Burnout and Cognitive Performance
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Burnout and Cognitive Functioning
3. Burnout, Depression, Anxiety and Cognitive Functioning
4. Burnout and Family Support
5. Objectives and Hypotheses of the Study
6. Materials and Methods
6.1. Ethics
6.2. Procedure and Participants
6.3. Assessment Tools
6.3.1. Burnout
6.3.2. Depression and Anxiety
6.3.3. Family Support
6.3.4. Assessment of Cognitive Functioning
7. Statistical Analyses
8. Results
8.1. Descriptive Characteristics
8.2. Correlation Analysis between Demographics, MBI-GS, HADS, FSS and Cognitive Performance
8.3. Cognitive Performance and Burnout
8.4. Burnout, Family Support, Depression and Anxiety
8.5. Cognitive Performance and Family Support
9. Discussion
10. Practical Implications
11. Limitations
12. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Cognitive Task | Description | Cognitive Domain |
---|---|---|
Taylor Complex Figure Test (TCFT) [97,98] | The TCFT consists of four phases in total; (1) copy; participants are asked to copy the complex figure, (2) immediate recall; participants are asked to draw by memory the complex figure, (3) delayed recall trial; after 30–45 min the participants are asked to draw again by memory the complex figure, and (4) recognition; the participants are asked to recognize parts of the complex figure’s items among disruptive ones. The maximum score for the three first conditions is 36, whereas the maximum score on the fourth condition is 24. The minimum score depends on the age range and the educational level of each participant. | Visuospatial Constructional Abilities & Visuospatial Memory |
Short Story [99] | This test consists of two phases; immediate and delayed recall trial. Firstly, a brief story is presented orally twice consecutively and then the participant is asked to recall it and retell it immediately after it was read. After approximately 20 min participants are asked to recall the story again (delayed recall trial). The story is divided in eight parts in total and the scoring depends by the participant’s ability to recall certain parts of the story of each of the eight parts as accurately as possible. Each parts chunk’s maximum score is two resulting to a total maximum score of 32 (combined score of the immediate recall trial) in the first condition and 16 in the second condition. The total score on the delayed recall trial is used for measuring the participant’s performance in episodic memory. | Learning & Episodic Memory |
Prospective Memory [16] | Self-made test; at the beginning of the examination the participants are instructed to remember asking the examiner the question: “What time is it?” after the completion of each task. The total score of the test is the total number of questions each participant asked this question after each cognitive task, resulting to a maximum score of 13. | Prospective Memory |
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV Digit Span (forward, backwards, ascending order) [100] | The participants firstly are asked to repeat ongoing sequences of numbers forward (a task of verbal short-term memory; a part of the working memory system), backwards and in an ascending order. A working memory index score is provided based on the performance in the above three conditions and is transformed in a standardized total score. | Short-term Memory, Verbal Working Memory |
Corsi Block-Tapping Span (backwards) [101] | In this test participants are shown an ongoing sequence of blocks and they are asked to recall each sequence backwards. | Visuospatial Working Memory |
Trail Making Test (TMT) (part A & B) [102] | Part A: the participants need to connect a sequence of numbers, as quickly as possible.Part B: the participants need to connect a sequence of numbers and letters while alternating between the two. The total time taken to complete each of the two tests is used as the primary performance metric. | Executive functions: visual attention & task switching |
Stroop Color-Word Test [103] | This consists of three conditions. Firstly, the participants are required to speedily read a list of color words, then name the color of a list of letters and, lastly, name the color of a list of color words printed in a different color as fast as possible within 45 s. | Executive Functions: Inhibition, Attention, Speed of processing |
Variable | n (%) |
---|---|
Age (mean, SD) | 40.40 (10.06) |
Range | 23–58 |
Males/Females | 24/80 (23.1/76.9) |
Education (mean, SD) | 16.82 (1.39) |
Range | 12–22 |
Sector | |
Public | 62 (59.6) |
Private | 42 (40.4) |
2nd Occupation | 22 (21.1) |
Years of Working Experience (mean, SD) | 15.20 (8.67) |
Range | 2–35 |
Family Status | |
Cohabitating | 8 (7.7) |
Married | 47 (45.2) |
In a relationship | 1 (1) |
Separated | 4 (3.8) |
Divorced | 3 (2.9) |
Single | 41 (39.4) |
Children | |
Yes/No | 40 (38.5)/64(61.5) |
HADS-Depression ≥ 8 (mean, SD) | 17 (10.70, 2.49) |
Range | 8–17 |
HADS-Anxiety ≥ 8 (mean, SD) | 39 (10.25, 2.44) |
Range | 8–18 |
Variable | Mean | SD | Exh. | CY | HADS-A | FSS | Short Story | Stroop-W | Digit Span-FW | Digit Span-Asc. |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Hours/week-main | 32.80 | 11.19 | 0.29 ** | 0.23 * | 0.20 * | 0.37 ** | 0.24 * | 0.00 | −0.08 | 0.17 |
Hours/week-second | 1.80 | 4.71 | 0.20 * | 0.24 * | −0.00 | −0.10 | −0.07 | −0.26 ** | −0.05 | −0.00 |
Hours/week-total | 34.60 | 12.38 | 0.36 ** | 0.19 * | 0.18 | 0.06 | 0.19 * | −0.10 | −0.10 | 0.21 * |
Sector | - | - | 0.23 * | 0.02 | −0.07 | −0.30 * | 0.05 | 0.13 | −0.04 | 0.09 |
Family Status | - | - | −0.17 | −0.02 | 0.06 | 0.23 * | 0.00 | 0.07 | 0.08 | 0.08 |
No of Children | 0.83 | 1.31 | −0.09 | −0.11 | −0.06 | −0.13 | −0.19 * | −0.05 | 0.21 * | −0.04 |
Variables | Mean | SD | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | 13 | 14 | 15 | 16 | 17 | 18 | 19 | 20 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. E | 2.89 | 1.54 | - | |||||||||||||||||||
2. CY | 2.08 | 1.30 | 0.42 | - | ||||||||||||||||||
3. PE | 5.05 | 0.96 | −0.09 | −0.31 | - | |||||||||||||||||
4. TCFT-C | 33.57 | 2.74 | 0.02 | −0.19 * | 0.17 | - | ||||||||||||||||
5. TCFT-IR | 25.31 | 5.22 | 0.02 | −0.16 | −0.03 | 0.09 | - | |||||||||||||||
6. TCFT-DR | 24.77 | 5.04 | −0.05 | −0.07 | 0.04 | 0.44 | 0.13 | - | ||||||||||||||
7. TCFT-Rec. | 19.27 | 2.15 | −0.07 | −0.09 | 0.00 | 0.21 * | 0.11 | 0.29 | - | |||||||||||||
8. Short Story-IR | 21.43 | 5.45 | 0.00 | 0.04 | 0.06 | 0.23 * | 0.00 | 0.14 | −0.03 | - | ||||||||||||
9. Short Story-DR | 11.69 | 2.92 | 0.10 | −0.01 | 0.10 | 0.37 | −0.02 | 0.27 | 0.08 | 0.74 | - | |||||||||||
10. Prosp.Mem | 8.88 | 3.93 | 0.02 | 0.02 | 0.10 | 0.05 | −0.20 * | 0.02 | −0.14 | 0.24 * | 0.17 | - | ||||||||||
11. STROOP-W | 104.08 | 15.49 | −0.02 | 0.19 * | 0.00 | −0.10 | 0.00 | −0.01 | −0.01 | 0.02 | −0.09 | 0.03 | - | |||||||||
12. STROOP-C | 74.24 | 10.23 | −0.03 | 0.02 | 0.07 | 0.09 | −0.08 | 0.05 | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.05 | 0.16 | 0.42 | - | ||||||||
13. STROOP-CW | 47.64 | 9.81 | −0.02 | −0.03 | 0.20 * | −0.09 | −0.12 | 0.07 | 0.06 | 0.15 | 0.13 | 0.07 | 0.30 | 0.42 | - | |||||||
14. STROOP-Int. | 4.47 | 9.96 | −0.00 | −0.10 | 0.19 | −0.07 | −0.08 | 0.04 | 0.03 | 20 * | 0.19* | 0.10 | −0.11 | 0.04 | 0.77 | - | ||||||
15. Digit Span-FW | 9.27 | 2.08 | 0.00 | −0.07 | −0.00 | −0.00 | −0.05 | −0.09 | 0.07 | −0.06 | 0.11 | 0.00 | −0.12 | −0.03 | 0.00 | 0.06 | - | |||||
16. Digit Span-Back. | 9.22 | 2.48 | 0.05 | −0.04 | 0.07 | 0.21 * | −0.02 | −0.19 * | 0.05 | 0.12 | 0.15 | 0.14 | 0.09 | 0.37 | 0.29 | 0.25 * | 0.22 * | - | ||||
17. Digit Span-Asc. | 9.86 | 2.50 | −0.01 | −0.09 | 0.01 | 0.27 | 0.07 | −0.30 | 0.16 | 0.21 * | 0.25 | 0.10 | −0.08 | 0.19 * | 0.06 | 0.14 | 0.15 | 0.41 | - | |||
18. Digit Span-tot. | 28.36 | 5.98 | −0.02 | −0.09 | 0.02 | 0.22 * | 0.02 | 0.23 * | 0.09 | 0.10 | 0.18 | 0.07 | −0.12 | 0.32 | 0.15 | 0.14 | 0.29 | 0.83 | 0.67 | - | ||
19. Corsi Span- back. | 28.43 | 5.52 | −0.11 | −0.06 | 0.01 | 0.19 * | −0.02 | 0.29 | 0.16 | 0.18 | 0.14 | 0.14 | 0.01 | 0.22 * | 0.32 | 0.31 | −0.01 | 0.46 | 0.26 | 0.37 | - | |
20. TMT-A | 38.68 | 17.30 | 0.03 | 0.04 | 0.04 | −0.01 | −0.02 | −0.13 | −0.18 | −0.12 | −0.03 | −0.04 | −0.27 | −0.13 | −0.27 | −0.17 | −0.12 | −0.13 | −0.01 | −0.03 | −0.19 | - |
21. TMT-B | 72.02 | 27.71 | 0.01 | 0.16 | 0.00 | −0.18 | −0.12 | −0.28 | −0.17 | −0.17 | −0.24 * | −0.00 | −0.15 | −0.31 | −0.25 | −0.19 * | −0.20 * | −0.37 * | −0.34 | −0.40 | −0.41 | 0.50 |
Model Path | b | SE B | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
Cynicism TCFT-copy | ||||
Constant | 0.00 [−0.19, 0.21] | 0.10 | 0.07 | 0.93 |
Cynicism (centered) | −0.15 [−0.30, −0.00] | 0.07 | −2.06 | 0.04 * |
Anxiety (centered) | 0.00 [−0.04, 0.05] | 0.02 | 0.25 | 0.79 |
Depression (centered) | −0.01 [−0.07, 0.05] | 0.03 | −0.32 | 0.74 |
Perceived Family Support (centered) | −0.00 [−0.02, 0.19] | 0.01 | −0.35 | 0.72 |
Cynicism × Anxiety | 0.03 [−0.00, 0.07] | 0.02 | 1.51 | 0.13 |
Cynicism × Depression | −0.00 [−0.05, 0.05] | 0.02 | −0.11 | 0.90 |
Cynicism × Perceived Family Support | 0.00 [−0.01, 0.01] | 0.00 | 0.03 | 0.96 |
Cynicism Stroop-W | ||||
Constant | 0.10 [−0.09, 0.30] | 0.10 | 1.00 | 0.31 |
Cynicism (centered) | 0.14 [−0.00, 0.28] | 0.72 | 1.96 | 0.051 |
Anxiety (centered) | 0.00 [−0.04, 0.30] | 0.02 | 0.03 | 0.97 |
Depression (centered) | 0.00 [−0.05, 0.06] | 0.03 | 0.01 | 0.98 |
Perceived Family Support (centered) | 0.00 [−0.02, 0.02] | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.99 |
Cynicism × Anxiety | 0.00 [−0.02, 0.02] | 0.12 | 0.00 | 0.64 |
Cynicism × Depression | −0.16 [−0.06, 0.03] | 0.26 | −0.60 | 0.54 |
Cynicism × Perceived Family Suport | −0.00 [−0.02, 0.01] | 0.02 | −0.59 | 0.55 |
Variables | Mean | SD | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. EX | 2.89 | 1.54 | (0.90) | |||||
2. CY | 2.08 | 1.30 | 0.42 ** | (.71) | ||||
3. PE | 5.05 | 0.96 | −0.09 | −0.31 ** | (0.84) | |||
4. HADS-D | 5.02 | 3.24 | 0.41 ** | 0.45 ** | −0.33 ** | (0.74) | ||
5. HADS-A | 6.33 | 3.82 | 0.41 ** | 0.45 ** | −0.25 ** | 0.80 ** | (0.84) | |
6. FSS | 51.40 | 8.88 | −0.24 * | −0.30 ** | 0.16 | −0.42 ** | −0.37 ** | (0.81) |
References
- Broadbent, D.E.; Cooper, P.F.; FitzGerald, P.; Parkes, K.R. The Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 1982, 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maslach, C.; Schaufeli, W.B.; Leiter, M.P. Job burnout. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2001, 52, 397–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schaufeli, W.; Enzmann, D. The Burnout Companion to Study and Practice: A Critical Analysis; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Eskildsen, A.; Andersen, L.P.; Pedersen, A.D.; Andersen, J.H. Cognitive impairments in former patients with work-related stress complaints–one year later. Stress 2016, 19, 559–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feuerhahn, N.; Stamov-Roßnagel, C.; Wolfram, M.; Bellingrath, S.; Kudielka, B.M. Emotional exhaustion and cognitive performance in apparently healthy teachers: A longitudinal multi-source study. Stress Health 2013, 29, 297–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oosterholt, B.G.; Linden, D.; Maes, J.H.; Verbraak, M.J.; Kompier, M.A. Burned out cognition-cognitive functioning of burnout patients before and after a period with psychological treatment. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 2012, 38, 358–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Oosterholt, B.G.; Maes, J.H.; van der Linden, D.; Verbraak, M.J.; Kompier, M.A. Getting better, but not well: A 1.5 year follow-up of cognitive performance and cortisol levels in clinical and non-clinical burnout. Biol. Psychol. 2016, 117, 89–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Österberg, K.; Skogsliden, S.; Karlson, B. Neuropsychological sequelae of work-stress-related exhaustion. Stress 2014, 17, 59–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stenlund, T.; Nordin, M.; Järvholm, L.S. Effects of rehabilitation programmes for patients on long-term sick leave for burnout: A 3-year follow-up of the REST study. J. Rehabil. Med. 2012, 44, 684–690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Deligkaris, P.; Panagopoulou, E.; Montgomery, A.J.; Masoura, E. Job burnout and cognitive functioning: A systematic review. Work Stress 2014, 28, 107–123. [Google Scholar]
- Miller, E.; Wallis, J. Executive function and higher-order cognition: Definition and neural substrates. Encycl. Neurosci. 2009, 4, 99–104. [Google Scholar]
- Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P. The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 21, 8–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van der Linden, D.; Keijsers, G.P.; Eling, P.; Schaijk, R.V. Work stress and attentional difficulties: An initial study on burnout and cognitive failures. Work Stress 2005, 19, 23–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Monsell, S. Control of mental processes. In Unsolved Mysteries of the Mind: Tutorial Essays in Cognition; Bruce, V., Ed.; Erlbaum: London, UK; Taylor & Francis: Abingdon, UK, 1996; pp. 93–148. [Google Scholar]
- Jonsdottir, I.; Nordlund, A.; Ellbin, S.; Ljung, T.; Glise, K.; Währborg, P.; Wallin, A. Cognitive impairment in patients with stress-related exhaustion. Stress 2013, 16, 181–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eskildsen, A.; Andersen, L.P.; Pedersen, A.D.; Vandborg, S.K.; Andersen, J.H. Work-related stress is associated with impaired neuropsychological test performance: A clinical cross-sectional study. Stress 2015, 18, 198–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jonsdottir, I.H.; Nordlund, A.; Ellbin, S.; Ljung, T.; Glise, K.; Währborg, P.; Sjörs, A.; Wallin, A. Working memory and attention are still impaired after three years in patients with stress-related exhaustion. Scand. J. Psychol. 2017, 58, 504–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Oosterholt, B.G.; Maes, J.H.; van der Linden, D.; Verbraak, M.J.; Kompier, M.A. Cognitive performance in both clinical and non-clinical burnout. Stress 2014, 17, 400–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Dam, A.; Keijsers, G.P.; Verbraak, M.J.; Eling, P.A.; Becker, E.S. Burnout patients primed with success did not perform better on a cognitive task than burnout patients primed with failure. Psychology 2012, 3, 583–589. [Google Scholar]
- Van Dam, A.; Keijsers, G.P.; Eling, P.A.; Becker, E.S. Testing whether reduced cognitive performance in burnout can be reversed by a motivational intervention. Work Stress 2011, 25, 257–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maslach, C.; Leiter, M.P. Understanding the burnout experience: Recent research and its implications for psychiatry. World Psychiatry 2016, 15, 103–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Herman, J.P.; Cullinan, W.E. Neurocircuitry of stress: Central control of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical axis. Trends Neurosci. 1997, 20, 78–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Belanoff, J.K.; Gross, K.; Yager, A.; Schatzberg, A.F. Corticosteroids and cognition. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2001, 35, 127–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marin, M.-F.; Lord, C.; Andrews, J.; Juster, R.-P.; Sindi, S.; Arsenault-Lapierre, G.; Fiocco, A.J.; Lupien, S.J. Chronic stress, cognitive functioning and mental health. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2011, 96, 583–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Durning, S.J.; Costanzo, M.; Artino Jr, A.R.; Dyrbye, L.N.; Beckman, T.J.; Schuwirth, L.; Holmboe, E.; Roy, M.J.; Wittich, C.M.; Lipner, R.S. Functional neuroimaging correlates of burnout among internal medicine residents and faculty members. Front. Psychiatry 2013, 4, 131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Liston, C.; McEwen, B.S.; Casey, B.J. Psychosocial stress reversibly disrupts prefrontal processing and attentional control. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2009, 106, 912–917. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Petrides, M.; Milner, B. Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia 1982, 20, 249–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Japee, S.; Holiday, K.; Satyshur, M.D.; Mukai, I.; Ungerleider, L.G. A role of right middle frontal gyrus in reorienting of attention: A case study. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2015, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Leech, R.; Sharp, D.J. The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain 2014, 137, 12–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Blix, E.; Perski, A.; Berglund, H.; Savic, I. Long-Term Occupational Stress Is Associated with Regional Reductions in Brain Tissue volumes. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e64065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Arnsten, A.F.T. Catecholamine modulation of prefrontal cortical cognitive function. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1998, 2, 436–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beversdorf, D.Q.; Hughes, J.D.; Steinberg, B.A.; Lewis, L.D.; Heilman, K.M. Noradrenergic modulation of cognitive flexibility in problem solving. NeuroReport 1999, 10, 2763–2767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Qin, S.; Hermans, E.J.; van Marle, H.J.; Luo, J.; Fernández, G. Acute psychological stress reduces working memory-related activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Biol. Psychiatry 2009, 66, 25–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sandström, A.; Rhodin, I.N.; Lundberg, M.; Olsson, T.; Nyberg, L. Impaired cognitive performance in patients with chronic burnout syndrome. Biol. Psychol. 2005, 69, 271–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- de Andrade, A.P.M.; Amaro, E., Jr.; Farhat, S.C.L.; Schvartsman, C. Higher burnout scores in paediatric residents are associated with increased brain activity during attentional functional magnetic resonance imaging task. Acta Paediatr. 2016, 105, 705–713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rydmark, I.; Wahlberg, K.; Ghatan, P.H.; Modell, S.; Nygren, Å.; Ingvar, M.; Åsberg, M.; Heilig, M. Neuroendocrine, cognitive and structural imaging characteristics of women on longterm sickleave with job stress-induced depression. Biol. Psychiatry 2006, 60, 867–873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Österberg, K.; Karlson, B.; Hansen, Å. Cognitive performance in patients with burnout, in relation to diurnal salivary cortisol: Original research report. Stress 2009, 12, 70–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McInerney, S.; Rowan, M.; Lawlor, B. Burnout and its effect on neurocognitive performance. Ir. J. Psychol. Med. 2012, 29, 176–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Castaneda, A.E.; Suvisaari, J.; Marttunen, M.; Perälä, J.; Saarni, S.I.; Aalto-Setälä, T.; Lönnqvist, J.; Tuulio-Henriksson, A. Cognitive functioning in relation to burnout symptoms and social and occupational functioning in a population-based sample of young adults. Nord. J. Psychiatry 2011, 65, 32–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, A.D. Working memory and reading. In Processing of Visible Language; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1979; pp. 355–370. [Google Scholar]
- Engle, R.W.; Cantor, J.; Carullo, J.J. Individual differences in working memory and comprehension: A test of four hypotheses. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1992, 18, 972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, A.D.; Logie, R.H. Working memory: The multiple-component model. In Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control; Miyake, A., Shah, P., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1999; pp. 28–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hallsten, L. Burning out: A framework. In Professional Burnout: Recent Developments in Theory and Research; Schaufeli, W.B., Maslach, C., Marek, T., Eds.; Taylor & Francis: London, UK, 1993; pp. 95–113. [Google Scholar]
- Williams, P.G.; Suchy, Y.; Rau, H.K. Individual differences in executive functioning: Implications for stress regulation. Ann. Behav. Med. 2009, 37, 126–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smeets, T.; Giesbrecht, T.; Jelicic, M.; Merckelbach, H. Context-dependent enhancement of declarative memory performance following acute psychosocial stress. Biol. Psychol. 2007, 76, 116–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Drummond, S.; O’Driscoll, M.P.; Brough, P.; Kalliath, T.; Siu, O.-L.; Timms, C.; Riley, D.; Sit, C.; Lo, D. The relationship of social support with well-being outcomes via work–family conflict: Moderating effects of gender, dependants and nationality. Hum. Relat. 2017, 70, 544–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhu, S.; Hu, J.; Efird, J.T. Role of social support in cognitive function among elders. J. Clin. Nurs. 2012, 21, 2118–2125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Koutsimani, P.; Montgomery, A.; Georganta, K. The relationship between burnout, depression and anxiety: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bianchi, R.; Schonfeld, I.S.; Laurent, E. Burnout-depression overlap: A review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2015, 36, 28–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Zhang, H.; Tang, L.; Ye, Z.; Zou, P.; Shao, J.; Wu, M.; Zhang, Q.; Qiao, G.; Mu, S. The role of social support and emotional exhaustion in the association between work-family conflict and anxiety symptoms among female medical staff: A moderated mediation model. BMC Psychiatry 2020, 20, 266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zhang, H.; Ye, Z.; Tang, L.; Zou, P.; Du, C.; Shao, J.; Wang, X.; Chen, D.; Qiao, G.; Mu, S.Y. Anxiety symptoms and burnout among chinese medical staff of intensive care unit: The moderating effect of social support. BMC Psychiatry 2020, 20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Airaksinen, E.; Larsson, M.; Forsell, Y. Neuropsychological functions in anxiety disorders in population-based samples: Evidence of episodic memory dysfunction. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2005, 39, 207–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DeLuca, A.K.; Lenze, E.J.; Mulsant, B.H.; Butters, M.A.; Karp, J.F.; Dew, M.A.; Pollock, B.G.; Shear, M.K.; Houck, P.R.; Reynolds, C.F., III. Comorbid anxiety disorder in late life depression: Association with memory decline over four years. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry J. Psychiatry Late Life Allied Sci. 2005, 20, 848–854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Basso, M.R.; Lowery, N.; Ghormley, C.; Combs, D.; Purdie, R.; Neel, J.; Davis, M.; Bornstein, R. Comorbid anxiety corresponds with neuropsychological dysfunction in unipolar depression. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 2007, 12, 437–456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eysenck, M.W.; Derakshan, N.; Santos, R.; Calvo, M.G. Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion 2007, 7, 336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Byers, A.L.; Covinsky, K.E.; Barnes, D.E.; Yaffe, K. Dysthymia and depression increase risk of dementia and mortality among older veterans. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2012, 20, 664–672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Peeters, M.C.; Montgomery, A.J.; Bakker, A.B.; Schaufeli, W.B. Balancing work and home: How job and home demands are related to burnout. Int. J. Stress Manag. 2005, 12, 43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rupert, P.A.; Stevanovic, P.; Hunley, H.A. Work-family conflict and burnout among practicing psychologists. Prof. Psychol. Res. Pract. 2009, 40, 54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beauregard, N.; Marchand, A.; Blanc, M.-E. What do we know about the non-work determinants of workers’ mental health? A systematic review of longitudinal studies. BMC Public Health 2011, 11, 439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Colichi, R.M.B.; Bocchi, S.C.M.; Lima, S.A.M.; Popim, R.C. Interactions between quality of life at work and family: Integrative review. Int. Arch. Med. 2017, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tselebis, A.; Anagnostopoulou, T.; Bratis, D.; Moulou, A.; Maria, A.; Sikaras, C.; Ilias, I.; Karkanias, A.; Moussas, G.; Tzanakis, N. The 13 item family support scale: Reliability and validity of the greek translation in a sample of greek health care professionals. Asia Pac. Fam. Med. 2011, 10, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hendricks, V.M.; Blanken, P. Snowball sampling: Theoretical and practical considerations. In Snowball Sampling: A Pilot Study on Cocaine Use; IVO: Rotterdam, The Netherlands, 1992; pp. 17–35. [Google Scholar]
- Maslach, C.; Jackson, S.E.; Leiter, M.P. Maslach Burnout Inventory; CPP: Pomona, CA, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Zigmond, A.S.; Snaith, R.P. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 1983, 67, 361–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pallant, J.F.; Tennant, A. An introduction to the rasch measurement model: An Example Using the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS). Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 2007, 46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Crawford, J.; Henry, J.; Crombie, C.; Taylor, E. Normative data for the HADS from a large non-clinical sample. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 2001, 40, 429–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Julkunen, J.; Greenglass, E. The Family Support Scale; York University: Toronto, ON, Canada, 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Benjamini, Y.; Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B Methodol. 1995, 57, 289–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Glickman, M.E.; Rao, S.R.; Schultz, M.R. False discovery rate control is a recommended alternative to bonferroni-type adjustments in health studies. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2014, 67, 850–857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Leiter, M.P.; Maslach, C. Latent burnout profiles: A new approach to understanding the burnout experience. Burn. Res. 2016, 3, 89–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Preacher, K.J.; Hayes, A.F. SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 2004, 36, 717–731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Ellbin, S.; Engen, N.; Jonsdottir, I.H.; Nordlund, A.I. Assessment of cognitive function in patients with stress-related exhaustion using the Cognitive Assessment Battery (CAB). J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2018, 40, 567–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krabbe, D.; Ellbin, S.; Nilsson, M.; Jonsdottir, I.H.; Samuelsson, H. Executive function and attention in patients with stress-related exhaustion: Perceived fatigue and effect of distraction. Stress 2017, 20, 333–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Diestel, S.; Cosmar, M.; Schmidt, K.-H. Burnout and impaired cognitive functioning: The role of executive control in the performance of cognitive tasks. Work Stress 2013, 27, 164–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Demerouti, E.; Bakker, A.B.; Nachreiner, F.; Schaufeli, W.B. The job demands-resources model of burnout. J. Appl. Psychol. 2001, 86, 499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hobfoll, S.E. Conservation of resources: A new attempt at conceptualizing stress. Am. Psychol. 1989, 44, 513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leiter, M.P.; Maslach, C. Six areas of worklife: A model of the organizational context of burnout. J. Health Hum. Serv. Adm. 1999, 21, 472–489. [Google Scholar]
- Demerouti, E.; Bakker, A.B.; Leiter, M. Burnout and job performance: The moderating role of selection, optimization, and compensation strategies. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2014, 19, 96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hockey, G.R.J. Compensatory control in the regulation of human performance under stress and high workload: A cognitive-energetical framework. Biol. Psychol. 1997, 45, 73–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morgan, C.A.; Russell, B.; McNeil, J.; Maxwell, J.; Snyder, P.J.; Southwick, S.M.; Pietrzak, R.H. Baseline burnout symptoms predict visuospatial executive function during survival school training in special operations military personnel. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2011, 17, 494–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ding, Y.; Qu, J.; Yu, X.; Wang, S. The mediating effects of burnout on the relationship between anxiety symptoms and occupational stress among community healthcare workers in China: A cross-sectional study. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e107130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Turnipseed, D.L. Anxiety and burnout in the health care work environment. Psychol. Rep. 1998, 82, 627–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Castaneda, A.E.; Tuulio-Henriksson, A.; Marttunen, M.; Suvisaari, J.; Lönnqvist, J. A review on cognitive impairments in depressive and anxiety disorders with a focus on young adults. J. Affect. Disord. 2008, 106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stordal, K.I.; Lundervold, A.J.; Egeland, J.; Mykletun, A.; Asbjørnsen, A.; Landrø, N.I.; Roness, A.; Rund, B.R.; Sundet, K.; Oedegaard, K.J. Impairment across executive functions in recurrent major depression. Nord. J. Psychiatry 2004, 58, 41–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chodosh, J.; Kado, D.M.; Seeman, T.E.; Karlamangla, A.S. Depressive symptoms as a predictor of cognitive decline: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2007, 15, 406–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Castaneda, A.E.; Marttunen, M.; Suvisaari, J.; Perälä, J.; Saarni, S.I.; Aalto-Setälä, T.; Aro, H.; Lönnqvist, J.; Tuulio-Henriksson, A. The effect of psychiatric co-morbidity on cognitive functioning in a population-based sample of depressed young adults. Psychol. Med. 2010, 40, 29–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Castaneda, A.E.; Suvisaari, J.; Marttunen, M.; Perälä, J.; Saarni, S.I.; Aalto-Setälä, T.; Lönnqvist, J.; Tuulio-Henriksson, A. Cognitive functioning in a population-based sample of young adults with anxiety disorders. Eur. Psychiatry 2011, 26, 346–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grant, M.M.; Thase, M.E.; Sweeney, J.A. Cognitive disturbance in outpatient depressed younger adults: Evidence of modest impairment. Biol. Psychiatry 2001, 50, 35–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ganguli, M. Depression, cognitive impairment and dementia: Why should clinicians care about the web of causation? Indian J. Psychiatry 2009, 51, S29–S34. [Google Scholar]
- Ganguli, M.; Du, Y.; Dodge, H.H.; Ratcliff, G.G.; Chang, C.-C.H. Depressive symptoms and cognitive decline in late life: A prospective epidemiological study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2006, 63, 153–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ahola, K.; Hakanen, J.; Perhoniemi, R.; Mutanen, P. Relationship between burnout and depressive symptoms: A study using the person-centred approach. Burn. Res. 2014, 1, 29–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bakusic, J.; Schaufeli, W.; Claes, S.; Godderis, L. Stress, burnout and depression: A systematic review on DNA methylation mechanisms. J. Psychosom. Res. 2017, 92, 34–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bakker, A.B.; Schaufeli, W.B.; Demerouti, E.; Janssen, P.P.; van der Hulst, R.; Brouwer, J. Using equity theory to examine the difference between burnout and depression. Int. J. 2000, 13, 247–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Toker, S.; Biron, M. Job burnout and depression: Unraveling their temporal relationship and considering the role of physical activity. J. Appl. Psychol. 2012, 97, 699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Naderifar, M.; Goli, H.; Ghaljaie, F. Snowball sampling: A purposeful method of sampling in qualitative research. SDMEJ 2017, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Chuang, C.-H.; Tseng, P.-C.; Lin, C.-Y.; Lin, K.-H.; Chen, Y.-Y. Burnout in the intensive care unit professionals: A systematic review. Medicine 2016, 95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hubley, A. Scoring System for the Modified Taylor Complex Figure (MTCF); University of Northern British Columbia: Prince George, BC, Canada, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Hubley, A. Quantified process scoring of complex figures. In The Quantified Process Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment; Swets & Zeitlinger: Lisse, The Netherlands, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Kosmidis, M.H.; Bozikas, V.; Vlahou, C.H.; Giaglis, G. Neuropsychological Battery; Aristotele University of Thessaloniki: Thessaloniki, Greece, 2011; Unpublished work. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler, D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 4th ed.; NCS Pearson: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2008; Volume 22, p. 498. [Google Scholar]
- Corsi, P. Memory and the Medial Temporal Region of the Brain. Ph.D. Thesis, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada, 1972. Unpublished work. [Google Scholar]
- Reitan, R.; Wolfson, D. The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Therapy and Clinical Interpretation; Tucson, A.Z., Ed.; Neuropsychological Press: Totowa, NJ, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Stroop, J.R. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 1935, 18, 643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Koutsimani, P.; Montgomery, A.; Masoura, E.; Panagopoulou, E. Burnout and Cognitive Performance. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 2145. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18042145
Koutsimani P, Montgomery A, Masoura E, Panagopoulou E. Burnout and Cognitive Performance. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(4):2145. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18042145
Chicago/Turabian StyleKoutsimani, Panagiota, Anthony Montgomery, Elvira Masoura, and Efharis Panagopoulou. 2021. "Burnout and Cognitive Performance" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 4: 2145. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18042145