Moral Reasoning about Aggressive Behavior in Relation to Type of Aggression, Age and Gender in South Korean Pupils
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Age Changes
1.2. Gender Differences
1.3. Moral Reasoning by Type of Aggression
1.4. Studies in South Korea
1.5. Present Study
1.6. Aims
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Measures
2.3. Coding of Open-Ended Questions
2.4. Statistical Analyses
2.5. Ethical Issues
3. Results
3.1. Harmfulness
3.2. Moral Judgment
3.3. Reason for Judgment
3.4. Causal Attribution
4. Discussion
4.1. Differences in Moral Reasoning by Type of Aggression
4.2. Differences in Moral Reasoning by Age
4.3. Differences in Moral Reasoning by Gender
5. Implications
Table Strengths and Limitations
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Helwig, C.C.; Turiel, E. Children’s social and moral reasoning. In The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development, 2nd ed.; Smith, P.K., Hart, C.H., Eds.; Wiley-Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2011; pp. 567–583. ISBN 9781444390933. [Google Scholar]
- Romera, E.M.; Casas, J.; Gómez-Ortiz, O.; Ortega-Ruiz, R. Moral domain as a risk and protective factor against bullying. An integrating perspective review on the complexity of morality. Aggress. Violent. Behav. 2019, 45, 75–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olweus, D. School bullying: Development and some important challenges. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2013, 9, 751–780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Greene, M.B. Bullying in schools: A plea for a measure of human rights. J. Soc. Issues 2006, 62, 63–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Monks, C.P.; Ortega-Ruiz, R.; Torrado Val, E. Unjustified aggression in preschool. Aggreg. Behav. 2002, 28, 458–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ey, L.-A.; Walker, S.; Spears, B. Young children’s thinking about bullying: Personal, social-conventional and moral reasoning perspectives. Australas. J. Early Child. 2019, 44, 196–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davidson, P.; Turiel, E.; Black, A. The effect of stimulus familiarity on the use of criteria and justifications in children’s social reasoning. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 1983, 1, 49–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turiel, E. The Development of Social Knowledge: Morality and Convention; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1983; ISBN 0521253098. [Google Scholar]
- Smith, P.K.; Talamelli, L.; Cowie, H.; Naylor, P.; Chauhan, P. Profiles of non-victims, escaped victims, continuing victims and new victims of school bullying. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2004, 74, 565–581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murray-Close, D.; Crick, N.R.; Galotti, K.M. Children’s moral reasoning regarding physical and relational aggression. Soc. Dev. 2006, 15, 345–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wright, M.F.; Yanagida, T.; Aoyama, I.; Dědková, L.; Li, Z.; Kamble, S.V.; Bayraktar, F.; Sevčiková, A.; Soudi, S.; Macháčková, H.; et al. Differences in attributions for public and private face-to-face and cyber victimization among adolescents in China, Cyprus, the Czech Republic, India, Japan and the United States. J. Genet. Psychol. 2017, 178, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kwak, K. Intervention programs in South Korea. In School Bullying in Different Cultures: Eastern and Western Perspectives; Smith, P.K., Kwak, K., Toda, Y., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2016; pp. 350–364. ISBN 9781139410878. [Google Scholar]
- Lee, S.; Smith, P.K.; Monks, C.P. Perception of bullying-like phenomena in South Korea: A qualitative approach from a lifespan perspective. J. Aggress. Confl. Peace. Res. 2011, 3, 210–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smetana, J.G.; Rote, W.M.; Jambon, M.; Tasopoulos-Chan, M.; Villalobos, M.; Comer, J. Developmental changes and individual differences in young children’s moral judgments. Child Dev. 2012, 83, 683–696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scholes, L.; Lunn Brownlee, J.; Walker, S.; Johansson, E. Changes in children’s reasoning about the social inclusion of aggressive children over the early years of elementary school. Int. J. Incl. Educ. 2017, 21, 991–1010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Helwig, C.C.; Hildebrandt, C.; Turiel, E. Children’s judgments about psychological harm in social context. Child Dev. 1995, 66, 1680–1693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nunner-Winkler, G.; Meyer-Nikele, M.; Wohlrab, D. Gender differences in moral motivation. Merrill-Palmer Q. 2007, 53, 26–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jaffee, S.; Hyde, J.S. Gender differences in moral orientation: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2000, 126, 703–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Park, Y.; Killen, M. When is peer rejection justifiable?: Children’s understanding across two cultures. Cogn. Dev. 2010, 25, 290–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Thornberg, R.; Pozzoli, T.; Gini, G.; Hong, J. Bullying and repeated conventional transgressions in Swedish schools: How do gender and bullying roles affect students’ conceptions? Psychol. Sch. 2017, 54, 1189–1201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Donenberg, G.R.; Hoffman, L.W. Gender differences in moral development. Sex Roles 1988, 18, 701–717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mulvey, K.L. Children’s reasoning about social exclusion: Balancing many factors. Child Dev. Perspect. 2016, 10, 22–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldstein, S.E.; Tisak, M.S.; Boxer, P. Preschoolers’ normative and prescriptive judgments about relational and overt aggression. Early Educ. Dev. 2002, 13, 23–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, P.K. Understanding School Bullying: Its Nature and Prevention Strategies; Sage: London, UK, 2014; ISBN 9781847879059. [Google Scholar]
- Goldstein, S.E.; Tisak, M.S. Adolescents’ social reasoning about relational aggression. J. Child. Fam. Stud. 2010, 19, 471–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Monks, C.P.; Robinson, S.; Worlidge, P. The emergence of cyberbullying: A survey of primary school pupils’ perceptions and experiences. Sch. Psychol. Int. 2012, 33, 477–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kowalski, R.M.; Giumetti, G.W.; Schroeder, A.N.; Lattanner, M.R. Bullying in the digital age: A critical review and meta-analysis of cyberbullying research among youth. Psychol. Bull. 2014, 140, 1073–1137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lee, S.; Smith, P.K.; Monks, C.P. Meaning and usage of a term for bullying-like phenomena in South Korea: A lifespan perspective. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 31, 342–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hofstede, G.; Hofstede, G.J.; Minkov, M. Cultures and Organizations: Software of the Mind, 3rd ed.; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 2010; ISBN 0071664181. [Google Scholar]
- Lau, Y.L.; Cameron, C.A.; Chieh, K.M.; O’Leary, J.; Fu, G.; Lee, K. Cultural differences in moral justifications enhance understanding of Chinese and Canadian children’s moral decisions. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 2012, 44, 461–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kwon, H. Analysis on relationships between types of participation in bullying and psychological factors of each type on elementary school students. Korean Elem. Moral Educ. Soc. 2013, 42, 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, B.; Choi, S. The effects of moral reasoning of early adolescents on the bullying: The moderating effects of adolescents peer conformity and the mediating effects of empathy. Korean J. Youth Stud. 2016, 23, 29–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, Y.; Yi, S. The moral judgment and justification reasoning in terms of aggressive behaviour by 3, 4 and 5 year olds: The relationship to children’s false belief understanding. Korean J. Child Stud. 2014, 35, 49–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Choi, J.; Heo, Y. Moral judgment and social interdependence according to perception and participant role in bullying situation. Korean J. Youth Stud. 2008, 15, 171–196. [Google Scholar]
- Shin, H.; Kim, S. Differences in attitudes toward bullying and moral disengagement among aggression clusters in middle school students. Korean J. Youth Stud. 2014, 21, 307–331. [Google Scholar]
- Lee, S.; Smith, P.K.; Monks, C.P. Participant roles in peer-victimization among young children in South Korea: Peer-, self-, and teacher-nominations. Aggress. Behav. 2016, 42, 287–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eslea, M.; Rees, J. At what age are children most likely to be bullied at school? Aggress. Behav. 2001, 27, 419–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, P.K.; Cowie, H.; Olafsson, R.F.; Liefooghe, A.P.; Almeida, A.; Araki, H.; del Barrio, C.; Costabile, A.; Dekleva, B.; Boundoumadi, A.; et al. Definitions of bullying: A comparison of terms used, and age and gender differences, in a fourteen–country international comparison. Child Dev. 2002, 73, 1119–1133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mayeux, L.; Underwood, M.; Risser, S. Perspectives on the ethics of sociometric research with children: How children, peers and teachers help to inform the debate. Merrill-Palmer Q. 2007, 53, 53–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wright, M.F. Cybervictims’ emotional responses, attributions, and coping strategies for cyber victimization: A qualitative approach. Safer Communities 2016, 15, 160–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krcmar, M.; Valkenburg, P.M. A scale to assess children’s moral interpretations of justified and unjustified violence and its relationship to television viewing. Commun. Res. 1999, 26, 608–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Monks, C.P.; Smith, P.K. Definitions of bullying: Age differences in understanding of the term, and the role of experience. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2006, 24, 801–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Graham, J.; Nosek, B.A.; Haidt, J.; Iyer, R.; Koleva, S.; Ditto, P.H. Mapping the moral domain. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 101, 366–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Turiel, E. The development of morality. In Child and Adolescent Development: An Advanced Course; Damon, W., Lerner, R.M., Eds.; Wiley: Hoboken, NK, USA, 2008; pp. 473–516. ISBN 9780470285770. [Google Scholar]
- Atari, M.; Lai, M.H.C.; Dehghani, M. Sex differences in moral judgments across 67 countries. Proc. R. Soc. B 2020, 287, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gilligan, C.; Ward, J.V.; Taylor, J.M.; Bardige, B. Mapping the Moral Domain: A Contribution of Women’s Thinking to Psychological Theory and Education; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1988; ISBN 9780674548329. [Google Scholar]
- Capraro, V.; Sippel, J. Gender differences in moral judgment and the evaluation of gender-specified moral agents. Cogn. Process. 2017, 18, 399–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Categories | Definition of Categories (with Examples) |
---|---|
1. Welfare | Appeals to victim’s physical, psychological harm, injury, loss, or negative affect (“The child got hurt”) |
2. Fairness | Appeals to maintaining a balance of rights between persons (“this is unfair because the child can’t defend himself if several children hit him/her”) |
3. Obligation | References to keeping rules or prohibition, or the act is bad itself (“We should not break other’s property” “We should be nice to friends” “Hitting is bad/wrong”) |
4. Authority and punishment avoidance | Appeals to the approval of specific authority and punishment of other persons (“My mum said this is bad” “The child would get arrested by policeman”) |
5. Peer relationships | Affects peer relationships between children (“Other classmates will get to know about the rumor”) |
6. Prudential reasons | Affects personal health, safety, or comfort (“If the child says bad words, he will get used to it” “the child will get a bad habit”) |
7. Intention | Intention of act (“It is OK if the child broke the other’s pencil by mistake”). |
8. Dismiss | Regard it as not serious or important, or as easy to solve (“It’s OK, you can buy one later”) |
9. Don’t know | “I don’t know” or child did not answer |
Categories | Definition of Categories (with Examples) |
---|---|
Aggressor related | Personal satisfaction: For fun, boredom, wanting to annoy others |
Characteristic problem: Venting one’s anger, obtaining other’s attention | |
Practical reasons: Obtaining resources (“The child is extorting money”, “The child is jealous of the other’s goods”) Disliking victim: Aggressor dislikes a victim or child but did not explain the reason for disliking | |
Victim related | Provocative victim: Victim provoked first |
Victim characteristics: Caused by victim’s oddness (“The child is ugly and odd”), characteristic problem (“The child is selfish, not kind”), competence (“The child is bad at play”), wang-ta (“The child is wang-ta”) | |
Situational factor | Attributed to situational factor, rather than to bully or to victim (“There is no room”, “There would be misunderstanding among these children”, “They fought”) |
Don’t know | “I don’t know” or child did not answer |
Type of Aggression | Age | Gender | Total | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
6 y (n = 61) | 11 y (n = 95) | Boy (n = 86) | Girl (n = 71) | N = 156 | |
Verbal | 3.66 (0.77) | 3.45 (0.84) | 3.48 (0.89) | 3.59 (0.73) | 3.53 (0.82) |
Phys. Ind | 3.79 (0.49) | 3.61 (0.70) | 3.63 (0.70) | 3.75 (0.53) | 3.68 (0.63) |
Soc. Excl | 3.49 (0.85) | 3.35 (0.82) | 3.31 (0.90) | 3.52 (0.73) | 3.41 (0.83) |
Rumor | 3.52 (0.79) | 3.60 (0.69) | 3.52 (0.79) | 3.63 (0.64) | 3.57 (0.73) |
Phys. Grp | 3.87 (0.39) | 3.90 (0.34) | 3.87 (0.37) | 3.90 (0.35) | 3.89 (0.36) |
Breaking | 3.72 (0.61) ** | 3.33 (0.71) | 3.41 (0.71) | 3.58 (0.67) | 3.48 (0.70) |
Mobile | 3.70 (0.72) | 3.44 (0.86) | 3.49 (0.88) | 3.61 (0.73) | 3.54 (0.81) |
3.85 (0.40) ** | 3.56 (0.66) | 3.63 (0.63) | 3.73 (0.53) | 3.68 (0.60) | |
Average | 3.71 (0.05) | 3.54 (0.04) | 3.56 (0.04) | 3.68 (0.04) |
Type of Aggression | Age | Gender | Total | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
6 y (n = 61) | 11 y (n = 95) | Boy (n = 86) | Girl (n = 71) | N = 156 | |
Verbal | 3.82 (0.50) | 3.67 (0.53) | 3.69 (0.60) | 3.80 (0.40) | 3.74 (0.52) |
Phys. Ind | 3.79 (0.58) | 3.70 (0.58) | 3.71 (0.63) | 3.76 (0.52)_ | 3.73 (0.52) |
Soc. Excl | 3.64 (0.68) | 3.42 (0.63) | 3.44 (0.70) | 3.58 (0.60) | 3.50 (0.66) |
Rumor | 3.66 (0.66) | 3.79 (0.43) | 3.69 (0.62) | 3.80 (0.40) | 3.74 (0.53) |
Phys. Grp | 3.90 (0.35) | 3.98 (0.14) | 3.93 (0.30) | 3.97 (0.17) | 3.95 (0.25) |
Breaking | 3.82 (0.43) | 3.54 (0.63) | 3.59 (0.66) | 3.72 (0.45) | 3.65 (0.58) |
Mobile | 3.77 (0.56) | 3.66 (0.54) | 3.69 (0.58) | 3.72 (0.51) | 3.70 (0.55) |
3.85 (0.40) | 3.65 (0.56) | 3.70 (0.58) | 3.76 (0.43) | 3.73 (0.51) | |
Average | 3.78 (0.04) | 3.68 (0.03) | 3.69 (0.03) | 3.78 (0.03) | 3.72 (0.28) |
Type of Aggression | Welfare | Fairness | Obligation | Don’t Know | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Verbal | Total | 38.9 | 0.6 | 36.9 | 17.8 |
Age (6/11) | 9.8/57.3 ** | 0/1 | 54.1/26.0 ** | 29.7/11.5 * | |
Gender (B/G) | 34.9/43.7 | 0/1.4 | 36.0/38.0 | 20.9/14.1 | |
Phys. Ind | Total | 26.1 | 1.3 | 44.6 | 20.4 |
Age (6/11) | 16.4/32.3 | 0/2 | 54.1/38.5 | 26.2/16.7 | |
Gender (B/G) | 20.9/32.4 | 1.2/1 | 46.5/42.3 | 24.4/15.5 | |
Soc. Excl | Total | 14.0 | 15.3 | 41.4 | 21.0 |
Age (6/11) | 11.5/15.6 | 8.2/19.8 | 49.2/36.5 | 27.9/16.7 | |
Gender (B/G) | 12.8/15.5 | 14.0/16.9 | 45.3/36.6 | 19.8/22.5 | |
Rumor | Total | 29.9 | 4.5 | 27.4 | 29.3 |
Age (6/11) | 14.8/39.6 * | 0/7.3 | 37.7/20.8 | 42.6/20.8 * | |
Gender (B/G) | 26.7/33.8 | 3.5/5.6 | 27.9/26.8 | 31.4/26.8 | |
Phys. Grp | Total | 14.6 | 49.7 | 21.7 | 11.5 |
Age (6/11) | 11.5/16.7 | 32.8/60.4 * | 31.1/15.6 | 23.0/4.2 * | |
Gender (B/G) | 5.8/25.4 ** | 60.5/36.6 ** | 18.6/25.4 | 11.6/11.3 | |
Break | Total | 19.7 | 3.2 | 46.5 | 15.3 |
Age (6/11) | 13.1/24.0 | 0/5.2 | 50.8/43.8 | 24.6/9.4 * | |
Gender (B/G) | 19.8/19.7 | 4.7/1.4 | 41.9/52.1 | 16.3/14.1 | |
Mobile | Total | 27.4 | 3.2 | 28.0 | 28.7 |
Age (6/11) | 13.1/36.5 | 0/5.2 | 39.3/20.8 | 39.3/21.9 * | |
Gender (B/G) | 18.6/38.0 ** | 3.5/2.8 | 30.2/25.4 | 32.6/23.9 | |
Total | 28.7 | 6.4 | 20.4 | 33.1 | |
Age (6/11) | 11.5/39.6 ** | 0/11.5 | 29.5/14.6 | 49.2/22.9 | |
Gender (B/G) | 22.1/36.6 | 7.0/5.6 | 25.6/14.1 | 32.6/33.8 | |
Average | Total Age (6/11) Gender (B/G) | 24.9 12.7/32.7 20.2/30.6 | 10.5 5.1/14.1 11.8/8.9 | 33.3 43.2/27.1 34.0/32.6 | 22.1 32.8/15.5 23.7/20.3 |
Type of Aggression | Aggressor Related | Victim Related | Situational | Don’t Know | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Verbal | Total | 43.3 | 19.1 | 7.6 | 29.9 |
Age (6/11) | 34.4/49.0 | 16.4/28.1 | 1.6/12.5 | 47.5/18.8 ** | |
Gender (B/G) | 40.7/46.5 | 22.1/15.5 | 7.0/8.5 | 30.2/29.6 | |
Phys. Ind | Total | 40.1 | 26.8 | 10.2 | 22.9 |
Age (6/11) | 29.5/46.9 | 32.8/33.3 | 4.9/13.5 | 36.1/14.6 * | |
Gender (B/G) | 36.0/45.1 | 24.4/29.6 | 15.1/4.2 | 24.4/21.1 | |
Soc. Excl | Total | 25.5 | 40.1 | 13.4 | 21.0 |
Age (6/11) | 21.3/28.1 | 31.1/46.9 | 16.8/13.5 | 34.1/12.5 ** | |
Gender (B/G) | 19.8/32.4 | 41.9/38.0 | 16.3/9.9 | 22.1/19.7 | |
Rumor | Total | 38.9 | 26.1 | 10.8 | 24.2 |
Age (6/11) | 24.6/47.9 * | 27.9/30.2 | 4.9/3.5 | 42.6/12.5 ** | |
Gender (B/G) | 33.7/451 | 26.7/25.4 | 10.5/11.3 | 29.1/18.3 | |
Phys. Grp | Total | 35.0 | 35.7 | 4.5 | 24.8 |
Age (6/11) | 21.3/43.8 * | 32.8/42.7 | 4.9/4.2 | 41.0/14.6 ** | |
Gender (B/G) | 33.7/36.6 | 40.7/29.6 | 1.2/8.5 | 24.4/25.4 | |
Break | Total | 46.5 | 19.7 | 5.1 | 28.7 |
Age (6/11) | 27.9/58.3 ** | 19.7/18.8 | 8.2/11.5 | 42.6/19.8 ** | |
Gender (B/G) | 44.2/49.3 | 15.1/25.4 | 7.0/2.8 | 33.7/45.2 | |
Mobile | Total | 60.5 | 13.4 | 4.5 | 21.7 |
Age (6/11) | 37.7/75.0 ** | 23.0/15.6 | 3.3/2.1 | 37.7/11.5 ** | |
Gender (B/G) | 65.1/54.9 | 11.6/15.5 | 2.3/7.0 | 20.9/22.5 | |
Total | 59.2 | 15.9 | 2.5 | 22.3 | |
Age (6/11) | 37.7/72.9 ** | 18.0/18.8 | 3.3/3.1 | 39.3/11.5 ** | |
Gender (B/G) | 64.0/53.5 | 10.5/22.5 | 1.2/4.2 | 24.4/19.7 | |
Average | Total Age (6/11) Gender (B/G) | 43.6 29.3/52.7 42.2/45.4 | 24.6 25.4/24.1 24.1/25.1 | 7.3 5.1/8.7 7.6/7.0 | 24.4 40.2/14.5 26.2/22.4 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Lee, S.-H.; Smith, P.K.; Monks, C.P. Moral Reasoning about Aggressive Behavior in Relation to Type of Aggression, Age and Gender in South Korean Pupils. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 2288. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18052288
Lee S-H, Smith PK, Monks CP. Moral Reasoning about Aggressive Behavior in Relation to Type of Aggression, Age and Gender in South Korean Pupils. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(5):2288. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18052288
Chicago/Turabian StyleLee, Seung-Ha, Peter K. Smith, and Claire P. Monks. 2021. "Moral Reasoning about Aggressive Behavior in Relation to Type of Aggression, Age and Gender in South Korean Pupils" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 5: 2288. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18052288
APA StyleLee, S. -H., Smith, P. K., & Monks, C. P. (2021). Moral Reasoning about Aggressive Behavior in Relation to Type of Aggression, Age and Gender in South Korean Pupils. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(5), 2288. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18052288