Next Article in Journal
Chemical Impurities: An Epistemological Riddle with Serious Side Effects
Previous Article in Journal
Cyberaggression in Adolescents of Bolivia: Connection with Psychopathological Symptoms, Adaptive and Predictor Variables
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Hypothetical Interventions on Risk Factors for Cognitive Impairment among Chinese Older Adults: An Application of the Parametric G-Formula

1
State Key Laboratory of Molecular Vaccine and Molecular Diagnostics, School of Public Affairs and School of Public Health, Xiamen University, Xiamen 361102, China
2
School of Public Health, Key Laboratory of Health Technology Assessment of Fujian Province University, Xiamen University, Xiamen 361102, China
3
School of Public Health, Xiamen University, Xiamen 361102, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17(3), 1021; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17031021
Submission received: 7 January 2020 / Revised: 3 February 2020 / Accepted: 5 February 2020 / Published: 6 February 2020

Abstract

:
The effects of psychosocial and dietary interventions on risk of cognitive impairment is not known. The aim of this study was to estimate the 10-year risks of cognitive impairment under hypothetical interventions of psychosocial factors and dietary intake among Chinese older adults. A sample of 7377 respondents aged 65 and over was drawn from the last four waves of the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey from 2002 to 2011/2012. The parametric g-formula was used to estimate the risk of cognitive impairment under independent hypothetical interventions of social engagement, psychological well-being (PWB), dietary intake, and the joint interventions of their different combination. The observed risk of cognitive impairment was 20.08% (95% confidence interval (CI): 18.81, 21.07). The risk ratios (RR) of cognitive impairment under the hypothetical interventions on higher social engagement, eating fruits at least sometimes, eating vegetables at least sometimes, positive PWB were 0.72 (95% CI: 0.65, 0.82), 0.93 (95% CI: 0.89, 0.95), 0.98 (95% CI: 0.89, 1.00) and 0.99 (95% CI: 0.98, 0.99), respectively. The RR of joint intervention was 0.64 (95% CI: 0.58, 0.73). Hypothetical interventions on psychosocial factors such as social engagement and PWB, as well as fruits and vegetable intake, were beneficial to protect older adults from cognitive impairment.

1. Introduction

With the largest number of older persons in the world, the aging process of China is accelerative because of the three-decade one-child policy and longer life expectancy. Elderly people’s cognitive function declines with increasing age [1,2]. The prevalence of cognitive impairment, including mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia, increases with advancing age [3]. MCI represents the transition between the state of normal cognition and dementia [3]. A meta-analysis in China reported that the pooled prevalence of MCI for Chinese older adults was 12.7%, and it was 9.6% and 14.7% respectively in eastern and western China [4]. The number of older adults living with dementia is expected to increase from 9.6 million in 2010 to more than 23.3 million by 2030 [5]. Cognitive impairment severely erodes functioning, results in memory loss, decreased exercise flexibility, unresponsiveness, and so on, which affect the elderly’s quality of life seriously and create burden and stress on families [6]. Those patients who were in a late period of cognitive impairment and did not receive sufficient therapy lost activity ability and memory, followed by death. Dementia confers a huge burden on families and society in China. The costs of dementia will increase from US$47.2 billion in 2010 to US$114.2 billion by 2030 [5]. There has been no curative therapy established since research investigating the treatment of dementia has been performed worldwide for more than 20 years [7]. There is no doubt that cognitive impairment is one of the largest social care and public health challenges facing people. Interventions to delay the onset of cognitive impairment become a public health priority.
A considerable number of prospective observational studies have explored risk factors for cognitive impairment, including socio-economic characteristics, biological factors, and life behavior factors. There are limited studies on the risk factors of cognitive impairment among old adults focused on psychosocial factors [8,9] and dietary intake [10]. A systematic review showed that older adults with poor social relationships and psychological distress had an increased risk of cognitive decline or cognitive impairment [11,12,13]. Recent studies showed that older adults with higher social engagement had a lower risk of cognitive impairment but the relationship varies across different domains of social engagement [14,15]. An increased intake of vegetables is associated with slower rates of cognitive decline and decreased dementia risk among older adults [16]. These findings make it conceivable that psychosocial factors and dietary intake may play an important role in delaying the incident cognitive impairment.
Cognitive interventions at the preclinical phases could be beneficial in delaying the onset of cognitive impairment [17,18,19]. Randomized control trials (RCTs) have offered some short-term impact of interventions that can improve cognitive function such as dance [20], exercise [21], physical activities [22], and psychological intervention [23]. RCTs for long-term effects of psychosocial and dietary factors were lacking because of some practical reasons such as cost, loss to follow up, and inoperability. Moreover, due to the selective enrolment of participants into trials, the conclusions from RCTs may not always be generalized to other studies or population [24].
Therefore, the aims of this study were to estimate the 10-year risks of cognitive impairment under independent hypothetical interventions of social engagement, psychological well-being (PWB), dietary intake, and the joint interventions of their different combination by using the parametric g-formula, and compare different intervention schemes to thus provide intervention strategies among Chinese older adults aged 65 and above, using a nationally representative cohort of the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey (CLHLS).

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Data

Data were drawn from a nationally representative study of community-dwelling older adults in China, the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey (CLHLS). The CLHLS began in 1998 and covered counties and cities in 22 of the 31 provinces in China by using a stratified cluster sampling method, representing approximately 85% of the Chinese population. The follow-up surveys were conducted in 2000, 2002, 2005, 2008/2009, and 2011/2012. CLHLS accumulated comprehensive information of the elderly via face-to-face interviews in China, including demographics, health status and functioning, cognition testing, lifestyle and health-related behaviors, activities of daily living (ADL), and so on. Details about the study design, sampling, and data quality of the CLHLS are fully reported elsewhere [25]. The last four waves of data from 2002 to 2011/2012 were used in the current study, since 2002 (as “baseline”) was the first wave to expand the age of participants from 80 to 65 years and older. The 2002 wave of the CLHLS included 16,064 respondents aged 65 years and over. At the same time, only the respondents who completed at least one follow-up interview were included to confirm the data’s quality. Finally, a total sample of 7377 respondents aged 65 years and over was produced (Figure 1). For each participant, the follow-up was ended at the time of onset of cognitive impairment, death, loss to follow, or in the examination of wave 2011/2012, whichever occurred first.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Outcome Variable

A Chinese revised version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) was used to assess the cognitive function of participants [26]. The MMSE in CLHLS, initially modified to reflect the socioeconomic and cultural context in China, has been found to be reliable and validated [27,28,29]. Adjusting the scores of cognition function according to age and education level, the respondents were considered as cognitive impaired if their MMSE scores were below the mean scores minus 1.5 times the standard deviation [30,31].

2.2.2. Intervention Variables

To be compatible with previous studies [32,33], social engagement was assessed using three categorical data on marital status (married/unmarried), living arrangement (living alone/living with others), social activity (yes/no), and two questions “Is there anyone you can talk to when you have something in mind?” (yes/no), and “Is there anyone can give you a hand when you are in trouble?” (yes/no) [25]. Each item was coded as 1 or 0. Social engagement scores ranged from 0–5; scores 0–3 were regarded as the low frequency in social engagement according to the frequency distribution, and scores 4–5 indicated the high frequency [34].
Six items were used to generate two indices, that have been documented in prior studies [35,36,37], representing the psychological well-being (PWB) among older adults, one for negative psychological well-being, and the other for positive psychological well-being. The items for negative PWB were as follows: “Do you feel the older you get the more useless you are?”, “Do you often feel lonely and isolated?”, and “Do you often feel fearful or anxious?” Five response options (always, often, sometimes, seldom, and never) were given. The three items for positive PWB were: “Are you as happy now as when you were younger?”, “Do you always look on the bright side of things?”, and “How do you think of your life at present?” Five response options (very good, good, so-so, bad, and very bad) were given for the three items. The scores ranged from 5 (very good or always) to 1 (very bad or never). Both positive PWB and negative PWB ranged from 3–15, PWB scores (−12~+12) were calculated by using positive PWB scores minus negative PWB scores [38]. The Cronbach’s alpha for the PWB was α = 0.71, which implied internal consistency. A higher score on this measure indicated a better level of PWB.
Dietary information was collected with the food frequency questionnaires. Considering the consistency of the survey waves, four common categories of food were included: vegetables, fruits, meat, and fish. The intake frequency of each food was converted to 3 categories based on the 2002 wave: very often or every day, sometimes, and seldom or never.

2.2.3. Covariates

Baseline variables included age, gender, education level (illiteracy/literacy), and occupation before 65 years old (farmer/others). Age was categorized into 65–74, 75–84, 85–94, and ≥95. Our primary analysis included 6 time-varying variables: (1) residence (rural/urban); (2) exercise (yes/no); (3) smoking (never/quitted/smoking); (4) drinking (never/quitted/drinking); (5) chronic diseases (hypertension, diabetes, stroke, and cerebrovascular disease (CVD)); and (6) activity of daily living. If the respondents could not complete bathing, dressing, toileting, transferring, continence, and feeding all independently by themselves, they were regarded as disabled.

2.3. Hypothetical Interventions on Risk Factors for Cognitive Impairment

The parametric g-formula was used to estimate the risk of cognitive impairment under each of the following hypothetical interventions.
  • Social engagement: every participant with low social engagement was intervened on high social engagement, and all those with high social engagement were not intervened on.
  • Psychological well-being: everyone with negative PWB scores was intervened on positive scores, and the others were not intervened on.
  • Dietary intake: the respondents who seldom ate those 4 categories of food were intervened on to increase their frequency to sometimes eat those foods. The entire population was divided into two categories: every day and sometimes; (3) a: vegetables; (3) b: fruits; (3) c: meat; and (3) d: fish.
  • Interventions (1) and (2) combined.
  • Interventions (3) a and (3) b combined.
  • Interventions (3) a, (3) b, and (1) combined.
  • Interventions (3) a, (3) b, and (2) combined.
  • Interventions (3) a, (3) b, and (1)–(2) combined.

2.4. Statistical Analyses

Ten-years risks of cognitive impairment under the hypothetical interventions were approximately estimated by applying the parametric g-formula, which can be used to consistently estimate the effect of hypothetical intervention under the assumption that time-dependent covariates been correctly assessed at all waves.
  • Pooled regression models were used to predict the risk of cognitive impairments, each risk factor and risk of loss to follow-up or death respectively, given prior risk factor history for each 3 years period between 2002 and 2011/2012. A Kaplan–Meier estimator was used to incorporate censoring owing to death and loss to follow-up.
  • Based on these estimated models, pseudo-cohorts under each of the interventions were generated by a Monte Carlo simulation of 10,000 individuals, using the following steps:
    (a)
    The time-varying confounders at the next time point were simulated by the estimated regression models under the intervention of interest.
    (b)
    The risk of cognitive impairment and censor were simulated at the next time point and were simulated by estimated regression models in step (1) and the simulated covariates in step (2)a.
  • To predict the risk of cognitive impairment under each selected intervention, step (2) should be repeated for the entire duration of follow-up.
The population risk ratio was calculated by comparing the simulated risks under various interventions with the risk under no intervention. The 95% confidence intervals (CI) were estimated by nonparametric bootstrapping with 500 samples. Subgroup analyses were done for individuals aged ≥80 years old versus <80 years old at baseline, for male and female and for individuals with education levels of illiteracy and literacy.
Sensitivity analyses were also conducted. First, the order of the included variables was changed in the model. Second, different interaction items were tried, such as social engagement and PWB, dietary intake and social engagement and PWB, educational level and social engagement and PWB. Third, in our original research, all the covariates were included at one period back as the predictors in the parametric estimation. The analyses were conducted again using a two-period lagged model in parametric estimation. Finally, the risk ratio (RR) values were calculated when the control variables included in the model were quadratic rather than linear. All analyses were conducted using SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA).

3. Results

3.1. Baseline Characteristics

The baseline characteristics of the 7377 participants are depicted in Table 1, of whom 1014 participants developed cognitive impairment during the 10-year follow-up period. Over 50% of respondents were aged in the 65~ and 75~ years groups. Approximately 54% of respondents were women and over half of the respondents were illiterate (57.2%) and farmers (59.2%). The mean score of PWB was 4.39 and the proportion of high frequency of social engagement was 51.9%. The mean MMSE score was 26.11. The prevalence of cognitive impairment in participants aged 80 and over and in the western region was much higher than older adults aged less than 80 years old and in the eastern region. Meanwhile, the prevalence rate of female older adults (8.23%–11.89%) was higher than male older adults (7.41%–8.66%). Not surprisingly, illiterate elderly had a higher prevalence rate than those who were literate.
The mean of the variables of simulated data was closely related to the data under no intervention, especially in 2005 (data not shown). For example, the mean differences between the simulated and observed number of cognitive impairment prevalence rate were <0.1 during the three follow-ups.

3.2. Single Interventions

As presented in Table 2, the 10-year risks of cognitive impairment under 6 single hypothetical interventions were estimated by g-formula. The estimated 10-year cognitive impairment risk under no intervention was 20.08% (95% CI: 18.81, 21.07). The most effective intervention in our study was when all participants were intervened on social engagement (RR = 0.72, 95% CI: 0.65, 0.82). The other interventions were as follows: eating fruits at least sometimes: RR = 0.93, 95% CI: 0.89, 0.95; eating vegetables at least sometimes: RR = 0.98, 95% CI: 0.89, 1.00; and those with minus PWB scores were intervened on positive scores: RR = 0.97, 95% CI: 0.98, 0.99. However, hypothetical interventions of both meat and fish intake could not reduce the risk of cognitive impairment significantly.
The subgroup analyses showed evidence of a difference in independent intervention stratified by age group (see Table S1), gender (see Table S2), and education level (see Table S3). The cognitive impairment risk of young old age is lower than the risk of oldest old age. The PWB intervention has a significant effect on young old age (RR = 0.94, 95% CI = 0.93–0.98), while it has no significant effect on oldest old age, but can reduce the risk of cognitive impairment in those of oldest old age. The social engagement and fruits are protective intervention in two age groups. Men are generally less likely to develop cognitive impairment disease than women. There are no other differences between male and female under single intervention. The PWB (RR = 0.96, 95% CI = 0.93–0.98) and eating vegetables (RR = 0.98, 95% CI = 0.97–1.00) can only protect illiterate older adults.

3.3. Joint Interventions

Table 3 presents the results of the analyses for joint interventions after excluding two interventions, meat and fish intake, which were insignificant in the analysis of single intervention. All the combinations can reduce the cognitive impairment risk of older adults significantly. The 5th complicated intervention, which included social engagement, PWB, and vegetable and fruits intake sometimes, reduced the risk the most (RR = 0.64, 95% CI = 0.58–0.73), followed by the interventions that combined diet and social engagement (RR = 0.66, 95% CI = 0.59–0.75), social engagement combined with PWB (RR = 0.70, 95% CI = 0.64–0.80), vegetables and fruits intake at least sometimes combined with PWB (RR = 0.89, 95% CI = 0.86–0.92), and vegetable and fruit intake at least sometimes RR = 0.92, 95% CI = 0.89–0.95. Our sensitivity analysis, which tested whether the order of the variables included and the different model settings could affect the results, showed that the results were robust (sensitivity analysis results not shown).

4. Discussion

We estimated the cognitive impairment risk of the elderly with results under various hypothetical psychosocial and dietary intake interventions by applying the g-formula to the nationally representative cohort of CLHLS. To our knowledge, this is the first attempt to investigate interventions on cognitive impairment using the parametric g-formula among older adults. We found that social engagement, PWB, and vegetable and fruit intake were associated with decreased cognitive impairment risk, both independently and jointly. Stronger effects of PWB on cognitive impairment risk were observed among those < 80 years old compared with those ≥80 years old and among those who are illiterate compared with those who are literate.
The results from our hypothetical interventions of social engagement on cognitive impairment, which could reduce the 10-year elderly population risk of cognitive impairment by 28%, are in line with those of former studies. A recent meta-analysis concluded that older adults with multiple aspects of social relationships had a decreased risk of cognitive decline [11]. Prospective studies found that older adults with higher social engagement had a lower risk of cognitive impairment and that those with a high frequency of emotional support had better cognition function [9,34,39]. ‘Use it or lose it’ theory suggests that social engagement stimulates the brain, better social relationships may delay the onset of cognitive impairment among older adults [40]. A decrease in social engagement may lead to the disuse of the brain which in turn may accelerate the process of cognitive decline [40].
We intervened on older adults with negative PWB to bring them to a balanced condition. Our finding of significant risk reduction for cognitive impairment by PWB is consistent with the previous evidence relating PWB to cognitive impairment. The prior study also concluded that higher levels of psychological well-being were associated with better cognitive functions [41]. Several explanations could be applied to interpret the significant association between PWB and cognitive impairment. First, older adults with better PWB may be more likely to maintain healthy habits and prudent lifestyles, such as physical activity, which are important protective factors against cognitive decline. Second, it was suggested by the stress-buffering hypothesis that the association between PWB and cognitive impairment might be mediated through stress, positive well-being might buffer against stress, which in turn resulted in cognitive decline due to structural changes in the hippocampus [1].
We found that the consumption of vegetables and fruits could protect older adults from the onset of cognitive impairment and the effects of fruits were better than the effects of vegetables. There are considerable studies showing that older adults with frequent consumption of vegetables had a lower risk of cognitive impairment [16,42]. A meta-analysis concluded that the increased consumption of vegetables and fruit had positive effects on preventing cognitive impairment or dementia [43]. However, former studies found the protective effect of frequent fruit consumption appears weaker than that for vegetable [16], which was different from our study, and might be due to eating habits of the Chinese elderly. There are a variety of explanations by which the frequent vegetables and fruits intakes may be associated with decreased risk of cognitive impairment, and many of these are associated with processes that are implicated in brain aging, such as increased oxidative stress and inflammation [44].
There are several limitations in our study. First, the validity of g-formula methods relies on three common assumptions for observational study: no model misspecification, no measurement error, and no unmeasured confounding [45,46]. We have adjusted for as many potential risk factors as possible to alleviate the issue of no measurement error and no unmeasured confounding, which are inevitable in the observed study. Besides, the evidence that simulated data under no intervention from the parametric g-formula was analogous to the observed data indicated the necessary condition of the absent of model misspecification, and we conducted sensitivity analyses that showed that the results were robust across different specifications. Second, the consistency, requiring that the counterfactual outcome for all scenarios should be the same as the observed outcome under the observed exposure history, was implicitly assumed to hold in our study. This assumption may be met for social engagement and dietary intake but is less likely to meet for PWB [23]. Therefore, the hypothetical effects for PWB should be interpreted as the effect of the combination of changes that led to the decrease in PWB scores in the CLHLS population from 2002/2012. Third, caution should be taken in generalizing these results to other populations, because the risk was standardized to the distribution of confounders in the parametric g-formula in this study. Fourth, cognitive impairment was solely assessed using the MMSE, without other neuropsychological tests or clinical evaluation to verify the results. MMSE may not be sensitive to detect early stage of cognitive impairment, but this issue may be partially alleviated by setting a relatively low cut-off for MMSE in this study [47]. Finally, given the limited food categories on food frequency questionnaire in the current study, future studies should include more food categories, such as carbohydrates, sugar, and fat.
The strengths of our study included its longitudinal design and the application of the parametric g-formula with adjustment for time-dependent confounders by risk factors for cognitive impairment and simulating interventions on psychosocial factors and dietary intake.

5. Conclusions

Randomized trials, which are considered the ‘golden standard’, are difficult to implement. By applying parametric g-formula to longitudinal sample from the CLHLS, we found that hypothetical interventions on psychosocial factors such as social engagement and PWB, as well as fruits and vegetable intake, were beneficial to protect older adults from cognitive impairment. The g-formula is especially well suited to estimating the effects of hypothetical interventions in the presence of time-varying confounders. Our findings suggest that efforts to promote psychosocial interventions in combination with dietary intake interventions may prove to be an effective strategy for delaying the onset of cognitive impairment among older adults.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/1660-4601/17/3/1021/s1, Table S1. Cognitive impairment risks under hypothetical interventions among different age groups, Table S2. Cognitive impairment risks under hypothetical interventions among different gender groups, Table S3. Cognitive impairment risks under hypothetical interventions in different education groups.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, Y.F. and Z.Z.; methodology, Z.Z., L.C. and J.F.; software, Z.Z., L.C. and J.F.; validation, Z.Z.; formal analysis, Z.Z., L.C. and J.F.; investigation, Z.Z., L.C. and J.F.; resources, Z.Z.; data curation, Z.Z., L.C. and J.F.; writing—original draft preparation, Z.Z., L.C. and J.F.; writing—review and editing, Z.Z., L.C., J.F. and Y.H.; visualization, L.C. and J.F.; supervision, Y.F.; project administration, Y.F.; funding acquisition, Y.F. and Z.Z. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China under Grant (number 81602941, 81573257).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the Center for Healthy Aging and Development Studies, Peking University for supporting this database. Data used for this research were provided by the study entitled “Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey” (CLHLS) managed by the Center for Healthy Aging and Development Studies, Peking University. CLHLS is supported by funds from the U.S. National Institutes on Aging (NIA), China Natural Science Foundation, China Social Science Foundation, and UNFPA.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Fratiglioni, L.; Paillard-Borg, S.; Winblad, B. An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurol. 2004, 3, 343–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Fichter, M.M.; Kohlboeck, G.; Quadflieg, N.; Wyschkon, A.; Esser, G. From childhood to adult age: 18-year longitudinal results and prediction of the course of mental disorders in the community. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2009, 44, 792–803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Knopman, D.S. Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias, in Goldman’s Cecil Medicine, 24th ed.; Elservier Saunder: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  4. Nie, H.W.; Xu, Y.; Liu, B.; Zhang, Y.D.; Lei, T.; Hui, X.P.; Zhang, L.; Wu, Y. The prevalence of mild cognitive impairment about elderly population in China: A meta-analysis. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2011, 26, 558–563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Xu, J.; Wang, J.; Wimo, A.; Fratiglioni, L.; Qiu, C. The economic burden of dementia in China, 1990–2030: Implications for health policy. Bull. World Health Organ. 2017, 95, 18–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Ku, L.-J.E.; Chang, S.-M.; Pai, M.-C.; Hsieh, H.-M. Predictors of caregiver burden and care costs for older persons with dementia in Taiwan. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2019, 31, 885–894. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Anand, P.; Singh, B. A review on cholinesterase inhibitors for Alzheimer’s disease. Arch. Pharm. Res. 2013, 36, 375–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Wang, H.X.; Karp, A.; Winblad, B.; Fratiglioni, L. Late-life engagement in social and leisure activities is associated with a decreased risk of dementia: A longitudinal study from the kungsholmen project. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2002, 155, 1081–1087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Zunzunegui, M.V.; Alvarado, B.E.; Del Ser, T.; Otero, A. Social networks, social integration, and social engagement determine cognitive decline in community-dwelling Spanish older adults. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. 2003, 58, S93–S100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Chen, X.; Huang, Y.; Cheng, H.G. Lower intake of vegetables and legumes associated with cognitive decline among illiterate elderly Chinese: A 3-year cohort study. J. Nutr. Health Aging 2012, 16, 549–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Kuiper, J.S.; Zuidersma, M.; Zuidema, S.U.; Burgerhof, J.G.M.; Stolk, R.P.; Voshaar, R.C.O.; Smidt, N. Social relationships and cognitive decline: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2016, 45, 1169–1206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Zhou, Z.; Mao, F.; Zhang, W.; Towne, S.D.; Wang, P.; Fang, Y. The Association Between Loneliness and Cognitive Impairment among Older Men and Women in China: A Nationwide Longitudinal Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 2877. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Kim, J.H.; Kim, Y.; Kwon, J.; Park, E.C. Association between Changes in Depressive State and Cognitive Function. Int J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 4944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Park, S.; Kwon, E.; Lee, H. Life Course Trajectories of Later-Life Cognitive Functions: Does Social Engagement in Old Age Matter? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Krueger, K.R.; Wilson, R.S.; Kamenetsky, J.M.; Barnes, L.L.; Bienias, J.L.; Bennett, D.A. Social Engagement and Cognitive Function in Old Age. Exp. Aging Res. 2009, 35, 45–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Loef, M.; Walach, H. Fruit, vegetables and prevention of cognitive decline or dementia: A systematic review of cohort studies. J. Nutr. Health Aging 2012, 16, 626–630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Solomon, A.; Mangialasche, F.; Richard, E.; Andrieu, S.; Bennett, D.A.; Breteler, M.; Fratiglioni, L.; Hooshmand, B.; Khachaturian, A.S.; Schneider, L.S.; et al. Advances in the prevention of Alzheimer’s disease and dementia. J. Intern. Med. 2014, 275, 229–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Galvin, J.E. Prevention of Alzheimer’s Disease: Lessons Learned and Applied. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2017, 65, 2128–2133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Cohen-Mansfield, J. Non-pharmacological interventions for persons with dementia: What are they and how should they be studied? Int. Psychogeriatr. 2018, 30, 281–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Mabire, J.-B.; Aquino, J.-P.; Charras, K. Dance interventions for people with dementia: Systematic review and practice recommendations. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2019, 31, 977–987. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Sungkarat, S.; Boripuntakul, S.; Chattipakorn, N.; Watcharasaksilp, K.; Lord, S.R. Effects of Tai Chi on Cognition and Fall Risk in Older Adults with Mild Cognitive Impairment: A Randomized Controlled Trial. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2017, 65, 721–727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Brasure, M.; Desai, P.; Davila, H.; Nelson, V.A.; Calvert, C.; Jutkowitz, E.; Butler, M.; Fink, H.A.; Ratner, E.; Hemmy, L.S.; et al. Physical Activity Interventions in Preventing Cognitive Decline and Alzheimer-Type Dementia A Systematic Review. Ann. Intern. Med. 2018, 168, 30–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Wuthrich, V.M.; Rapee, R.M.; Draper, B.; Brodaty, H.; Low, L.-F.; Naismith, S.L. Reducing risk factors for cognitive decline through psychological interventions: A pilot randomized controlled trial. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2019, 31, 1015–1025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Murthy, V.H.; Krumholz, H.M.; Gross, C.P. Participation in cancer clinical trials - Race-, sex-, and age-based disparities. JAMA J. Am. Med. Assoc. 2004, 291, 2720–2726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Zeng, Y. Towards Deeper Research and Better Policy for Healthy Aging --Using the Unique Data of Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey. China Econ. J. 2012, 5, 131–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Katzman, R.; Zhang, M.Y.; Ouang Ya, Q.; Wang, Z.Y.; Liu, W.T.; Yu, E.; Wong, S.C.; Salmon, D.P.; Grant, I. A Chinese version of the Mini-Mental State Examination; impact of illiteracy in a Shanghai dementia survey. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 1988, 41, 971–978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Zhang, Z. Gender differentials in cognitive impairment and decline of the oldest old in China. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. 2006, 61, S107–S115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Gu, D.; Dupre, M.E. Assessment of Reliability of Mortality and Morbidity in the 1998–2002 CLHLS Waves. Healthy Longev. China 2008, 20, 99–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Zeng, Y.; Vaupel, J.; Xiao, Z.; Zhang, C.; Liu, Y. The Healthy Longevity Survey and the Active Life Expectancy of the Oldest Old in China. Popul. Engl. Sel. 2001, 13, 95–116. [Google Scholar]
  30. Mitchell, A.J.; Shiri-Feshki, M. Rate of progression of mild cognitive impairment to dementia--meta-analysis of 41 robust inception cohort studies. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2009, 119, 252–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Ravaglia, G.; Forti, P.; Lucicesare, A.; Pisacane, N.; Rietti, E.; Bianchin, M.; Dalmonte, E. Physical activity and dementia risk in the elderly: findings from a prospective Italian study. Neurology 2008, 70, 1786–1794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Saczynski, J.S.; Pfeifer, L.A.; Masaki, K.; Korf, E.S.; Laurin, D.; White, L.; Launer, L.J. The Effect of Social Engagement on Incident Dementia The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2006, 163, 433–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Zhou, Z.; Wang, P.; Fang, Y. Social Engagement and Its Change are Associated with Dementia Risk among Chinese Older Adults: A Longitudinal Study. Sci. Rep. Uk 2018, 8, 1551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Zhou, Z.; Mao, F.; Han, Y.; Fu, J.; Fang, Y. Social Engagement and Cognitive Impairment in Older Chinese Adults: The Mediating Role of Psychological Well-Being. J. Aging Health 2019, 0, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Zhou, Z.; Cai, L.; Zhuang, M.; Hong, Y.A.; Fang, Y. A longitudinal analysis of the association between the living arrangements and psychological well-being of older Chinese adults: the role of income sources. BMC Geriatr. 2019, 19, 347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Wang, J.; Chen, T.; Han, B. Does co-residence with adult children associate with better psychological well-being among the oldest old in China? Aging Ment. Health 2014, 18, 232–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Zhang, W.; Liu, G. Childlessness, psychological well-being, and life satisfaction among the elderly in China. J. Cross Cult. Gerontol. 2007, 22, 185–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Sanjuan, P. Affect Balance as Mediating Variable Between Effective Psychological Functioning and Satisfaction with Life. J. Happiness Stud. 2011, 12, 373–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Barnes, L.L.; de Leon, C.F.M.; Wilson, R.S.; Bienias, J.L.; Evans, D.A. Social resources and cognitive decline in a population of older African Americans and whites. Neurology 2004, 63, 2322–2326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Hultsch, D.F.; Hertzog, C.; Small, B.J.; Dixon, R.A. Use it or lose it: Engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychol. Aging 1999, 14, 245–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Llewellyn, D.J.; Lang, I.A.; Langa, K.M.; Huppert, F.A. Cognitive function and psychological well-being: findings from a population-based cohort. Age Ageing 2008, 37, 685–689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Burns, A.; Guthrie, E.; Marino-Francis, F.; Busby, C.; Morris, J.; Russell, E.; Margison, F.; Lennon, S.; Byrne, J. Brief psychotherapy in Alzheimer’s disease - Randomised controlled trial. Br. J. Psychiatry 2005, 187, 143–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  43. Jiang, X.; Huang, J.; Song, D.; Deng, R.; Wei, J.; Zhang, Z. Increased Consumption of Fruit and Vegetables Is Related to a Reduced Risk of Cognitive Impairment and Dementia: Meta-Analysis. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2017, 9, 18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Lamport, D.J.; Saunders, C.; Butler, L.T.; Spencer, J.P. Fruits, vegetables, 100% juices, and cognitive function. Nutr. Rev. 2014, 72, 774–789. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Garcia-Aymerich, J.; Varraso, R.; Danaei, G.; Camargo, C.A.; Hernan, M.A. Incidence of Adult-onset Asthma After Hypothetical Interventions on Body Mass Index and Physical Activity: An Application of the Parametric G-Formula. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2014, 179, 20–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Nascimento, G.G.; Peres, M.A.; Mittinty, M.N.; Peres, K.G.; Do, L.G.; Horta, B.L.; Gigante, D.P.; Correa, M.B.; Demarco, F.F. Diet-Induced Overweight and Obesity and Periodontitis Risk: An Application of the Parametric G-Formula in the 1982 Pelotas Birth Cohort. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2017, 185, 442–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Zheng, J.; Liu, J.; An, R. Functional limitation and cognitive impairment among 80+ year old Chinese. Australas. J. Ageing 2016, 35, 266–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Flow chart of sample selection, CLHLS 2002–2012. * CI = cognitive impairment.
Figure 1. Flow chart of sample selection, CLHLS 2002–2012. * CI = cognitive impairment.
Ijerph 17 01021 g001
Table 1. The distribution of covariates at baseline (n/%).
Table 1. The distribution of covariates at baseline (n/%).
VariablesN%VariablesN%
Cognitive functioning *26.11 (4.6) Drinking
 PWB *4.39 (3.7) not drinking484465.7
Social engagement quitting78410.6
 low355148.1drinking174923.7
 high382651.9Hypertension
Age (years) no634586.0
 65~234131.7yes103214.0
 75~222330.1Diabetes
 85~179424.3no724398.2
 95~101913.8yes1341.8
Gender Stroke
 male339546.0no708796.1
 female398254.0yes2903.1
Residence Disabled
 urban329444.7no627885.1
 rural408355.3yes109914.9
Marital status Fruit intake
 married421457.1every day259635.2
 not married316342.9sometimes299040.5
Living arrangements seldom179124.3
 live with others634586.0Vegetable intake
 live alone103214.0every day658789.3
Education sometimes6508.4
 illiteracy421957.2seldom1702.3
 literacy315842.8Meat intake
Profession every day296340.2
 farmer435859.1sometimes316042.8
 others301940.9seldom125417.0
Exercise regularly Fish intake
 no451761.2every day171723.3
 yes286038.8sometimes362149.1
Smoking seldom203927.6
 not smoking159421.6Region
 quitting465063.0eastern335545.5
 smoking113315.4middle180424.5
western221830.1
* mean (standard deviation); PWB = psychological well-being.
Table 2. Cognitive impairment risks under hypothetical single interventions.
Table 2. Cognitive impairment risks under hypothetical single interventions.
InterventionRisk95% CIRR95% CI
Natural course (no intervention)20.0817.81, 21.071.00——
Social engagement14.5412.60, 16.640.720.65, 0.82
PWB19.4617.51, 20.300.970.98, 0.99
Vegetables19.7317.63, 20.890.980.98, 1.00
Fruits18.6316.22, 19.940.930.89, 0.95
Meat20.0417.88, 21.281.000.97, 1.02
Fish20.0718.16, 21.221.000.97, 1.04
Notes: CI = confidence interval, RR = risk ratio, PWB = psychological well-being.
Table 3. Cognitive impairment risks under hypothetical joint interventions.
Table 3. Cognitive impairment risks under hypothetical joint interventions.
InterventionRisk95% CIRR95% CI
No intervention20.0817.81, 21.071.00——
Social engagement + PWB14.1412.34, 16.380.700.64, 0.80
Diet (vegetables + fruits)18.5016.09, 19.880.920.89, 0.95
Diet (vegetables + fruits) + Social engagement13.3211.42, 15.460.660.59, 0.75
Diet (vegetables + fruits) + PWB17.9515.66, 19.150.890.86, 0.92
All factors (Social engagement + PWB + Diet (vegetables + fruits)12.9311.24, 15.050.640.58, 0.73
Notes: CI = confidence interval, RR = risk ratio, PWB = psychological well-being.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Zhou, Z.; Cai, L.; Fu, J.; Han, Y.; Fang, Y. Hypothetical Interventions on Risk Factors for Cognitive Impairment among Chinese Older Adults: An Application of the Parametric G-Formula. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 1021. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17031021

AMA Style

Zhou Z, Cai L, Fu J, Han Y, Fang Y. Hypothetical Interventions on Risk Factors for Cognitive Impairment among Chinese Older Adults: An Application of the Parametric G-Formula. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020; 17(3):1021. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17031021

Chicago/Turabian Style

Zhou, Zi, Lun Cai, Jian Fu, Yaofeng Han, and Ya Fang. 2020. "Hypothetical Interventions on Risk Factors for Cognitive Impairment among Chinese Older Adults: An Application of the Parametric G-Formula" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 3: 1021. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17031021

APA Style

Zhou, Z., Cai, L., Fu, J., Han, Y., & Fang, Y. (2020). Hypothetical Interventions on Risk Factors for Cognitive Impairment among Chinese Older Adults: An Application of the Parametric G-Formula. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(3), 1021. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17031021

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop