
Figure 7. Males exposed to deltamethrin (DM) only exhibited increased distance traveled in the Y maze, but there were no significant differences in locomotor activity and Y maze attention performance in males and females exposed to DM, corticosterone (CORT), or both. Locomotor activity and Y maze assays were conducted in male and female offspring exposed to one of four treatment groups at 8 weeks of age. In (A) data are expressed as the average number of beam breaks in 60 min after a 30-min habituation period. In (BD) mice were allowed to freely explore the Y maze for 8 min and arm entries were recorded to assess working memory, attention, and total distance traveled. Exposure group differences were assessed via one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc analyses and stratified by sex. * p < 0.05, N = 7–9, and error bars represent SEM.
Figure 7. Males exposed to deltamethrin (DM) only exhibited increased distance traveled in the Y maze, but there were no significant differences in locomotor activity and Y maze attention performance in males and females exposed to DM, corticosterone (CORT), or both. Locomotor activity and Y maze assays were conducted in male and female offspring exposed to one of four treatment groups at 8 weeks of age. In (A) data are expressed as the average number of beam breaks in 60 min after a 30-min habituation period. In (BD) mice were allowed to freely explore the Y maze for 8 min and arm entries were recorded to assess working memory, attention, and total distance traveled. Exposure group differences were assessed via one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc analyses and stratified by sex. * p < 0.05, N = 7–9, and error bars represent SEM.
Toxics 07 00024 g007
Figure 8. There was no significant difference in impulsivity, as measured by marble burying, in male (A) and female (B) offspring exposed to one of four treatment groups. Twenty marbles were placed in a 4 × 5 pattern on top of clean bedding. Offspring were tested at 8 weeks of age and allowed to roam freely for 30 min total. The number of marbles buried at least 50%, as scored by two independent scorers, is reported. N = 7–9, differences between exposure groups were assessed via one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc analyses and were stratified by sex. Alpha level of 0.05 was used and error bars represent SEM.
Figure 8. There was no significant difference in impulsivity, as measured by marble burying, in male (A) and female (B) offspring exposed to one of four treatment groups. Twenty marbles were placed in a 4 × 5 pattern on top of clean bedding. Offspring were tested at 8 weeks of age and allowed to roam freely for 30 min total. The number of marbles buried at least 50%, as scored by two independent scorers, is reported. N = 7–9, differences between exposure groups were assessed via one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc analyses and were stratified by sex. Alpha level of 0.05 was used and error bars represent SEM.
Toxics 07 00024 g008

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Vester, A.I.; Chen, M.; Marsit, C.J.; Caudle, W.M. A Neurodevelopmental Model of Combined Pyrethroid and Chronic Stress Exposure. Toxics 2019, 7, 24. https://doi.org/10.3390/toxics7020024

AMA Style

Vester AI, Chen M, Marsit CJ, Caudle WM. A Neurodevelopmental Model of Combined Pyrethroid and Chronic Stress Exposure. Toxics. 2019; 7(2):24. https://doi.org/10.3390/toxics7020024

Chicago/Turabian Style

Vester, Aimée I., Merry Chen, Carmen J. Marsit, and W. Michael Caudle. 2019. "A Neurodevelopmental Model of Combined Pyrethroid and Chronic Stress Exposure" Toxics 7, no. 2: 24. https://doi.org/10.3390/toxics7020024

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop