Next Article in Journal
Can Resistance Exercise Be a Tool for Healthy Aging in Post-Menopausal Women with Type 1 Diabetes?
Next Article in Special Issue
The Influence of Episodic Future Thinking and Graphic Warning Labels on Delay Discounting and Cigarette Demand
Previous Article in Journal
Development and Implementation of a Total Worker Health® Mentoring Program in a Correctional Workforce
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Future Thinking Priming Especially Effective at Modifying Delay Discounting Rates among Cigarette Smokers

1
Department of Health Behavior, Roswell Park Comprehensive Cancer Center, Buffalo, NY 14203, USA
2
Virginia Tech, The Fralin Biomedical Research Institute at VTC, Roanoke, VA 24016, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(16), 8717; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18168717
Submission received: 23 June 2021 / Revised: 8 August 2021 / Accepted: 13 August 2021 / Published: 18 August 2021

Abstract

:
Background: Tobacco use remains one of the world’s greatest preventable causes of death and disease. While most smokers want to quit, few are successful, highlighting a need for novel therapeutic approaches to support cessation efforts. Lower delay discounting (DD) rates are associated with increased smoking cessation success. Future thinking priming (FTP) reliably reduces DD rates in large populations. Smokers consistently discount more than nonsmokers, and evidence suggests that changes in DD rates are rate dependent. This study examined whether smoking status moderated the effect of FTP on DD rates and, if so, if the moderation effect could be attributed to differences in baseline rates of DD. Methods: Moderation analysis was conducted to determine whether the effect of FTP, versus neutral priming (NP), on DD differed among smokers and nonsmokers. Results: Smoking status moderated the effect of condition (FTP vs. NP) on post-intervention DD scores (b = −0.2919, p = 0.0124) and DD change scores (b = −0.2975, p = 0.0130). There was no evidence of rate dependence effects in the current sample. Conclusions: FTP had a greater effect on decreasing DD rates among smokers than nonsmokers. FTP is effective and simple to administer, which makes it a promising therapeutic approach for aiding smoking cessation.

1. Introduction

“The future interests me—I’m going to spend the rest of my life there.”—Mark Twain.
Despite the success of tobacco control efforts, tobacco use remains one of the most significant preventable causes of death and disease in the world today [1]. Annually, cigarette smoking alone causes more than 8 million deaths worldwide, over 600,000 of whom die from secondhand smoke [2,3,4,5,6,7,8]. In the US, smoking cigarettes causes nearly half a million deaths annually and over 30% of all cancer deaths [9,10]. In the US, most individuals who smoke express the desire to quit, and over half make at least one quit attempt nearly every year, but within 6–12 months, over 90% reverse this decision despite significant effort [11]. This pernicious conundrum remains one of the most significant public health challenges today, calling for innovative therapeutic targets and novel approaches to support efforts to quit smoking [10].
Smoking cigarettes is a highly reinforcing experience for many individuals, and achieving long-term abstinence from smoking is a process whereby individuals must repeatedly choose options other than the immediate reinforcing experience of smoking. While the biological rewards (dopamine release) of cigarette smoking are not as significant as those of other drugs, the fact that smoking is so repetitive, and so often performed in conjunction with other activities, increases the association of those rewards with many activities engaged in on a daily basis, which both enhances the pleasure and the motivation derived from the activities and the reinforcing nature of smoking [12,13]. Delay discounting (DD) describes the degree to which the reinforcer’s subjective value declines as the time to receipt increases. Most humans and many animals prefer immediate reinforcement but are willing to wait for a period of time for rewards that are perceived to be of more value [14,15,16,17]. Individuals who smoke demonstrate higher DD rates than individuals who do not, suggesting that, for smokers, reinforcers lose their value quickly when received outside the temporal window for which they are valued [2,3,6,7,8,18,19,20,21,22,23]. Good health, long life, and prudent far-sighted health-related decision-making are all temporally distant reinforcers. Among smokers, lower DD rates predict greater success at achieving and maintaining abstinence from smoking [24,25,26,27,28,29]. DD rate is also modifiable [2,6,26,27,30,31,32,33,34], and thus has become a novel therapeutic target in the treatment of tobacco dependence and other unhealthy behaviors.
Methods to decrease DD rates include framing techniques intended to alter the temporal window in which decisions are made [35]. Episodic future thinking is a framing technique that shows promise for reducing DD rates concurrently with other health behaviors, including cigarette smoking [8,36,37,38,39,40]. Future thinking priming (FTP) is another framing technique administered remotely that reliably reduces DD rates in large populations [41,42]. Framing techniques have been shown to impact the brain via the activation of neural networks involved in decision-making and prospective thinking, resulting in reductions in DD [43,44,45,46].
The effect of FTP on DD rates has been replicated in two studies and with multiple measures of DD with samples that include smokers and nonsmokers [41,42]. However, smokers consistently discount more than nonsmokers [47]. Therefore, understanding whether and how FTP specifically impacts DD rates among individuals who smoke is critical, and should be conducted prior to examining the therapeutic potential of FTP for cigarette smoking. For instance, the higher DD rates found among smokers might be less modifiable given that they appear to be so strongly associated with smoking status. On the other hand, evidence suggests that changes in DD rates are rate dependent [48,49,50,51,52], which suggests that FTP might have a greater impact on smokers, for whom there is more room for change in DD rates.
This study examined whether smoking status moderated the effect of FTP on DD rates and then examined if any differences found could be attributed to rate dependency. This is a secondary analysis of a randomized controlled trial in which FTP significantly decreased DD rates in a large sample of participants [42]. Given the evidence for rate dependence, we hypothesized that FTP would have a greater effect on individuals who smoke than those who do not smoke, and that this would be due to a rate-dependent difference in baseline measures of DD.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Adult Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) workers (age ≥ 18 years) who spoke English and resided in North America were eligible to participate. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of City University of New York (#680011-1) and Roswell Park Comprehensive Cancer Center (#BDR082917). Informed consent was obtained from all participants. Data was collected in 2016. MTurk is an online worker platform. MTurk research participants provided responses comparable to those of laboratory participants [53,54,55,56].

2.2. Study Design and Procedures

In this study, we conceptualized smoking status as a moderator and conducted a moderation analysis to determine whether the relation between the conditions (FTP, NP) and the dependent variable (DD) was moderated by smoking status.
This is a secondary data analysis of a study that compared participants randomized to FTP or neutral priming (NP) in a pre/post-test control group design [42]. Participants (n = 1532) were enrolled and completed baseline measures. Two weeks later, they were randomized, completed the FTP or NP tasks as assigned, and administered the post-intervention assessments.

2.3. The Future Thinking Priming and Neutral Tasks

The FTP stimuli consisted of 10 future-oriented words and phrases: “future”, “self-discipline”, “willpower”, “self-control”, “long-term”, “save”, “planned”, and “investment”. NP stimuli consisted of 10 words and phrases that reflected a neutral focus and were shown to have no effect on DD (e.g., “pale”, “informative”, “dispassionate”, “formal”, etc.). Participants were first instructed to write 10 different sentences describing themselves and incorporating at least one of the words provided. After submitting the sentences, participants were instructed to write a short paragraph (not exceeding 250 words) about themselves, incorporating all the words/phrases provided.

2.4. Measures

Standard sociodemographic measures were collected at baseline, including sex, age, race, ethnicity, education, and income. Additionally, tobacco use (number of cigarettes smoked per day) and alcohol use (number of alcoholic drinks per week) were collected. Because priming effects are influenced by social self-monitoring [57], we administered the self-monitoring scale. High social self-monitors may initially respond to priming stimuli in a manner consistent with the prime, but they are less likely to maintain primed behaviors because they tend to shift behaviors to match social expectations [57]. Time perspective (future-oriented or not) was assessed with the time perspective questionnaire [58]. Perceived social status (SSS) was measured by the MacArthur Scale of Subjective Social Status [59].
DD was the primary outcome measure. DD of USD 100 was assessed by the 5-Trial adjusting delay discounting task [60]. The 5-Trial task is an interactive instrument that automatically adjusts to respondents’ choices to produce a result after a maximum of five trials [60]. Respondents were asked on the first trial whether they would prefer USD 50 now or USD 100 in three weeks. If the immediate option is selected, then the second trial shortens the delay to one day (USD 50 now or USD 100 in one day). If the delayed option is selected on the first trial, then the second trial lengthens the delay (i.e., USD 50 now or USD 100 in two years). Delays on all subsequent trials are adjusted based on responses from the preceding trial. The 5-trial task output was expressed as the natural logarithm of k in Mazur’s hyperbolic discounting model, with k increasing as the preference for smaller sooner rewards increases [17]. Lower k values mean that individuals are more willing to wait for a larger reward.

2.5. Data Analysis

The analysis was carried out using IBM SPSS, Version 23 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). Descriptive analyses were conducted to characterize the sample. One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) and χ2 analysis were used to examine characteristic differences between smokers and nonsmokers. Statistically significant differences between the smokers and nonsmokers were included in the final models to control for baseline differences between the groups.
Moderation analysis was conducted using Hayes PROCESS v3.5.3 with bootstrapping at 1000 samples to examine potential moderation of smoking status on the effects of FTP on DD. We used two methods of conceptualizing the outcome: (1) DD post-intervention, and (2) change in DD (change = post interventions − baseline). Each outcome was analyzed in a separate model.
In the two models, condition (FTP vs. NP) was entered as a fixed variable, smoking status (smoker vs. nonsmoker) was entered as the moderator, and partner status, income, education, alcoholic drinks, and SSS were entered as covariates to control for baseline differences between smokers and nonsmokers. In the first model, baseline DD rate was also entered to be consistent with Blomqvist’s method of controlling for the baseline measure of interest in the regression analyses [61]. Smoking status was considered a significant moderator if the interaction between smoking status and condition was significant for one or both smoking status categories. If the overall interaction between condition and smoking status was below p < 0.10, following the guidance from Hayes, further probing was performed to measure the effects of this interaction on different levels of the moderator [62].
The Oldham correlation method was used to assess rate dependence overall and in smokers versus nonsmokers, using the following equation where x is the baseline measure, y is the post-intervention measure, s2x is baseline variance, s2y is post-test variance, and rxy is the correlation between baseline and post-intervention:
O l d h a m   C o r r   ( ( x y ) ,   ( x + y ) 2 ) = s x 2 s y 2 ( s x 2 + s y 2 ) 2 4 r x y 2 s x 2 s y 2
The Oldham correlation method was chosen because it was used to successfully identify rate dependence in DD in other studies and because it removes mathematical coupling, regression to the mean, and mathematical bias [51,53,63]. If the Oldham correlation is above 0.3, the comparison is considered rate dependent [50,51,53,64].

3. Results

3.1. Participants

Participants’ (n = 1532) mean age was 35.7 years (SD 11.3); 54.8% were female; 82.5% were White; and about half were partnered (58.7%). Two-thirds attended at least some college (65.9%). Annual household incomes ranged from <USD 10,000 (5.1%) to >USD 99,000 (14.3%). The mean score for self-monitoring was intermediate (M = 10.7, SD = 4.7). In the parent study, no characteristic differences among participants randomized to the FTP and NP conditions were found with the exception of other tobacco use (FTP 14.6% versus NP 10.7%; χ2 = 5.21, p = 0.02) [42].
About one-fifth (22%; n = 333) of participants reported smoking >0 cigarettes per day and were considered smokers. Nonsmokers were more likely to be partnered than smokers (61% vs. 52%); nonsmokers had significantly more education, higher income, and higher SSS than smokers. Smokers reported significantly more alcoholic drinks per week than nonsmokers (5.13 vs. 2.73). See Table 1 for details.

3.2. Moderation Effects

Delay discounting post-intervention. The overall model for the moderation effect of smoking status on post-test DD rate was significant (R2 = 0.6690, F = 341.758, p < 0.001). The interaction between condition and smoking status was less than 0.10 (b = −0.2437, p = 0.0651). Probing revealed that this effect was significant for smokers (b = −0.2919, p = 0.0124). See Table 2 and Figure 1.
Change in delay discounting. The overall model for the moderation effect of smoking status on DD change score was not significant (R2 = 0.0070, F = 1.3334, p = 0.2223). The interaction between condition and smoking status was less than 0.10 (b = −0.2525, p = 0.0620). Probing revealed this effect was significant for smokers (b = −0.2975, p = 0.0130) (see Table 3 and Figure 2).

3.3. Rate-Dependence Effects

No evidence of rate dependence was found. All Oldham correlations were below the 0.3 threshold. The overall DD outcomes showed an Oldham correlation of −0.113; DD among smokers showed an Oldham correlation of −0.010; DD among nonsmokers showed an Oldham correlation of −0.140.

4. Discussion

Our findings indicate that a one-time remote administration of FTP decreased DD among individuals who smoked cigarettes. The FTP task had a greater effect on DD rates among individuals who smoked cigarettes than among those who did not. This suggests that FTP can modify DD rates among smokers, which enhances its therapeutic potential as a novel intervention to reduce DD and concurrently reduce cigarette consumption among smokers, similar to episodic future thinking. Our robust approach examined the moderating effects of smoking status on the effects of FTP on DD rates, using two different methods to account for baseline DD rates. Smoking status had a significant moderating effect in both models, which reinforces the reliability and validity of the findings.
FTP shows promise for development into a novel intervention to support smoking cessation. More research is needed to determine if FTP can decrease cigarette consumption as it decreases DD rates and to determine whether repeated exposure provides cumulative effects. Determination of optimal exposure patterns to support short- and long-term cessation is needed prior to efficacy testing.
If found to support smoking cessation, FTP has the potential to be disseminated widely with minimal infrastructure, which bodes well for potential reach. The FTP task was developed and tested via remote administration using an easily accessible survey program such as Qualtrics or RedCap. Individuals’ engagement in the FTP task is practical and relatively undemanding. Individuals can engage in the task via a link sent by text or email and complete the task on a computer or mobile device.
Given the role of temporal distance in the subjective value of reinforcers, temporal orientation is increasingly being recognized as an important factor in attaining and maintaining abstinence from smoking. Efforts to develop novel interventions that target DD are gaining traction. These findings support previous research focused on DD as a therapeutic target in the treatment of tobacco use [2,3,6,7,8] and represent the next step in the examination of FTP as an effective intervention.
We did not find rate-dependence effects, which might be indicative of the characteristics of the sample. Rate dependence is understudied, but we believe the process of calculating rate dependence is accessible, and future studies should continue to investigate rate dependence as part of the standard procedure [48,49,51,52,65].
This study has several important strengths. First, the current sample was relatively large and representative, which increases the generalizability of findings. Second, results were replicated in two different models, which reduces the likelihood of potential model error or bias. Third, the rigorous design of the parent study helped ensure participant understanding of instructions and accuracy of measurements. Fourth, the current study addresses rate-dependence effects, which has recently been recognized as a methodological standard in research on treatment effectiveness [49,51,52,65]. Finally, the fact that the current study is a secondary data analysis helps increase the impact of the parent study and expands the research knowledge by investigating additional hypotheses cost-effectively and efficiently, as well as helping identify differences in treatment among subgroups of the parent study, which in turn can inform tailoring of group-specific treatments [64,66].
This study has several limitations as well. The moderating effect of smoking status on the effects of FTP on DD rates might be explained by an unknown third factor, for example, having a higher income bracket might contribute to more future thinking orientation in general. Although relatively large, the sample sizes were different for nonsmokers and smokers. This might have contributed to a lack of significance in the overall interactions. There was no conditional effect for nonsmokers and they were a much larger group. Therefore, their weight in the standard error was greater. Finally, smoking status is a self-selected characteristic which ultimately may have resulted in some differences in psychosocial measures, even though these were controlled statistically in all models.

5. Conclusions

The current study showed that one-time administration of FTP had a greater effect on decreasing DD rates among smokers than nonsmokers. The FTP is a simple task accessible remotely on a wide variety of devices. The FTP task has potential to be developed as a therapeutic support for smoking cessation.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.S., C.E.S., W.K.B., and E.C.; methodology, A.S., C.E.S., W.K.B., and E.C.; formal analysis, A.S.; investigation, C.E.S.; resources, A.S.; data curation, C.E.S.; writing—original draft preparation, A.S.; writing—review and editing, A.S., W.K.B., C.E.S., and E.C.; visualization, A.S.; supervision, W.K.B., and C.E.S.; project administration, C.E.S.; funding acquisition, C.E.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities (R01 MD007054 PI: Sheffer), the National Cancer Institute (5P20CA192993 and 5P20CA192991 PI: Sheffer).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Boards of City University of New York (#680011-1) and Roswell Park Comprehensive Cancer Center (#BDR082917). Informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

No new data were created or analyzed in this study. Data sharing is not applicable to this article.

Acknowledgments

The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. WHO. WHO Report on the Global Tobacco Epidemic, 2017: Monitoring Tobacco Use and Prevention Policies; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  2. Barlow, P.; McKee, M.; Reeves, A.; Galea, G.; Stuckler, D. Time-discounting and tobacco smoking: A systematic review and network analysis. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2017, 46, 860–869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Friedel, J.E.; DeHart, W.B.; Madden, G.J.; Odum, A.L. Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: Discounting of monetary and consumable outcomes in current and non-smokers. Psychopharmacology 2014, 231, 4517–4526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Öberg, M.; Jaakkola, M.S.; Prüss-Ustün, A.; Peruga, A.; Woodward, A.; World Health, O. Global Estimate of the Burden of Disease from Second-Hand Smoke/by Mattias Öberg...[et al]; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  5. Oberg, M.; Jaakkola, M.S.; Woodward, A.; Peruga, A.; Prüss-Ustün, A. Worldwide burden of disease from exposure to second-hand smoke: A retrospective analysis of data from 192 countries. Lancet 2011, 377, 139–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Rung, J.M.; Madden, G.J. Experimental reductions of delay discounting and impulsive choice: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2018, 147, 1349–1381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Rung, J.M.; Peck, S.; Hinnenkamp, J.; Preston, E.; Madden, G.J. Changing Delay Discounting and Impulsive Choice: Implications for Addictions, Prevention, and Human Health. Perspect. Behav. Sci. 2019, 42, 397–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Stein, J.S.; Wilson, A.G.; Koffarnus, M.N.; Daniel, T.O.; Epstein, L.H.; Bickel, W.K. Unstuck in time: Episodic future thinking reduces delay discounting and cigarette smoking. Psychopharmacology 2016, 233, 3771–3778. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. ACS. Cancer Facts and Figures 2016; American Cancer Society: Atlanta, GA, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  10. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. The Health Consequences of Smoking–50 Years of Progress. A Report of the Surgeon General; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Office on Smoking and Health: Atlanta, GA, USA, 2014.
  11. Babb, S.; Malarcher, A.; Schauer, G.; Asman, K.; Jamal, A. Quitting Smoking Among Adults-United States, 2000–2015. MMWR Morb. Mortal Wkly. Rep. 2017, 65, 1457–1464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Rupprecht, L.E.; Smith, T.T.; Schassburger, R.L.; Buffalari, D.M.; Sved, A.F.; Donny, E.C. Behavioral Mechanisms Underlying Nicotine Reinforcement. In The Neuropharmacology of Nicotine Dependence; Springer International Publishing: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2015; pp. 19–53. [Google Scholar]
  13. Perkins, K.A.; Karelitz, J.L. Reinforcement enhancing effects of nicotine via smoking. Psychopharmacology 2013, 228, 479–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Ainslie, G. Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychol. Bull. 1975, 82, 463–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Kirby, K.N. Bidding on the future: Evidence against normative discounting of delayed rewards. J. Exp. Psychol. General 1997, 126, 54–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Logue, A.W. Research on self-control: An integrating framework. Behav. Brain Sci. 1988, 11, 665–709. [Google Scholar]
  17. Mazur, J.E. An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In The Effect of Delay and of Intervening Events on Reinforcement Value, Quantitative Analyses of Behavior; Commons, M.L., Mazur, J.E., Nevin, J.A., Rachlin, H., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1987; pp. 55–73. [Google Scholar]
  18. Baker, F.; Johnson, M.W.; Bickel, W.K. Delay discounting in current and never-before cigarette smokers: Similarities and differences across commodity, sign, and magnitude. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2003, 112, 382–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Bickel, W.K.; Madden, G.J. A comparison of measures of relative reinforcing efficacy and behavioral economics: Cigarettes and money in smokers. Behav. Pharmacol. 1999, 10, 627–637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Bickel, W.K.; Bickel, W.K.; Yi, R.; Kowal, B.P.; Gatchalian, K.M. Cigarette smokers discount past and future rewards symmetrically and more than controls: Is discounting a measure of impulsivity? Drug Alcohol Depend. 2008, 96, 256–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  21. Mitchell, S.H. Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers. Psychopharmacology 1999, 146, 455–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Odum, A.L. Delay discounting: I’m a k, you’re a k. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 2011, 96, 427–439. [Google Scholar]
  23. Reynolds, B. Do high rates of cigarette consumption increase delay discounting? A cross-sectional comparison of adolescent smokers and young-adult smokers and nonsmokers. Behav. Process. 2004, 67, 545–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Krishnan-Sarin, S.; Reynolds, B.; Duhig, A.M.; Smith, A.; Liss, T.; McFetridge, A.; Cavallo, D.A.; Carroll, K.M.; Potenza, M.N. Behavioral impulsivity predicts treatment outcome in a smoking cessation program for adolescent smokers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007, 88, 79–82. [Google Scholar]
  25. MacKillop, J.; Kahler, C.W. Delayed reward discounting predicts treatment response for heavy drinkers receiving smoking cessation treatment. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2009, 104, 197–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Sheffer, C.; Mackillop, J.; McGeary, J.; Landes, R.; Carter, L.; Yi, R.; Jones, B.; Christensen, D.; Stitzer, M.; Jackson, L.; et al. Delay discounting, locus of control, and cognitive impulsiveness independently predict tobacco dependence treatment outcomes in a highly dependent, lower socioeconomic group of smokers. Am. J. Addict. 2012, 21, 221–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Sheffer, C.E.; Christensen, D.R.; Landes, R.; Carter, L.P.; Jackson, L.; Bickel, W.K. Delay discounting rates: A strong prognostic indicator of smoking relapse. Addict. Behav. 2014, 39, 1682–1689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  28. Stanger, C.; Ryan, S.R.; Fu, H.; Landes, R.D.; Jones, B.A.; Bickel, W.K.; Budney, A.J. Delay discounting predicts adolescent substance abuse treatment outcome. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2012, 20, 205–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Yoon, J.H.; Higgins, S.T.; Heil, S.H.; Sugarbaker, R.J.; Thomas, C.S.; Badger, G.J. Delay discounting predicts postpartum relapse to cigarette smoking among pregnant women. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2007, 15, 176–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Bickel, W.K.; Yi, R.; Landes, R.D.; Hill, P.F.; Baxter, C. Remember the future: Working memory training decreases delay discounting among stimulant addicts. Biol. Psychiatry 2011, 69, 260–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Black, A.C.; Rosen, M.I. A money management-based substance use treatment increases valuation of future rewards. Addict. Behav. 2011, 36, 125–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  32. Sheffer, C.E.; Prashad, N.; Lunden, S.; Malhotra, R.; O’Connor, R.J. To smoke or not to smoke: Does delay discounting affect the proximal choice to smoke? Subst. Use Misuse 2019, 54, 1237–1246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Bickel, W.K.; George Wilson, A.; Franck, C.T.; Terry Mueller, E.; Jarmolowicz, D.P.; Koffarnus, M.N.; Fede, S.J. Using crowdsourcing to compare temporal, social temporal, and probability discounting among obese and non-obese individuals. Appetite 2014, 75, 82–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Athamneh, L.N.; Stein, J.S.; Bickel, W.K. Will delay discounting predict intention to quit smoking? Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2017, 25, 273–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Koffarnus, M.N.; Jarmolowicz, D.P.; Mueller, E.T.; Bickel, W.K. Changing delay discounting in the light of the competing neurobehavioral decision systems theory: A review. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 2013, 99, 32–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Daniel, T.O.; Stanton, C.M.; Epstein, L.H. The future is now: Reducing impulsivity and energy intake using episodic future thinking. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 2339–2342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Dassen, F.C.M.; Jansen, A.; Nederkoorn, C.; Houben, K. Focus on the future: Episodic future thinking reduces discount rate and snacking. Appetite 2016, 96, 327–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. O’Neill, J.; Daniel, T.O.; Epstein, L.H. Episodic future thinking reduces eating in a food court. Eat. Behav. 2016, 20, 9–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Snider, S.E.; LaConte, S.M.; Bickel, W.K. Episodic Future Thinking: Expansion of the Temporal Window in Individuals with Alcohol Dependence. Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res. 2016, 40, 1558–1566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Sze, Y.Y.; Daniel, T.O.; Kilanowski, C.K.; Collins, R.L.; Epstein, L.H. Web-Based and Mobile Delivery of an Episodic Future Thinking Intervention for Overweight and Obese Families: A Feasibility Study. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth 2015, 3, e97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Sheffer, C.E.; Mackillop, J.; Fernandez, A.; Christensen, D.; Bickel, W.K.; Johnson, M.W.; Panissidi, L.; Pittman, J.; Franck, C.T.; Williams, J.; et al. Initial examination of priming tasks to decrease delay discounting. Behav. Process. 2016, 128, 144–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Shevorykin, A.; Pittman, J.C.; Bickel, W.K.; O’Connor, R.J.; Malhotra, R.; Prashad, N.; Sheffer, C.E. Primed for Health: Future Thinking Priming Decreases Delay Discounting. Health Behav. Policy Rev. 2019, 6, 363–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Barutchu, A.; Sahu, A.; Humphreys, G.W.; Spence, C. Multisensory processing in event-based prospective memory. Acta Psychol. 2019, 192, 23–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Barutchu, A.; Spence, C.; Humphreys, G.W. Multisensory enhancement elicited by unconscious visual stimuli. Exp. Brain Res. 2018, 236, 409–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Elgendi, M.; Kumar, P.; Barbic, S.; Howard, N.; Abbott, D.; Cichocki, A. Subliminal Priming-State of the Art and Future Perspectives. Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Peters, J.; Buchel, C. Episodic future thinking reduces reward delay discounting through an enhancement of prefrontal-mediotemporal interactions. Neuron 2010, 66, 138–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Mitchell, S.H.; Wilson, V.B. Differences in delay discounting between smokers and nonsmokers remain when both rewards are delayed. Psychopharmacology 2012, 219, 549–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Bickel, W.K.; Landes, R.D.; Kurth-Nelson, Z.; Redish, A.D. A Quantitative Signature of Self-Control Repair. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 2, 685–695. [Google Scholar]
  49. Bickel, W.K.; Quisenberry, A.J.; Snider, S.E. Does impulsivity change rate dependently following stimulant administration? A translational selective review and re-analysis. Psychopharmacology 2016, 233, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Oldham, P.D. A note on the analysis of repeated measurements of the same subjects. J. Chronic Dis. 1962, 15, 969–977. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Quisenberry, A.J.; Snider, S.E.; Bickel, W.K. The Return of Rate Dependence. Behav. Anal. 2016, 16, 215–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  52. Snider, S.E.; Quisenberry, A.J.; Bickel, W.K. Order in the absence of an effect: Identifying rate-dependent relationships. Behav. Process. 2016, 127, 18–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Buhrmester, M.; Kwang, T.; Gosling, S.D. Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A New Source of Inexpensive, Yet High-Quality, Data? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2011, 6, 3–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Crump, M.J.; McDonnell, J.V.; Gureckis, T.M. Evaluating Amazon’s Mechanical Turk as a tool for experimental behavioral research. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e57410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Paolacci, G.; Chandler, J.; Ipeirotis, P.G. Running experiments on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2010, 5, 411–419. [Google Scholar]
  56. Ross, J.; Irani, I.; Silberman, M.; Zaldivar, A.; Tomlinson, B. Who are the Crowdworkers? Shifting Demographics in Amazon Mechanical Turk. In Proceedings of the Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Atlanta, GA, USA, 10–15 April 2010; pp. 2863–2872. [Google Scholar]
  57. Wheeler, S.C.; Demarree, K.G.; Petty, R.E. Understanding the role of the self in prime-to-behavior effects: The Active-Self account. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2007, 11, 234–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Fong, G.T.; Hall, P.A. The importance of time perspective in predicting, understanding, and reducing health risk behaviors among adolescents. In Reducing Adolescent Risk: Toward an Integrated Approach; Romer, D., Ed.; Sage: Newbury Park, CA, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  59. Adler, N.; Stewart, J. The MacArthur Scale of Subjective Social Status; MacArthur Research Network on SES & Health: San Francisco, CA, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  60. Koffarnus, M.N.; Bickel, W.K. A 5-trial adjusting delay discounting task: Accurate discount rates in less than one minute. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2014, 22, 222–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Chiolero, A.; Paradis, G.; Rich, B.; Hanley, J.A. Assessing the Relationship between the Baseline Value of a Continuous Variable and Subsequent Change Over Time. Front. Public Health 2013, 1, 29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  62. Hayes, A.F. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach; Guilford Publications: New York, NY, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  63. Nunes, E.V.; Pavlicova, M.; Hu, M.-C.; Campbell, A.N.; Miele, G.; Hien, D.; Klein, D.F. Baseline Matters: The Importance of Covariation for Baseline Severity in the Analysis of Clinical Trials. Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abus. 2011, 37, 446–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Selby, J.V.; Beal, A.C.; Frank, L. The Patient-Centered Outcomes Research Institute (PCORI) National Priorities for Research and Initial Research Agenda. JAMA 2012, 307, 1583–1584. [Google Scholar]
  65. Snider, S.E.; Deshpande, H.U.; Lisinski, J.M.; Koffarnus, M.N.; LaConte, S.M.; Bickel, W.K. Working Memory Training Improves Alcohol Users’ Episodic Future Thinking: A Rate-Dependent Analysis. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 2018, 3, 160–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Cheng, H.G.; Phillips, M.R. Secondary Analysis of Existing Data: Opportunities and Implementation. Shanghai Arch. Psychiatry 2014, 26, 371–375. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Future thinking priming appears to have a greater effect on smokers than nonsmokers.
Figure 1. Future thinking priming appears to have a greater effect on smokers than nonsmokers.
Ijerph 18 08717 g001
Figure 2. Future thinking priming produced larger change in delayed discounting rate than neutral priming among smokers.
Figure 2. Future thinking priming produced larger change in delayed discounting rate than neutral priming among smokers.
Ijerph 18 08717 g002
Table 1. Participant characteristics.
Table 1. Participant characteristics.
Characteristic/VariableRange, Level,
Category
Mean (SD) or Percent (N)χ2 or F
Nonsmokers
(n = 1199)
Smokers
(n = 333)
SociodemographicSexFemale55.96 (671)50.75 (169)χ2 = 2.859, p = 0.091
Age18–7435.40 (11.27)36.74 (11.201)F = 3.714, p = 0.054
RaceWhite81.81 (981)84.98 (284)χ2 = 3.361, p = 0.339
Black6.26 (75)6.31 (21)
Asian6.00 (72)3.60 (12)
Other5.92 (71)5.11 (17)
HispanicYes6.42 (77)6.91 (23)χ2 = 3.361, p = 0.751
Partner status *Partnered60.71 (728)51.65 (172)χ2 = 8.838, p = 0.003
Education in years *1–2815.81 (2.686)14.62 (2.611)F = 51.561, p < 0.001
Annual household income *<USD 24,99917.10 (205)28.53 (95)χ2 = 52.556, p < 0.001
USD 25,000–USD 49,99928.86 (346)35.74 (119)
USD 50,000–USD 74,99922.85 (274)22.52 (75)
USD 75,000–USD 99,99915.01 (180)5.71 (19)
>USD 100,00016.18 (194)7.51 (25)
Alcohol useNumber of drinks per week *0–702.73 (5.155)5.13 (8.049)F = 43.077, p < 0.001
PsychosocialSelf-monitoring scale0–2510.59 (4.598)11.01 (4.960)F = 2.157, p < 0.142
Time perspectiveFuture oriented66.31 (795)63.96 (213)χ2 = 0.635, p = 0.426
SSS *0–104.91 (1.839)4.17 (1.838)F = 41.979, p < 0.001
*p < 0.05. SSS is perceived social status.
Table 2. Moderating effect of smoking status on post-intervention delay discounting rate.
Table 2. Moderating effect of smoking status on post-intervention delay discounting rate.
Outcome MeasureFactors/VariablesBSEtp95% CI
LowerUpper
Post-intervention delay discounting rate Interaction + effectsnonsmokers−0.04820.0616−0.78360.4334−0.16900.0725
smokers−0.29190.1168−2.4990.0124−0.5209−0.0629
Constant −0.56310.1964−2.86720.0042−0.9483−0.1779
Condition−0.04860.0616−0.79010.4296−0.16940.0721
Smoking status 0.30770.09683.17770.00150.11770.4976
Baseline delay discounting rate0.85790.016352.64980.00000.82590.8898
Partnered status0.00950.05920.15980.8731−0.10660.1256
Education −0.01920.0106−1.80750.0709−0.04010.0016
Income−0.00760.0183−0.41390.6790−0.04360.0284
Alcohol (number of drinks per week)−0.00570.0046−1.22340.2214−0.01470.0034
Perceived social status (SSS)0.00390.01830.21250.8317−0.03200.0398
+ Interaction is the conditional effects of the condition type (FTP vs. NP) based on the moderator variable (nonsmokers vs. smokers).
Table 3. Moderating effect of smoking status on change in delay discounting score.
Table 3. Moderating effect of smoking status on change in delay discounting score.
Outcome MeasureFactors/VariablesbSEtP95% CI
LowerUpper
The difference between post-intervention and baseline delayed discounting rate Interaction + effectsnonsmokers−0.04500.0631−0.71340.4757−0.16870.0787
smokers−0.29750.1196−2.48720.0130−0.5321−0.0629
Constant 0.00040.19000.00200.9984−0.37230.3730
Condition −0.04500.0631−0.71340.4757−0.16870.0787
Smoking status 0.22110.09872.24100.02520.02760.4146
Partner 0.03070.06060.50600.6129−0.08820.1495
Education −0.00970.0108−0.89330.3718−0.03090.0116
Income0.00320.01880.17290.8628−0.03350.0400
Alcohol (number of drinks per week)−0.00570.0047−1.20650.2278−0.01500.0036
Perceived social status (SSS)0.00550.01880.29110.7710−0.03130.0423
+ Interaction is the conditional effects of the condition type (FTP vs. NP) based on the moderator variable levels (nonsmokers vs. smokers).
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Shevorykin, A.; Bickel, W.K.; Carl, E.; Sheffer, C.E. Future Thinking Priming Especially Effective at Modifying Delay Discounting Rates among Cigarette Smokers. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 8717. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18168717

AMA Style

Shevorykin A, Bickel WK, Carl E, Sheffer CE. Future Thinking Priming Especially Effective at Modifying Delay Discounting Rates among Cigarette Smokers. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(16):8717. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18168717

Chicago/Turabian Style

Shevorykin, Alina, Warren K. Bickel, Ellen Carl, and Christine E. Sheffer. 2021. "Future Thinking Priming Especially Effective at Modifying Delay Discounting Rates among Cigarette Smokers" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 16: 8717. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18168717

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop