Next Article in Journal
Lipid Metabolism and Cancer Immunotherapy: Immunosuppressive Myeloid Cells at the Crossroad
Next Article in Special Issue
Interactions among Genetic Background, Anesthetic Agent, and Oxygen Concentration Shape Blunt Traumatic Brain Injury Outcomes in Drosophila melanogaster
Previous Article in Journal
Multicopy Suppressor Analysis of Strains Lacking Cytoplasmic Peptidyl-Prolyl cis/trans Isomerases Identifies Three New PPIase Activities in Escherichia coli That Includes the DksA Transcription Factor
Previous Article in Special Issue
A Metabolic Mechanism for Anaesthetic Suppression of Cortical Synaptic Function in Mouse Brain Slices—A Pilot Investigation
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Propofol Affects Cortico-Hippocampal Interactions via β3 Subunit-Containing GABAA Receptors

1
Department of Anesthesiology and Intensive Care, Klinikum rechts der Isar, Technical University of Munich, School of Medicine, Munich, Ismaninger Str. 22, 81675 München, Germany
2
Werner Reichardt Centre for Integrative Neuroscience, Eberhard-Karls-University, Otfried-Müller-Str. 25, 72076 Tübingen, Germany
3
Department of Anesthesiology, Neuroanesthesia Division, Columbia University Medical Center, New York Presbyterian Hospital, 622 West 168th Street, New York City, NY 10032, USA
4
Department of Comparative Biosciences, College of Veterinary Medicine, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 2001 South Lincoln Avenue, Urbana, IL 61802-6178, USA
5
Carl R. Woese Institute for Genomic Biology, University of Illiniois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL 61801, USA
6
Department of Anaesthesiology, Experimental Anaesthesiology Section, Eberhard-Karls-University, Waldhörnlestrasse 22, 72072 Tübingen, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2020, 21(16), 5844; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms21165844
Submission received: 22 July 2020 / Revised: 5 August 2020 / Accepted: 7 August 2020 / Published: 14 August 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Molecular Mechanisms and Neural Correlates of General Anesthesia)

Abstract

:
Background: General anesthetics depress neuronal activity. The depression and uncoupling of cortico-hippocampal activity may contribute to anesthetic-induced amnesia. However, the molecular targets involved in this process are not fully characterized. GABAA receptors, especially the type with β3 subunits, represent a main molecular target of propofol. We therefore hypothesized that GABAA receptors with β3 subunits mediate the propofol-induced disturbance of cortico-hippocampal interactions. Methods: We used local field potential (LFP) recordings from chronically implanted cortical and hippocampal electrodes in wild-type and β3(N265M) knock-in mice. In the β3(N265M) mice, the action of propofol via β3subunit containing GABAA receptors is strongly attenuated. The analytical approach contained spectral power, phase locking, and mutual information analyses in the 2–16 Hz range to investigate propofol-induced effects on cortico-hippocampal interactions. Results: Propofol caused a significant increase in spectral power between 14 and 16 Hz in the cortex and hippocampus of wild-type mice. This increase was absent in the β3(N265M) mutant. Propofol strongly decreased phase locking of 6–12 Hz oscillations in wild-type mice. This decrease was attenuated in the β3(N265M) mutant. Finally, propofol reduced the mutual information between 6–16 Hz in wild-type mice, but only between 6 and 8 Hz in the β3(N265M) mutant. Conclusions: GABAA receptors containing β3 subunits contribute to frequency-specific perturbation of cortico-hippocampal interactions. This likely explains some of the amnestic actions of propofol.

1. Introduction

Commonly used general anesthetics such as propofol influence information processing in the brain. Hypnotic doses of this anesthetic, for instance, impair the transfer of information between the frontal and parietal cortical regions [1,2,3] as well as between the thalamus and the cortex [4,5]. While impairment of thalamocortical activity may relate to the hypnotic component of anesthesia, propofol-induced changes in cortico-hippocampal interactions may contribute to the amnestic component of this drug. Memory-related processes are strongly linked to interactions between these areas [6]. Both regions interact with each other via neural oscillating activity in the mid-frequency (4–12 Hz) range [7]. Volatile anesthetics cause a slowing of the peak oscillation frequency in the hippocampus. This may represent one mechanism of anesthetic-induced amnesia [8,9]. It is difficult to determine which specific molecular pathways are involved in this process, because volatile anesthetics cause many changes via multiple molecular targets in the brain, e.g., potassium channels, HCN channels, acetylcholine receptors, glutamate receptors and GABAA receptors. [10,11,12,13]. Compared to volatile anesthetics, propofol is much more specific for GABAA receptors [11,14]. The anesthetic actions of propofol are mediated almost exclusively via βsubunit containing GABAA receptors [15,16]. Using genetically engineered mice, it was shown that β2subunit-containing GABAA receptors, a frequently expressed subtype of GABAA receptors [17] are involved in the sedative properties of intravenous anesthetics [18,19]. On the other hand, β3 subunit-containing GABAA receptors, being only a minor subtype of GABAA receptors [17], largely contribute to the hypnotic and immobilizing effects of propofol [20]. Furthermore, the electrophysiological effects of propofol in the cerebral cortex critically depend on the presence of β3 subunit-containing GABAA receptors [21]. Therefore, we used wild-type and β3(N265M) knock-in mutant mice to elucidate the impact of β3 subunit-containing GABAA receptors on the effects of propofol on cortico-hippocampal interactions. The β3(N265M) mice are largely resistant to the actions of propofol at this receptor-subtype [22]. We hypothesized that the GABAergic anesthetic propofol would reduce connectivity between the cortex and hippocampus and that this reduction in connectivity is at least in part mediated by β3 subunit-containing GABAA receptors. We tested this hypothesis by (i) analyzing the local field potential (LFP) spectral power in (prefrontal) cortex and hippocampus, (ii) the interaction between these brain areas using phase locking and (iii) the shared information content using the mutual information.

2. Results

We did not observe significant differences in body weight of the animals from both groups. (WT: 33.4 ± 5.0 g vs. β3(N265M): 36.4 ± 5.9 g). Animals were recorded under awake control conditions for ten minutes, injected i.v. with propofol, and immediately monitored again until they showed recovery from anesthesia in their LFP recordings. Head-fixation impeded with simultaneous test of return of righting reflex (RoRR). Therefore, time to RoRR after propofol injection was measured in separate experiments. We determined RoRR after approximately 10 min in wild-type and 2 min in β3(N265M)-mutant mice.

2.1. Effects of Propofol on the Power Spectrum of Cortical and Hippocampal Oscillations in Wild-Type and β3(N265M) Knock-in Mice

Propofol did not change spectral power in the 4–12 Hz range at cortical recording sites in wild-type mice, yet there was a significant increase in power in frequencies above 12 Hz. This increase in higher-frequency power was also observed in data recorded from hippocampal sites. The most striking difference between the hippocampus and cortex, however, was a strong activation of hippocampal but not of cortical oscillations in the 4–6 Hz range. For the β3(N265M) mice, the results are different. Propofol did not have a significant effect on the spectral band power in cortex. The activation of 4–6 Hz oscillatory activity we observed in the wild-type mice in the hippocampus was also absent in the β3(N265M) animals. However, in β3(N265M) mice, we observed a small but significant increase in 8–10 Hz band power. The detailed effects of propofol on spectral band power are presented in Figure 1.

2.2. Actions of Propofol on Interactions between Cortex and Hippocampus in Wild-Type and β3(N265M) Knock-in Mice

In the next step, we focused on changes in interactions between the cortex and hippocampus using the phase locking value (PLV). In wild-type mice, we found a strong de-synchronization of phase in the 6 to 12 Hz range, indicating that propofol impairs the oscillatory connectivity between the cortex and hippocampus. The β3(N265M) knock-in mice did not show that strong a decrease in PLV. In these animals, PLV showed a significant and strong decrease only in a smaller frequency window from 6–8 Hz. Figure 2 displays the PLV for the wild-type and β3(N265M) mice for all frequency ranges. This demonstrates that GABAA receptors containing the β3 subunit might be involved in communicating neuronal information between the cortex and hippocampus in the 8–14 Hz range while receiving propofol sedation/anesthesia.

2.3. Effects of Propofol on Shared Information Content between Cortex and Hippocampus in Wild-Type and β3(N265M) Knock-in Mice

Finally, the use of mutual information (MI) served the purpose to estimate propofol-induced changes of shared information between cortex and hippocampus. In wild-type mice, propofol caused a strong reduction in shared information in frequencies from 6–16 Hz. In contrast, MI in β3(N265M) knock-in mice did not show a decrease to that degree, and the effect was limited to a smaller frequency range from 6 to 8 Hz. This is in line with the finding for PLV, where the effects in the β3(N265M) knock-ins were attenuated, i.e., limited to a smaller frequency range as well. Figure 3 shows the effect of propofol on MI in the single frequency ranges for wild-type and β3(N265M) knock-in mice.
The Hedges’ g values and their CI for all experiments (power spectral density (PSD), PLV, and MI) in the single frequency intervals are listed in Table 1.

3. Discussion

We present results regarding different effects of propofol on cortico-hippocampal interactions in two animal models, i.e., in wild type and β3(N265M) knock-in mice. The animals showed an attenuated response to propofol. Investigations of anesthetic-induced modulation of interactions between brain regions have been in the focus of research for some decades. In the mid-1990s, MacIver and colleagues demonstrated that the intravenous anesthetic thiopental affects cortico-hippocampal synchronicity in a dose-dependent manner [23]. More recently, Perouansky and co-workers showed that volatile anesthetics slow hippocampal oscillations in the 4–12 Hz range, thereby inducing amnesia [8]. This knowledge is extended by the present study by showing that the GABAergic anesthetic propofol also affects hippocampal oscillatory activity in this frequency range, weakens cortico-hippocampal interactions and lowers the shared information content between these two brain regions. Based on the observed differences between wild-type and β3(N265M) mutant mice, we conclude that this process critically depends on GABAA receptors containing β3 subunits.
Our experiments served the purpose to investigate propofol-induced effects on interactions between cortex and hippocampus in the range from 4 to 12 Hz that are strongly related to processes that are necessary for memory consolidation [24]. The cortex and the hippocampus play an important role in memory formation. The observed effects of propofol on oscillatory activity in these regions are likely to be associated with the amnestic component of anesthesia. Rodent studies have also demonstrated that parts of the frontal cortex and hippocampus are among the cerebral regions that are most sensitive to anesthetics [11]. The actions of propofol in the CNS are almost exclusively mediated via GABAA receptors containing β subunits [15,25]. There are three different isoforms of β subunits (β1-β3), resulting in at least three different subtypes of GABAA receptors as defined by their β subunits [26,27]. Currently, studies measuring behavioral endpoints of the actions of propofol on β1 subunit-containing GABAA receptors are limited. However, roughly a decade ago, two landmark studies by Reynolds et al. [18] and Jurd et al. [20] provided evidence that sedation by intravenous anesthetics is primarily mediated by β2 subunit-containing GABAA receptors, while hypnosis and immobility involves β3 subunit-containing GABAA receptors. Furthermore, findings using cortical slice cultures from β3(N265M) knock-in mice demonstrate that the typical electrophysiological actions of propofol—the prolongation of inhibitory postsynaptic currents—critically depend on the presence of the β3 subunit-containing GABAA receptor subtype [21]. With this in mind, we evaluated the actions of propofol on cortico-hippocampal interactions in wild-type and β3(N265M) knock-in mice to elucidate the impact of β3 subunit-containing GABAA receptors.
As mentioned above, the cortex is strongly associated with working, i.e., short-term, memory tasks, but also requires interactions with other brain areas like thalamus and hippocampus [28,29]. In order to consolidate memory, the hippocampus is required. Siapas et al. described that activity in the 4–10 Hz frequency range seems important to gate information flow across cortico-hippocampal circuits. The subsequent plastic changes are believed to underlie the storage of information across these networks [30].
We structured our experiments in three steps. The first investigation was to evaluate propofol-induced changes in spectral band-power in the cortex and hippocampus. The analysis of spectral band-power revealed that propofol does not affect oscillations in the mid-frequency range in the cortex. This result complies with findings from other studies showing that propofol, at least at low concentrations, does not affect the (prefrontal) cortex [31]. In our experiments in wild-type mice, propofol caused a strong activation in the 4–6 Hz range, relating to the theta range, in the hippocampus. This observation is in line with previous experimental findings. Perouansky et al. discovered a slowing of oscillations in the 4–12 Hz range in the rat’s hippocampus as an effect of subanesthetic doses of inhaled anesthetics. It was hypothesized that this slowing led to impaired learning. The authors concluded that modulating hippocampal θ-oscillations may present a key process of anesthetic-induced amnesia [8]. Pan et al. found a crucial frequency of around 6.5 Hz for hippocampal oscillations. If peak frequency drops below 6.5 Hz, learning impairments occur. This may be due to a slowing of these θ-frequencies or due to a reduction in high frequency θ-oscillations [32]. The activation of low θ-frequencies was absent in the β3(N265M) knock-in mice. This finding may indicate that β3 subunit-containing GABAA receptors, which are heavily expressed in both the cortex and hippocampus [33,34,35,36] are necessary to mediate propofol-induced amnestic actions.
The use of PLV can help to determine effects on oscillation synchrony between cortex and hippocampus and the concept of MI may help to detect changes in the amount of information that is shared by these regions through oscillatory activity in the selected frequency range. The next step consisted of an analysis of cortico-hippocampal interactions using PLV to evaluate phase synchrony and hence the strength of connectivity between the two recording sites. This synchrony may be necessary for interactions between different cerebral structures [37]. Recently, a study using PLV showed that phase coupling in the 3–9 Hz range between the cortex and visual area after a visual stimulus is enhanced during active working memory processes [38]. Furthermore, synchrony between the cortex and hippocampus increases during spatial working memory tasks [39], underlining the importance of synchrony for mnemonic processes. Our analysis was targeted at cortico-hippocampal interactions, possibly reflecting memory consolidation processes rather than working memory activity. Propofol negatively affected cortico-hippocampal PLV in the 6–12 Hz range, i.e., synchrony and hence strength of connection decreased. Propofol also negatively influenced PLV in β3(N265M) knock-in mice, but to a lesser degree as compared to the wild-type, since only PLV in the 6–8 Hz range was significantly reduced.
MI analysis indicated significantly reduced shared information content between the cortex and hippocampus with propofol in the 6–16 Hz oscillation range in wild-type animals. This reduction may be explained by the observed drop in PLV at these very frequencies with propofol. Synchrony and hence possibly the strength of cortico-hippocampal interaction become reduced. This reduction consequently leads to reduced information that is shared between both regions. The shared information is also reduced by propofol in the β3(N265M) mice, but to a lesser degree and in a narrower frequency range as in the wild-types.

Limitations

First of all, we are aware of our small sample size. Nevertheless, we found significant and strong effects that highlight the differences in LFP activity between the strains. The delivery of a single bolus dose of propofol via the tail vein leads to a peak and then decreasing concentrations of propofol in the brain. Hence, we did not investigate the effects at steady state conditions, which is very difficult to do in mice, but rather at selected, artifact-free and non-suppressed episodes that followed the (burst) suppression states after propofol delivery. Our findings highlight differences in LFP activity between the wild type and β3(N265M) mutant mice. LFP effects at distinct propofol concentrations need to be investigated in the future.
We also could not investigate the return of the righting reflex, because the head of the animals was fixated. We also did not perform behavioral experiments to investigate memory performance. This should also be subject to further investigations. Finally, we focused on studying a single point mutation in the β3 subunit of the GABAA receptor. The β3 subunit was selected, as the β3-containing GABAA receptors have been shown to be of major importance for the molecular and behavioral actions of propofol [20,22].

4. Materials and Methods

Every endeavor was made to minimize both the suffering and the number of mice sacrificed for the study. All experiments and procedures performed were approved by the local animal care committee (1 January 2007–31 December 2010, Eberhard-Karls-University, Tübingen, Germany) and in accordance with the German law on animal experimentation. Wild-type (n = 4) and GABAA receptor β3(N265M) knock-in mice (n = 3) of the same genetic background (87.5% 129 × 1/SvJ, 12.5% 129/Sv) of both sexes were used [20]. The β3(N265M) point mutation renders β3 subunit-containing GABAA receptors insensitive to propofol, but not towards its natural ligand GABA [20,22].

4.1. Surgical Procedures

The electrode implantation procedure was described in detail in previous publications [28,40,41,42,43]. During general anesthesia with isoflurane, a warming apparatus (Fine Science Tools, Heidelberg, Germany) kept body temperature close to 36 °C. The electrode arrays, consisting of four linear electrodes, were implanted after craniotomy using a stereotactic frame, two arrays in each animal on one hemisphere. The cortical array was placed 200 µm below the pia, the other array was placed 900 µm below the pia to target the hippocampus. Electrode arrays were fixed along with their connecting wires to the skull using dental cement (Flowline, Heraeus Kulzer, Hanau, Germany). Finally, the skin incision was sutured and attached to the implant. Post-operatively, the animals were kept warm and analgesia was achieved using carprofen.

4.2. Electrophysiology

Custom-built microelectrode arrays were used. A 1 × 4 array of polyimide tubing (HV Technologies, Trenton, GA, USA) served as a frame for electrodes made of platinum and tungsten that was glass coated. Electrodes (Thomas Recording, Giessen, Germany) had a shank diameter of 80 µm, a metal core diameter of 23 µm, and a free tip length of 10 µm and impedance was higher than 1 MΩ. The inter-tip distance in the assembled arrays was around 300 µm. Teflon-insulated silver wires (Science Products, Hofheim, Germany) were soldered to the electrodes on one end and connected to a micro-plug (Bürklin, Munich, Germany). Local field potentials were recorded using a multichannel extracellular amplifier (MultiChannelSystems, Reutlingen, Germany; gain 5000, digital sampling rate 20 kHz) in a head-restraint apparatus.

4.3. Recording and Pre-Processing of Local Field Potentials

Adult wild-type and β3(N265M) mice were housed individually for at least one week in a room maintained on a 12-hr light/dark cycle with free access to food and water before electrophysiological recordings. Propofol (10 mg/kg, B.Braun, Melsungen, Germany) was injected intravenously through the tail vein. Electrophysiological recording was started after placing the animal in a head-restraint apparatus. For each experiment, neuronal activity was monitored for ten minutes immediately before propofol treatment (baseline condition) and after propofol administration. The wild-type mice entered a state of isoelectric suppression and required around 50 min to return to baseline LFP activity. A similar state was reached by the mutant mice, already at around 20 min. In the effective period, LFP burst suppression was observed, without ever developing isoelectric LFP.
After the termination of the experiments, the mice were deeply anesthetized and an intracardial perfusion of 0.1 M phosphate puffer prior to 4% paraformaldehyde in phosphate buffer was delivered. The recording sites were marked by electrolytic lesions of the brain tissue, which were identified in the Nissl-stained coronal sections [28].
The initial step of signal preprocessing contained a 100 Hz low pass filtering with a MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA)-based Butterworth filtering routine and a down sampling from fs = 20 kHz to fds = 250 Hz. We present all used LFP episodes in Figure A1. For analysis in the distinct frequency ranges, the recordings were additionally band pass filtered with the MATLAB routine. We used the filtfilt function, allowing filtering without phase shift. Then, we extracted a simultaneous artifact-free 5 s LFP episode from each of the eight channels from all experiments for analysis. In our analyses, we focused on the 2–16 Hz frequency range of the LFP. In order to evaluate effects in the single oscillation frequencies, we investigated oscillatory activity in non-overlapping 2 Hz frequency bands.

4.4. Spectral Power

We used MATLAB to calculate the relative band power for each frequency band represented in the histogram. First, the power spectral density of the 5 s LFP episode over the entire frequency range and for each channel was estimated using the fast Fourier transform. The absolute power spectral density vector was divided through its sum to obtain the relative power. The sum of relative power in the desired frequency range represents the relative band power. Relative band power was calculated for both regions, cortex and hippocampus, to investigate propofol induced effects.

4.5. Phase Locking

We used the phase locking value (PLV) to investigate interactions between cortex and hippocampus. Lachaux et al. introduced PLV to detect synchronic behavior in a defined frequency range between two channels [37]. PLV calculation is based on the analytic phase φ(t) relationship between both channels. φ(t) describes the momentary position of the oscillation for each data point. In contrast to Lachaux, we used the Hilbert transform [44] instead of the wavelet approach to calculate φ(t). PLV is based on the difference in analytic phase between two channels, here the difference in the hippocampal and prefrontal cortical analytic phase, φhippocampus(t) and φcortex(t). The details are described in the next section. Figure A2 shows exemplary signals that are either completely phase locked or completely independent.

4.6. Description of Hilbert Transform and PLV

The analytic signal X(t) of a recorded and filtered signal x(t), here the envelope of the LFP in the respective frequency range, can be derived with the Hilbert transform [44]. In order to correctly apply this method, the LFP has to be filtered to a narrow frequency range [45], here the described frequency bins of 2 Hz width.
X ( t ) = x R E ( t ) + x I M ( t )
is a series of complex numbers with xRE(t) as the real part and xIM(t) as the imaginary part.
The real part xRE(t) complies with the original signal x(t) and xIM(t) complies with x(t) after a 90° phase shift. Based on X(t), the analytic amplitude A(t) and the analytic phase φ(t) can be calculated.
A(t) represents the envelope of x(t) and is derived by A ( t ) = x R E ( t ) 2 + x I M ( t ) 2 .
φ(t), also called the instantaneous phase, is calculated as ϕ ( t ) = arctan ( x I M ( t ) / x R E ( t ) ) .
The difference Φ(t) is defined as Φ ( t ) = ϕ h i p p o c a m p u s ( t ) ϕ c o r t e x ( t )
PLV can be consequently calculated according to P L V = 1 N exp ( i Φ ( t ) ) where N represents the number of data points in the time series.
PLV ranges between 1 and 0 and high values indicate little variance in phase difference, i.e., high synchronization and possibly strong connectivity, whereas a PLV close to 0 indicates that the signals are not connected.
We calculated and averaged the PLV from the extracted 5 s LFP episodes over 5 non-overlapping 1 s (N = 250) segments. Figure A2 presents a graphical description of the PLV calculation.

4.7. Mutual Information

Mutual information (MI) evaluates shared information content between two recording sites [46] i.e., the dependence of two channels. The higher the MI, the higher the dependence between two channels. An MI of zero describes two independent channels. MI was calculated over the entire 5 s segments for all hippocampal-cortical channel combinations.
MI calculation is based on the evaluation of probability distributions—in our case, either distribution of LFP amplitudes of a sequence of LFP amplitudes in both selected channels.
Therefore, in a first step, the filtered 5 s LFP sequences were normalized, i.e., presented as z-scores according to x n [ t ] = x [ t ] m e a n ( x [ t ] ) s t d ( x [ t ] ) . x[t] is a discrete time series, here the filtered and down sampled 5 s LFP episode. In order to generate the amplitude-based probability distributions and the LFP amplitude values of the normalized series xn[t], we used 10 equally spaced bins, as in Figure A3. The width of one bin was the maximum amplitude range, i.e., max(xn[t])-min(xn[t]) divided by the number of bins. For each of the two channels, xn[t] and yn[t], the probability distributions p(x) and p(y) are calculated based on their bin histograms as displayed in Figure A3. Furthermore, the joint probabilities p(x,y) are determined. The mutual information can be estimated by: M I = x N y N p ( x , y ) log ( p ( x , y ) p ( x ) , p ( y ) ) , where N is the number of bins [47].

4.8. Statistics

We used the Hedge’s g effect size measures for dependent and independent samples. Calculation was performed with the MATLAB based MES toolbox [48]. We also performed a 10,000-fold bootstrapping to generate 95% confidence intervals (CI)—95% CI excluding zero indicates significance at p < 0.05. As a rule of thumb, absolute Hedge’s g values > 0.8 indicate a strong effect and values above 0.5 indicate a medium effect [49]. Hence, we discuss the results from the Hedge’s g analyses according to this notation, i.e., we describe a strong effect if g > 0.8 and a medium effect if g > 0.5.

5. Conclusions

Our results suggest that the observed propofol-induced changes in interactions between cortex and hippocampus are largely mediated by β3 subunit-containing GABAA receptors and that this GABAA receptor subtype potentially participates in the process of inducing amnesia by propofol. The decrease in spectral band-power in the 4–6 Hz range was absent in β3(N265M) knock-in mice and the reduction in PLV and MI in our analyses of the θ-frequency range was not as pronounced as in the wild-types.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, B.D., B.A., C.S.; Methodology, B.D., M.K., B.A., C.S., S.B.; Data Analysis, M.K., S.B.; Discussion of the results: M.K., B.D., B.A., P.S.G., G.S. Writing–Original Draft Preparation, M.K., B.D., S.B.; Writing–Review & Editing, M.K., B.D., S.B., B.A., C.S., P.S.G., G.S., U.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by grants from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (AN321/2-1 and SCHW577/81).

Acknowledgments

We thank Ursula Pascht for excellent technical assistance.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Abbreviations

GABAGamma-aminobutyric acid
LFPLocal field potential
MIMutual information
PLVPhase locking value
PSDPower spectral density

Appendix A

Figure A1. Prefrontal cortical (blue) and hippocampal (green) LFP episodes of 5 s used for analysis: (A) episodes derived from the wild types, (B) episodes derived from β3(N265M) mutants. LFP: local field potential.
Figure A1. Prefrontal cortical (blue) and hippocampal (green) LFP episodes of 5 s used for analysis: (A) episodes derived from the wild types, (B) episodes derived from β3(N265M) mutants. LFP: local field potential.
Ijms 21 05844 g0a1
Figure A2. Example for the calculation of PLV for two signals of “locked phase”, i.e., a perfect PLV = one and two signals with “fading phase”, i.e., no phase locking leading to a PLV = 0.001 as well as for real prefrontal cortical and hippocampal LFP. (A) “Locked phase” represents the association between the original (black) signal and a signal of same frequency and amplitude shifted by 270 degrees. “Fading phase” represents the association between the original (black) signal and a signal of higher frequency (yellow) leading to a constantly changing phase relationship. (B) The polar representation of the “locked phase” shows that at all times, the phase difference resides in the 270-degree corridor. For the “fading phase”, the phase difference is equally distributed over all angles. (C) LFP recordings simultaneously recorded from the prefrontal cortex and the hippocampus that were filtered to the 10–12 Hz range. (D) The polar representation of the phase differences shows an accumulation of observations in the upper right quadrant. The resulting PLV is 0.465. PLV: phase locking value; LFP: local field potential.
Figure A2. Example for the calculation of PLV for two signals of “locked phase”, i.e., a perfect PLV = one and two signals with “fading phase”, i.e., no phase locking leading to a PLV = 0.001 as well as for real prefrontal cortical and hippocampal LFP. (A) “Locked phase” represents the association between the original (black) signal and a signal of same frequency and amplitude shifted by 270 degrees. “Fading phase” represents the association between the original (black) signal and a signal of higher frequency (yellow) leading to a constantly changing phase relationship. (B) The polar representation of the “locked phase” shows that at all times, the phase difference resides in the 270-degree corridor. For the “fading phase”, the phase difference is equally distributed over all angles. (C) LFP recordings simultaneously recorded from the prefrontal cortex and the hippocampus that were filtered to the 10–12 Hz range. (D) The polar representation of the phase differences shows an accumulation of observations in the upper right quadrant. The resulting PLV is 0.465. PLV: phase locking value; LFP: local field potential.
Ijms 21 05844 g0a2
Figure A3. Example for the binning of the LFP amplitudes as used for the calculation of the mutual information. (A) Five seconds of normalized LFP recorded from the cortex, filtered to the 4–8 Hz range and down sampled to 250 Hz (black) and a binned representation of the signal using 10 equally spaced bins (orange). (B) Corresponding histogram of the binned amplitudes. LFP: local field potential.
Figure A3. Example for the binning of the LFP amplitudes as used for the calculation of the mutual information. (A) Five seconds of normalized LFP recorded from the cortex, filtered to the 4–8 Hz range and down sampled to 250 Hz (black) and a binned representation of the signal using 10 equally spaced bins (orange). (B) Corresponding histogram of the binned amplitudes. LFP: local field potential.
Ijms 21 05844 g0a3

References

  1. Imas, O.A.; Ropella, K.M.; Ward, B.D.; Wood, J.D.; Hudetz, A.G. Volatile anesthetics disrupt frontal-posterior recurrent information transfer at gamma frequencies in rat. Neurosci. Lett. 2005, 387, 145–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Jordan, D.; Ilg, R.; Riedl, V.; Schorer, A.; Grimberg, S.; Neufang, S.; Omerovic, A.; Berger, S.; Untergehrer, G.; Preibisch, C.; et al. Simultaneous Electroencephalographic and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Indicate Impaired Cortical Top–Down Processing in Association with Anesthetic-induced Unconsciousness. Anesthesiology 2013, 119, 1031–1042. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Ku, S.-W.; Lee, U.; Noh, G.-J.; Jun, I.-G.; Mashour, G.A. Preferential Inhibition of Frontal-to-Parietal Feedback Connectivity Is a Neurophysiologic Correlate of General Anesthesia in Surgical Patients. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e25155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Kratzer, S.; Kreuzer, M.; Mattusch, C.; Kochs, E.F.; Rammes, G. Propofol Modulates Spatial Cortical Signal Propagation in Acute Thalamocortical Brain Slices. In Proceedings of the American Society of Anesthesiology annual meeting (ASA 2014), New Orleans, LA, USA, 11–15 October 2014. [Google Scholar]
  5. Hentschke, H.; Raz, A.; Krause, B.M.; Murphy, C.A.; Banks, M.I. Disruption of cortical network activity by the general anaesthetic isoflurane. Br. J. Anaesth. 2017, 119, 685–696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Eichenbaum, H. A cortical—hippocampal system for declarative memory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2000, 1, 41–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Benchenane, K.; Tiesinga, P.H.; Battaglia, F.P. Oscillations in the prefrontal cortex: A gateway to memory and attention. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2011, 21, 475–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Perouansky, M.; Rau, V.; Ford, T.; Oh, S.I.; Perkins, M.; Eger, E.I.I.; Pearce, R.A. Slowing of the Hippocampal Theta Rhythm Correlates with Anesthetic-Induced Amnesia. Anesthesiology 2010, 113, 1299–1309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. MacIver, M.B.; Bland, B.H. Chaos analysis of EEG during isoflurane-induced loss of righting in rats. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Zhou, C.; Liang, P.; Liu, J.; Ke, B.; Wang, X.; Li, F.; Li, T.; Bayliss, D.A.; Chen, X. HCN1 channels contribute to the effects of amnesia and hypnosis but not immobility of volatile anesthetics. Anesth. Analg. 2015, 121, 661–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Rudolph, U.; Antkowiak, B. Molecular and neuronal substrates for general anaesthetics. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2004, 5, 709–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Sonner, J.M.; Antognini, J.F.; Dutton, R.C.; Flood, P.; Gray, A.T.; Harris, R.A.; Homanics, G.E.; Kendig, J.; Orser, B.; Raines, D.E.; et al. Inhaled Anesthetics and Immobility: Mechanisms, Mysteries, and Minimum Alveolar Anesthetic Concentration. Anesth. Analg. 2003, 97, 718–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Heurteaux, C.; Guy, N.; Laigle, C.; Blondeau, N.; Duprat, F.; Mazzuca, M.; Lang-Lazdunski, L.; Widmann, C.; Zanzouri, M.; Romey, G.; et al. TREK-1, a K+ channel involved in neuroprotection and general anesthesia. EMBO J. 2004, 23, 2684–2695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Ying, S.-W.; Abbas, S.Y.; Harrison, N.L.; Goldstein, P.A. Propofol block of Ih contributes to the suppression of neuronal excitability and rhythmic burst firing in thalamocortical neurons. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2006, 23, 465–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Hill-Venning, C.; Belelli, D.; Peters, J.A.; Lambert, J.J. Subunit—dependent interaction of the general anaesthetic etomidate with the γ-aminobutyric acid type A receptor. Br. J. Pharmacol. 1997, 120, 749–756. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Sanna, E.; Mascia, M.P.; Klein, R.L.; Whiting, P.J.; Biggio, G.; Harris, R.A. Actions of the general anesthetic propofol on recombinant human GABAA receptors: Influence of receptor subunits. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 1995, 274, 353–360. [Google Scholar]
  17. Benke, D.; Fakitsas, P.; Roggenmoser, C.; Michel, C.; Rudolph, U.; Mohler, H. Analysis of the presence and abundance of GABAA receptors containing two different types of alpha subunits in murine brain using point-mutated alpha subunits. J. Biol. Chem. 2004, 279, 43654–43660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Reynolds, D.S.; Rosahl, T.W.; Cirone, J.; O’Meara, G.F.; Haythornthwaite, A.; Newman, R.J.; Myers, J.; Sur, C.; Howell, O.; Rutter, A.R.; et al. Sedation and anesthesia mediated by distinct GABA(A) receptor isoforms. J. Neurosci. 2003, 23, 8608–8617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Drexler, B.; Antkowiak, B.; Engin, E.; Rudolph, U. Identification and characterization of anesthetic targets by mouse molecular genetics approaches. Can. J. Anaesth. 2011, 58, 178–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Jurd, R.; Arras, M.; Lambert, S.; Drexler, B.; Siegwart, R.; Crestani, F.; Zaugg, M.; Vogt, K.E.; Ledermann, B.; Antkowiak, B.; et al. General anesthetic actions in vivo strongly attenuated by a point mutation in the GABAA receptor β3 subunit. FASEB J. 2003, 17, 250–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Drexler, B.; Jurd, R.; Rudolph, U.; Antkowiak, B. Distinct actions of etomidate and propofol at β3-containing γ -aminobutyric acid type A receptors. Neuropharmacology 2009, 57, 446–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Siegwart, R.; Jurd, R.; Rudolph, U. Molecular determinants for the action of general anesthetics at recombinant alpha(2)beta(3)gamma(2)gamma-aminobutyric acid(A) receptors. J. Neurochem. 2002, 80, 140–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. MacIver, M.B.; Mandema, J.W.; Stanski, D.R.; Bland, B.H. Thiopental uncouples hippocampal and cortical synchronized electroencephalographic activity. Anesthesiology 1996, 84, 1411–1424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Young, C.K.; McNaughton, N. Coupling of Theta Oscillations between Anterior and Posterior Midline Cortex and with the Hippocampus in Freely Behaving Rats. Cereb. Cortex 2009, 19, 24–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Smith, A.J.; Oxley, B.; Malpas, S.; Pillai, G.V.; Simpson, P.B. Compounds exhibiting selective efficacy for different beta subunits of human recombinant gamma-aminobutyric acidA receptors. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 2004, 311, 601–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Li, M.; De Blas, A.L. Coexistence of two beta subunit isoforms in the same gamma-aminobutyric acid type A receptor. J. Biol. Chem. 1997, 272, 16564–16569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Fisher, J.L.; Macdonald, R.L. Functional properties of recombinant GABA(A) receptors composed of single or multiple beta subunit subtypes. Neuropharmacology 1997, 36, 1601–1610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Butovas, S.; Rudolph, U.; Jurd, R.; Schwarz, C.; Antkowiak, B. Activity Patterns in the Prefrontal Cortex and Hippocampus during and after Awakening from Etomidate Anesthesia. Anesthesiology 2010, 113, 48–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Goldman-Rakic, P.S. Cellular basis of working memory. Neuron 1995, 14, 477–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Siapas, A.G.; Lubenov, E.V.; Wilson, M.A. Prefrontal Phase Locking to Hippocampal Theta Oscillations. Neuron 2005, 46, 141–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Veselis, R.A.; Pryor, K.O.; Reinsel, R.A.; Mehta, M.; Pan, H.; Johnson, R. Low-dose propofol—induced amnesia is not due to a failure of encoding: Left inferior prefrontal cortex is still active. Anesthesiology 2008, 109, 213–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Pan, W.-X.; McNaughton, N. The medial supramammillary nucleus, spatial learning and the frequency of hippocampal theta activity. Brain Res. 1997, 764, 101–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Heldt, S.A.; Ressler, K.J. Forebrain and midbrain distribution of major benzodiazepine-sensitive GABAA receptor subunits in the adult C57 mouse as assessed with in situ hybridization. Neuroscience 2007, 150, 370–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Sperk, G.; Kirchmair, E.; Bakker, J.; Sieghart, W.; Drexel, M.; Kondova, I. Immunohistochemical distribution of 10 GABA(A) receptor subunits in the forebrain of the rhesus monkey Macaca mulatta. J. Comp. Neurol. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Wisden, W.; Laurie, D.J.; Monyer, H.; Seeburg, P.H. The distribution of 13 GABAA receptor subunit mRNAs in the rat brain. I. Telencephalon, diencephalon, mesencephalon. J. Neurosci. 1992, 12, 1040–1062. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Fritschy, J.M.; Mohler, H. GABAA-receptor heterogeneity in the adult rat brain: Differential regional and cellular distribution of seven major subunits. J. Comp. Neurol. 1995, 359, 154–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Lachaux, J.P.; Rodriguez, E.; Martinerie, J.; Varela, F.J. Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals. Hum. Brain Mapp. 1999, 8, 194–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Liebe, S.; Hoerzer, G.M.; Logothetis, N.K.; Rainer, G. Theta coupling between V4 and prefrontal cortex predicts visual short-term memory performance. Nat. Neurosci. 2012, 15, 456–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. O’Neill, P.-K.; Gordon, J.A.; Sigurdsson, T. Theta oscillations in the medial prefrontal cortex are modulated by spatial working memory and synchronize with the hippocampus through its ventral subregion. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 14211–14224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Hentschke, H.; Schwarz, C.; Antkowiak, B. Neocortex is the major target of sedative concentrations of volatile anaesthetics: Strong depression of firing rates and increase of GABAA receptor-mediated inhibition. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2005, 21, 93–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Hofmann, J.I.; Schwarz, C.; Rudolph, U.; Antkowiak, B. Effects of Diazepam on Low-Frequency and High-Frequency Electrocortical γ-Power Mediated by α1- and α2-GABA(A) Receptors. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2019, 20, 3486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Haiss, F.; Butovas, S.; Schwarz, C. A miniaturized chronic microelectrode drive for awake behaving head restrained mice and rats. J. Neurosci. Methods 2010, 187, 67–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Schwarz, C.; Hentschke, H.; Butovas, S.; Haiss, F.; Stüttgen, M.C.; Gerdjikov, T.V.; Bergner, C.G.; Waiblinger, C. The head-fixed behaving rat—procedures and pitfalls. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 2010, 27, 131–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Freeman, W. Hilbert transform for brain waves. Scholarpedia 2007, 2, 1338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Le Van Quyen, M.; Foucher, J.; Lachaux, J.; Rodriguez, E.; Lutz, A.; Martinerie, J.; Varela, F.J. Comparison of Hilbert transform and wavelet methods for the analysis of neuronal synchrony. J. Neurosci. Methods 2001, 111, 83–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Schreiber, T. Measuring information transfer. Phys. Rev. Lett. 2000, 85, 461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Kraskov, A.; Stögbauer, H.; Grassberger, P. Estimating mutual information. Phys. Rev. E 2004, 69, 066138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Hentschke, H.; Stuttgen, M.C. Computation of measures of effect size for neuroscience data sets. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2011, 34, 1887–1894. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed.; L. Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1988; p. 567. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. PSD error bar plots of the 2 Hz frequency ranges (mean ± sem). (A) LFP recorded from the cortex in the wild-type mice. Propofol has a significant and medium effect on the 12–14 Hz and a significant and strong effect on the 14–16 Hz (Hedges’ g: 1.01; CI: (0.38 to 1.96)) frequency range. In both frequency ranges, propofol causes an increase in relative power. (B) There is no significant effect of propofol on the PSD of LFP activity recorded from cortex in the β3(N265M) group. (C) In the hippocampal LFP recordings from wild-types, propofol significantly and strongly increased relative power in 4–6 Hz range and in the 14–16 Hz range (Hedges’ g: 0.90; CI: (0.26 to 1.78)). (D) Propofol caused a significant and strong increase in relative power in the 8–10 Hz range of LFP recorded from the hippocampus of the β3(N265M) mice. The ivory areas indicate significance, i.e., a 95% confidence interval of Hedge’s g exclusive 0; wild-type (n = 4), β3(N265M) knock-in mice (n = 3); PSD: power spectral density. LFP: local field potential, CI: 95% confidence interval.
Figure 1. PSD error bar plots of the 2 Hz frequency ranges (mean ± sem). (A) LFP recorded from the cortex in the wild-type mice. Propofol has a significant and medium effect on the 12–14 Hz and a significant and strong effect on the 14–16 Hz (Hedges’ g: 1.01; CI: (0.38 to 1.96)) frequency range. In both frequency ranges, propofol causes an increase in relative power. (B) There is no significant effect of propofol on the PSD of LFP activity recorded from cortex in the β3(N265M) group. (C) In the hippocampal LFP recordings from wild-types, propofol significantly and strongly increased relative power in 4–6 Hz range and in the 14–16 Hz range (Hedges’ g: 0.90; CI: (0.26 to 1.78)). (D) Propofol caused a significant and strong increase in relative power in the 8–10 Hz range of LFP recorded from the hippocampus of the β3(N265M) mice. The ivory areas indicate significance, i.e., a 95% confidence interval of Hedge’s g exclusive 0; wild-type (n = 4), β3(N265M) knock-in mice (n = 3); PSD: power spectral density. LFP: local field potential, CI: 95% confidence interval.
Ijms 21 05844 g001
Figure 2. PLV for the wild-type (A) and β3(N265M) mutant recordings (B) for non-overlapping 2 Hz frequency bands. (A) Propofol strongly and significantly affects PLV in the 6–12 Hz range in the wild-type group. (B) Propofol only shows a significant (strong: g = 1.06; CI: (0.38, 2.85)) effect on PLV in the 6–8 Hz range in the β3(N265M) mutant group. The ivory areas indicate significance, i.e., a 95% confidence interval of Hedge’s g exclusive 0; wild-type (n = 4), β3(N265M) knock-in mice (n = 3); PLV: phase locking value; CI: 95% confidence interval.
Figure 2. PLV for the wild-type (A) and β3(N265M) mutant recordings (B) for non-overlapping 2 Hz frequency bands. (A) Propofol strongly and significantly affects PLV in the 6–12 Hz range in the wild-type group. (B) Propofol only shows a significant (strong: g = 1.06; CI: (0.38, 2.85)) effect on PLV in the 6–8 Hz range in the β3(N265M) mutant group. The ivory areas indicate significance, i.e., a 95% confidence interval of Hedge’s g exclusive 0; wild-type (n = 4), β3(N265M) knock-in mice (n = 3); PLV: phase locking value; CI: 95% confidence interval.
Ijms 21 05844 g002
Figure 3. Propofol-induced change in mutual information MI (10 bins) in wild-type (A) and β3(N265M) mice (B) at control conditions and in the presence of propofol. (A) In the wild-type analysis, propofol significantly reduces MI in frequencies above 6 Hz. The observed effects are classified as strong effects by the Hedges’ g test. (B) In the β3(N265M) group propofol has a significant (medium: g < 0.8) decreasing effect on MI only in the frequency range of 6–10 Hz. The ivory areas indicate significance, i.e., a 95% confidence interval of Hedge’s g exclusive 0; wild-type (n = 4), β3(N265M) knock-in mice (n = 3); MI: mutual information.
Figure 3. Propofol-induced change in mutual information MI (10 bins) in wild-type (A) and β3(N265M) mice (B) at control conditions and in the presence of propofol. (A) In the wild-type analysis, propofol significantly reduces MI in frequencies above 6 Hz. The observed effects are classified as strong effects by the Hedges’ g test. (B) In the β3(N265M) group propofol has a significant (medium: g < 0.8) decreasing effect on MI only in the frequency range of 6–10 Hz. The ivory areas indicate significance, i.e., a 95% confidence interval of Hedge’s g exclusive 0; wild-type (n = 4), β3(N265M) knock-in mice (n = 3); MI: mutual information.
Ijms 21 05844 g003
Table 1. The table shows the Hedges’ g values and corresponding 95% CI intervals of the comparison of spectral power, PLV, and MI at control conditions and in the presence of propofol. Grey entries are not significant. A bold representation in black indicates a significant strong effect. A bold and italic representation in black indicates a significant medium effect. PLV: phase locking value; MI: mutual information; CI: 95% confidence interval.
Table 1. The table shows the Hedges’ g values and corresponding 95% CI intervals of the comparison of spectral power, PLV, and MI at control conditions and in the presence of propofol. Grey entries are not significant. A bold representation in black indicates a significant strong effect. A bold and italic representation in black indicates a significant medium effect. PLV: phase locking value; MI: mutual information; CI: 95% confidence interval.
TypeFrequencyHedge’s g (95% CI)
Spectral PowerPLVMI
CortexHippocampus 10 Bins
Wild-type4–6 Hz−0.40 (−1.25, 0.32)−1.55 (−2.68, −1.05)−0.60 (−2.31, 0.18)0.39 (−1.13, 1.19)
6–8 Hz0.29 (−0.45, 1.04)0.10 (−0.71, 0.65)0.78 (0.21, 1.60)1.04 (0.39, 1.90)
8–10 Hz0.50 (−0.29, 1.41)0.48 (−0.53, 1.39)1.80 (1.20, 3.01)1.16 (0.68, 2.06)
10–12 Hz−0.42 (−1.07, 0.10)−0.46 (−1.17, 0.22)1.11 (0.68, 1.88)1.09 (0.80, 1.81)
β3(N265M)Mutant4–6 Hz0.29 (−1.02, 1.98)−0.28 (−0.70, 0.16)−1.03 (−4.30, 0.33)−0.09 (−2.21, 0.85)
6–8 Hz−0.42 (−2.93, 1.02)0.38 (−0.16, 1.54)1.06 (0.38, 2.85)0.78 (0.03, 4.69)
8–10 Hz0.40 (−1.23, 1.31)−0.87 (−3.50, −0.69)−0.62 (−3.49, 0.25)0.68 (0.59, 1.84)
10–12 Hz−0.43 (−6.27, 0.57)−0.28 (−1.12, 0.95)−0.27 (−2.66, 8.03)0.64 (−0.18, 2.34)

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Kreuzer, M.; Butovas, S.; García, P.S.; Schneider, G.; Schwarz, C.; Rudolph, U.; Antkowiak, B.; Drexler, B. Propofol Affects Cortico-Hippocampal Interactions via β3 Subunit-Containing GABAA Receptors. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2020, 21, 5844. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms21165844

AMA Style

Kreuzer M, Butovas S, García PS, Schneider G, Schwarz C, Rudolph U, Antkowiak B, Drexler B. Propofol Affects Cortico-Hippocampal Interactions via β3 Subunit-Containing GABAA Receptors. International Journal of Molecular Sciences. 2020; 21(16):5844. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms21165844

Chicago/Turabian Style

Kreuzer, Matthias, Sergejus Butovas, Paul S García, Gerhard Schneider, Cornelius Schwarz, Uwe Rudolph, Bernd Antkowiak, and Berthold Drexler. 2020. "Propofol Affects Cortico-Hippocampal Interactions via β3 Subunit-Containing GABAA Receptors" International Journal of Molecular Sciences 21, no. 16: 5844. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms21165844

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop