Next Article in Journal
Advancing Prevention of STIs by Developing Specific Serodiagnostic Targets: Trichomonas vginalis as a Model
Next Article in Special Issue
Empirical Relationships between Problematic Alcohol Use and a Problematic Use of Video Games, Social Media and the Internet and Their Associations to Mental Health in Adolescence
Previous Article in Journal
The Impact of COVID-19 Related Lockdown on Dental Practice in Central Italy—Outcomes of A Survey
Previous Article in Special Issue
Problematic Relationships with Smartphones of Spanish and Colombian University Students
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Assessing the Psychometric Properties of the Internet Addiction Test in Peruvian University Students

by
Arnold Alejandro Tafur-Mendoza
1,
Julio César Acosta-Prado
2,3,*,
Rodrigo Arturo Zárate-Torres
4 and
Duván Emilio Ramírez-Ospina
3
1
Research Center (CIUP), Universidad del Pacífico, Lima 15072, Peru
2
School of Business Science, Universidad del Pacífico, Lima 15072, Peru
3
School of Accounting, Economic and Business Sciences, Universidad de Manizales, Manizales 170001, Colombia
4
Colegio de Estudios Superiores de Administración, Bogota 111071, Colombia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17(16), 5782; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17165782
Submission received: 30 May 2020 / Revised: 9 July 2020 / Accepted: 24 July 2020 / Published: 10 August 2020

Abstract

:
The use of the Internet has been gradually and unstoppably gaining ground in all areas of life, from recreational activities to how social relations are established. However, the existence of clinical cases indicates that the addictive use of the Internet is a problem that seriously affects some people. Among the instruments that measure this construct, the Internet Addiction Test (IAT) stands out. However, instrumental studies of this test are scarce in Latin America. The present study sought to analyze the psychometric properties of the IAT in a sample of 227 Peruvian undergraduate university students. Confirmatory factor analysis was used to provide validity evidence based on the internal structure, and evidence based on the relationship with other variables was also provided. Reliability was estimated through the ordinal alpha coefficient. The results indicated that the IAT adequately fits a bifactor model (with two specific factors, time/control and stress/compensate), obtaining good levels of reliability. Additionally, the IAT scores correlate significantly with the average number of hours per day on the internet and social skills. The results lead to the conclusion that the scores in the IAT have evidence of validity and reliability for its use.

1. Introduction

In the framework of a society in which both communication and the free flow of information are closely related to the development of the network, it is necessary to know to what extent reality and the virtual sphere intermingle. From the lowering of costs, the multiplicity of ways of accessing the network, and the advent of social networks, the Internet grows exponentially every day [1]. As studies in Sweden [2] and Spain [3] seem to indicate, the fierce proliferation of the network as a means of communication has brought negative consequences (cyberbullying, problematic Internet use, sexting, nomophobia, etc.) that have a stronger impact on the young population, having identified a series of problems among which stand out the addiction to this environment and that affect above all the social sphere of the individual.
However, the use of the Internet has expanded gradually and unstoppably in all areas of life [4]. The existence of clinical cases indicates that maladaptive use of the Internet is an existing problem that seriously affects some people, mainly those with special emotional needs and young people and adolescents [5]. The use or abuse of the Internet arises from disciplines, such as psychology or psychiatry [6]. In this sense, it is not surprising that the term Internet addiction was used for the first time by the psychiatrist Ivan Golberg in 1995 [7]. The literature on problematic use of the Internet shows a very varied terminology to describe the problems derived from the use of the Internet, among which are: Computer addiction, excessive use of the Internet, pathological use of the Internet, Internet dependence, compulsive use of the Internet, disorder by impulsive use, and compulsive use of the Internet or Internet addiction [8,9].
Kimberly Young [10], after reviewing a series of investigations, which indicated that some online users became addicted to the Internet in the same way that others became addicted to drugs, alcohol, or gambling, suggests the need to empirically investigate the concept of addictive use of the Internet. In addition to the review, the study sought to identify whether excessive use of the Internet can be considered addictive and to know the magnitude of the problems created by these abuses. However, Young’s approaches to Internet addiction sparked a controversial debate among doctors and academics at the time [11].
On the other hand, there are proposals [12] that suggested that Internet addiction is a disorder that should have been considered in the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-V) since it is an impulsive-compulsive spectrum disorder [13], which consists of the use of computers. However, these recommendations or proposals were not reflected in the DSM-V, since the term Internet addiction does not appear in the manual [14]. Among the diagnoses referenced in the DSM-V, Internet gaming disorder (IGD) is strongly related to the pathological nature of Internet use. The excessive use of the Internet not only involves the reproduction of online games (for example, it also implies the excessive use of social networks, such as Facebook or Twitter). Therefore, IGD is within the Internet addiction. Thus, research on other excessive uses of the Internet should follow guidelines analogous to those suggested with IGD [14].
Among the theoretical models proposed to understand Internet addiction, Davis’ cognitive-behavioral model [15] is the one that has had the most development and has been empirically tested in different contexts [16]. Two parts are distinguished in this model, one specific and the other general. The first involves excessive use of an aspect of the internet, while generalized use encompasses multiple excessive uses of the Internet [15]. Generalized use is associated with social support and social service uses of the Internet, where maladaptive cognitions are a strong predictor of this component and to a lesser extent of specific use [17]. Under this model, symptoms of Internet addiction are primarily affective or behavioral and are usually preceded or caused by cognitive symptoms, mediated by specific and general pathological Internet use [15,16]. The value of the cognitive-behavioral model lies in that it contemplates a continuum of severity regarding Internet use, allowing a better understanding by mental health specialists of the way and degree to which the excessive use of the internet can affect the lives of people [18].
To measure Internet addiction, various tests have been developed, which, mainly, are based on the diagnosis of this disorder, including the Problematic Internet Use Questionnaire (PIUQ) [19], the Compulsive Internet Use Scale (CIUS) [20], Internet-Related Problem Scale [21], and the Internet-Related Experiences Questionnaire (IREQ) [22]. The PIUQ, made up of 18 items, was built for the general population and measures problems related to Internet use through three subscales: Obsession, neglect, and control disorder. The CUIS is made up of 14 items that measure the severity of compulsive Internet use. The Internet-Related Problem Scale has 20 items that address DSM-IV symptoms for substance abuse: Tolerance, escape from other problems, reduced activities, loss of control, negative effects, withdrawal, craving, and introversion. The IREQ was developed in middle-school students and is made up of 10 items that measure possible Internet addiction based on intrapersonal and interpersonal experiences. However, the test that has had the most development and that has been adapted to various contexts is the Internet Addiction Test (IAT) [23]. Thus, the objective of this study was to analyze the psychometric properties of the IAT in a sample of Peruvian undergraduate university students.

2. Literature Review

For this study, in a search conducted in Scopus and Web of Science (WoS) on studies of the psychometric properties of the IAT, 46 studies were found (April 2020). Table 1 presents a summary of 39 of the 46 studies found, which were those that analyzed the internal structure of the IAT, an aspect that does not have a consensus in all the studies carried out. Most of the studies were carried out in Europe and Asia; in North America, only one study was found in the United States and another in Canada, while, in Latin America, the IAT has been studied in Colombia and Chile. Regarding the populations where these studies have been carried out, the majority were in university students, probably due to the accessibility of the sample and that it is the age group that spends the most time on the Internet (young people between 18 and 25 years old).
The reliability of the IAT has usually been estimated through the alpha coefficient, obtaining, in most cases, acceptable values above 0.70, and even exceeding 0.90. However, most of these studies have not considered the assumptions that this coefficient has, such as the one-dimensionality of the data, that the measurement model is tau-equivalent, and that there are no correlated errors [63]. Violation of any of these assumptions would distort the values reported in these studies. Additionally, there is no consideration of the ordinality of the items, which must be considered when working with Likert-type scales [64]. A meta-analysis of the internal consistency of the IAT (reliability generalization) indicates a combined alpha of 0.90 for samples of university students [65].
Regarding the internal structure of the IAT, the literature shows factorial models with one to six factors. Although, the diversity of models increases, because two studies may have the same number of factors, but they differ in the number of items used (not all studies end their analyses with the original 20 items but eliminate some of them because they do not fit their proposed model), in the addition of correlated errors (in many cases to improve the adjustment indices in the confirmatory factor analysis, without taking into account the substantive justification of the correlated errors [66]) or in the distribution of the items by the factor (two models can coincide in the number of factors and items, but the grouping of the items is different). This large number of models does not allow for a uniform IAT structure to be available, and for studies seeking to study this aspect in a particular sample, they have to resort to exploratory techniques or to test a large number of existing models.
An aspect linked to the above is the analysis technique used to find the factor structure in these studies. Almost half of the studies reported in Table 1 indicate that the principal component analysis (PCA) has been performed as the first or only technique, which is not suitable for psychological constructs, since it is a variable reduction technique; therefore, it seeks to explain the greater proportion of explained variance, considering both the variance shared by the items and the error variance. On the contrary, the factor analysis only considers the extraction of the factors the variance that the items share among themselves [67]. The use of the PCA could lead to obtaining structures that do not fit the data, especially since the measurement nature of the data is not considered.
In Peru, only one study of the psychometric properties of IAT [68] in middle-school students between the ages of 13 and 19 years was found. Item 1 was removed from the scale because it presented a low discriminatory capacity; therefore, this version had 19 items. Reliability was estimated through the alpha coefficient, obtaining a value of 0.870. On the other hand, the internal structure was evaluated by PCA, finding a factorial solution of four components. This structure was corroborated in the same sample through confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). It is important to note that the use of middle-school students and university students is different. University students use the Internet primarily for information search; also, they spend more time online and have a wider range of Internet uses than middle-school students [69].
Another source of validity evidence that has been explored in these background studies is evidence based on the relationship with other variables, specifically convergent evidence, where the hours of daily Internet use was used as a measure, in addition to the IAT [38,41,45,50]. In these studies, correlation coefficients between 0.29 and 0.48 were found. Additionally, Internet use has an impact on a person’s social life, and studies have shown that Internet addiction is negatively related to social skills [70,71,72].
Considering all the above, the IAT is one of the most used instruments for measuring Internet addiction and that, in Peru, it has only worked with middle-school students in the adolescence stage but not in other samples, such as university students, which is precisely in this group where most adaptations have been made in other countries. Furthermore, the statistical procedures used in previous studies may be questionable and, in some cases, inadequate. Thus, the IAT does not have a version for Peruvian university students, so its psychometric properties are not known in this population and the extrapolation of results in other contexts would not be appropriate, since validity and reliability are not inherent in a test but correspond to the specific interpretations and uses of the scores obtained in a test [73]. Therefore, the objective of this study was to analyze the psychometric properties of the IAT in a sample of Peruvian undergraduate university students.

3. Materials and Methods

3.1. Design

According to the classification system of research designs in psychology [74], the objective of the study corresponds to an instrumental investigation, since the psychometric properties of a psychological instrument are analyzed in a specific sample. For the development of the study, various standards, guidelines, good practices, and recommendations were followed in instrumental studies in the behavioral and health sciences [73,75].

3.2. Participants

The selection of the participants was carried out through an intentional non-probability sampling [76], where the individuals in the sample were directly and selectively chosen, seeking to obtain similar proportions in each of the categories of the variables sex, year of study, and academic discipline. Regarding the sample size, a priori statistical power analysis was performed to determine the minimum recommended sample size. Statistical power is the ability of a statistical test (for example, U Mann–Whitney or H Kruskal–Wallis) to reject a null hypothesis when it is false; in other words, it is the probability of not committing the type II error [77]. The input parameters for this analysis were based on a simple two-tailed correlational model (similar to the one that will be carried out in the collection of validity evidence based on the relationship with other variables), the significance level (α) being 0.05, the expected effect size equal to 0.20 (recommended minimum value representing practical significance in social science data [78]), and an expected statistical power of 0.80 (recommended minimum in behavioral science [79]). The recommended minimum sample size was 191 (Figure 1).
The final sample was made up of 227 Peruvian undergraduate university students from a public university located in Metropolitan Lima (Peru). The ages of the participants were between 18 and 40 years (M = 20.81, SD = 2.92). The highest proportion of students were females (57.30%), in their second year of studies (30.00%), and belonged to professional careers in the engineering area (19.80%). Table 2 presents a more detailed description of the sample characteristics.

3.3. Instruments

3.3.1. IAT—Internet Addiction Test

The Spanish version of the IAT was used (Appendix A), adapted to a sample of Colombian university students [32]. The IAT is made up of 20 items, all in a positive sense, on a five-point Likert-type scale (1 = rarely; 2 = occasionally; 3 = frequently; 4 = usually; 5 = always), the minimum score being 20 and the maximum score 100, where the student was asked to choose the alternative that best suits their reality. The IAT items were reviewed by the authors of this study, pointing out that it was not necessary to adjust their wording. To corroborate this assumption, a pilot study was carried out with 10 university students, who indicated that they had no problems in understanding the items.
In the adaptation study, the total scale had a good level of reliability (α = 0.89). Likewise, through an analysis of the main components, they found an internal structure of three factors (consequences for the use of the Internet, cognitive-emotional dimension, and time control), which together had an explained variance of 47.80%, and correlated positively with the number of hours of daily Internet access, although the magnitudes were low [32].
For the application of this study, in addition to the IAT, a sociodemographic and Internet usage file was added to collect information on the career that the participants studied, as well as the year of studies they were studying, age, gender, and the average daily hours spent on the Internet.

3.3.2. EHS—Social Skills Scale

The Social Skills Scale, developed by Gismero [80], is made up of 33 items. Five items are written in the direct sense, while 28 items are in an inverse sense, seeking to detect the lack of assertion or deficit in social skills. The original instrument is composed of six factors: (1) Self-expression in social situations, eight items; (2) defense of one’s rights as a consumer, five items; (3) expression of anger or disagreement, four items; (4) assertiveness, six items; (5) making requests, five items; and (6) starting interactions with the opposite sex, five items. Each item is answered using a four-point response format (A = I do not identify myself at all; B = Rather it does not have to do with me, even if it ever happens to me; C = It describes me approximately, although I do not always act or feel like this; and D = Strongly agree, and I would feel or act like this in most cases). A higher global score indicates that the person has a higher level of social skills and better insertion in various contexts or situations.
This instrument has been studied in various countries, presenting adequate psychometric properties, good levels of reliability, adequate adjustment to the six-factor structure, and discriminant evidence [81]. In the present study, the scores on the items showed good internal consistency at the global level (ω = 0.890). Likewise, in the factors, the omega coefficient (ω) varied from 0.500 to 0.748.

3.4. Procedure

The data collection process began with the request of permits, both to the directors of the schools of each academic discipline and the teachers in charge of some courses for the application of the instrument in classrooms. Before the application of the IAT, the students were given an informed consent form that contained the objective of the evaluation and where they were guaranteed the confidentiality and anonymity of their answers, as well as the possibility of withdrawing from their participation at any time of the evaluation without consequence. Only those students who voluntarily signed the informed consent participated in the study.
The application was collective, with an average duration of 15 min. Data collection lasted approximately five weeks. At the end of each evaluation, the examiners reviewed the application protocols to verify that there are no unanswered items or items with more than one marked answer option. If one or both situations described occurred, the evaluated person was asked to correct and provide their definitive answer. After data collection, each evaluation protocol was coded for the elaboration of the database in a Microsoft Excel 2016 spreadsheet.
During the evaluation process, the ethical guidelines for working with humans outlined in the code of ethics of the American Psychological Association (APA) were followed. Additionally, the ethical principles of the Declaration of Helsinki were respected, including its recent updates and regulations for research on human beings.

3.5. Data Analysis

The statistical analysis was divided into four stages: (1) Item analysis; (2) validity evidence based on the internal structure of the test through CFA; (3) validity evidence based on the relationship with other variables from the convergent evidence; and (4) reliability analysis using the internal consistency method.
The descriptive analysis of the items was performed using the mean and standard deviation, as measures of central tendency and dispersion, respectively. As descriptive measures of the distribution of the items, the skewness and kurtosis coefficients were used, where values between −2.00 and 2.00 indicate that the items follow a normal distribution [82]. Likewise, item discrimination was estimated through the item-rest polyserial correlation, which considers the ordinality of the items, considering values above 0.30 as adequate [83]. Finally, possible floor and ceiling effects in the items were analyzed, that is, to identify the proportion of participants who chose the lowest alternative (floor effect) and the highest (ceiling effect), taking as acceptable effects those that were between 1% and 15% [84].
The CFA was carried out using the 23 models presented in Table 1, considering only those models made up of the 20 original items and whose factors had at least three items. The estimation method used was the weighted least squares with mean and variance adjusted (WLSMV), appropriate for observable ordinal variables and that performs well against slightly non-normal underlying distributions [85]. The WLSMV involves the use of the diagonal weighted least squares (DWLS) estimator with robust standard errors and a statistical test with adjusted mean and variance (using a scale-shifted approach). To assess the level of adequacy of the models, the following fit indices were used [86,87]: SSχ2/df < 2.00, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) < 0.08, comparative fit index (CFI) > 0.90, Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) > 0.90, standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) < 0.08, and weighted root mean square residual (WRMR) < 1.00.
The convergent evidence involved correlating the scores obtained in the IAT with another measure that seeks to evaluate the same construct or other construct with which it is expected is correlated; in this case, these measures were the average of daily hours that the study participants spent on the Internet and a social skills scale. Before selecting the correlation statistic to be used, the presence of outliers in these two variables with which the IAT scores were correlated was examined. Univariate analysis of outliers was visually inspected through boxplots for each variable (including the six dimensions of the social skills scale). A few outliers were found in two factors of the social skills scale and in the average of daily hours that the study participants spent on the Internet. Therefore, a robust statistic was chosen for the correlation between variables, the skipped correlation coefficient. This coefficient is robust to slight changes in any distribution and has the advantage of treating outliers in a way that considers the general structure of a data set [88]. As a measure of the effect size, the coefficient of determination was used, considering its interpretation values of 0.04, 0.25, and 0.64 as the minimum, moderate, and strong effect, respectively [78].
The internal consistency of the items was estimated using the ordinal alpha coefficient [89] since it works with the inter-item polychoric correlation matrix. Values above 0.70 were considered acceptable [90].
A priori statistical power analysis to determine the minimum sample size required for the study was performed using the G*Power 3.1.9.7 software [91]. The other analyses were performed in the R software version 4.0.2 (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria, 2016) [92], using the following packages: pacman 0.5.1 [93], readxl 1.3.1 [94], tidyverse 1.3.0 [95], psych 1.9.12.31 [96], lavaan 0.6–6 [97], BifactorIndicesCalculator 0.2.0 [98], semTools 0.5–2.925 [99], and WRS 0.36 [100].

4. Results

4.1. Item Analysis

Table 3 presents the results of the item analysis. The means of the items were between 1.352 (item 20) and 3.084 (items 1 and 7), indicating that the participants’ responses were concentrated on the lowest alternatives (rarely, occasionally, and frequently). Likewise, the standard deviation fluctuated between 0.658 (item 20) and 1.211 (item 7), showing low variability in the data. Regarding the skewness and kurtosis measures, most of the items presented values between −2.00 and 2.00. However, items 9, 15, 19, and 20 showed an excess of kurtosis, and these last two also indicated an excess of skewness. Therefore, these four items showed a deviation from a normal distribution.
Concerning the item-rest polyserial correlation, all the items were above the threshold of 0.30, ranging from 0.371 (item 7) to 0.684 (item 15), indicating good discrimination by the IAT items. On the other hand, in the analysis of the response options, a soil effect was found in all the items, except for items 1 and 7. However, regarding the ceiling effect, most of the items had acceptable values (between 1% and 15%). These effects reflect the tendency of the participants to choose the lowest response options, being the most prominent in items 19 and 20, where more than 70% of the sample chose the response option “rarely”. On the contrary, the “always” alternative was selected in a low percentage, even reaching 0% in some items (items 6, 8, 10, 11, 15, and 20).

4.2. Validity Evidence Based on the Internal Structure

The models tested were taken from Table 1. However, of the 39 studies presented, the studies selected were those where the factor structure was configured by all the items of the IAT (20 items) and that the proposed factors have at least three items in its composition, which is recommended to achieve an adequate representation of a factor [86]. Twenty-four studies met the two requirements and their factor models were tested. Four studies had the same unifactorial structure [29,45,51,53]. Whereas, five models [26,32,38,46,59] presented problems in the analysis: Covariance matrix of latent variables was not positive definite [59], some estimated latent variable variances were negative [26,38,46], or the model did not converge [32].
Table 4 presents the fit indices of the models tested. The bifactor model of Watters et al. (2013) [34] was the one that obtained the best results (SSχ2/df < 2.00, RMSEA < 0.08, CFI > 0.90, TLI > 0.90, SRMR < 0.08, and WRMR < 1.00). Figure 2 shows the factor structure of the bifactor model, made up of a general factor that measures Internet addiction through 20 items and two specific factors, time/control and stress/compensate, the first factor consisting of five items (1, 2, 7, 16, and 17) and the second factor of 11 items (3, 4, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 15, 18, 19, and 20). Figure 2 also presents the factor loadings of the items in the general factor and the corresponding specific factors.
Additionally, complementary statistical indices were calculated that allowed assessment of the robustness of the general factor, as well as the contribution of specific factors [101]. The omega hierarchical was used for the general factor and the specific factors, expecting values above 0.70 (ωH) for the first and greater than 0.30 (ωHS) for the seconds. The results indicate a value of 0.704 for the general factor, 0.234 for time/control, and 0.478 for stress/compensate. The H coefficient was also calculated, considering values greater than 0.70 to be adequate. The general factor obtained a coefficient of 0.888: For the time/control factor it was 0.785 while, for the stress/compensate factor, the H coefficient was equal to 0.513.
The explained common variance (ECV) for the general factor was 0.612, while the factors (ECV of a specific factor concerning itself) showed an ECV of 0.368 for time/control and 0.562 for stress/compensate. On the other hand, the proportion of uncontaminated correlations (PUCs) was equal to 0.658. At the item level, the ECV index (I-ECV) was between 0.126 and 1.000. Thus, the results allow the use of this bifactor model [102].

4.3. Validity Evidence Based on Relations to Other Variables

The convergent evidence was collected from the correlation between the two specific factors of IAT and the general factor with the average hours per day that study participants spent on the Internet and social skills. The minimum time of hours per day was less than one hour and the maximum 12 h (M = 3.368, SD = 2.277). The skipped correlation coefficients between the average daily hours on the Internet with Internet addiction (and time/control factor) were statistically significant (Table 5). Likewise, the effect size (skipped correlation coefficient squared) in the time/control factor and total Internet addiction was greater than 0.04, indicating a recommended minimum effect size that represents practical significance in social science data [78]. Regarding social skills, the correlations between Internet addiction (and its two factors) with social skills (and the defense of rights factor) were statistically significant (Table 5). Similar results were obtained for correlations between Internet addiction (and stress/compensate factor) with self-expression, disagreement, and assertiveness (Table 5). The size of the effect between Internet addiction and social skills presented practical significance. These findings provide convergent evidence to the IAT.
On the other hand, the correlation between the specific factors of the IAT presented a minimum effect size (skipped correlation coefficient squared > 0.04). For the correlation between time/control and stress/compensate with Internet addiction, the effect size was strong (skipped correlation coefficient squared > 0.64). In all three cases, the correlations were statistically significant (Table 5).

4.4. Reliability

Reliability was evaluated by the internal consistency method, using for this purpose the ordinal alpha coefficient, which considers the ordinal nature of the items for its calculation. The specific factors and the general factor obtained satisfactory values, above 0.70 (Table 5). Likewise, the coefficients obtained were not remarkably high (0.90 or higher), indicating that the IAT, in the sample studied, does not include redundant items [103].

5. Discussion

The present study analyzed the psychometric properties of the IAT in a sample of Peruvian university students. The 20 items that make up the instrument presented adequate levels of discrimination, although the analysis of the response options indicated the presence of a floor effect in 18 items. Likewise, the levels of skewness and kurtosis were acceptable in most of the items. Regarding the validity evidence based on the internal structure, different models were tested through CFA, being the bifactor model with two specific factors (time/control and stress/compensate), the one that presented the best indexes of adjustment. Another source of validity evidence that was used was that based on the relationship with other variables, specifically the convergent evidence, finding statistically significant correlations between the two specific factors and the general factor with the average number of hours per day on the Internet and social skills. Finally, the reliability, estimated through the ordinal alpha coefficient, was acceptable for the general factor and the specific factors.
The items showed discrimination indexes above 0.30, which implies that each item is related to the other items taken together, which would also justify the presence of an underlying general factor. These results agree with a previous study [43], where all the items were higher than the cut-off point used in this study. However, other studies showed problems only with item 7 (“How often do you check your email before something else that you need to do?”), finding values of 0.170 [42], 0.250 [41], −0.098 [104], and 0.195 [32], while all the other items were greater than 0.30. In this study, item 7 had the lowest item-rest correlation (0.371).
Item 7 is problematic in the literature because it would be mainly relevant for university students or people whose jobs involve communication by this means. In university students, virtual communication occurs mainly with their professors by email, for sending papers, receiving corrected papers, or notifying activities on virtual platforms, as well as with their peers to exchange information (e.g., articles or books) or share working documents. It is important to highlight that, in this study, the item-rest correlation was estimated using the polyserial correlation, unlike previous studies that worked with the Pearson correlation coefficient. The polyserial correlation coefficient considers the ordinality of the items, being more precise in estimating the degree of item discrimination.
The presence of a floor effect in the items, together with low averages in these, shows the low level of Internet addiction of the participants. This result may be due to the characteristics of the study participants, who belong entirely to a public sector university, where most of the population belongs to a medium or medium-low socioeconomic level, having some limitations regarding the Internet accessibility as it involves spending on devices (cell phones, tablets, laptops, etc.) and mobile data for connectivity. On the other hand, Internet addiction is a clinical construct, so its presence in a non-clinical population (university students) should be low under normal conditions.
Regarding the internal structure of the IAT, this study recollected a large part of the models reported in previous studies to test them and find out how the IAT is structured in the Peruvian sample. The bifactor model (one general factor and two specific factors) [34] presented the best fit. In the reviewed literature, the other study that reported a bifactor model found a general factor and three specific factors [56]. Comparing the results obtained in this study with those reported by Watters et al. [34], many points of agreement were observed, both at the level of fit indices and in factor loadings. In both studies, the factor loadings had higher values in the general factor than in the specific factors. In both studies, several items presented factor loadings below 0.30 or 0.40, which are the usual cut-off points in this type of study. However, the results of the present bifactor model indicate that those items that had low factor loadings in the general factor, had higher factor loadings in the specific factors, and vice versa.
The characteristic described above is typical of bifactor models, which allow the simultaneous evaluation of the influence of the general factor and specific factors on the variability of each item. The evaluation of the bifactor model through the omega hierarchical (ωH and ωHS) and H coefficients (also known as construct reliability), ECV, I-ECV, and PUC, provided evidence regarding the relevance of the model. The remaining 15 models reviewed and tested presented convergence or adjustment problems. The difference in fit between the previous studies and the present study is probably due to the different estimation methods used. In this study, the WLSMV estimator was used that considers the categorical nature of the items. Furthermore, many of the previous studies had problems in choosing the appropriate statistical technique or methods.
The use of the PCA for work with psychological variables is not appropriate since in its conception it considers formative models, useful in other disciplines (economics, marketing, among others), where it seeks to group indicators or reduce the number of variables. By contrast, factor analysis works with reflective models, where an underlying variable (factor) causes certain behaviors (indicators or items). For the factor analysis, the exploratory or non-restrictive version involves making a series of decisions during the analysis, the most critical being the determination of the number of factors. Additionally, the choice of the rotation method should be justified by how the factors are related. In the reviewed antecedents, the use, in most of the studies, of the “Little Jiffy” was observed, which supposes a routine of analysis in the three mentioned aspects: PCA, eigenvalues greater than one (method to choose the number of factors) and Varimax rotation (consider that the factors are not correlated). The use of Little Jiffy has been heavily criticized and its use is not recommended, as it may lead to the acceptance of erroneous factor models [67].
From a theoretical point of view, the time/control factor is related to behavioral symptoms (e.g., neglect household chores to spend more time online), while the stress/compensate factor is made up of cognitive and affective symptoms (e.g., block out disturbing thoughts about your life with soothing thoughts of the Internet or snap, yell, or act annoyed if someone bothers you while you are online). This bifactor model is framed within the cognitive-behavioral perspective, explaining the symptoms of Internet addiction (of varied nature) from specific and generalized uses, which simultaneously influence the symptoms [15]. The model obtained in this study theoretically differs from the models presented in the literature review, because a component (behavioral, cognitive, or affective) is not emphasized, but rather, the components are worked on simultaneously.
Convergent evidence from the IAT was also provided, finding statistically significant correlations with the hours of daily Internet use, and time/control and total Internet addiction presented a recommended minimum effect size that represents practical significance in social science data. These results were like those obtained by other researchers [38,41,45,50]. Additionally, the IAT negatively correlates with social skills measures (total score and self-expression, disagreement, and assertiveness factors). Previous studies also report these relationships with similar degrees of correlation. Internet addiction is associated with greater difficulties in social skills, probably since the emotional burden produced by being connected to the Internet interferes with social aspects. In this way, the development of social skills is left aside due to the few social interactions that the subject experiences, since most of his time is online [2,71].
Regarding the reliability of the scores on the IAT, the ordinal alpha coefficient showed acceptable levels for the general factor and the specific factors. On this point, most of previous studies coincide, including meta-analytical studies [65].
Regarding the limitations of the study, the main one focuses on the size and variety of the sample. Regarding the first aspect, although globally, the sample size is justified in an a priori statistical power analysis, the number of participants within the groups of sociodemographic variables is small, which limits the possibility of carrying out additional analyses in the items. For example, knowing the differential functioning of items or knowing the factorial invariance of the IAT. Regarding the variety of the sample, the students belonged to a public university; therefore, they share various characteristics that make it a homogeneous group, and therefore, the variability in the responses to the items was low.
To know and deepen other characteristics of the IAT, future studies should focus their objectives on analyses that provide evidence of its clinical utility. In this way, the appropriate cut-off points should be determined to be able to classify people addicted to the Internet and, in turn, evaluate the intensity of this addiction. Likewise, it is necessary to previously know how the instrument works in people with a presumption of Internet addiction, as well as to what extent it is related to other tests that measure clinical constructs (e.g., depression or anxiety), being relevant measures to obtain validity evidence based on relations to other variables. Therefore, working with clinical samples is a necessity, since the IAT could have different uses in the diagnosis and treatment of Internet addiction.
Likewise, the study of the IAT in non-academic populations must be accompanied by a review of the content of the items, since some of them may only be valid for the population of university students, for example, item 6 “How often do your grades or school work suffer because of the amount of time you spend online? ”, being, in this case, the rewriting of the item or its exclusion from the test. Additionally, given the complexity of the IAT structure, other multivariate techniques could be tried to corroborate what was found here or to propose more stable structures. Techniques, such as network analysis, exploratory structural equation modeling (ESEM), or Bayesian approaches to factor analysis, would help in this regard. Regarding reliability, it is relevant to obtain evidence on temporal stability (test-retest reliability), particularly useful in the IAT, due to the high variability that scores in this type of test can have.

6. Conclusions

This study represents a contribution to the study of the IAT in Latin America, where it has been little studied, unlike other contexts, such as Europe or Asia. The findings indicate that the IAT, in the sample of Peruvian university students, is made up of a general factor and two specific factors (time/control and stress/compensate), or a bifactor model. Likewise, added to the validity evidence based on the internal structure, the IAT showed evidence based on the relationship with other variables (average hours per day on the Internet). On the other hand, the reliability of the scores was acceptable.
The results lead to the conclusion that the scores in the IAT have evidence of validity and reliability for its use. This has implications for both researchers and those who are primarily involved in the patient practice. For researchers, the IAT constitutes an instrument that would allow studies on Internet addiction to be carried out, for example, knowing its prevalence in certain groups, identifying the factors associated with its genesis and evolution in people, or knowing the degree of sensitivity and specificity with which one can diagnose a person with Internet addiction. For professionals, the IAT is a tool that would help diagnose Internet addiction, and it would also allow evaluation of the effects produced by a treatment or therapy that seeks to decrease the level of Internet addiction.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.A.T.-M., J.C.A.-P., R.A.Z.-T. and D.E.R.-O.; methodology, A.A.T.-M., J.C.A.-P., R.A.Z.-T. and D.E.R.-O.; software, A.A.T.-M., J.C.A.-P., R.A.Z.-T. and D.E.R.-O.; validation, A.A.T.-M., J.C.A.-P., R.A.Z.-T. and D.E.R.-O.; formal analysis, A.A.T.-M., J.C.A.-P., R.A.Z.-T. and D.E.R.-O.; investigation, A.A.T.-M., J.C.A.-P., R.A.Z.-T. and D.E.R.-O.; resources, A.A.T.-M., J.C.A.-P., R.A.Z.-T. and D.E.R.-O.; data curation, A.A.T.-M., J.C.A.-P., R.A.Z.-T. and D.E.R.-O.; writing—original draft preparation, A.A.T.-M., J.C.A.-P., R.A.Z.-T. and D.E.R.-O.; writing—review and editing, A.A.T.-M., J.C.A.-P., R.A.Z.-T. and D.E.R.-O.; visualization, A.A.T.-M., J.C.A.-P., R.A.Z.-T. and D.E.R.-O.; supervision, A.A.T.-M., J.C.A.-P., R.A.Z.-T. and D.E.R.-O.; project administration, A.A.T.-M., J.C.A.-P., R.A.Z.-T. and D.E.R.-O. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

English and Spanish version of the Internet Addiction Test (IAT)
  • How often do you find that you stay online longer than you intended?
    ¿Con qué frecuencia se conecta a internet más de lo previsto?
  • How often do you neglect household chores to spend more time online?
    ¿Con qué frecuencia descuida las actividades de la casa para estar más tiempo conectado?
  • How often do you prefer the excitement of the internet to intimacy with your partner?
    ¿Con qué frecuencia prefiere más la emoción que le produce estar conectado a la intimidad con su pareja o la relación directa con sus amigos?
  • How often do you form new relationships with fellow online users?
    ¿Con qué frecuencia forma nuevas relaciones con usuarios de Internet?
  • How often do others in your life complain to you about the amount of time you spend online?
    ¿Con qué frecuencia las personas cercanas a usted se quejan por la cantidad de tiempo que permanece conectado?
  • How often do your grades or school work suffer because of the amount of time you spend online?
    ¿Con qué frecuencia sus calificaciones o actividades académicas se afectan negativamente por la cantidad de tiempo que permanece en Internet?
  • How often do you check your email before something else that you need to do?
    ¿Con qué frecuencia revisa su correo electrónico antes de realizar otra tarea que necesita hacer?
  • How often does your job performance or productivity suffer because of the internet?
    ¿Con qué frecuencia el tiempo que pasa en Internet afecta negativamente su desempeño o productividad en el trabajo?
  • How often do you become defensive or secretive when anyone asks you what you do online?
    ¿Con qué frecuencia está a la defensiva o se muestra reservado cuando alguien le pregunta qué hace en Internet?
  • How often do you block out disturbing thoughts about your life with soothing thoughts of the internet?
    ¿Con qué frecuencia bloquea los pensamientos desagradables de su vida con pensamientos agradables relacionados con Internet?
  • How often do you find yourself anticipating when you will go online again?
    ¿Con qué frecuencia anticipa cuando estará conectado de nuevo?
  • How often do you fear that life without the internet would be boring, empty, and joyless?
    ¿Con qué frecuencia teme que la vida sin Internet sería aburrida, vacía o triste?
  • How often do you snap, yell, or act annoyed if someone bothers you while you are online?
    ¿Con qué frecuencia se enoja si alguien lo molesta mientras está conectado?
  • How often do you lose sleep due to being online?
    ¿Con qué frecuencia se queda sin dormir por conectarse durante la noche?
  • How often do you feel preoccupied with the internet when off-line, or fantasize about being online?
    ¿Con qué frecuencia se siente preocupado por no estar conectado o imagina estarlo?
  • How often do you find yourself saying “just a few more minutes” when online?
    ¿Con qué frecuencia dice: “unos minutos más”, cuando está conectado?
  • How often do you try to cut down the amount of time you spend online and fail?
    ¿Con qué frecuencia trata de disminuir el tiempo que pasa en Internet y no lo logra?
  • How often do you try to hide how long you’ve been online?
    ¿Con qué frecuencia intenta ocultar el tiempo que permanece conectado?
  • How often do you choose to spend more time online over going out with others?
    ¿Con qué frecuencia prefiere pasar más tiempo en Internet que salir con otras personas?
  • How often do you feel depressed, moody, or nervous when you are off-line, which goes away once you are back online?
    ¿Con qué frecuencia se siente deprimido, malhumorado o nervioso cuando no está conectado, pero se siente mejor cuando se conecta de nuevo?

References

  1. Freeman, C.G. The implications of online connectivity for world heritage in a digital platform society. Hist. Environ. 2018, 30, 84–95. [Google Scholar]
  2. Engelberg, E.; Sjöberg, L. Internet use, social skills, and adjustment. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 2004, 7, 41–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Gracia Blanco, M.D.; Vigo Anglada, M.; Fernández Perez, M.J.; Marcó Arbones, M. Problemas conductuales relacionados con el uso de Internet: Un estudio exploratorio. Psicologia 2002, 18, 273–292. [Google Scholar]
  4. Mayer, M.A. The use of the Internet among adolescents; Risks and benefits. Aten. Primaria 2011, 43, 287–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Sánchez-Carbonell, X.; Beranuy, M.; Castellana, M.; Chamarro, A.; Oberst, U. La adicción a Internet y al móvil: ¿moda o trastorno? Adicciones 2008, 20, 149–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Garcia del Castillo, J.A.; Terol, M.D.C.; Nieto, M.; Lledó, A.; Sánchez, S.; Martín-Aragón, M.; Sitges, E. Uso y abuso de Internet en jóvenes universitarios. Adicciones 2008, 20, 131–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Domínguez, C.; Geijo, S.; Sánchez, I.; Imaz, C.; Cabús, G. Revision of the mechanisms involved in the problematic use of the Internet. Rev. Asoc. Esp. Neuropsiquiatr. 2012, 32, 691–705. [Google Scholar]
  8. Kuss, D.J.; Lopez-Fernandez, O. Internet addiction and problematic Internet use: A systematic review of clinical research. World J. Psychiatry 2016, 6, 143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Starcevic, V.; Aboujaoude, E. Internet addiction: Reappraisal of an increasingly inadequate concept. CNS Spectr. 2017, 22, 7–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Young, K.S. Internet addiction: The emergence of a new clinical disorder. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 1998, 1, 237–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Young, K.S. Internet addiction: A new clinical phenomenon and its consequences. Am. Behav. Sci. 2004, 48, 402–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Block, J.J. Issues for DSM-V: Internet addiction. Am. J. Psychiatry 2008, 165, 306–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Young, K.S.; Nabuco de Abreu, C. Internet Addiction: A Handbook and Guide to Evaluation and Treatment; John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2010; ISBN 978-0-470-55116-5. [Google Scholar]
  14. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013; ISBN 978-0-89042-554-1. [Google Scholar]
  15. Davis, R.A. A cognitive-behavioral model of pathological Internet use. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2001, 17, 187–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Teo, C.P.A.; Lim, K.; Oei, T.P. Problematic Internet Use: Variants of cognitive-behavioural model under path analysis. Int. J. Psychol. Behav. Anal. 2017, 3, 130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Díaz-Aguado, M.J.; Martín-Babarro, J.; Falcón, L. Problematic Internet use, maladaptive future time perspective and school context. Psicothema 2018, 30, 195–200. [Google Scholar]
  18. Fernandes, B.; Rodrigues, B.; Pontes, H.M. Internet addiction or problematic internet use ? Which term should be used ? Psicol. USP 2019, 30, e190020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Demetrovics, Z.; Szeredi, B.; Rózsa, S. The three-factor model of Internet addiction: The development of the Problematic Internet Use Questionnaire. Behav. Res. Methods 2008, 40, 563–574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Meerkerk, G.-J.; Van Den Eijnden, R.J.J.M.; Vermulst, A.A.; Garretsen, H.F.L. The Compulsive Internet Use Scale (CIUS): Some psychometric properties. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 2008, 12, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Armstrong, L.; Phillips, J.G.; Saling, L.L. Potential determinants of heavier Internet usage. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 2000, 53, 537–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Casas, J.A.; Ruiz-Olivares, R.; Ortega-Ruiz, R. Validation of the internet and social networking experiences questionnaire in Spanish adolescents. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 2013, 13, 40–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Young, K.S. Caught in the Net: How to Recognize the Signs of Internet Addiction—And a Winning Strategy for Recovery; John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.: New York, NY, USA, 1998; ISBN 978-0-471-19159-9. [Google Scholar]
  24. Widyanto, L.; McMurran, M. The psychometric properties of the Internet Addiction Test. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 2004, 7, 443–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Ngai, S.S.Y. Exploring the validity of the Internet Addiction Test for students in grades 5–9 in Hong Kong. Int. J. Adolesc. Youth 2007, 13, 221–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Chang, M.K.; Man, S.P. Factor structure for Young’s Internet Addiction Test: A confirmatory study. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2008, 24, 2597–2619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Khazaal, Y.; Billieux, J.; Thorens, G.; Khan, R.; Louati, Y.; Scarlatti, E.; Theintz, F.; Lederrey, J.; Van Der Linden, M.; Zullino, D. French validation of the Internet Addiction Test. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 2008, 11, 703–706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Widyanto, L.; Griffiths, M.D.; Brunsden, V. A psychometric comparison of the Internet Addiction Test, the Internet-Related Problem Scale, and self-diagnosis. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2011, 14, 141–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Panayides, P.; Walker, M.J. Evaluation of the psychometric properties of the Internet Addiction Test (IAT) in a sample of Cypriot high school students: The Rasch measurement perspective. Eur. J. Psychol. 2012, 8, 327–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Jelenchick, L.A.; Becker, T.; Moreno, M.A. Assessing the psychometric properties of the Internet Addiction Test (IAT) in US college students. Psychiatry Res. 2012, 196, 296–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Barke, A.; Nyenhuis, N.; Kröner-Herwig, B. The German version of the Internet Addiction Test: A validation study. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2012, 15, 534–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Puerta-Cortés, D.X.; Carbonell, X.; Chamarro, A. Análisis de las propiedades psicométricas de la versión en español del Internet Addiction Test. Trastor. Adict. 2012, 14, 99–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Faraci, P.; Craparo, G.; Messina, R.; Severino, S. Internet Addiction Test (IAT): Which is the best factorial solution? J. Med. Internet Res. 2013, 15, e225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Watters, C.A.; Keefer, K.V.; Kloosterman, P.H.; Summerfeldt, L.J.; Parker, J.D.A. Examining the structure of the Internet Addiction Test in adolescents: A bifactor approach. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2013, 29, 2294–2302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Pawlikowski, M.; Altstötter-Gleich, C.; Brand, M. Validation and psychometric properties of a short version of Young’s Internet Addiction Test. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2013, 29, 1212–1223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Lee, K.; Gyeong, H.; Yu, B.; Song, Y.M.; Lee, H.K.; Kim, D. Reliability and validity of the Korean version of the Internet Addiction Test among college students. J. Korean Med. Sci. 2013, 28, 763–768. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Hawi, N.S. Arabic validation of the Internet Addiction Test. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2013, 16, 200–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Lai, C.-M.; Mak, K.-K.; Watanabe, H.; Ang, R.P.; Pang, J.S.; Ho, R.C.M. Psychometric properties of the Internet Addiction Test in Chinese adolescents. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 2013, 38, 794–807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Pontes, H.M.; Patrão, I.M.; Griffiths, M.D. Portuguese validation of the Internet Addiction Test: An empirical study. J. Behav. Addict. 2014, 3, 107–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Karim, A.K.M.R.; Nigar, N. The Internet Addiction Test: Assessing its psychometric properties in bangladeshi culture. Asian J. Psychiatr. 2014, 10, 75–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Tsimtsiou, Z.; Haidich, A.B.; Kokkali, S.; Dardavesis, T.; Young, K.S.; Arvanitidou, M. Greek version of the Internet Addiction Test: A validation study. Psychiatr. Q. 2014, 85, 187–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Chong, N.; Isa, S.M.; Hashim, A.H.; Kumar, S.; Harbajan, M.K. Validity of the Malay version of the internet addiction test: A study on a group of medical students in Malaysia. Asia-Pac. J. Public Heath 2015, 27, NP2210–NP2219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Fernández-Villa, T.; Molina, A.J.; García-Martín, M.; Llorca, J.; Delgado-Rodríguez, M.; Martín, V. Validation and psychometric analysis of the Internet Addiction Test in Spanish among college students. BMC Public Health 2015, 15, 953. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Lu, X.; Yeo, K.J. Psychometric properties of the Internet Addiction Test in a sample of Malaysian undergraduate students. Psicol. Educ. 2015, 21, 17–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Dhir, A.; Chen, S.; Nieminen, M. Psychometric validation of Internet Addiction Test with Indian adolescents. J. Educ. Comput. Res. 2015, 53, 15–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Lai, C.M.; Mak, K.K.; Cheng, C.; Watanabe, H.; Nomachi, S.; Bahar, N.; Young, K.S.; Ko, H.C.; Kim, D.; Griffiths, M.D. Measurement invariance of the Internet Addiction Test among Hong Kong, Japanese, and Malaysian adolescents. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2015, 18, 609–617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  47. Fioravanti, G.; Casale, S. Evaluation of the psychometric properties of the Italian internet addiction test. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2015, 18, 120–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Hawi, N.S.; Blachnio, A.; Przepiorka, A. Polish validation of the Internet Addiction Test. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2015, 48, 548–553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Kaya, F.; Delen, E.; Young, K.S. Psychometric properties of the Internet Addiction Test in Turkish. J. Behav. Addict. 2016, 5, 130–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Servidio, R. Assessing the psychometric properties of the Internet Addiction Test: A study on a sample of Italian university students. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2017, 68, 17–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Boysan, M.; Kuss, D.J.; Barut, Y.; Ayköse, N.; Güleç, M.; Özdemir, O. Psychometric properties of the Turkish version of the Internet Addiction Test (IAT). Addict. Behav. 2017, 64, 247–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Samaha, A.A.; Fawaz, M.; El Yahfoufi, N.; Gebbawi, M.; Abdallah, H.; Baydoun, S.A.; Ghaddar, A.; Eid, A.H. Assessing the psychometric properties of the Internet Addiction Test (IAT) among Lebanese college students. Front. Public Health 2018, 6, 365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Waqas, A.; Farooq, F.; Raza, M.; Javed, S.T.; Khan, S.; Ghumman, M.E.; Naveed, S.; Haddad, M. Validation of the Internet Addiction Test in students at a Pakistani medical and dental school. Psychiatr. Q. 2018, 89, 235–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Neelapaijit, A.; Pinyopornpanish, M.; Simcharoen, S.; Kuntawong, P.; Wongpakaran, N.; Wongpakaran, T. Psychometric properties of a Thai version Internet Addiction Test. BMC Res. Notes 2018, 11, 69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  55. Tsermentseli, S.; Karipidis, N.; Samaras, P.; Thompson, T. Assessing the factorial structure of the Internet Addiction Test in a sample of Greek adolescents. Hell. J. Psychol. 2018, 15, 274–288. [Google Scholar]
  56. Hernández, C.; Rivera, D. A Chilean transcultural adaptation and assessment of the Internet Addiction Test’s factor structures: Development of a brief version. Rev. Iberoam. Diag. Eval. 2018, 49, 143–155. [Google Scholar]
  57. Černja, I.; Vejmelka, L.; Rajter, M. Internet Addiction Test: Croatian preliminary study. BMC Psychiatry 2019, 19, 388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Tudorel, O.I.; Vintilă, M.; Vlaicu, L.; Bălăuță, D.; Goian, C.; Rusu, A. Romanian version of the Internet Addiction Test: Psychometric properties and cross-gender invariance. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 2019, 17, 234–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Ndasauka, Y.; Pitafi, A.; Kayange, G.M. Psychometric properties of Young’s Internet Addiction Test (IAT) in Urdu language. Asian J. Psychiatr. 2019, 40, 39–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Yaffe, Y.; Seroussi, D.-E. Further evidence for the psychometric properties of young’s Internet Addiction Test (IAT): A study on a sample of Israeli-Arab male adolescents. Am. J. Health Behav. 2019, 43, 1030–1039. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Talwar, P.; Mohamad, F.S.; Mustaffa, M.S.; Othman, M.K.; Bee, O.Y.; Hian, C.C. Internet addiction among university students: Psychometric properties of three factor model Internet Addiction Test. Online J. Health Allied Sci. 2019, 18, 1–7. [Google Scholar]
  62. Lu, X.; Yeo, K.J.; Guo, F.; Zhao, Z. Factor structure and a multiple indicators multiple cause model of Internet Addiction Test: The effect of socio-demographic and internet use variables. Curr. Psychol. 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Raykov, T.; Marcoulides, G.A. Thanks coefficient alpha, we still need you! Educ. Psychol. Meas. 2019, 79, 200–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Viladrich, C.; Angulo-Brunet, A.; Doval, E. A journey around alpha and omega to estimate internal consistency reliability. Psicoogia 2017, 33, 755–782. [Google Scholar]
  65. Moon, S.J.; Hwang, J.S.; Kim, J.Y.; Shin, A.L.; Bae, S.M.; Kim, J.W. Psychometric properties of the Internet Addiction Test: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2018, 21, 473–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Marcus, C.; Peter, H. Questionable research practices when using confirmatory factor analysis. J. Manag. Psychol. 2019, 34, 18–30. [Google Scholar]
  67. Sellbom, M.; Tellegen, A. Factor analysis in psychological assessment research: Common pitfalls and recommendations. Psychol. Assess. 2019, 31, 1428–1441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Matalinares, M.; Raymundo, O.; Baca, D. Psychometric adaptation of the Test of Internet Addiction. Rev. Peru. Psicol. Trab. Soc. 2014, 3, 45–66. [Google Scholar]
  69. Wang, L.; Luo, J.; Gao, W.; Kong, J. The effect of Internet use on adolescents’ lifestyles: A national survey. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2012, 28, 2007–2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Zegarra, C.O.; Cuba, M.S. Frequency of Internet addiction and development of social skills in adolescents in an urban area of Lima. Medwave 2017, 17, e6857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Torrente, E.; Piqueras, J.A.; Orgilés, M.; Espada, J.P. Association of Internet addiction with social anxiety and lack of social skills in spanish adolescents. Ter. Psicol. 2014, 32, 175–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  72. Chou, W.; Huang, M.; Chang, Y.; Chen, Y.; Hu, H.; Yen, C. Social skills deficits and their association with Internet addiction and activities in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Behav. Addict. 2017, 6, 42–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. American Educational Research Association; American Psychological Association; National Council on Measurement in Education. Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing; American Educational Research Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2014; ISBN 978-0935-302-356. [Google Scholar]
  74. Ato, M.; López, J.J.; Benavente, A. A classification system for research designs in psychology. Psicologia 2013, 29, 1038–1059. [Google Scholar]
  75. Zickar, M.J. Measurement development and evaluation. Annu. Rev. Organ. Psychol. Organ. Behav. 2020, 7, 213–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  76. Kerlinger, F.N.; Lee, H.B. Foundations of Behavioral Research, 4th ed.; Harcourt College Publishers: Fort Worth, TX, USA, 2000; ISBN 9780155078970. [Google Scholar]
  77. Turner, D.P.; Houle, T.T. The importance of statistical power calculations. Headache J. Head Face Pain 2018, 58, 1187–1191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Ferguson, C.J. An effect size primer: A guide for clinicians and researchers. Prof. Psychol. Res. Pr. 2009, 40, 532–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1988; ISBN 0-8058-0283-5. [Google Scholar]
  80. Gismero, E. EHS: Escala de Habilidades Sociales; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 2000; ISBN 84-7174-609-3. [Google Scholar]
  81. Miranda-Zapata, E.; Riquelme-Mella, E.; Cifuentes-Cid, H.; Riquelme-Bravo, P. Análisis factorial confrmatorio de la Escala de habilidades sociales en universitarios chilenos. Rev. Lat. Psicol. 2014, 46, 73–82. [Google Scholar]
  82. Tabachnick, B.G.; Fidell, L.S. Using Multivariate Statistics, 7th ed.; Pearson: New York, NY, USA, 2019; ISBN 9780134792866. [Google Scholar]
  83. Meyer, J.P. Applied Measurement with Jmetrik; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2014; ISBN 978-0-203-11519-0. [Google Scholar]
  84. McHorney, C.A.; Tarlov, A.R. Individual-patient monitoring in clinical practice: Are available health status surveys adequate? Qual. Life Res. 1995, 293–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Li, C.-H. Confirmatory factor analysis with ordinal data: Comparing robust maximum likelihood and diagonally weighted least squares. Behav. Res. Methods 2016, 48, 936–949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  86. Keith, T.Z. Multiple Regression and Beyond: An Introduction to Multiple Regression and Structural Equation Modeling, 3rd ed.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2019; ISBN 9781351667937. [Google Scholar]
  87. DiStefano, C.; Liu, J.; Jiang, N.; Shi, D. Examination of the Weighted Root Mean Square Residual: Evidence for trustworthiness? Struct. Equ. Model. A Multidiscip. J. 2018, 25, 453–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Wilcox, R.R.; Rousselet, G.A.; Pernet, C.R. Improved methods for making inferences about multiple skipped correlations. J. Stat. Comput. Simul. 2018, 88, 3116–3131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  89. Zumbo, B.D.; Gadermann, A.M.; Zeisser, C. Ordinal versions of coefficients alpha and theta for Likert rating scales. J. Mod. Appl. Stat. Method 2007, 6, 21–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Nunnally, J.; Bernstein, I. Psychometric Theory, 3rd ed.; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 1994; ISBN 9780070478497. [Google Scholar]
  91. Erdfelder, E.; Faul, F.; Buchner, A.; Lang, A.-G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 2009, 41, 1149–1160. [Google Scholar]
  92. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2020; Available online: https://www.r-project.org (accessed on 9 June 2020).
  93. Rinker, T.W.; Kurkiewicz, D. Pacman: Package Management for R. 2018. Available online: http://github.com/trinker/pacman (accessed on 9 June 2020).
  94. Hadley, W.; Bryan, J. Readxl: Read Excel Files. 2019. Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/package=readxl (accessed on 9 June 2020).
  95. Wickham, H.; Averick, M.; Bryan, J.; Chang, W.; Mcgowan, L.D.A.; François, R.; Grolemund, G.; Hayes, A.; Henry, L.; Hester, J.; et al. Welcome to the tidyverse. J. Open Source Softw. 2019, 4, 1686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Revelle, W. Psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research. 2019. Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/package=psych (accessed on 9 June 2020).
  97. Rosseel, Y. Lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. J. Stat. Softw. 2012, 48, 1–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  98. Dueber, D. BifactorIndicesCalculator: Bifactor Indices Calculator. 2020. Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/package=BifactorIndicesCalculator (accessed on 9 June 2020).
  99. Jorgensen, T.D.; Pornprasertmanit, S.; Schoemann, A.M.; Rosseel, Y. SemTools: Useful tools for structural equation modeling. 2020. Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/package=semTools (accessed on 9 June 2020).
  100. Wilcox, R.R.; Schönbrodt, F.D. The WRS Package for Robust Statistics in R. 2019. Available online: https://github.com/nicebread/WRS (accessed on 9 June 2020).
  101. Dominguez-Lara, S.; Rodriguez, A. Statistical indices from bifactor models. Interacciones 2017, 3, 59–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  102. Rodriguez, A.; Reise, S.P.; Haviland, M.G. Evaluating bifactor models: Calculating and interpreting statistical indices. Psychol. Methods 2016, 21, 137–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Tavakol, M.; Dennick, R. Making sense of Cronbach’s alpha. Int. J. Med. Educ. 2011, 2, 53–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Conti, M.A.; Pelegrino, A.; Hearst, N.; Athanássios, T.; Tavares, H.; Nabuco de Abreu, C. Evaluation of semantic equivalence and internal consistency of a Portuguese version of the Internet Addiction Test (IAT). Rev. Psiquiatr. Clin. 2012, 39, 106–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. A priori statistical power analysis to determine the minimum recommended sample size.
Figure 1. A priori statistical power analysis to determine the minimum recommended sample size.
Ijerph 17 05782 g001
Figure 2. Factorial structure of the bifactor model.
Figure 2. Factorial structure of the bifactor model.
Ijerph 17 05782 g002
Table 1. Previous studies on psychometric properties of the Internet Addiction Test (IAT).
Table 1. Previous studies on psychometric properties of the Internet Addiction Test (IAT).
StudyCountrySample SizeFactorsData AnalysisItemsReliability (α)
1. Widyanto et al. (2004) [24]-866PCA20>0.50 c
2. Ngai (2007) [25]Hong Kong9884PCA20>0.60 c
3. Chang et al. (2008) [26]Hong Kong4103PCA, CFA a20>0.80 d
4. Khazaal et al. (2008) [27]France2461EFA, CFA200.93
5. Widyanto et al. (2011) [28]-2253PCA20-
6. Panayides et al. (2012) [29]Cyprus6041PCA200.89
7. Jelenchick et al. (2012) [30]United States2152EFA20>0.80 c
8. Barke et al. (2012) [31]Germany8412PCA, CFA200.89
9. Puerta-Cortés et al. (2012) [32]Colombia11173PCA200.89
10. Faraci et al. (2013) [33]Italy4851, 2EFA, CFA20, 18>0.70 c
11. Watters et al. (2013) [34]Canada19482CFA b200.93
12. Pawlikowski et al. (2013) [35]Germany10492PCA, CFA11>0.80 c
13. Lee et al. (2013) [36]Korea2794PCA200.91
14. Hawi (2013) [37]Lebanon8171PCA, CFA200.92
15. Lai et al. (2013) [38]Hong Kong8443CFA a200.93
16. Pontes et al. (2014) [39]Portugal5931CFA200.90
17. Karim et al. (2014) [40]Bangladesh1774PCA180.89
18. Tsimtsiou et al. (2014) [41]Greece1513EFA200.91
19. Chong et al. (2015) [42]Malaysia1625PCA200.91
20. Fernández-Villa et al. (2015) [43]Spain9632EFA, CFA190.91
21. Lu et al. (2015) [44]Malaysia1046EFA200.93 e
22. Dhir et al. (2015) [45]India19141EFA, CFA200.88
23. Lai et al. (2015) [46]Hong Kong, Japan and Malaysia25353CFA20-
24. Fioravanti et al. (2015) [47]Italy8402EFA, CFA20>0.80 c
25. Hawi (2015) [48]Poland12452PCA, CFA200.90
26. Kaya (2016) [49]Turkey9904EFA, CFA200.92
27. Servidio (2017) [50]Italy6592PCA, CFA180.89
28. Boysan et al. (2017) [51]Turkey4551PCA, CFA200.93
29. Samaha et al. (2018) [52]Lebanon2564EFA, CFA190.91
30. Waqas et al. (2018) [53]Pakistan5221EFA, CFA200.90
31. Neelapaijit et al. (2018) [54]Thailand3243EFA, CFA200.89
32. Tsermentseli et al. (2018) [55]Greece7253EFA, CFA b19>0.70 f
33. Hernández et al. (2018) [56]Chile4252CFA100.85
34. Černja et al. (2019) [57]Croatia3523PCA, CFA200.91
35. Tudorel et al. (2019) [58]Romania4212EFA, CFA200.86
36. Ndasauka et al. (2019) [59]Pakistan5064EFA200.88
37. Yaffe et al. (2019) [60]Israel1802PCA, CFA18>0.70 c
38. Talwar et al. (2019) [61]Malaysia3073PCA, CFA19>0.70 c
39. Lu et al. (2019) [62]Malaysia11204EFA, CFA17-
Note. a Hierarchical model; b Bifactor model; c α coefficients of the factors; d Construct reliability; e Rasch model (person reliability); f ω coefficients of the factors; PCA = Principal Component Analysis; EFA = Exploratory Factor Analysis; CFA = Confirmatory Factor Analysis.
Table 2. Sociodemographic characteristics of participants (n = 227).
Table 2. Sociodemographic characteristics of participants (n = 227).
Characteristicn%
Gender
Male9742.70
Female13057.30
Year of study
First2410.60
Second6830.00
Third6327.80
Fourth4218.50
Fifth3013.20
Academic discipline
Health Sciences4118.10
Humanities3013.20
Social Sciences2711.90
Basic sciences4318.90
Engineering4519.80
Economic-Business4118.10
Table 3. Item analysis for the Internet Addiction Test (IAT).
Table 3. Item analysis for the Internet Addiction Test (IAT).
ItemMSDSkKuItem-Rest CorrelationFloor (%)Ceiling (%)
Item 13.0841.2000.115−0.9890.378816
Item 22.0660.9020.7340.3750.570281
Item 31.8851.1461.1180.2030.381534
Item 41.6700.8261.3291.8820.381511
Item 51.9251.0211.0920.7750.560423
Item 61.8280.9081.0130.4370.532430
Item 73.0841.2110.168−1.0160.371717
Item 81.7970.8890.9680.1630.591450
Item 91.6560.8601.5062.4040.471531
Item 101.7530.9131.0240.2450.465510
Item 111.8550.8980.798−0.0720.496430
Item 121.6260.9001.4891.8680.451591
Item 131.7710.9601.2751.2000.527502
Item 141.8771.0061.0530.4420.515452
Item 151.5020.7721.5712.2510.684640
Item 162.2861.0940.8330.1490.443246
Item 172.0931.0660.9060.1380.558333
Item 181.7361.0001.4151.4090.538542
Item 191.4140.7962.2755.4990.626721
Item 201.3520.6582.1765.6400.512730
Note. M = Mean; SD = Standard Deviation; Sk = Skewness; Ku = Kurtosis.
Table 4. Confirmatory factor analysis for the IAT.
Table 4. Confirmatory factor analysis for the IAT.
ModelSSχ2dfSSχ2/dfRMSEA (90% CI)CFITLISRMRWRMR
1. One-factor [29,45,51,53]387.2851612.4050.079 (0.069; 0.089)0.8900.8710.0921.168
2. Khazaal et al. (2008) [27]449.3571692.6590.086 (0.076; 0.095)0.8640.8470.1011.288
3. Widyanto et al. (2011) [28]423.7791672.3580.082 (0.073; 0.092)0.8760.8590.0951.244
4. Jelenchick et al. (2012) [30]393.8961692.3310.077 (0.067; 0.087)0.8910.8780.0921.189
5. Barke et al. (2012) [31]382.2151672.2890.076 (0.066; 0.086)0.8960.8810.0911.163
6. Watters et al. (2013) [34]285.7411541.8550.062 (0.050; 0.073)0.9360.9210.0730.939
7. Hawi (2013) [37]462.2051662.7840.089 (0.079; 0.099)0.8570.8360.1021.311
8. Lee et al. (2013) [36]340.2371642.0750.069 (0.059; 0.079)0.9150.9010.0841.086
9. Pontes et al. (2014) [39]437.2161682.6020.084 (0.075; 0.094)0.8700.8530.0991.267
10. Tsimtsiou et al. (2014) [41]375.2811672.2470.074 (0.064; 0.084)0.8990.8850.0901.156
11. Fioravanti et al. (2015) [47]390.0651652.3640.078 (0.068; 0.088)0.8910.8750.0931.182
12. Hawi (2015) [48]342.8711692.0290.067 (0.057; 0.078)0.9160.9050.0851.097
13. Dhir et al. (2015) [45]437.2351662.6340.085 (0.075; 0.095)0.8690.8500.0991.266
14. Waqas et al. (2018) [53]475.7091702.7980.089 (0.080; 0.099)0.8520.8350.1041.336
15. Tudorel et al. (2019) [58]388.4491672.3260.077 (0.067; 0.087)0.8930.8780.0921.176
16. Černja et al. (2019) [57]399.8181672.3940.079 (0.069; 0.088)0.8870.8720.0931.200
Note. RMSEA = Root Mean Square Error of Approximation; CI = Confidence Interval; CFI = Comparative Fit Index; TLI = Tucker–Lewis Index; SRMR = Standardized Root Mean Square Residual; WRMR = Weighted Root Mean Square Residual.
Table 5. Interfactorial matrix, convergent evidence, and reliability of the IAT.
Table 5. Interfactorial matrix, convergent evidence, and reliability of the IAT.
VariableTime/ControlStress/CompensateInternet Addiction
1. Time/Control-
2. Stress/Compensate0.400 ***-
3. Internet Addiction0.804 ***0.861 ***-
4. Self-Expression−0.117−0.197 **−0.331 ***
5. Defense of rights−0.185 **−0.189 **−0.350 ***
6. Disagreement−0.055−0.205 **−0.311 ***
7. Assertiveness−0.054−0.189 **−0.196 **
8. Making requests0.019−0.029−0.118
9. Starting interactions−0.1270.058−0.118
10. Social Skills−0.137 *−0.186 **−0.257 ***
11. Hours on the Internet0.397 ***0.1120.337 ***
12. Ordinal Alpha0.7270.8560.888
Note. * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Tafur-Mendoza, A.A.; Acosta-Prado, J.C.; Zárate-Torres, R.A.; Ramírez-Ospina, D.E. Assessing the Psychometric Properties of the Internet Addiction Test in Peruvian University Students. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 5782. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17165782

AMA Style

Tafur-Mendoza AA, Acosta-Prado JC, Zárate-Torres RA, Ramírez-Ospina DE. Assessing the Psychometric Properties of the Internet Addiction Test in Peruvian University Students. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020; 17(16):5782. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17165782

Chicago/Turabian Style

Tafur-Mendoza, Arnold Alejandro, Julio César Acosta-Prado, Rodrigo Arturo Zárate-Torres, and Duván Emilio Ramírez-Ospina. 2020. "Assessing the Psychometric Properties of the Internet Addiction Test in Peruvian University Students" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 16: 5782. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17165782

APA Style

Tafur-Mendoza, A. A., Acosta-Prado, J. C., Zárate-Torres, R. A., & Ramírez-Ospina, D. E. (2020). Assessing the Psychometric Properties of the Internet Addiction Test in Peruvian University Students. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(16), 5782. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17165782

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop