A Novel Tactile Function Assessment Using a Miniature Tactile Stimulator
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Protocol
2.1. Participants
- This study and the experimental protocol were reviewed and approved by the institutional review board (IRB) of the Chang Gung Medical Foundation (IRB no.: 202001628B0A3). All of the methods were performed in accordance with the regulations of the Taiwan Human Subjects Research Act and the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki 1975. The details of the study and the procedures were clearly explained to each participant.
- Sixteen healthy participants were recruited for the main experiment, and written informed consent was obtained from all participants (Figure 1).
- The inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) aged between 40 and 65 years old, and (2) normal cognitive function as indicated on a judgment, orientation, memory, abstract thinking, and calculation (JOMAC) scale.
- The exclusion criteria were as follows: (1) sensory loss (anesthesia or hypoesthesia) or sensation change (paresthesia or hyperesthesia), (2) lesion at peripheral or central nervous systems, (3) entrapment neuropathy, (4) alcoholism or history of alcoholism, (5) rheumatoid disease, (6) complex regional pain syndrome, (7) hypothyroidism, (8) fibromyalgia, (9) end stage renal disease (ESRD), (10) inability to perform the button press to report perceived orientation or direction, and (11) obvious calluses or wounds noted on the index fingertip.
2.2. Experimental Setup
- 5.
- The MTS system was set up on a desk and connected to a PC through RS-232 ports (Figure 2A).
- 6.
- The MTS system comprised a three-motor controller for manipulating the indentation depth, directions of motion, and rotation speeds of the grating ball; a finger support surface for the participant to place their finger on; and a human–machine interface for controlling the MTS [17].
- 7.
- MTS consisted of three degrees of freedom (DoF) (one of which is illustrated in Figure 2B): rotation for producing motion, vertical excursion for controlling the depth of indentation into the skin, and arm orientation for controlling the direction of motion. Each DoF was controlled by a single motor: two DC motors controlled the rotational motion and direction of motion, while one DC motor controlled the depth of indentation.
- 8.
- The user interface (UI) included several parts: the first part was for registering information on the participants’ general characteristics; the second part was for setting up the three control parameters (indentation depth, motion direction, and rotation speed) of the grating ball; the third part was for recording the participant’ responses in the orientation discrimination task, where they selected the corresponding vertically and horizontally oriented pictures on the screen; and the fourth part was for analyzing the results of the task immediately after each training or testing session.
- 9.
- The grating ball, engraved with 2 mm sine-wave gratings with a peak-to-peak amplitude of 0.5 mm, was installed on the stimulator (Figure 2C).
- 10.
- On each trial, the grating ball first rotated the orientation of the grating ball (Figure 2B), and then rotated the grating ball to reach the defined surface scanning speed. Next, the rotating grating ball compressed the fingerpad with a fixed indentation depth of 1 mm for 1 s to present the tactile stimulation. Finally, the rotated grating ball moved upward, left the skin, and then stopped rotating.
- 11.
- Headphones and a blindfold were prepared, which prevented the participants from seeing the grating orientation and hearing the motor noise of the MTS.
- 12.
- Information on the participants’ basic characteristics was registered, and tactile stimuli parameters were set into the control program via the UI.
- 13.
- The participants’ index finger was secured inside the finger holder using Velcro. The forearm and hand were positioned palm side up on the forearm supporter and the base of the MTS.
- 14.
- The grating ball was adjusted to the surface of the participants’ finger, to make sure that the indentation of the tactile stimuli during tests was precise (Figure 2B).
2.2.1. Instruction Session
- 15.
- The participants were informed of the procedure, safety precautions, and purpose of the experiment. The participants were then asked to sign the written informed consent form.
- 16.
- The experimenter measured TSA using the MTS and comprised three sessions: eye-opened training, eye-closed training, and testing.
- 17.
- The MTS performed tactile stimulations on the index fingertip of the participant’s right hand. The procedure was noninvasive and painless, and the total duration was approximately 30 min.
- 18.
- In the main experiment, in each trial, the participants were presented with one of the stimulus conditions from a factorial design of 16 combinations (4 directions × 4 speeds) in a pseudorandom order. Specifically, the directions were 0°, 90°, 180°, or 270° and the surface moving speeds were 5, 10, 40, or 160 mm/s. The tactile stimulus had an indentation depth of 1 mm, and each stimulus lasted for 1s.
- 19.
- The reference frame was defined with respect to the forearm when the patients were in a neutral position. Directions of 0° and 180° indicated that the grating ball was moving rightward and leftward, respectively, and thus yielded a vertical orientation as the grating orientation was orthogonal to the scanning direction. Similarly, 90° and 270° indicated that the grating ball was moving distally and proximately, respectively, and thus yielded a horizontal orientation (Figure 2D). Although the participants were presented with one of the four directions, the participants’ task was to report the perceived grating orientation (horizontal or vertical).
2.2.2. Eye-Opened Training Session
- 20.
- The participants’ fingers were fixed at the appropriate location inside the finger holder (Please see numbers 9 and 10 in Section 2.2).
- 21.
- The aim of this task was to confirm that the participants understood the task structure.
- 22.
- The participants were instructed to visually inspect the orientation of the grating engraved on the stimulation ball during stimulation.
- 23.
- The tactile stimulation was identical to that described in numbers 4 and 5 in Section 2.2.1, with a fixed surface moving speed of 40 mm/s. We employed a total of four combinations (4 directions × 1 speed) and repeated each combination three times, yielding a total of 12 trials (4 directions × 1 speed × 3 repetitions). In each trial, the participants were thus presented with one of the vertical or horizontal orientations (see number 5 in Section 2.2.1). As a two-alternative forced choice design, the participants verbally reported the perceived orientation after each tactile stimulus.
- 24.
- Based on our inclusion criteria of cognitive function, we expected that all participants would meet the passing criteria during training.
2.2.3. Eye-Closed Training Session
- 25.
- This was a 24-trial task where the participants’ eyes remained closed.
- 26.
- The participants were asked to wear a blindfold and headphones that played white noise so that they could not see or hear the stimuli.
- 27.
- The aim of the eye-closed training session was to ensure that the participants could perform the task under conditions analogous to those of the testing session.
- 28.
- Each stimulation was performed as described in number 4 and 5 of Section 2.2.1, with a fixed surface moving speed of 40 mm/s. We employed a total of four combinations (4 directions × 1 speed) and each of the combinations was repeated six times (4 directions × 1 speed × 6 repetitions = 24 trials).
- 29.
- The accuracy, quantified by the probability that the participants would give a correct answer, was computed from the aforementioned 24 trials. If the participants’ accuracy was ≥75%, we proceeded to the testing session; if not, we repeated the eye-opened training (number 3 and 4 in Section 2.2.2) to make sure they understood the task.
- 30.
- The participants were excluded if their accuracy did not reach 100% during the second round of eye-opened training.
2.2.4. Testing Session
- 31.
- The setup of the testing session was identical to that in the eye-closed training session (see number 2 in Section 2.2.3), and the stimulus protocol was identical to that delineated in number 4 in Section 2.2.1.
- 32.
- The testing session involved three blocks, and each block had a factorial design of 32 combinations (4 directions × 4 speeds × 2 repetitions), for a total of 96 trials.
- 33.
- The results of each patient in the trial were analyzed.
- 34.
- In order to investigate the effect of motion of rotation with the grating edge on the fingerpad, some of the participants (8 of 16) also received additional stimulations by using 1 mm and 4 mm sine-wave grating balls (Figure 2C).
- 35.
- The procedure for the stimulation using 1mm and 4mm grating balls was the same as that of the 2 mm grating ball.
2.3. Usability Test
- 36.
- 37.
- The recruited participants were briefly instructed with our MTS-based protocol.
- 38.
- Our examiner firstly operated MTS to test the participants’ tactile acuity using the protocol.
- 39.
- Next, the participants would try to operate MTS and test the examiner’s tactile acuity independently.
- 40.
- Finally, the participants completed the SUS questionnaire.
2.4. Data Analysis
- 41.
- The statistical analyses were performed using Statistical Program for Social Sciences (SPSS). The Wilcoxon signed-rank test was applied to compare the inter-orientation accuracy between the vertical and horizontal orientations. The Friedman test was applied to compare the accuracy between speeds, and if the results were significant, the Wilcoxon signed-rank test was used as the post hoc test. The repeated measure ANOVA was applied to the full 3 wavelength × 4 speed model, in which factors are wavelength (1, 2, and 4 mm sine-wave grating ball) and speed (5, 10, 40, and 160 mm/s). The confidence interval was corrected using the Bonferroni method.
- 42.
- All data are presented in terms of the mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM) with 95% confidence intervals.
- 43.
- The SUS questionnaire consisted of 10 statements with five positive and five negative statements and each question had a five-point scale (strongly disagree = 1 point and strongly agree = 5 points).
- 44.
- For analysis of SUS, the odd-numbered questions, Q1, 3, 5, 7, and 9, were positive questions, and the recorded scores were: original score − 1.
- 45.
- Even numbered questions, Q2, 4, 6, 8, and 10, were negative questions, and the recorded scores were: 5 − original scores.
- 46.
- The recorded scores from the ten questions were summed up and then multiplied by 2.5 to yield the total SUS score.
- 47.
- The total SUS score ranged from 0 to 100. When it came to an acceptable SUS score, products were at least passable when SUS scores were over 70. Good products could score in the mid-70s to low-80s. Excellent products would score better than 85. Products with scores less than 70 needed to be considered for scrutiny and improvement [22].
3. Results
4. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Klingner, C.M.; Witte, O.W. Somatosensory deficits. In Handbook of Clinical Neurology; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2018; Volume 151, pp. 185–206. [Google Scholar]
- Van Boven, R.W.; Johnson, K.O. The limit of tactile spatial resolution in humans: Grating orientation discrimination at the lip, tongue, and finger. Neurology 1994, 44, 2361–2366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Boven, R.W.; Johnson, K.O. A psychophysical study of the mechanisms of sensory recovery following nerve injury in humans. Brain 1994, 117 Pt 1, 149–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Grant, A.C.; Fernandez, R.; Shilian, P.; Yanni, E.; Hill, M.A. Tactile spatial acuity differs between fingers: A study comparing two testing paradigms. Percept. Psychophys. 2006, 68, 1359–1362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Johnson, K.O.; Phillips, J.R. Tactile spatial resolution. I. Two-point discrimination, gap detection, grating resolution, and letter recognition. J. Neurophysiol. 1981, 46, 1177–1192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tong, J.; Mao, O.; Goldreich, D. Two-Point Orientation Discrimination Versus the Traditional Two-Point Test for Tactile Spatial Acuity Assessment. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, P.B.; Koerber, H.R.; Millecchia, R. From innervation density to tactile acuity: 1. Spatial representation. Brain Res. 2004, 1011, 14–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Craig, J.C. Grating orientation as a measure of tactile spatial acuity. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 1999, 16, 197–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bara-Jimenez, W.; Shelton, P.; Hallett, M. Spatial discrimination is abnormal in focal hand dystonia. Neurology 2000, 55, 1869–1873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Grant, A.C.; Zangaladze, A.; Thiagarajah, M.C.; Sathian, K. Tactile perception in developmental dyslexia: A psychophysical study using gratings. Neuropsychologia 1999, 37, 1201–1211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grant, A.C.; Thiagarajah, M.C.; Sathian, K. Tactile perception in blind Braille readers: A psychophysical study of acuity and hyperacuity using gratings and dot patterns. Percept Psychophys. 2000, 62, 301–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Van Boven, R.W.; Hamilton, R.H.; Kauffman, T.; Keenan, J.P.; Pascual-Leone, A. Tactile spatial resolution in blind braille readers. Neurology 2000, 54, 2230–2236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sakai, S.; Saito, K.; Kojima, S.; Otsuru, N.; Onishi, H. Grating orientation task trial numbers for short- and long-term tactile discrimination learning. J. Clin. Neurosci. 2021, 93, 195–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bleyenheuft, Y.; Cols, C.; Arnould, C.; Thonnard, J.L. Age-related changes in tactile spatial resolution from 6 to 16 years old. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 2006, 23, 83–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Remblay, F.; Backman, A.; Cuenco, A.; Vant, K.; Wassef, M.A. Assessment of spatial acuity at the fingertip with grating (JVP) domes: Validity for use in an elderly population. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 2000, 17, 61–66. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Tremblay, F.; Wong, K.; Sanderson, R.; Cote, L. Tactile spatial acuity in elderly persons: Assessment with grating domes and relationship with manual dexterity. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 2003, 20, 127–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pei, Y.C.; Lee, T.C.; Chang, T.Y.; Ruffatto, D., 3rd; Spenko, M.; Bensmaia, S. A multi-digit tactile motion stimulator. J. Neurosci. Methods 2014, 226, 80–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hsu, Y.C.; Yeh, C.I.; Huang, J.J.; Hung, C.H.; Hung, C.P.; Pei, Y.C. Illusory Motion Reversal in Touch. Front. Neurosci. 2019, 13, 605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pei, Y.-C.; Bensmaia, S.J. The neural basis of tactile motion perception. J. Neurophysiol. 2014, 112, 3023–3032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brooke, J. SUS: A quick and dirty usability scale. Usability Eval. Ind. 1996, 189, 4–7. [Google Scholar]
- Brooke, J. SUS: A retrospective. J. Usability Stud. 2013, 8, 29–40. [Google Scholar]
- Bangor, A.; Kortum, P.; Miller, J.A. The System Usability Scale (SUS): An Empirical evaluation. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact. 2008, 24, 574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, Y.P.; Yeh, C.I.; Lee, T.C.; Huang, J.J.; Pei, Y.C. Relative posture between head and finger determines perceived tactile direction of motion. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 5494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Sathian, K.; Zangaladze, A. Tactile spatial acuity at the human fingertip and lip: Bilateral symmetry and interdigit variability. Neurology 1996, 46, 1464–1466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Johnson, K.O. The roles and functions of cutaneous mechanoreceptors. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2001, 11, 455–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bleyenheuft, Y.; Thonnard, J.L. Tactile spatial resolution measured manually: A validation study. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 2007, 24, 111–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pei, Y.C.; Hsiao, S.S.; Craig, J.C.; Bensmaia, S.J. Neural mechanisms of tactile motion integration in somatosensory cortex. Neuron 2011, 69, 536–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Loken, L.S.; Lundblad, L.C.; Elam, M.; Olausson, H.W. Tactile direction discrimination and vibration detection in diabetic neuropathy. Acta Neurol. Scand. 2010, 121, 302–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Olausson, H.; Norrsell, U. Observations on human tactile directional sensibility. J. Physiol. 1993, 464, 545–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vierck, C.J., Jr. Tactile movement detection and discrimination following dorsal column lesions in monkeys. Exp. Brain Res. 1974, 20, 331–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gleeson, B.T.; Horschel, S.K.; Provancher, W.R. Perception of Direction for Applied Tangential Skin Displacement: Effects of Speed, Displacement, and Repetition. IEEE Trans. Haptics 2010, 3, 177–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lundblad, L.C.; Olausson, H.; Wasling, P.; Jood, K.; Wysocka, A.; Hamilton, J.P.; McIntyre, S.; Backlund Wasling, H. Tactile direction discrimination in humans after stroke. Brain Commun. 2020, 2, fcaa088. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Norrsell, U.; Olausson, H. Spatial cues serving the tactile directional sensibility of the human forearm. J. Physiol. 1994, 478 Pt 3, 533–540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Participant | Age (Years) | Sex | Test Hand | Education Level |
---|---|---|---|---|
#1 | 55 | Female | Right | University |
#2 | 49 | Female | Excluded * | Junior high school |
#3 | 51 | Male | Right | University |
#4 | 40 | Male | Right | University |
#5 | 49 | Female | Excluded * | Senior high school |
#6 | 56 | Male | Right | Elementary school |
#7 | 54 | Female | Right | Senior high school |
#8 | 55 | Female | Right | University |
#9 | 50 | Male | Right | University |
#10 | 49 | Male | Right | University |
#11 | 43 | Female | Right | University |
#12 | 41 | Male | Right | University |
#13 | 49 | Male | Right | University |
#14 | 45 | Female | Right | University |
#15 | 41 | Male | Right | University |
#16 | 50 | Female | Right | University |
Participant | Accuracy in Training Sessions (%) | Pass Training Criteria? | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Eye-Opened Training (1st) | Eye-Closed Training | Eye-Opened Training (2nd) | ||
#1 | 100 | 95.8 | - | Yes |
#3 | 100 | 62.5 | 100 | Yes |
#4 | 100 | 83.3 | - | Yes |
#6 | 100 | 87.5 | - | Yes |
#7 | 100 | 100 | - | Yes |
#8 | 100 | 95.8 | - | Yes |
#9 | 100 | 87.5 | - | Yes |
#10 | 100 | 95.8 | - | Yes |
#11 | 100 | 91.7 | - | Yes |
#12 | 100 | 83.3 | - | Yes |
#13 | 100 | 45.8 | 100 | Yes |
#14 | 100 | 83.3 | - | Yes |
#15 | 100 | 83.3 | - | Yes |
#16 | 100 | 58.3 | 100 | Yes |
Participant | Accuracy in Different Speeds (%) | Accuracy in Different Orientations (%) | Total Accuracy (%) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
5 mm/s | 10 mm/s | 40 mm/s | 160 mm/s | Vertical | Horizontal | ||
#1 | 95.8 | 95.8 | 87.5 | 70.8 | 89.6 | 85.4 | 87.5 |
#3 | 95.8 | 95.8 | 91.7 | 58.3 | 79.2 | 91.7 | 85.4 |
#4 | 87.5 | 91.7 | 100 | 79.2 | 85.4 | 95.8 | 89.6 |
#6 | 91.7 | 100 | 95.8 | 95.8 | 95.8 | 95.8 | 95.8 |
#7 | 100 | 100 | 100 | 58.3 | 79.2 | 100 | 89.6 |
#8 | 100 | 100 | 91.7 | 75 | 93.8 | 89.6 | 91.7 |
#9 | 95.8 | 100 | 91.7 | 58.3 | 87.5 | 85.4 | 86.5 |
#10 | 100 | 95.8 | 100 | 87.5 | 93.8 | 97.9 | 95.8 |
#11 | 91.7 | 91.7 | 87.5 | 87.5 | 89.6 | 89.6 | 89.6 |
#12 | 95.8 | 95.8 | 91.7 | 79.2 | 85.4 | 95.8 | 90.6 |
#13 | 95.8 | 91.7 | 75 | 54.2 | 77.1 | 89.6 | 79.2 |
#14 | 95.8 | 87.5 | 87.5 | 83.3 | 81.3 | 87.5 | 88.5 |
#15 | 95.8 | 95.8 | 91.7 | 79.2 | 85.4 | 95.8 | 90.6 |
#16 | 100 | 91.7 | 95.8 | 66.7 | 91.7 | 87.5 | 88.5 |
Participant | Age (Years) | Sex | EDUCATIONAL LEVEL |
---|---|---|---|
#1 | 25 | Male | Senior high school * |
#2 | 31 | Male | University |
#3 | 25 | Male | Senior high school * |
#4 | 27 | Female | University |
#5 | 41 | Female | University |
#6 | 32 | Male | University |
#7 | 32 | Male | University |
Item | Content | Score |
---|---|---|
All Subjects (n = 7) | ||
Q1 | I would like to use the miniature tactile stimulator often | 3.57 ± 0.20 |
Q2 | I think the miniature tactile stimulator is complex to use | 3.43 ± 0.20 |
Q3 | I think the miniature tactile stimulator is easy to use | 4.00 ± 0.00 |
Q4 | I required technical assistance to use the miniature tactile stimulator | 2.71 ± 0.52 |
Q5 | I think the functionalities of the miniature tactile stimulator are well integrated | 3.71 ± 0.18 |
Q6 | I think the functionalities of the miniature tactile stimulator are not consistent | 3.43 ± 0.20 |
Q7 | I think most users can quickly learn to use the miniature tactile stimulator | 3.86 ± 0.14 |
Q8 | I think most users have difficulties learning to use the miniature tactile stimulator | 3.86 ± 0.14 |
Q9 | I am confident when using the miniature tactile stimulator | 3.43 ± 0.30 |
Q10 | I need to learn more background information of the miniature tactile stimulator before use | 3.43 ± 0.20 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Sung, C.-T.; Wang, Y.-J.; Huang, J.-J.; Pei, Y.-C.; Lin, L.-C.; Mai, W.-H.; Chang, B.-L. A Novel Tactile Function Assessment Using a Miniature Tactile Stimulator. Sensors 2023, 23, 1844. https://doi.org/10.3390/s23041844
Sung C-T, Wang Y-J, Huang J-J, Pei Y-C, Lin L-C, Mai W-H, Chang B-L. A Novel Tactile Function Assessment Using a Miniature Tactile Stimulator. Sensors. 2023; 23(4):1844. https://doi.org/10.3390/s23041844
Chicago/Turabian StyleSung, Chung-Tung, Yung-Jung Wang, Jian-Jia Huang, Yu-Cheng Pei, Lei-Chi Lin, Wen-Hsin Mai, and Bao-Luen Chang. 2023. "A Novel Tactile Function Assessment Using a Miniature Tactile Stimulator" Sensors 23, no. 4: 1844. https://doi.org/10.3390/s23041844
APA StyleSung, C.-T., Wang, Y.-J., Huang, J.-J., Pei, Y.-C., Lin, L.-C., Mai, W.-H., & Chang, B.-L. (2023). A Novel Tactile Function Assessment Using a Miniature Tactile Stimulator. Sensors, 23(4), 1844. https://doi.org/10.3390/s23041844