Next Article in Journal
Intermedin Alleviates Diabetic Cardiomyopathy by Up-Regulating CPT-1β through Activation of the Phosphatidyl Inositol 3 Kinase/Protein Kinase B Signaling Pathway
Previous Article in Journal
High-Risk HPV Detection in Paraffin-Embedded Tissue from Cervical Lesions
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Inhibition of Nitric Oxide Synthesis Prevents the Effects of Intermittent Social Defeat on Cocaine-Induced Conditioned Place Preference in Male Mice

by
María Ángeles Martínez-Caballero
1,
María Pilar García-Pardo
2,
Claudia Calpe-López
3,
María Carmen Arenas
4,
Carmen Manzanedo
4 and
María Asuncion Aguilar
1,*
1
Neurobehavioural Mechanisms and Endophenotypes of Addictive Behavior Research Unit, Department of Psychobiology, University of Valencia, 46010 Valencia, Spain
2
Department of Psychology and Sociology, Faculty of Social Sciences, University of Zaragoza, 50009 Teruel, Spain
3
Institute of Psychopharmacology, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, University of Heidelberg, 68167 Mannheim, Germany
4
Department of Psychobiology, University of Valencia, 46010 Valencia, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Pharmaceuticals 2024, 17(9), 1203; https://doi.org/10.3390/ph17091203 (registering DOI)
Submission received: 8 August 2024 / Revised: 5 September 2024 / Accepted: 9 September 2024 / Published: 12 September 2024

Abstract

:
We have previously observed that mice exposed to social defeat stress are more sensitive to cocaine in the conditioned place preference (CPP) paradigm. In this context, it has been suggested that the nitric oxide (NO) pathway plays a role in the effects of stress. The present study evaluates the role of a neuronal NO synthase (nNOS) inhibitor (7-nitroindazole, 7-NI) in the short- and long-term behavioural effects of intermittent social defeat (ISD). Four groups of mice were employed for the study: a control group and three stressed groups, one treated with vehicle and two treated with 7-NI (7.25 or 12.5 mg/kg). After the last episode of defeat, mice were tested in the elevated plus maze (EPM), social interaction, object recognition and tail suspension tests. Three weeks later, mice were conditioned with cocaine (1 mg/kg). Stressed mice, irrespective of the treatment received, showed anxiety in the EPM, presented a deficit of social interaction and spent less time immobile in the tail suspension test. However, only stressed mice treated with vehicle developed CPP. Thus, although 7-NI did not modify the short-term behavioural effects of ISD, it prevented ISD-induced potentiation of the rewarding properties of cocaine in adulthood. These results support a specific role of nNOS in the effects of social stress on drug reward.

1. Introduction

Environmental factors, such as stress exposure, can make individuals more vulnerable to developing drug abuse [1,2,3]. In previous studies, we have observed that adult male mice exposed to intermittent social defeat (ISD) during late adolescence (post-natal day (PND) 47, 50, 53 and 56) are more sensitive to the rewarding effects of cocaine on the conditioned place preference (CPP) paradigm [4,5,6,7]. In addition, we have seen that ISD induces several short-term behavioural alterations that could be related with the subsequent resilience or vulnerability of mice to the development of cocaine CPP, such as a decrease in the number of entries and time spent in the open arms of the elevated plus maze (EPM), social avoidance in the social interaction test, greater immobility in the tail suspension test, and a reduction of grooming in the splash test [5].
Several peripheral and central stress responses, including autonomic or hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) responses and structural and functional brain alterations (decreased prefrontal functioning and increased limbic–striatal activation), have been associated with the pathophysiology of substance use disorders [8]. Regarding the neurobiological substrates of the effects of ISD on cocaine reward, we have observed that adult mice exposed to ISD during late adolescence show decreased expression of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (proBDNF) in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and its receptor tropomyosin receptor kinase B (TrkB) in the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) [9]. In addition, we have seen how the enhanced response to cocaine after social stress is reversed by antagonism of the corticotropin-releasing factor type 1 (CRF1) [10] and N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) glutamate receptors [4].
Using a different protocol of SD stress (four episodes of SD on alternate days), we have previously observed that NMDA and AMPA (α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid) glutamate receptors and neuronal nitric oxide synthase (nNOS), a downstream signal molecule of NMDA receptor activation, are implicated in the effects of SD on the rewarding properties of the psychostimulant drug MDMA (3,4 methylenedioxymethamphetamine) in mice [11,12]. In particular, exposure to an episode of defeat immediately before each conditioning session with MDMA (PND 54, 56, 58 and 60) blunted the rewarding effects of this drug in the CPP paradigm, and this effect was prevented by administration of the NMDA antagonist memantine [11] and the nNOS inhibitor 7-nitroindazole (7-NI) [12]. Moreover, exposure to this protocol of SD decreased the expression of several subunits of NMDA and AMPA receptors—mainly GluN1 and GluA1—in the striatum and hippocampus [11] and increased nitrite levels in these structures [12] 48 h after the last episode of defeat. Considered as a whole, these results demonstrate that glutamatergic–nitric oxide plasticity is involved in the effects of SD stress on MDMA reward.
The role of NO in other behavioural consequences of social stress, such as depression- and anxiety-like symptomatology, has only been studied using the protocol of chronic SD stress (CSDS), in which experimental animals are exposed to daily episodes of defeat on 10 consecutive days and are in sensory contact with opponents for a 24 h interval between each episode. Social avoidance and depression-like behaviours observed in mice exposed to CSDS are known to be mediated by nNOS enzyme activity and NO production, as CSDS increases the expression of nNOS in the shell of the NAcc [13]. Similarly, increased anxiety-like behaviour induced by CSDS is accompanied by the activation of nNOS-containing neurons in the right (but not left) medial prefrontal cortex [14].
In light of the above evidence, the aim of the present study was to evaluate the role of the NO pathway in the behavioural effects of ISD. With this purpose in mind, we tested whether the nNOS enzyme inhibitor 7-NI can prevent the short-term effects of ISD exposure in young mice undergoing the EPM, social interaction, object recognition and tail suspension tests 24/48 h after the last defeat episode (anxiety- and depression-like symptoms, cognitive deficits and stress hyperreactivity). In addition, we explored whether 7-NI can block the long-term effects of ISD on cocaine reward, i.e., the potentiation of the rewarding effects of cocaine observed in adult mice exposed to ISD during late adolescence.

2. Results

2.1. Behavioural Analysis during Episodes of Defeat

The data of the time spent in the different behavioural categories and latencies to show these behaviours are shown in Table 1. ANOVA of the threat data showed that the variables labelled as days [F(1,31) = 4.696; p < 0.05] and treatment [F(2,31) = 4.694; p < 0.05] were significant, but not their interaction. Mice spent more time under threat from the opponent in the first than in the last episode of defeat (p < 0.05), and the 7-NI12 + SD group spent less time under threat from the opponent than the vehicle + SD group (p < 0.05). In addition, ANOVA of the latency of avoidance/flee behaviour revealed that the variable labelled treatment was significant [F(2,31) = 4.349; p < 0.05] and that mice in the 7-NI12 + SD group showed lower latency to avoid or flee the opponent than those in the vehicle + SD group (p < 0.05). As data for latency of avoidance/flee in the first episode of defeat and of latency of defence/submission in the fourth episode have unequal variances we performed ANOVA–Welch, the results of which are not significant.

2.2. Role of 7-NI in the Short-Term Effects of ISD

Regarding the elevated plus maze (EPM), the ANOVAs revealed that the values for entries in open arms [EOA F(3,40) = 3.801; p < 0.05], percentage of entries in open arms [%EOA F(3,40) = 5.526; p < 0.01], time in open arms [TOA F(3,40) = 5.521; p < 0.01] and percentage of time in open arms [%TOA F(3,40) = 5.452; p < 0.01] were significant. Post-hoc comparison of the treatment variable showed a reduction in these measurements in all stressed mice in comparison with the control group, irrespective of whether they received treatment with vehicle or 7-NI. In particular, mice treated with vehicle + SD and 7-NI7 + SD differed from controls (ps < 0.05) in EOA, %EOA, TOA and %TOA (Figure 1a–d, respectively), and the differences between mice treated with 7-NI12 + SD and controls were more significant (p < 0.01 for EOA, TOA and %TOA and p < 0.001 for %EOA). ANOVAs for latency to enter into the open arms (LOA) and number of total entries into the arms (TotalE) did not reveal significance.
ANOVA of the social interaction data (Figure 2) also confirmed significance [F(3,40) = 4.802; p < 0.01]. Post-hoc comparison of the treatment variable revealed a reduction of the index of social interaction (ISI) in comparison with controls among mice in the vehicle + SD, 7NI7 + SD and 7-NI12 + SD groups (p < 0.05, p < 0.01 and p < 0.01, respectively).
In relation to the tail suspension test (TST), ANOVA of the time spent immobile (Figure 3a) showed that values were significant [F(3,40) = 7.261; p < 0.001]. Post-hoc comparison of the treatment variable revealed that mice in the vehicle + SD, 7NI7 + SD and 7-NI12 + SD groups spent less time immobile than controls (p < 0.01, p < 0.001 and p < 0.01, respectively). ANOVA of the latency to become immobile data (Figure 3b) also confirmed significance [F(3,40) = 5.714; p < 0.002]; however, in this case, post-hoc comparison revealed that all the groups differed from the vehicle + SD group (p < 0.05 vs. control; ps < 0.01 vs. 7-NI7 + SD and 7-NI12 + SD groups).
ANOVA of the object recognition data did not show values to be significant (Supplementary Figure S1).

2.3. Role of 7-NI in the Long-Term Effects of ISD

ANOVA of the CPP score (Figure 4a) confirmed significance [F(3,35) = 4.157, p < 0.05]. Post-hoc comparison of the treatment variable revealed that only the vehicle + SD group obtained a higher CPP score than the control group (p < 0.01). In addition, ANOVA of the time spent in the drug-paired compartment (Figure 4b) revealed that the days variable [F(1,35) = 9.259, p < 0.01] and the interaction days X group variable [F(3,35) = 4.157, p < 0.05] were significant, while the treatment variable was not significant. Post-hoc comparison of the interaction variable showed that only stressed mice treated with vehicle (and not those treated with 7-NI) developed CPP (p < 0.001; significant difference in the time spent in the drug-paired compartment in pre-C vs. post-C in the vehicle + SD group).

2.4. Correlations between Behavioural Measurements

Significant Pearson correlations are shown in Table 2 and Table 3. Among the results, it is notable that preference for the open arms of the EPM correlated with several behaviours in the fourth episode of defeat; in particular, TOA and %TOA correlated positively with attack and avoidance/flee, but negatively with defence/submission (ps < 0.05). ISI negatively correlated with threat in the first episode of defeat and attack in the fourth episode of defeat (ps < 0.05). In addition, immobility in the TST and CPP score correlated positively with defence/submission in the fourth episode of defeat (ps < 0.05), while positive correlations were observed between immobility in the TST and ISI (p < 0.01), and between CPP score and LOA (p < 0.01).

3. Discussion

The results of the present study indicate that administration of the selective nNOS inhibitor 7-NI does not modify the short-term behavioural effects of ISD, such as the presence of anxiety-like symptoms in the EPM, the deficit of social interaction and hyperreactivity in a stressful situation (TST). In contrast, 7-NI is able to prevent the increase in latency of immobility in the TST that we observed in defeated mice. More importantly, administration of 7-NI before each experience of defeat during late-adolescence prevents ISD-induced potentiation of the rewarding properties of cocaine in adulthood.
Administration of 7-NI did not induce effects on the behaviour of mice during social defeat episodes, with the exception of a decrease in the latency when avoiding or fleeing the opponent (which consequently showed lower levels of threat) observed in mice treated with the dose of 12.5 mg/kg. Although this behavioural trait of mice treated with the high dose of 7-NI could be interpreted as a more active coping strategy (which could reduce the short-term negative consequences of social defeat, such as anxiety- or depression-like symptoms), this idea is not supported by the results observed. There are no differences between groups of defeated mice regarding other behaviours frequently associated with active coping, such as higher time spent in avoidance/fleeing or lower time spent in defence/submission. In addition, mice of all groups exposed to ISD (irrespective of the treatment with vehicle or any dose of 7-NI) showed a similar behavioural response in every test performed shortly after the last episode of defeat (EPM, social interaction, TST and object recognition).
Exposure to ISD during late adolescence reduced the number of entries and time spent in the open arms of the EPM, as well as the percentages of these measures, thus indicating the presence of anxiety-like symptoms in these defeated mice, in line with previous studies carried out in our laboratory [5,6]. We expected that inhibition of nNOS with 7-NI would prevent the anxiety-like effects of ISD, as this compound has anxiolytic properties in animal models of anxiety [15], and we have previously observed an increase in nitrite levels in the striatum and hippocampus after SD [12]. In addition, other protocols of repeated stress have been shown to activate the NO pathway in mice; for example, chronic unpredictable stress increased NOS activity in the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus [16], while CSDS activated nNOS-containing neurons in the right medial prefrontal cortex and reduced open-arm exploration [14]. However, in the present study, 7-NI failed to produce anxiolysis in defeated mice, in line with studies in which similar results were obtained with acute (6 h) immobilization-induced restraint stress, a protocol that also increases nitrite levels [17,18]. 7-NI was not found to induce an anxiolytic effect in the EPM in restrained mice [17,18,19,20] and failed to block the hippocampal NOS activation induced by exposure to a protocol of stress–restress [21]. Authors of these papers proposed that overproduction of NO following stress is likely to involve iNOS and not nNOS, as they observed that the iNOS inhibitor aminoguanidine was effective in reducing anxiety in stressed animals [17,18,19,20,21]. In addition, it has been suggested that the behavioural effects of NOS inhibitors can vary in stressed animals. For example, 7-NI produces an anxiolytic effect in unstressed mice, but not in stressed mice [15,17,18,19,20], which can present increased anxiety after 7-NI administration [22].
As we have previously observed, ISD led to avoidance of social exploration in our mice [5,7]. We expected 7-nitroindazole to reverse this effect of ISD, as nNOS has been implicated in the pathophysiology of depressive disorders [13,23,24] and overexpression of nNOS in NAcc-induced social avoidance [13]. Moreover, administration of the nNOS-specific inhibitor L-VNIO in the NAcc prevented the deficit of social interaction induced by CSDS [13]. Although the effects of 7-NI on social avoidance induced by ISD have not been studied previously, this compound has been shown to reverse the reduction in social interaction observed in Shank3 mutant mice, a mouse model of autism, when administered at 80 mg/kg 7 days before SIT [25,26]. Conversely to our hypothesis, we have observed in the present study that administration of 7-NI before each episode of defeat does not prevent the social avoidance induced by ISD. Differences in the protocol used to induce stress (ISD vs. CSDS) and in the nNOS inhibitor employed (7-NI vs. L-VNIO) could explain the divergence between previous results [13] and the present data.
ISD reduced the time of immobility in the TST, in accordance with previous studies performed in our laboratory [5,6], and 7-NI did not reverse this effect. In line with the conventional interpretation of immobility in the TST as behavioural despair, several studies have indicated a role of nNOS in depression-like behaviours. nNOS overexpression in the NAcc of mice is enough to increase immobility in the TST and reduce sucrose preference [13]. In addition, the increase in immobility induced by chronic mild stress (CMS) in the TST and forced swim test (FST) was shown to be reversed by the systemic administration of 7-NI [23], and the same effects were observed after intra-NAcc administration of L-VNIO to mice exposed to CSDS [13]. Conversely, 7-NI (25 mg/kg, 30 min before TST) did not reverse the increase in the time spent immobile in the TST following chronic isolation stress in adolescent mice, although it did reduce levels of nitrites in their hippocampus [27]. The reduction in the time spent immobile in the TST by mice exposed to ISD can be interpreted as an enhanced reactivity of defeated mice to the situation of moderate inescapable stress that the TST represents, rather than a reduction of depressive-like behaviour (see a more detailed discussion in [5]). In support of this idea, we have observed in the present study that the time spent immobile correlated positively with the ISI, which was clearly decreased in defeated mice. Our results show that 7-NI did not modify the enhanced reactivity of defeated mice to the stressful situation of TST, though this compound did reverse the increase in the latency to become immobile induced by ISD.
In the present study we aimed to evaluate the effects of ISD on the cognitive performance of mice with the ORT and to explore the potential role of 7-NI in such effects, as previous studies have reported controversial results concerning the consequences of defeat on recognition memory. CSDS was found to reduce the discrimination index in the ORT in some studies [28,29], but not in others [30]. In a previous study, we also failed to observe effects of four episodes of social defeat (on alternate days) in the ORT [31]. In accordance with this, ISD, alone or with 7-NI, did not induce changes in recognition memory in the present study. An important factor in the effects of social defeat in the ORT may be the time elapsed between defeat exposure and testing, as CSDS has been shown to reduce the discrimination index at 7 days (but not at 1 or 21 days) after the last episode of defeat [32]. Future studies should take into account this variable to evaluate the potential role of nNOS in memory recognition, particularly in light of a recent report that 7-NI (80 mg/kg, 4–5 days before ORT) reversed the cognitive deficits observed in a mouse model of autism [26].
The most relevant result of the present paper is that blockade of nNOS with 7-NI during episodes of social defeat in late adolescence reversed the effects of ISD on the sensitivity of adult mice to cocaine reward. In agreement with previous studies of our laboratory the dose of 1 mg/kg of cocaine is ineffective to induce CPP in non-stressed mice [4,5,6,7]. Conversely, mice treated with vehicle before each episode of ISD acquired cocaine CPP after conditioning with this subthreshold dose (as reflected by the significant increase in the time spent in cocaine-paired compartment in post-C with respect to pre-C, Figure 4a). The fact that mice treated with 7-NI before defeat episodes did not acquire CPP (the same observed in control mice without stress exposure) indicates that both doses of this compound prevented the effects of ISD, although it is important to note that there are not significant differences between the CPP score of mice exposed to ISD treated with vehicle or 7-NI (Figure 4b). Thus, it seems that inhibition of nNOS with 7-NI reduces, but does not completely block, the effects of ISD on cocaine reward. There are no previous studies on the involvement of the NO pathway in the effects of social defeat on the rewarding properties of cocaine; however, in line with the results observed here, we have previously observed that 7-NI reversed the effects of a similar protocol of ISD on MDMA-induced CPP [12]. In addition, blockade of the NMDA receptors, which are closely related with nNOS inhibition, also reversed the effects of ISD on cocaine reward [11]. nNOS is a downstream signal molecule of NMDA receptor activation. Stimulation of NMDA receptors induces a calcium influx and, in the presence of calmodulin, activates nNOS and the production of NO. In addition, post-synaptically produced NO acts as a retrograde neurotransmitter on pre-synaptic neurons and causes glutamate release, which in turn activates post-synaptic NMDA receptors to increase calcium and further activate nNOS and NO production. This close relationship between NMDA receptors and nNOS can explain why memantine (that blocks NMDA receptors) acts in the same way as 7-NI (which inhibits nNOS), in this case, preventing the effects of social defeat on cocaine-induced CPP. Irrespective of stress exposure, there is experimental evidence that supports a role for nNOS in the addiction-like effects of cocaine in rats. Abstinence from cocaine self-administration increases nNOS gene and protein expression in the NAcc, but not in the PFC [33], while re-exposure to cocaine-conditioned stimuli following self-administration and extinction increases extracellular glutamate, leading to release of NO in the NAcc core [34]. Moreover, elimination of nNOS-expressing interneurons in the NAcc has been shown to reduce cue-induced cocaine seeking, while stimulation of these interneurons was reported to provoke drug-seeking in the absence of cues [35]. Administration of 7-NI attenuates the symptoms of abstinence induced by cocaine withdrawal and protects against cocaine-induced oxidative stress [36], attenuates CPP and the increase of opioid mu receptors in the NAcc induced by cocaine [37], and reduces the expression of behavioural sensitization and hippocampal synaptic plasticity induced by cocaine [38]. Similarly, the selective nNOS inhibitor L-NPA prevents cocaine CPP [39], undermines the reinstatement of cocaine self-administration induced by cocaine priming or drug-associated cues [33] and, when administered intra-PFC, prevents cocaine-induced presynaptic plasticity in cholinergic neurons of the laterodorsal tegmentum [39]. As a whole, these studies support a role for nNOS in the effects of cocaine in basal conditions and after stress exposure, mainly in the hippocampus and NAcc. Regarding the possible molecular mechanism of action of 7-NI underlying its ability to prevent the long-term effects of stress on cocaine reward, the most probable mechanism is the neuroprotective effect due to the inhibition of nNOS which reduced the neurotoxicity induced by overstimulation of NMDA receptors and nNOS-mediated production of NO (which in turn produces the free radicals responsible for oxidative injury) [36]. In addition, nNOS activation in the hippocampus altered the function of glucocorticoid receptors, an effect that can be reversed by 7-NI [40]. The increase in corticosterone levels induced by stress can activate nNOS through the mineral–corticoid receptors, resulting in the downregulation of hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors and the increase in hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing hormone [41]. On the other hand, 7-NI has antioxidant properties that are independent of nNOS [42].
In the present study, the potent effect of 7-NI in reversing the long-term potentiation of cocaine reward induced by ISD contrasts with the null influence of this compound on the behaviours of defeated mice in the short term after the last episode of ISD (anxiety-like symptoms in the EPM, social avoidance in the SIT and stress hyperreactivity in the TST). As mentioned above, some studies have suggested that the increase in anxiety induced by different protocols of stress is related with the inhibition of iNOS rather than the inhibition of nNOS [17,18,19,20,21,27]. Another possible explanation is that the anxiety- and depression-like behaviours induced by ISD were evaluated 24–48 h after the last episode of defeat and thus at the same time point after 7-NI administration. It is relevant to note that the effects of nNOS inhibitors on depression-like behaviour induced by stress depends on several factors, including the type of stressor, the protocol of drug administration and the moment at which the behaviour is evaluated. For example, treatment for 7 days with a daily injection of 7-NI (30 mg/kg) reduced the time spent immobile in the TST and FST of mice exposed to CMS 2 and 4 weeks (but not 24 h) after drug administration [23]. Future studies with longer intervals between 7-NI administration/ISD and behavioural evaluation should be performed to determine if anxiety-like symptoms, social avoidance and stress hyperreactivity induced by ISD depend on the integrity of nNOS. It would also be of interest to evaluate the role played by a selective iNOS inhibitor in the short- and long-term effects of ISD.
In conclusion, our results support a specific role for nNOS signalling in the effects of ISD stress on cocaine reward and point to the inhibition of NOS as a therapeutic option to enhance resilience to the effects of social stress on vulnerability to develop cocaine-use disorders.

4. Materials and Methods

4.1. Subjects

An amount of 44 C57BL/6 male mice and 17 OF1 male mice (Charles River, France) arrived in the laboratory at 21 days and 42 days of age, respectively. Experimental mice (C57BL/6) were grouped (n = 4–5) in plastic cages (25 × 25 × 14.5 cm) while aggressive opponents (OF1) were housed individually in plastic cages (23 × 32 × 20 cm) to induce heightened aggression [43]. Mice were maintained under standard laboratory conditions with food and water ad libitum and a reversed light schedule (lights on 19:30–07:30). All experimental protocols were initiated 26 days after the arrival of the mice at the laboratory. Procedures involving mice and their care were approved by the Ethics Committee of Experimental Research of the University of Valencia (2023-VSC-PEA-0288) and were performed according to Directive 2010/63/EU.

4.2. Drugs

Ninety minutes before each episode of defeat, experimental mice were injected with 7.25 or 12.5 mg/kg of 7-NI (Merck, Darmstadt, Germany) dissolved in physiological saline (NaCI 0.9%) with DMSO (dimethyl sulfoxide, 2/3 and 1/3 of the total volume, respectively). Control groups non exposed to stress (control) and exposed to stress (vehicle + ISD) were treated with the vehicle used to dissolve 7-NI (2/3 of physiological saline and 1/3 of DMSO). To induce CPP, experimental mice were injected with 1 mg/kg of cocaine (Alcaliber Laboratory, Madrid, Spain) dissolved in physiological saline. All compounds were injected intraperitoneally in a volume of 0.01 mL/g of weight. The doses of cocaine and 7-NI was selected on the basis of previous studies carried out in our laboratory ([5,12], respectively).

4.3. Experimental Protocols

4.3.1. Intermittent Social Defeat (ISD)

Each experimental mouse underwent an agonistic encounter of 25 min (on PND 47, 50, 53 and 56) with an OF1 isolated mouse, which defeated the experimental animal. Each encounter consisted of three phases. In the first phase (10 min) an experimental mouse (intruder) was introduced into the home cage of an aggressive opponent (resident); the two animals were separated by a wire mesh wall, which protected the intruder from attack but allowed social interaction and threats from the resident. In the second phase (5 min) the wire mesh was removed allowing confrontation between the two mice. In the third phase (10 min), the wire mesh was again introduced to separate the two animals but allow for social threats by the resident. All experimental mice displayed defeat (a specific posture characterized by an upright submissive position, limp forepaws, upwardly angled head, and retracted ears [44,45]), given that they all faced resident mice with high levels of aggression. Intruder mice were exposed to a different aggressor mouse during each episode of social defeat. The first and fourth episodes were videotaped and later an observer blind to the treatment [46] recorded, with a computerized system (Raton Time 1.0 software; Fixma SL, Valencia, Spain), the time spent in and the latencies of different behavioural categories in the experimental mice (avoidance/flee and defence/submission) and in the resident mice (threat and attack). The control (non-stressed) group underwent the same protocol in an empty cage (without the presence of a resident mouse).

4.3.2. Elevated Plus Maze (EPM)

Experimental mice underwent the EPM on PND 57 in order to evaluate the anxiogenic effects of ISD. The apparatus, made of plexiglass, consisted of two open arms and two enclosed arms (30 × 5 cm each one) with a central platform (5 × 5 cm) and was elevated 45 cm above floor level. Mice were placed on the central platform facing an open arm and were allowed to explore the maze for 5 min. The behaviour of the mice was video recorded and later analysed by an observer blind to the treatment, using a computerized method (Raton Time 1.0 software; Fixma SL, Valencia, Spain). The EPM test is based on the natural aversion of mice to open areas and the spontaneous exploratory behaviour they exhibit in novel environments. Anxiety levels are considered to be lower when the measurements in the open arms (time spent in, number of entries and percentages of time and entries) are higher than in the closed arms [47]. Besides these measurements, latency to enter in each arm, the time spent in and number of entries in the central platform and the total number of entries into the arms (a locomotor activity score [48]) were recorded. An arm was considered to have been visited when the animal placed all four paws on it. Measures of open arms (latency (LOA), time spent in (TOA), number of entries (EOA), percentage of time [(open/open + closed) × 100] spent in (%TOA), percentage of entries (%EOA), and total entries (TotalE) into the arms) were taken into account for the statistical analyses.

4.3.3. Social Interaction Test

The effects of ISD on the social behaviour of mice was evaluated on PND 57 in the social interaction test. The apparatus used was an open field (37 × 37 × 30 cm) which contained a perforated plexiglass cage (10 × 6.5 × 30 cm) placed in the middle of one wall of the open field. The test has two phases in which the mouse is placed in the centre of the open field and allowed to explore it twice for 10 min, under two different experimental conditions: with the perforated plexiglass cage empty (object phase) or containing an unfamiliar OF1 mouse (social phase). The perforated cage safeguards the experimental mouse from attack in both phases, the time spent in the 8 cm area surrounding the perforated cage—the interaction zone—was registered and automatically sent to a computer using the Ethovision 2.0 software package (Noldus, Wageningen, The Netherlands). Between both phases, the experimental mouse was returned to its home cage for 2 min. An index of social interaction (ISI) was obtained [time spent in the interaction zone during the social phase/(time spent in the interaction zone during the social phase + time spent in the interaction zone during the object phase)] [49]. The ISI is commonly used as an index of social preference or avoidance [50].

4.3.4. Object Recognition Test (ORT)

To evaluate the effects of ISD on relational memory we used the ORT [48,51]. This test consists of three phases. In the first phase (habituation), mice are placed in the centre of a box and allowed to explore it for two minutes (this phase was performed on PND 57). Twenty-four hours later, in the second phase (training session, T1) mice are once again placed (for five minutes) in the box, with two stones placed in opposite corners. Afterwards, mice are returned to their home cage for one minute (memory retention interval). In the last phase (test session, T2) one of the stones is replaced with a small non-toxic plastic toy. Subsequently, the mice are placed once again in the box for another five minutes. The following measures are recorded using a computerized system (Raton Time 1.0 software; Fixma SL, Valencia, Spain): total time spent exploring the two stones in T1; and time spent exploring the stone and the toy in T2. These measures are used to calculate the discrimination index [DI = (Tnovel − Tfamiliar)/(Tnovel + Tfamiliar) × 100%], as previously described [31].

4.3.5. Tail Suspension Test (TST)

On PND 58, we investigated whether ISD modified the behavioural variable of immobility in the TST, which is considered to represent despair [52]. This test is based on the observation that when mice are placed in an inescapable and uncontrollable stressful situation, such as being hung by their tail, after initial escape attemps, they develop an immobile posture [53]. The TST has been used as a measure of behavioural depression because animals treated with antidepressant drugs engage in escape-directed behaviours for more time [52]. Mice were suspended by the tail (using adhesive tape) from a hook for 6 min [54] and their behaviour was video recorded and later analyzed by an observer blind to the treatment, using a computerized method (Raton Time 1.0 software; Fixma SL, Valencia, Spain) to measure the total time spent immobile (Immobility) and the latency to become immobile (LImmobility).

4.3.6. Conditioned Place Preference (CPP)

The long-term effects of ISD on cocaine reward were evaluated three weeks after the last episode of social defeat (on PND 77) using the CPP procedure. Eight identical plexiglass boxes with two equal-sized compartments (30.7 cm long × 31.5 cm wide × 34.5 cm high) with different coloured walls (black vs. white) and distinct floor textures (fine vs. wide grid), separated by a gray central area (13.8 cm long × 31.5 cm wide × 34.5 cm high) were used. The boxes, equipped with infrared light beams to record the position of the animal, were controlled by three IBM PC computers using MONPRE 2Z 1.0 software (Cibertec SA, Madrid, Spain). Mice underwent an unbiased CPP procedure with three phases (see details in [55]). In the first phase (pre-conditioning, pre-C), the time spent by the animal in each compartment was recorded for 15 min over 3 consecutive days. Animals showing a strong unconditioned aversion or a preference for a given compartment (less than 33% or more than 66% of the total session time, respectively) were excluded from the study (n = 3). In the second phase (conditioning, 4 days) mice received saline and were confined to the vehicle-paired compartment for 30 min, and, after an interval of 4 h, were injected with 1 mg/kg of cocaine and were immediately confined to the drug-paired compartment for 30 min. During the third phase (post-conditioning, post-C), the time spent by the untreated mice in each compartment during a 15 min period was again recorded.

4.4. Experimental Design

According to the treatment received, four groups of mice were used (Figure 5): a control group (vehicle + no stress), a stressed group (vehicle + SD), and two stressed groups treated with 7-NI (7.25 or 12.5 mg/kg) before each episode of SD (7-NI7 + SD and 7-NI12 + SD). Shortly after the last episode of SD (24/48 h) the mice were tested in the EPM, SIT, ORT and TST. Behavioural tests took place in a dimly illuminated experimental room during the dark period (08.30–16.30) and were transported to this room 1 h prior to testing in order to facilitate adaptation. The order of the behavioural tests was based on a previous study, according to the degree of stress that the same tests had induced in the mice; in this way, we aimed to prevent previous experience in a test from affecting the mouse’s performance in subsequent tests [5]. As the open arms measurements are very sensitive to environmental conditions and prior manipulation of the animal, we decided to perform the EPM first. Following the same logic, the TST was performed last because it is the most stressful test. Subsequently, after an interval of 3 weeks, all mice were conditioned in the CPP paradigm with a subthreshold dose of cocaine (1 mg/kg).

4.5. Statistical Analyses

Sample distributions were assessed for normality (Kolmogorov–Smirnov test) and homogeneity (Levene’s test). In the case of unequal variance, we used the Welch ANOVA as alternative to the classic ANOVA. Post-hoc comparisons after ANOVA were performed with Bonferroni.
Regarding behaviour during the episodes of defeat, differences between groups and between the first and fourth episodes were evaluated with mixed ANOVAs, using the treatment (with three levels, vehicle + SD, 7-NI7 + SD and 7-NI12 + SD) and days variables (first and fourth episode of defeat).
The data obtained in the tests performed shortly after ISD and the CPP scores (time spent in post-C minus time spent in pre-C) were analysed by one-way ANOVA with a between variable—treatment—with four levels (vehicle + no stress, vehicle + SD, 7-NI7 + SD and 7-NI12 + SD). The following measurements were included in the statistical analyses: TOA, EOA, LOA, %TOA, %EOA and TotalE in the EPM, ISI, Immobility and LImmobility in TST, recognition Index in the ORT and CPP score. In addition, the data regarding the time spent in the drug-paired compartment were analysed with a two-way ANOVA with a between variable—treatment—with four levels (vehicle + no stress, vehicle + SD, 7-NI7 + SD and 7-NI12 + SD) and a within variable—days—with two levels (pre-C and post-C).
Pearson correlation tests were used to determine relationships among the different behaviours during the defeat episodes and the performance of stressed mice in each behavioural test. In the case of significant correlations, we also performed lineal regression analysis. All statistical analyses were performed with the SPSS program, version 29.0.2.0.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ph17091203/s1, Figure S1: Role of 7-nitroindazole in the effects of intermittent social defeat (SD) on the behaviour of mice in the object recognition test.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.A.A. and M.P.G.-P.; methodology, software and validation, M.A.A., M.C.A. and C.M.; formal analysis, investigation and visualization, C.C.-L. and M.Á.M.-C.; resources, M.A.A.; data curation and writing—original draft preparation, M.P.G.-P. and M.Á.M.-C.; writing—review and editing, M.A.A. and M.C.A.; supervision, C.M. and M.A.A.; project administration and funding acquisition, M.A.A. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación, Spain, MCIN/AEI/10.13039/501100011033, grant I+D+i PID2020-118945RB-I00.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The animal study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Valencia (protocol code: 2023-VSC-PEA-0288, date of approval: November 2023).

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors on request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Ahmed, S.H.; Badiani, A.; Miczek, K.A.; Müller, C.P. Non-pharmacological factors that determine drug use and addiction. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2020, 110, 3–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Koob, G.F.; Schulkin, J. Addiction and stress: An allostatic view. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2019, 106, 245–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Ruisoto, P.; Contador, I. The role of stress in drug addiction. An integrative review. Physiol. Behav. 2019, 202, 62–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. García-Pardo, M.P.; Calpe-López, C.; Miñarro, J.; Aguilar, M.A. Role of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors in the long-term effects of repeated social defeat stress on the rewarding and psychomotor properties of cocaine in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 2019, 361, 95–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Calpe-López, C.; García-Pardo, M.P.; Martínez-Caballero, M.A.; Santos-Ortíz, A.; Aguilar, M.A. Behavioral traits associated with resilience to the effects of repeated social defeat on cocaine-induced conditioned place preference in mice. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2020, 13, 278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Calpe-López, C.; Martínez-Caballero, M.A.; García-Pardo, M.P.; Aguilar, M.A. Intermittent voluntary wheel running promotes resilience to the negative consequences of repeated social defeat in mice. Physiol. Behav. 2022, 254, 113916. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Calpe-López, C.; Martínez-Caballero, M.A.; García-Pardo, M.P.; Aguilar, M.A. Brief maternal separation inoculates against the effects of social stress on depression-like behavior and cocaine reward in mice. Front. Pharmacol. 2022, 13, 825522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Milivojevic, V.; Sinha, R. Central and peripheral biomarkers of stress response for addiction risk and relapse vulnerability. Trends Mol. Med. 2018, 24, 173–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Montagud-Romero, S.; Nuñez, C.; Blanco-Gandia, M.C.; Martínez-Laorden, E.; Aguilar, M.A.; Navarro-Zaragoza, J.; Almela, P.; Milanés, M.V.; Laorden, M.L.; Miñarro, J.; et al. Repeated social defeat and the rewarding effects of cocaine in adult and adolescent mice: Dopamine transcription factors, proBDNF signaling pathways, and the TrkB receptor in the mesolimbic system. Psychopharmacology 2017, 234, 2063–2075. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Ferrer-Pérez, C.; Reguilón, M.D.; Manzanedo, C.; Aguilar, M.A.; Miñarro, J.; Rodríguez-Arias, M. Antagonism of corticotropin-releasing factor CRF(1) receptors blocks the enhanced response to cocaine after social stress. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2018, 823, 87–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. García-Pardo, M.P.; Miñarro, J.; Llansola, M.; Felipo, V.; Aguilar, M.A. Role of NMDA and AMPA glutamatergic receptors in the effects of social defeat on the rewarding properties of MDMA in mice. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2019, 50, 2623–2634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. García-Pardo, M.P.; LLansola, M.; Felipo, V.; De la Rubia Ortí, J.E.; Aguilar, M.A. Blockade of nitric oxide signalling promotes resilience to the effects of social defeat stress on the conditioned rewarding properties of MDMA in mice. Nitric Oxide 2020, 98, 29–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Yin, C.Y.; Huang, S.Y.; Gao, L.; Lin, Y.H.; Chang, L.; Wu, H.Y.; Zhu, D.Y.; Luo, C.X. Neuronal nitric oxide synthase in nucleus accumbens specifically mediates susceptibility to social defeat stress through cyclin-dependent kinase 5. J. Neurosci. 2021, 41, 2523–2539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Faria, M.P.; Laverde, C.F.; Nunes-de-Souza, R.L. Anxiogenesis induced by social defeat in male mice: Role of nitric oxide, NMDA, and CRF1 receptors in the medial prefrontal cortex and BNST. Neuropharmacology 2020, 166, 107973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Volke, V.; Soosaar, A.; Kõks, S.; Bourin, M.; Männistö, P.T.; Vasar, E. 7-Nitroindazole, a nitric oxide synthase inhibitor, has anxiolytic-like properties in exploratory models of anxiety. Psychopharmacology 1997, 131, 399–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Santos-Rocha, J.B.; Rae, M.; Teixeira, A.M.A.; Teixeira, S.A.; Munhoz, C.D.; Muscará, M.N.; Marcourakis, T.; Szumlinski, K.K.; Camarini, R. Involvement of neuronal nitric oxide synthase in cross-sensitization between chronic unpredictable stress and ethanol in adolescent and adult mice. Alcohol 2018, 68, 71–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Gilhotra, N.; Dhingra, D. GABAergic and nitriergic modulation by curcumin for its antianxiety-like activity in mice. Brain Res. 2010, 1352, 167–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Gilhotra, N.; Dhingra, D. Possible involvement of GABAergic and nitriergic systems for antianxiety-like activity of piperine in unstressed and stressed mice. Pharmacol. Rep. 2014, 66, 885–891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Gilhotra, N.; Jain, H.; Dhingra, D. Differential effects of nitric oxide synthase inhibitors on anxiety in unstressed and stressed mice. Indian J. Exp. Biol. 2010, 48, 365–372. [Google Scholar]
  20. Dhingra, D.; Chhillar, R.; Gupta, A. Antianxiety-like activity of gallic acid in unstressed and stressed mice: Possible involvement of nitriergic system. Neurochem. Res. 2012, 37, 487–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Harvey, B.H.; Oosthuizen, F.; Brand, L.; Wegener, G.; Stein, D.J. Stress-restress evokes sustained iNOS activity and altered GABA levels and NMDA receptors in rat hippocampus. Psychopharmacology 2004, 175, 494–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Pokk, P.; Väli, M. The effects of the nitric oxide synthase inhibitors on the behaviour of small-platform-stressed mice in the plus-maze test. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2002, 26, 241–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Zhou, Q.G.; Hu, Y.; Hua, Y.; Hu, M.; Luo, C.X.; Han, X.; Zhu, X.J.; Wang, B.; Xu, J.S.; Zhu, D.Y. Neuronal nitric oxide synthase contributes to chronic stress-induced depression by suppressing hippocampal neurogenesis. J. Neurochem. 2007, 103, 1843–1854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Zhou, Q.G.; Zhu, X.H.; Nemes, A.D.; Zhu, D.Y. Neuronal nitric oxide synthase and affective disorders. IBRO Rep. 2018, 5, 116–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Abdel-Haq, M.; Ojha, S.K.; Hamoudi, W.; Kumar, A.; Kumar Tripathi, M.; Khaliulin, I.; Domb, A.J.; Amal, H. Effects of extended-release 7-nitroindazole gel formulation treatment on the behavior of Shank3 mouse model of autism. Nitric Oxide 2023, 140–141, 41–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Tripathi, M.K.; Ojha, S.K.; Kartawy, M.; Hamoudi, W.; Choudhary, A.; Stern, S.; Aran, A.; Amal, H. The NO Answer for Autism Spectrum Disorder. Adv. Sci. 2023, 10, e2205783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Amiri, S.; Haj-Mirzaian, A.; Rahimi-Balaei, M.; Razmi, A.; Kordjazy, N.; Shirzadian, A.; Mehr, S.E.; Sianati, H.; Dehpour, A.R. Co-occurrence of anxiety and depressive-like behaviors following adolescent social isolation in male mice; possible role of nitrergic system. Physiol. Behav. 2015, 145, 38–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. da Costa, V.F.; Caipa Ramírez, J.C.; Viatela Ramírez, S.; Avalo-Zuluaga, J.H.; Baptista-de-Souza, D.; Canto-de-Souza, L.; Planeta, C.S.; Rico Rodríguez, J.L.; Nunes-de-Souza, R.L. Emotional- and cognitive-like responses induced by social defeat stress in male mice are modulated by the BNST, amygdala, and hippocampus. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2023, 17, 1168640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Jin, H.M.; Muna, S.; Bagalkot, T.R.; Cui, Y.; Yadav, B.K.; Chung, Y.C. The effects of social defeat on behavior and dopaminergic markers in mice. Neuroscience 2015, 288, 167–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Dang, R.; Wang, M.; Li, X.; Wang, H.; Liu, L.; Wu, Q.; Zhao, J.; Ji, P.; Zhong, L.; Licinio, J.; et al. Edaravone ameliorates depressive and anxiety-like behaviors via Sirt1/Nrf2/HO-1/Gpx4 pathway. J. Neuroinflammation 2022, 19, 41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. García-Pardo, M.P.; Roger-Sánchez, C.; Rodríguez-Arias, M.; Miñarro, J.; Aguilar, M.A. Cognitive and behavioural effects induced by social stress plus MDMA administration in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 2017, 319, 63–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Wendelmuth, M.; Willam, M.; Todorov, H.; Radyushkin, K.; Gerber, S.; Schweiger, S. Dynamic longitudinal behavior in animals exposed to chronic social defeat stress. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0235268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Frankowska, M.; Smaga, I.; Gawlińska, K.; Pieniążek, R.; Filip, M. Further proof on the role of accumbal nNOS in cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. Pharmacol. Rep. 2024, 76, 338–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Siemsen, B.M.; McFaddin, J.A.; Haigh, K.; Brock, A.G.; Leath, M.N.; Hooker, K.N.; McGonegal, L.K.; Scofield, M.D. Amperometric measurements of cocaine cue and novel context-evoked glutamate and nitric oxide release in the nucleus accumbens core. J. Neurochem. 2020, 153, 599–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Smith, A.C.W.; Scofield, M.D.; Heinsbroek, J.A.; Gipson, C.D.; Neuhofer, D.; Roberts-Wolfe, D.J.; Spencer, S.; Garcia-Keller, C.; Stankeviciute, N.M.; Smith, R.J.; et al. Accumbens nNOS interneurons regulate cocaine relapse. J. Neurosci. 2017, 37, 742–756. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Vitcheva, V.; Simeonova, R.; Kondeva-Burdina, M.; Mitcheva, M. Selective nitric oxide synthase inhibitor 7-Nitroindazole protects against cocaine-induced oxidative stress in rat brain. Oxid. Med. Cell Longev. 2015, 2015, 157876. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Thériault, R.K.; Leri, F.; Kalisch, B. The role of neuronal nitric oxide synthase in cocaine place preference and mu opioid receptor expression in the nucleus accumbens. Psychopharmacology 2018, 235, 2675–2685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Artur de la Villarmois, E.; Gabach, L.A.; Bianconi, S.; Poretti, M.B.; Occhieppo, V.; Schiöth, H.B.; Carlini, V.P.; Pérez, M.F. Pharmacological NOS-1 inhibition within the hippocampus prevented expression of cocaine sensitization: Correlation with reduced synaptic transmission. Mol. Neurobiol. 2020, 57, 450–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Kamii, H.; Taoka, N.; Minami, M.; Kaneda, K. Nitric oxide in the medial prefrontal cortex contributes to the acquisition of cocaine place preference and synaptic plasticity in the laterodorsal tegmental nucleus. Neurosci. Lett. 2017, 660, 39–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Zhou, Q.G.; Zhu, L.J.; Chen, C.; Wu, H.Y.; Luo, C.X.; Chang, L.; Zhu, D.Y. Hippocampal neuronal nitric oxide synthase mediates the stress-related depressive behaviors of glucocorticoids by downregulating glucocorticoid receptor. J. Neurosci. 2011, 31, 7579–7590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Chen, H.-J.C.; Spiers, J.G.; Sernia, C.; Lavidis, N.A. Response of the nitrergic system to activation of the neuroendocrine stress axis. Front. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Di Monte, D.A.; Royland, J.E.; Anderson, A.; Castagnoli, K.; Castagnoli, N.; Langston, J.W. Inhibition of monoamine oxidase contributes to the protective effect of 7-nitroindazole against MPTP neurotoxicity. J. Neurochem. 1997, 69, 1771–1773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Rodríguez-Arias, M.; Miñarro, J.; Aguilar, M.A.; Pinazo, J.; Simón, V.M. Effects of risperidone and SCH 23390 on isolation-induced aggression in male mice. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 1998, 8, 95–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Miczek, K.A.; Thompson, M.L.; Shuster, L. Opioid-like analgesia in defeated mice. Science 1982, 215, 1520–1522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Ribeiro Do Couto, B.; Aguilar, M.A.; Manzanedo, C.; Rodríguez-Arias, M.; Armario, A.; Miñarro, J. Social stress is as effective as physical stress in reinstating morphine-induced place preference in mice. Psychopharmacology 2006, 185, 459–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Brain, P.F.; McAllister, K.H.; Walmsley, S. Drug effects on social behaviors. In Psychopharmacology; Boulton, A.A., Bake, G.B., Greenshaw, A.J., Eds.; The Humana Press Inc: Clifton, NJ, USA, 1989; pp. 687–739. [Google Scholar]
  47. Rodgers, R.J.; Dalvi, A. Anxiety, defence and the elevated plus-maze. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 1997, 21, 801–810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Campos, A.C.; Fogaca, M.V.; Aguiar, D.C.; Guimarães, F.S. Animal models of anxiety disorders and stress. Braz. J. Psychiatry 2013, 35, S101–S111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Henriques-Alves, A.M.; Queiroz, C.M. Ethological evaluation of the effects of social defeat stress in mice: Beyond the social interaction ratio. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2016, 9, 364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Krishnan, V.; Han, M.H.; Graham, D.L.; Berton, O.; Renthal, W.; Russo, S.J.; LaPlant, Q.; Graham, A.; Lutter, M.; Lagace, D.C.; et al. Molecular adaptations underlying susceptibility and resistance to social defeat in brain reward regions. Cell 2007, 131, 391–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Winters, B.D.; Saksida, L.M.; Bussey, T.J. Object recognition memory: Neurobiological mechanisms of encoding, consolidation and retrieval. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2008, 32, 1055–1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Pollak, D.D.; Rey, C.E.; Monje, F.J. Rodent models in depression research: Classical strategies and new directions. Ann. Med. 2010, 42, 252–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Cryan, J.F.; Mombereau, C.; Vassout, A. The tail suspension test as a model for assessing antidepressant activity: Review of pharmacological and genetic studies in mice. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2005, 29, 571–625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Vaugeois, J.M.; Passera, G.; Zuccaro, F.; Costentin, J. Individual differences in response to imipramine in the mouse tail suspension test. Psychopharmacology 1997, 134, 387–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Maldonado, C.; Rodríguez-Arias, M.; Castillo, A.; Aguilar, M.A.; Miñarro, J. Effect of memantine and CNQX in the acquisition, expression and reinstatement of cocaine-induced conditioned place preference. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2007, 31, 932–939. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Role of 7-nitroindazole in the effects of intermittent social defeat (SD) on the behaviour of mice in the open arms (OA) of the elevated plus maze (EPM). Four groups of mice were used according to the treatment received: Control (vehicle + no stress), vehicle + SD (vehicle 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat), 7-NI7 + SD (7.25 mg/kg of 7-nitroindazole 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat) and 7-NI12 + SD (12.5 mg/kg of 7-nitroindazole 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat). The animals’ behaviours in the EPM were evaluated on PND 57 (24 h after the last episode of social defeat). Bars represent the mean (±SD) number of entries into the OA (a), percentage of entries into the OA (b), time spent in the OA (c) and percentage of time spent in the OA (d) for each group. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, significant difference with respect to the control group.
Figure 1. Role of 7-nitroindazole in the effects of intermittent social defeat (SD) on the behaviour of mice in the open arms (OA) of the elevated plus maze (EPM). Four groups of mice were used according to the treatment received: Control (vehicle + no stress), vehicle + SD (vehicle 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat), 7-NI7 + SD (7.25 mg/kg of 7-nitroindazole 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat) and 7-NI12 + SD (12.5 mg/kg of 7-nitroindazole 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat). The animals’ behaviours in the EPM were evaluated on PND 57 (24 h after the last episode of social defeat). Bars represent the mean (±SD) number of entries into the OA (a), percentage of entries into the OA (b), time spent in the OA (c) and percentage of time spent in the OA (d) for each group. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, significant difference with respect to the control group.
Pharmaceuticals 17 01203 g001
Figure 2. Role of 7-nitroindazole in the effects of intermittent social defeat (SD) on the behaviour of mice in the social interaction test. Four groups of mice were used according to the treatment received: Control (vehicle + no stress), vehicle + SD (vehicle 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat), 7-NI7 + SD (7.25 mg/kg of 7-nitroindazole 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat) and 7-NI12 + SD (12.5 mg/kg of 7-nitroindazole 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat). The animals’ behaviours were evaluated on PND 57 (24 h after the last episode of social defeat). Bars represent the mean (±SD) index of social interaction (ISI) in each group. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, significant difference with respect to the Control group.
Figure 2. Role of 7-nitroindazole in the effects of intermittent social defeat (SD) on the behaviour of mice in the social interaction test. Four groups of mice were used according to the treatment received: Control (vehicle + no stress), vehicle + SD (vehicle 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat), 7-NI7 + SD (7.25 mg/kg of 7-nitroindazole 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat) and 7-NI12 + SD (12.5 mg/kg of 7-nitroindazole 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat). The animals’ behaviours were evaluated on PND 57 (24 h after the last episode of social defeat). Bars represent the mean (±SD) index of social interaction (ISI) in each group. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, significant difference with respect to the Control group.
Pharmaceuticals 17 01203 g002
Figure 3. Role of 7-nitroindazole in the effects of intermittent social defeat (SD) on the behaviour of mice in the tail suspension test. Four groups of mice were used according to the treatment received: Control (vehicle + no stress), vehicle + SD (vehicle 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat), 7-NI7 + SD (7.25 mg/kg of 7-nitroindazole 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat) and 7-NI12 + SD (12.5 mg/kg of 7-nitroindazole 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat). The animals’ behaviours were evaluated on PND 58 (48 h after the last episode of social defeat). Bars represent the mean (±SD) time (in seconds) spent immobile (Time Immob.) (a) and the latency (in seconds) to show immobility (Latency Immob.) (b) in each group. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, significant difference with respect to the control group. ++ p < 0.01, significant difference with respect to the vehicle + SD group.
Figure 3. Role of 7-nitroindazole in the effects of intermittent social defeat (SD) on the behaviour of mice in the tail suspension test. Four groups of mice were used according to the treatment received: Control (vehicle + no stress), vehicle + SD (vehicle 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat), 7-NI7 + SD (7.25 mg/kg of 7-nitroindazole 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat) and 7-NI12 + SD (12.5 mg/kg of 7-nitroindazole 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat). The animals’ behaviours were evaluated on PND 58 (48 h after the last episode of social defeat). Bars represent the mean (±SD) time (in seconds) spent immobile (Time Immob.) (a) and the latency (in seconds) to show immobility (Latency Immob.) (b) in each group. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, significant difference with respect to the control group. ++ p < 0.01, significant difference with respect to the vehicle + SD group.
Pharmaceuticals 17 01203 g003
Figure 4. Role of 7-nitroindazole in the effects of intermittent social defeat (SD) on the behaviour of mice in the conditioned place preference (CPP) paradigm. Four groups of mice were used according to the treatment received: Control (vehicle + no stress), vehicle + SD (vehicle 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat), 7-NI7 + SD (7.25 mg/kg of 7-nitroindazole 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat) and 7-NI12 + SD (12.5 mg/kg of 7-nitroindazole 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat). The CPP procedure was initiated on PND 77 (3 weeks after the last episode of social defeat). (a) Acquisition of CPP. Bars represent the mean (±SD) time (in seconds) spent in drug-paired compartment before conditioning (pre-C, white bars) and after conditioning (post-C, black bars) with 1 mg/kg of cocaine. *** p < 0.001, significant difference in the time spent in pre-C vs. post-C. (b) CPP score. Bars represent the mean (±SD) CPP score (time spent in post-C minus time spent in pre-C in the drug-paired compartment). ** p < 0.01, significant difference with respect to the control group.
Figure 4. Role of 7-nitroindazole in the effects of intermittent social defeat (SD) on the behaviour of mice in the conditioned place preference (CPP) paradigm. Four groups of mice were used according to the treatment received: Control (vehicle + no stress), vehicle + SD (vehicle 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat), 7-NI7 + SD (7.25 mg/kg of 7-nitroindazole 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat) and 7-NI12 + SD (12.5 mg/kg of 7-nitroindazole 90 min before exposure to each episode of social defeat). The CPP procedure was initiated on PND 77 (3 weeks after the last episode of social defeat). (a) Acquisition of CPP. Bars represent the mean (±SD) time (in seconds) spent in drug-paired compartment before conditioning (pre-C, white bars) and after conditioning (post-C, black bars) with 1 mg/kg of cocaine. *** p < 0.001, significant difference in the time spent in pre-C vs. post-C. (b) CPP score. Bars represent the mean (±SD) CPP score (time spent in post-C minus time spent in pre-C in the drug-paired compartment). ** p < 0.01, significant difference with respect to the control group.
Pharmaceuticals 17 01203 g004
Figure 5. Timetable and experimental design. PND: post-natal day; 7-NI: 7-nitroindazole; EPM: elevated plus maze; SIT: social interaction test; ORT: object recognition test; TST: tail suspension test.
Figure 5. Timetable and experimental design. PND: post-natal day; 7-NI: 7-nitroindazole; EPM: elevated plus maze; SIT: social interaction test; ORT: object recognition test; TST: tail suspension test.
Pharmaceuticals 17 01203 g005
Table 1. Behavioural categories evaluated during episodes of social defeat.
Table 1. Behavioural categories evaluated during episodes of social defeat.
Experimental
Animals
Avoidance/
Flee
Latency Avoid/FleeDefence/
Submission
Latency
Defence/Subm.
Encounter14141414
Defeated mice treated with VEH117.39168.72[26.525.15]15.6420.8640.7173.2
(n = 10)±31.24±22.61±13.71±1.53±3.23±5.38±17.17±34.82
Defeated mice treated with 7-NI7142.87155.494.644.329.515.3974.6737.86
(n = 12)±20.28±21.24±2.07±1.31±2.43±3.89±27.12±12.81
Defeated mice treated with 7-NI1296.5125.21[2.692.67] *14.2717.5465.8431.75
(n = 12)±17.73±24.63±0.55±0.91±4.32±3.38±26.53±12.03
Aggressive
Opponents
ThreatLatency
Threat
AttackLatency
Attack
Encounter14 #141414
Confronted with VEH-treated mice[15.8910.32]23.4843.5640.9636.4722.286.98
(n = 10)±2.77±2.17±13.26±19.25±5.18±7.7±12.79±2.06
Confronted with 7-NI7 treated mice14.018.3215.2625.3935.129.093.783.73
(n = 12)±3.33±1.84±7.1±9.93±3.69±2.9±0.98±1.12
Confronted with 7-NI12 treated mice[6.846.4] *22.4641.3231.1826.042.674.6
(n = 12)±1.53±1.59±8.35±15.65±4.35±5.22±0.55±2.27
Mean time (±SEM) spent in different behavioural categories by experimental animals (avoidance/flee, defence/submission and latency to initiate these behaviours) treated with vehicle (VEH) or 7-nitroindazole at the doses of 7.5 (7-NI7) or 12.5 (7-NI12) mg/kg and by aggressive opponents confronted by them (threat, attack and latency to initiate these behaviours) in the first (1) and fourth (4) agonistic encounter. As interaction days X treatment was not significant, squared brackets [] indicate the treatments that differ significantly considering the data from the first and fourth episodes together. * p < 0.05, significant difference with respect to mice treated with vehicle (7-NT12 + SD vs. vehicle + SD group); # p < 0.05, significant difference with respect to the first encounter.
Table 2. Pearson correlations between behavioural categories shown by experimental and opponent mice during agonistic encounters.
Table 2. Pearson correlations between behavioural categories shown by experimental and opponent mice during agonistic encounters.
Threat 1Threat 4LThreat 1LThreat 4LAttack 1Av/Flee 1Av/Flee 4LAv/Flee 1LAv/Flee 4Def/S 1Def/S 4LDef/S 1LDef/S 4
Threat 1 nsnsnsnsr = −0.357; R2 = 0.127nsnsnsnsnsnsns
Threat 4ns nsnsnsnsr = −0.444; R2 = 0.197nsnsnsr = 0.613;
R2 = 0.375
nsns
LThreat 1nsns nsr = 0.638;
R2 = 0.407
nsnsr = 0.58;
R2 = 0.336
nsnsnsr = 0.461;
R2 = 0.213
ns
LThreat 4nsnsns nsnsnsnsnsnsr = −0.357; R2 = 0.128nsr = 0.516;
R2 = 0.266
LAttack 1nsns0.001ns nsnsr = 0.937;
R2 = 0.879
nsnsnsnsns
Av/Flee 10.05nsnsnsns nsr = −0.36;
R2 = 0.129
nsr = −0.458; R2 = 0.210nsnsns
Av/Flee 4ns0.01nsnsnsns nsnsnsr = −0.731; R2 = 0.535nsr = 0.423;
R2 = 0.179
LAv/Flee 1nsns0.001ns0.010.05ns nsnsnsnsns
LAv/Flee 4nsnsnsnsnsnsnsns nsnsnsns
Def/S 1nsnsnsnsns0.01nsnsns nsr = −0.498; R2 = 0.248ns
Def/S 4ns0.001ns0.05nsns0.01nsnsns nsr = −0.485; R2 = 0.235
LDef/S 1nsns0.01nsnsnsnsnsns0.01ns ns
LDef/S 4nsnsns0.01nsns0.05nsnsns0.01ns
Behavioural measurements: Threat, latency of threat, latency of attack, avoidance/flee, latency of avoidance/flee, defence/submission, latency of defence/submission. 1: first episode of defeat; 4, fourth episode of defeat. Upper part: r and R2 values for significant correlations, ns: no significant correlation. Lower part: level of significance. Only measurements with significant correlations were shown.
Table 3. Pearson correlations between behaviours in episodes of defeat and behavioural tests.
Table 3. Pearson correlations between behaviours in episodes of defeat and behavioural tests.
Threat 1Attack 1Attack 4LAttack 1Av/Flee 4LAv/Flee 1Def/S 4ImmobLImmobTOA%TOAEOA%EOAISICPP
Threat 1 nsnsnsnsnsnsnsnsnsnsnsnsr = −0.356; R2 = 0.126ns
Attack 1ns nsnsnsnsnsnsnsr = 0.361; R2 = 0.130r = 0.371; R2 = 0.137r = 0.361; R2 = 0.130nsnsns
Attack 4nsns nsnsnsnsnsnsr = 0.401; R2 = 0.161r = 0.363; R2 = 0.131nsr = 0.342; R2 = 0.117r = 0.369; R2 = 0.136ns
LAttack 1nsnsns nsr = 0.937;
R2 = 0.879
nsnsr = 0.507; R2 = 0.257nsnsnsnsnsns
Av/Flee 4nsnsnsns nsr = −0.731; R2 = 0.535nsns r = 0.366; R2 = 0.134nsnsnsns
LAv/Flee 1nsnsns0.001ns nsnsr = 0.475; R2 = 0.225nsnsnsnsnsns
Def/S 4nsnsnsns0.001ns r = 0.345; R2 = 0.119nsr = −0.496; R2 = 0.246r = −0.431; R2 = 0.186r = −0.424; R2 = 0.179nsnsr = 0.371; R2 = 0.138
Immobnsnsnsnsnsns0.05 nsnsnsnsnsr = 0.443; R2 = 0.196ns
L Immobnsnsns0.01ns0.01nsns nsnsnsnsnsns
TOAns0.050.05ns0.05ns0.01nsns r = 0.96;
R2 = 0.922
r = 0.878; R2 = 0.771r = 0.821; R2 = 0.674nsns
%TOAns0.050.05ns0.05ns0.05nsns0.001 r = 0.852; R2 = 0.725r = 0.888; R2 = 0.788nsns
EOAns0.05nsnsnsns0.05nsns0.0010.001 r = 0.825; R2 = 0.681r = −0.381; R2 = 0.145ns
%EOAnsns0.05nsnsnsnsnsns0.0010.0010.001 nsns
ISI0.05ns0.05nsnsnsns0.01nsnsns0.05ns ns
CPPnsnsnsnsnsns0.05nsnsnsnsnsnsns
Behavioural measurements: Threat, attack, latency of attack, avoidance/flee, latency of avoidance/flee, defence/submission, time of immobility in the TST, latency of immobility in the TST, measurements of the open arms (OA) in the EPM (time, percentage of time, entries and percentage of entries), index of social interaction (ISI), CPP score. 1: first episode of defeat; 4, fourth episode of defeat. Upper part: r and R2 values for significant correlations, ns: no significant correlation. Lower part: level of significance. Only measurements with significant correlations were shown.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Martínez-Caballero, M.Á.; García-Pardo, M.P.; Calpe-López, C.; Arenas, M.C.; Manzanedo, C.; Aguilar, M.A. Inhibition of Nitric Oxide Synthesis Prevents the Effects of Intermittent Social Defeat on Cocaine-Induced Conditioned Place Preference in Male Mice. Pharmaceuticals 2024, 17, 1203. https://doi.org/10.3390/ph17091203

AMA Style

Martínez-Caballero MÁ, García-Pardo MP, Calpe-López C, Arenas MC, Manzanedo C, Aguilar MA. Inhibition of Nitric Oxide Synthesis Prevents the Effects of Intermittent Social Defeat on Cocaine-Induced Conditioned Place Preference in Male Mice. Pharmaceuticals. 2024; 17(9):1203. https://doi.org/10.3390/ph17091203

Chicago/Turabian Style

Martínez-Caballero, María Ángeles, María Pilar García-Pardo, Claudia Calpe-López, María Carmen Arenas, Carmen Manzanedo, and María Asuncion Aguilar. 2024. "Inhibition of Nitric Oxide Synthesis Prevents the Effects of Intermittent Social Defeat on Cocaine-Induced Conditioned Place Preference in Male Mice" Pharmaceuticals 17, no. 9: 1203. https://doi.org/10.3390/ph17091203

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Article metric data becomes available approximately 24 hours after publication online.
Back to TopTop